Abstract
Background:
Unipolar depression is the third highest contributor to the global burden of disease, yet current pharmacotherapies typically take about 6 weeks to have an effect. A rapid-onset agent is an attractive prospect, not only to alleviate symptoms before first-line antidepressants display therapeutic action, but as a further treatment option in nonresponsive cases. It has been suggested that 3,4-methylene-dioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) could play a part in the treatment of depression, either as a rapid-onset pharmacological agent or as an adjunct to psychotherapy. Whilst these hypotheses are in keeping with the monoamine theory of depression and the principles surrounding psychotherapy, explicit experimental evidence of an antidepressant effect of MDMA has rarely been established.
Aims:
To address the hypothesis surrounding MDMA as a rapid-onset antidepressant by examining pharmacological, psychological and behavioural studies. We consider whether this therapy could be safe by looking at the translation of neurotoxicity data from animals to humans.
Method:
A literature review of the evidence supporting this hypothesis was performed.
Conclusions:
The pharmacology of MDMA offers a promising target as a rapid-onset agent and MDMA is currently being investigated for use in psychotherapy in anxiety disorders; translation from these studies for use in depression may be possible. However, experimental evidence and safety analysis are insufficient to confirm or reject this theory at present.
Keywords: MDMA, monoamine theory, mood disorder, psychotherapy, rapid-onset antidepressant
Introduction
3,4-Methylene-dioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), the active component of ‘ecstasy’, is a recreational drug which has recently been proposed to exhibit antidepressant properties [Riedlinger and Montagne, 2001; Sessa and Nutt, 2007]. This postulate is based on the acute pharmacology of MDMA and is consistent with the hallmark theory of depression, the monoamine hypothesis [Krishnan and Nestler, 2008]. MDMA rapidly increases availability of extracellular 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) at the synapse, mirroring the action of commonly prescribed antidepressants. However, current first-line treatments for depression, such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), typically take about 6 weeks to produce optimum therapeutic change [Frazer and Benmansour, 2002]; MDMA could offer instantaneous relief. This rapid onset is an attractive prospect for treatment-resistant depression (TRD), where currently the only therapeutic option is electroconvulsive therapy (ECT). Although there is a theoretical basis, at present experimental evidence of antidepressant action of MDMA is low, with just one rodent [Majumder et al. 2011] and one volunteer study [Majumder et al. 2012] suggesting direct rapid-onset antidepressant action.
MDMA is a controlled substance in most countries, typically in the most restrictive category (Class A in the UK). Despite this, MDMA is already in clinical trials for anxiety disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), as an adjunct to psychotherapy where it is thought to assist therapy sessions by strengthening the therapeutic relationships between patient and therapist [MAPS, 2015; Mithoefer et al. 2011, 2013; Oehen et al. 2013]. Psychotherapy, in the form of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), is one of the most effective treatments for depression, indicating that the translation of MDMA-assisted therapy to depression may also be beneficial.
To date, experiments using MDMA have been of limited scope, in part due to its Class A scheduling, as well as concerns over its safety and neurotoxicity profile. Through recreational use, it has garnered a reputation for being dangerous in the media, with reports focused on the rare deaths of young adults; and in the scientific literature through demonstration of neurotoxicity in animals. In humans, MDMA overdose can produce hyperthermia, with subsequent muscle break down resulting in hyperkalaemia and organ failure [Carvalho et al. 2012]. However, the doses required for these effects are excessive and thus reports of death solely from MDMA overdose are scarce; MDMA is more commonly linked to death as a contributing factor in a polydrug environment [Kaye et al. 2009]. To date, there are no published studies demonstrating neurotoxicity of repeated low dose MDMA in drug-naïve volunteers; indeed this regimen is in use in PTSD trials and has been postulated to have antidepressant activity [Sessa and Nutt, 2007].
Animal studies highlight physiological and psychological changes that occur after MDMA exposure, but these studies are limited, employing aggressive dose regimes over intense time periods that do not reflect patterns of human recreational use or those utilized in clinical trials [Mithoefer et al. 2011, 2013; Oehen et al. 2013]. Data from recreational users are also inadequate, restricted by retrospective design that cannot rule out premorbid differences, as well as reliance on self-reported variables of time and dose. Additionally, such data are confounded by polydrug use and ambiguity over whether ‘ecstasy’ tablets contain MDMA; a recent government report states that ‘most ecstasy tablets sold in the UK do not contain MDMA [Jones et al. 2011]. If MDMA is to be assessed for its antidepressant properties, there is an urgent need to eliminate equivocal data, as interpretation and relevant translation to patient populations is currently impossible. In this article we examine the conclusions of both animal and human literature to assess whether MDMA could prove a safe and effective therapy for TRD.
MDMA pharmacology
Positive indications
MDMA is an amphetamine derivative, capable of crossing the blood–brain barrier to the central nervous system, where its major substrates are the vesicular and presynaptic monoamine transporters [Elliott and Beveridge, 2005]. Early work identifying MDMA substrates, using post mortem brain material, or in vitro tissue samples, concluded that MDMA acts to reverse transport across these proteins, inducing a rapid, dose-dependent, nonexocytic monoamine release into the synapse [Schmidt, 1987; Rudnick and Wall, 1992; Rothman and Baumann, 2002]. These finding have been recapitulated in conscious rats [Baumann et al. 2005].
Of the monoamines, MDMA has highest efficacy for 5-HT release via the serotonin reuptake transporter (SERT) [Steele et al. 1987; Elliott and Beveridge, 2005]. Thus, MDMA acts to increase 5-HT availability in the acute setting, as SSRIs do chronically. Indeed, many of MDMA’s effects are abolished in SERT-deficient mice [Bengel et al. 1998; Fox et al. 2007], as well as by SSRIs, which act as competitive antagonists to SERT [Schmidt and Taylor, 1987]. In humans, SSRIs also diminish the effects of MDMA [Liechti and Vollenweider, 2000], an important consideration when thinking about use as a rapid-onset antidepressant or augmentation therapy. That is, giving MDMA in conjunction with commonly used antidepressants may prevent the therapeutic effects of MDMA, due to substrate competition [Hysek et al. 2012]. In addition to monoamine modulation itself, MDMA-induced 5-HT release acts to increase oxytocin in humans [Dumont et al. 2009]. Oxytocin characteristically precipitates prosocial behaviours and an improved ability to infer the mental state of others [Domes et al. 2007]; this has the potential to offer a gateway to successful psychotherapy.
Negative indications
Following acute MDMA-induced 5-HT release from serotonergic terminals, in conjunction with inhibition of 5-HT reuptake, marked depletions in 5-HT and its metabolite, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), have been reported in post mortem brain tissue of animals and humans [Schmidt et al. 1986], as well as in vivo from cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) measurements [Ricaurte et al. 1990; Taffe et al. 2003]. This does not bode well for use as an antidepressant following the monoamine theory of depression. Yet, these data are not definitive; rodent studies drawing such conclusions have used single doses, more than 10 times greater than an average recreational dose [Schmidt et al. 1986] or multiple high doses in rapid succession [Wallinga et al. 2011]. Primate data follow similar regimes [Ali et al. 1993].
In order to make measurements of CSF markers relevant, recreational doses need to be paralleled, as evidence suggests there is no need for interspecies scaling [Baumann et al. 2007]. Studies investigating CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in recreational users [Ricaurte et al. 1990; McCann et al. 1994] have consistently reported decreased levels of the metabolite, repeating animal literature, but these demonstrations come from polydrug users with unknown dosing. Tryptophan hydroxylase (Tph) activity also diminishes post-MDMA exposure [Bonkale and Austin, 2008, Schenk et al. 2013], although these measurements have rarely exceeded 2 weeks in studies. Furthermore, a recent rodent study found that diminished Tph after MDMA administration is transient and can recover in all brain regions [Adori et al. 2011].
Human data on changes in Tph typically come from post mortem tissues and these studies lack statistical power. One case report demonstrates that Tph was severely decreased in an autopsied brain of a ‘chronic’ MDMA user. Yet, this report also indicates that the subject took MDMA 4–5 nights per week, for 3 years prior to death and, on the weekend, would ingest 6–8 tablets [Kish et al. 2010]. Hair analysis further confirmed chronic cocaine use in this subject. Thus, the results represent an atypical pattern of heavy use, which would be unlikely to given therapeutically and are confused by polydrug use.
Thus, although current studies are heavily confounded, the evidence currently highlights a discrepancy between the acute and chronic pharmacology of MDMA. Acutely, MDMA works to increase 5-HT availability, suggestive of rapid-onset antidepressant properties and positive changes to emotion, but this transient effect may be accompanied by later depletions of 5-HT. However, studies examining long-term effects are inadequate and thus it cannot be concluded that MDMA significantly depletes 5-HT stores at doses that would be used therapeutically.
MDMA neurotoxicity
Reduced levels of 5-HT and its metabolites in brain tissue and the CSF have been interpreted to indicate that MDMA is neurotoxic, although definitive data must come from histological validation of neurodegeneration [Kaye et al. 2009]. To assess this in vivo, reduced SERT density is thought to represent neurotoxicity [Urban et al. 2012]. Incidentally, a low SERT density is also associated with depression [van Eijndhoven et al. 2011]. In humans, such investigations use imaging techniques such as positron emission tomography (PET), which rely on radioligands to label SERT. Currently, [11C] 3-amino-4-(2-dimethylaminomethyl-phenylsulfanyl)benzonitrile (DASB) is considered the most specific ligand [Frankle et al. 2004].
[11C]DASB studies on recreational users indicate global SERT depletion, in part, positively correlated with MDMA consumption, but also that SERT densities have a capacity to recover, at least in subcortical regions [McCann et al. 2005]. These results are notable but incomplete because they were obtained retrospectively. Attempts to overcome these limitations include recruitment of ‘chronic’ users, in the hope that they have had at least some exposure to MDMA, and hair analysis, allowing levels of MDMA to be verified up to approximately 3 months prior to testing. For example, in one study comparing SERT levels between MDMA users and MDMA-naïve controls, hair analyses confirmed the subjects’ drug history and ‘chronic’ MDMA users were defined as taking the substance for a mean of 4 years, taking 1–2 tablets every 2 months [Kish et al. 2010]. The authors found that SERT binding was significantly lower in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus, and correlated to the time and dose of drug use. Converging on similar conclusions, one of the most recent [11C]DASB studies found decreased SERT binding in cortical regions in MDMA users [Urban et al. 2012]. Depletion was correlated with increases in 5-HT2A receptor availability, a postsynaptic 5-HT receptor known to upregulate in response to chronic 5-HT depletion, suggesting that the brain is adapting to insult.
Other studies have extended the 5-HT2A data, repeating the findings that 5-HT2A receptors are upregulated in MDMA users [Benningfield and Cowan, 2013]. However, the perceived receptor deficiencies cannot be attributed to MDMA alone in these subjects. Hence, it is impossible to determine what dose of MDMA alters serotonergic function, accompanied by neurotoxicity, post exposure. Furthermore, behavioural and biochemical analysis must be coupled to determine whether these changes have a detrimental effect on MDMA users. If no significant behavioural effects accompany these changes, perhaps we need to question whether these changes are a problem. To address this, a prospective volunteer study that can be tightly regulated is desirable to examine both biochemical and behavioural differences after low-dose MDMA.
Despite MDMA passing safety measures for its use in clinical trials of PTSD, we believe that until the discrepancies in neurotoxicity data are resolved, it is unlikely that MDMA will be explored as a rapid-onset antidepressant because of the emphasis in both depression pathophysiology and MDMA neurotoxicity on 5-HT. MDMA is the second most popular illicit substance in Europe, peaking in popularity in the 1980s, and thus if MDMA use posed a chronic, detrimental effect, a resultant epidemic should emerge shortly. As of yet this does not appear to have surfaced and thus it might be hypothesized to be unlikely that MDMA has a noticeable lasting effect.
Psychological effects of MDMA
Animal studies
Animal studies carried out to address the psychological impacts of MDMA are often nontranslatable to therapeutic settings because of limitations outlined previously. Following a 10mg/kg dose of MDMA for 10 days in rats, measures of anxiety-like behaviours, such as open-field ambulation, indicate an increase in anxious phenotypes 3 months later [Kolyaduke and Hughes, 2013]. A dose of 5mg/kg of MDMA given to rats 4 times in 4 hours, on 2 consecutive days, diminished responses on the forced swim test and increased immobility up to 12 weeks post-MDMA exposure, indicating long-term negative behavioural changes [Thompson et al. 2004]. This study also analysed levels of 5-HT and 5-HIAA post mortem, with both being decreased in cortical areas of MDMA-treated rats. This can be interpreted as MDMA-induced chronic depletion of 5-HT, leading to anxious or depressed phenotypes. However, aggressive dosing was employed; these effects may not be seen with a modest dosing regimen.
Primate data show that, although CSF depletions of up to 93% in 5-HT and around 50% 5-HIAA are seen up to 1 year after a brief, high-dose MDMA regime [Domes et al. 2007], no behavioural changes, such as memory and reaction time, accompany these apparently drastic reductions. However, it is unfortunate, for the purposes of this debate, that this study did not also assess mood.
Human data
Numerous studies have compiled data from recreational users to investigate the relationship between MDMA and mood in humans. In one self-reported study, it was stated that the feeling of depression post-MDMA exposure in a cohort of 20 ‘ecstasy’ users was 55% [Davison and Parrott, 1997]. In a larger study, 52 frequent ‘ecstasy’ users were questioned, and the prevalence of depression reported was just 4% [De Almeida and Silva, 2003]. It is difficult to determine which figure is more representative as both studies used retrospective designs lacking biochemical analysis to confirm MDMA or other drug use. Furthermore, in humans, there is an argument that the environment in which MDMA is taken, rather than MDMA itself, causes negative changes to long-term mood, a factor that these self-reported studies do not consider. Indeed, it is suggested that reduced quality of sleep following recreational MDMA use, rather than the use itself, can lead to depression, anxiety and aggression [Scott et al. 2013].
Prospective studies have also been conducted, attempting to clarify whether MDMA use precipitates depression, or whether the drug is used recreationally as self-medication. One such study investigated behavioural outcomes of MDMA use in an initially MDMA-naïve population who were likely to use MDMA in the near future [De Win et al. 2006]. These volunteers underwent a number of behavioural tests over a mean period of 16 months, with data suggesting that premorbid depression did not predict MDMA use. In addition, this study found that depression post-MDMA was not significantly increased. However, these findings are not consistent across the prospective study literature [Lieb et al. 2002; Huizink et al. 2006; Briere et al. 2012].
A large Dutch population study explored the relationship between behavioural traits exhibited by school children and their future MDMA use, assessed 14 years later via questionnaire [Huizink et al. 2006]. The work found that the children who had fulfilled the criteria for anxious or depressed childhood behaviours were significantly more likely to used MDMA as they grew up and into young adulthood. A 4-year prospective study of 14–24 year olds from Munich demonstrated that ecstasy users had a 3-fold odds increase in the presence of mental disorders compared with nondrug users and a 2-fold odds increase compared with non-ecstasy users [Lieb et al. 2002]. As a prospective study the temporal nature of this relationship was investigated and it was demonstrated that, in 80.4% of ecstasy users with mental disorders, the presence of mental illness predated ecstasy use. Major depressive disorder was associated with increased ecstasy use and, within the cohort of ecstasy users with comorbid depression, the diagnosis of depression predated ecstasy use in 44.5%. In both these studies the authors proposed that these data were evidence that mental illness leads to increased rates of ecstasy use, in fitting with a hypothesis of self-medication. Differences between prospective studies still leave ambiguity, likely due to confounding factors such as polydrug use, dose and compound uncertainty.
Research into MDMA and its biochemical and/or behavioural effects on healthy volunteers now has considerable statistical power, with about 900 people having partaken in clinical trials. Kirkpatrick and colleagues undertook an analysis of data of the effects of MDMA across three different laboratories, with 220 healthy volun-teers [Kirkpatrick et al. 2014a]. They demonstrated consistent subjective effects, as well as physical changes. The subjective effects were assessed using visual analogue scales and covered perception of a drug effect, liking of drug effect, closeness to others and feelings of anxiety. Significant positive shifts were demonstrated on each of these factors. Physical changes related to an increase in both heart rate and blood pressure, resulting in a significant cardiovascular profile, which may prove problematic for patients in older age and those with pre-existing medical health problems. The overarching conclusion of this work is that the measured subjective and physical effects of MDMA are reproducible, irrelevant of population, demographic and experimental methodology.
In 1998, Vollenweider and colleagues safely conducted the first trial examining the effects of MDMA on nonpatient volunteers [Vollenweider et al. 1998]. Recently, a number of studies have furthered this work to characterize the acute effects of MDMA in a controlled environment [Hartman et al. 2013; Carhart-Harris et al. 2014a, 2014b; De Sousa Fernandes Perna et al. 2014; Frye et al. 2014; Hysek et al. 2014; Kirkpatrick et al. 2014b, 2014c; Kuypers et al. 2014; Schmid et al. 2014, 2015; Seibert et al. 2014; Spronk et al. 2014; Wardle and De Wit, 2014; Yubero-Lahoz et al. 2014; Clark et al. 2015]. Two papers of note, examining the psychological effects of MDMA were published by the groups of Nutt and De Wit respectively [Carhart-Harris et al. 2014b; Frye et al. 2014]. Nutt’s group looked at the effects of a modest MDMA dose on the recall of emotionally important memories, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and subjective ratings [Carhart-Harris et al. 2014a. De Wit’s group investigated the effects of MDMA on the perception of social rejection using a computer simulated task and subjective ratings [Frye et al. 2014a]. In both studies, MDMA was shown to enhance positivity. In the work by Nutt’s group, participants reported that during the use of MDMA, worst memories, such as the death of a close friend, were significantly less negative, whilst favourite memories were more positive, vivid and emotionally intense. In the study by De Wit’s group, enhanced positivity is demonstrated by a decreased subjective response to rejection. These two papers address differing areas of MDMA’s psychopharmacology, with effects of enhanced positivity apparently appearing consistent across both autobiographical recollection and contemporaneous subjective experience. These findings are highly supportive of the putative use of MDMA in depression for elevating mood, as well as further supporting the use of MDMA as an adjunct to psychotherapy. A modulation enabling increased positivity has the potential to increase engagement in the therapeutic process, to assist in the exploration of the underlying cognitions and to create a platform for subsequent behavioural change.
These studies have paved the way for exploration of the acute effects of MDMA on psychology. For the purposes of an antidepressant therapy, it will be important in the future to explore whether the positive changes in psychology demonstrated can persist beyond the duration of the acute actions of MDMA. In addition, translation to patient groups will need to be addressed. We have found one study looking at the effects of MDMA on mood in a putative target population [Majumder et al. 2012], which reported that in a population of MDMA users, with and without a predisposition of depression, the administration of the drug decreases depressive symptoms. The authors argue that this effect could underlie the popularity of MDMA as a form of self-medication; we would argue that this further supports the hypothesis that MDMA may provide a helpful therapy in depression.
Recreational drugs as clinical therapies
Rapid-onset antidepressants
In the 1940s, the first antidepressant, amphetamine, was used for its rapid-acting pharmacology. In his excellent book, On Speed: The Many Lives of Amphetamine, Rasmussen discusses the history of amphetamines and their rise to fame as wonder drugs that claimed to cure a whole host of ailments from obesity to mood disorders [Rasmussen, 2008]. Today the use of amphetamine has clearly fallen out of vogue, in part due to the prolific side effects observed from prescription and their highly addictive nature that led to the patterns of abuse that built up around them. Taboo now shrouds the conversion of illicit substances to clinical therapy; nevertheless, ketamine is in a number of clinical trials for TRD [Murrough and Charney, 2012]. Ketamine’s therapeutic use in anaesthesia ensures safety margins are established, but it is still significant that it is being considered as a rapid-onset antidepressant. Many trials of ketamine as a novel antidepressant have taken place in the past year, including a small naturalistic open label study by Diamond and colleagues looking at the efficacy and safety of delivering ketamine treatment within an NHS clinic setting [Diamond et al. 2014]. The model used a regime of once weekly intravenous (IV) doses of ketamine for 3 weeks, followed by twice weekly doses for a further 3 weeks, demonstrating a potential model for the safe clinical use of controlled substances which could be applied to MDMA.
Initial evidence supports ketamine’s role as a therapy for TRD, but this substance is markedly psychotropic, which may not be desirable for severely ill patients. MDMA is not overtly psychotropic and thus may provide a more attractive therapy. Alternatively, specific analogues to these drugs, lacking unwanted effects, can be developed. One group has synthesized a ketamine analogue for use in TRD [Burgdorf et al. 2013] and UWA-101, an MDMA analogue, has also been developed for use in Parkinson’s disease. UWA-101 apparently lacks cytotoxicity in primates [Johnston et al. 2012]. This could be applied to TRD if the monoaminergic actions of MDMA are decreed to be antidepressant as a designer agent will be less controversial and neurotoxic.
Psychotherapy adjuncts
In the middle of the last century, the precedent was set for the use of psychostimulants as rapid acting antidepressants and a simultaneous interest into the use of psychedelics as adjuncts to psychotherapy developed. In 1967, Naranjo and colleagues published a paper on MDA, a substance structurally related to MDMA, and its potential as an adjunct to psychotherapy on the basis of its ability to heighten affect, increase emotional empathy and increase self-insight, without the distracting perceptual alterations of LSD [Naranjo et al. 1967]. In 1986, Grinspoon and Bakalar discussed the history of drug use for the enhancement of psychotherapy and highlighted MDMA as one of the most promising agents of interest [Grinspoon and Bakalar, 1986]. Modern trials of MDMA as an augmentation strategy in cognitive therapies have been initiated with treatment-resistant PTSD patients. PTSD is an anxiety disorder, related and often comorbid with depression, and the protocols from the groups researching the efficacy of MDMA assisted psychotherapy in PTSD could be translated into work on depression. Current protocols consist of a number of MDMA-assisted sessions interspersed with nondrug based psychotherapy sessions, with an overnight inpatient stay following MDMA assisted-sessions to ensure safety [Oehen et al. 2013].
It is worth considering that many patients undergoing psychotherapy are on a variety of psychotropic medications and some data suggest that current antidepressants combined with CBT may have reduced effects on outcome. SSRIs negatively affect amygdala function, which is thought to be one of the structural targets of psychotherapy [Browning et al. 2011]. Episodic memory, mediated in part by the amygdala, is thought to be impaired in depression and hyperactive responses in the amygdala, to neutral stimuli, are well-documented in depressed subjects [Nestler et al. 2002]. MDMA acutely reduces activity in the amygdala [Gamma et al. 2000], suggesting that it may help overcome the cognitive insufficiencies linked to depression in addition to correcting monoamine imbalances. However, whether a reduction in amygdala function, as a result of MDMA exposure, affects psychotherapy outcomes, is unknown.
Conclusion
MDMA has the potential to act as a rapid-onset antidepressant via its modulation of the 5-HT system and as an augmentation strategy in cognitive therapy. Acutely, MDMA acts to increase extracellular 5-HT availability, portraying an ideal antidepressant agent according to the monoamine hypothesis. A single rodent study has confirmed this acute effect, but use as an antidepressant is restricted by inadequate experimental evidence and safety uncertainties. A precedent for the safe use of MDMA has been set by its use in clinical trials of PTSD, but a more concrete analysis needs to be resolved through human volunteer studies. Large doses appear neurotoxic in animals, thus small doses of subrecreational values, are most appropriate for use in initial trials. A preliminary study to assess safety would need a prospective design, measuring outcomes of therapeutic doses, in drug-naïve volunteers, using PET with [11C]DASB to measure neurotoxicity, in conjunction with psychological tests of emotions linked to 5-HT dysfunction. Once a safety profile and therapeutic index are concluded, the next step will be to look definitively at the antidepressant action of MDMA. There is no doubt that MDMA acutely causes feelings of empathy in almost all users. However, if antidepressant properties are tested, it should be remembered that when healthy volunteers are administered SSRIs, noticeable effects on mood are not recorded and thus it is important to emphasize studies on patients rather than healthy volunteers. In conclusion, the data from PTSD trials of MDMA assisted psychotherapy set a promising precedent that can likely be applied to depression. The use of MDMA as a standalone rapid-onset antidepressant is theoretically well-grounded, but lacks proof of concept.
Footnotes
Funding: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Conflict of interest statement: The authors declare no conflicts of interest in preparing this article.
Contributor Information
Rachel Patel, Green Templeton College, Woodstock Road, Oxford, OX2 6HG, UK.
Daniel Titheradge, St Hugh’s College, Oxford, UK.
References
- Adori C., Ando R., Szekeres M., Gutknecht L., Kovacs G., Hunyady L., et al. (2011) Recovery and aging of serotonergic fibers after single and intermittent MDMA treatment in dark agouti rat. J Comp Neurol 519: 2353–2378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ali S., Newport G., Scallet A., Binienda Z., Ferguson S., Bailey J., et al. (1993) Oral Administration of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) produces selective serotonergic depletion in the nonhuman primate. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15: 91–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumann M., Clark R., Budzynski A., Partilla J., Blough B., Rothman R. (2005) N-substituted piperazines abused by humans mimic the molecular mechanism of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, or ‘ecstasy’). Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 550–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumann M., Wang X., Rothman R. (2007) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) neurotoxicity in rats: a reappraisal of past and present findings. Psychopharmacology 189: 407–424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bengel D., Murphy D., Andrews A., Wichems C., Feltner D., Heils A., et al. (1998) Altered brain serotonin homeostasis and locomotor insensitivity to 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (‘ecstasy’) in serotonin transporter-deficient mice. Mol Pharmacol 53: 649–655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benningfield M., Cowan R. (2013) Brain serotonin function in MDMA (ecstasy) users: evidence for persisting neurotoxicity. Neuropsychopharmacology 38: 253–255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonkale W., Austin M. (2008) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine induces differential regulation of tryptophan hydroxylase 2 protein and mRNA levels in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuroscience 155: 270–276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briere F., Fallu J., Janosz M., Pagani L. (2012) Prospective associations between meth/amphetamine (speed) and MDMA (ecstasy) use and depressive symptoms in secondary school students. J Epidemiol Community Health 66: 990–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browning M., Grol M., Ly V., Goodwin G., Holmes E., Harmer C. (2011) Using an experimental medicine model to explore combination effects of pharmacological and cognitive interventions for depression and anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 2689–2697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgdorf J., Zhang X., Nicholson K., Balster R., Leander J., Stanton P., et al. (2013) GLYX-13, a NMDA receptor glycine-site functional partial agonist, induces antidepressant-like effects without ketamine-like side effects. Neuropsychopharmacology 38: 729–742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris R., Murphy K., Leech R., Erritzoe D., Wall M., Ferguson B., et al. (2014a) The effects of acutely administered 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine on spontaneous brain function in healthy volunteers measured with arterial spin labeling and blood oxygen level-dependent resting state functional connectivity. Biol Psychiatry. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris R., Wall M., Erritzoe D., Kaelen M., Ferguson B., De Meer I., et al. (2014b) The effect of acutely administered MDMA on subjective and bold-fMRI responses to favourite and worst autobiographical memories. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 17: 527–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carvalho M., Carmo H., Costa V., Capela J., Pontes H., Remiao F., et al. (2012) Toxicity of amphetamines: an update. Arch Toxicol 86: 1167–1231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark C., Frye C., Wardle M., Norman G., De Wit H. (2015) Acute effects of MDMA on autonomic cardiac activity and their relation to subjective prosocial and stimulant effects. Psychophysiology 52: 429–435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davison D., Parrott A. (1997) Ecstasy (MDMA) in recreational users: self-reported psychological and physiological effects. Human Psychopharmacol Clin Exp 12: 221–226. [Google Scholar]
- De Almeida S., Silva M. (2003) Ecstasy (MDMA): effects and patterns of use reported by users in Sao Paulo. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 25: 11–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Sousa Fernandes Perna E., Theunissen E., Kuypers K., Heckman P., De La Torre R., Farre M., et al. (2014) Memory and mood during MDMA intoxication, with and without memantine pretreatment. Neuropharmacology 87: 198–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Win M., Schilt T., Reneman L., Vervaeke H., Jager G., Dijkink S., et al. (2006) Ecstasy use and self-reported depression, impulsivity, and sensation seeking: a prospective cohort study. J Psychopharmacol 20: 226–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond P., Farmery A., Atkinson S., Haldar J., Williams N., Cowen P., et al. (2014) Ketamine infusions for treatment resistant depression: a series of 28 patients treated weekly or twice weekly in an ECT clinic. J Psychopharmacol 28: 536–544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domes G., Heinrichs M., Michel A., Berger C., Herpertz S. (2007) Oxytocin improves ‘mind-reading’ in humans. Biol Psychiatry 61: 731–733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumont G., Sweep F., van Der Steen R., Hermsen R., Donders A., Touw D., et al. (2009) Increased oxytocin concentrations and prosocial feelings in humans after ecstasy (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) administration. Soc Neurosci 4: 359–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott J., Beveridge T. (2005) psychostimulants and monoamine transporters: upsetting the balance. Curr Opin Pharmacol 5: 94–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox M., Andrews A., Wendland J., Lesch K., Holmes A., Murphy D. (2007) A pharmacological analysis of mice with a targeted disruption of the serotonin transporter. Psychopharmacology 195: 147–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankle W., Huang Y., Hwang D., Talbot P., Slifstein M., van Heertum R., et al. (2004) Comparative evaluation of serotonin transporter radioligands 11C-DASB and 11C-McN 5652 in healthy humans. J Nucl Med 45: 682–694. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frazer A., Benmansour S. (2002) Delayed pharmacological effects of antidepressants. Mol Psychiatry 7(Suppl. 1): S23–S28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frye C., Wardle M., Norman G., De Wit H. (2014) MDMA decreases the effects of simulated social rejection. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 117: 1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gamma A., Buck A., Berthold T., Liechti M., Vollenweider F. (2000) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) modulates cortical and limbic brain activity as measured by [H(2)(15)O]-PET in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 23: 388–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinspoon L., Bakalar J. (1986) Can drugs be used to enhance the psychotherapeutic process? Am J Psychother 40: 393–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartman R., Desrosiers N., Barnes A., Yun K., Scheidweiler K., Kolbrich-Spargo E., et al. (2013) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and metabolites disposition in blood and plasma following controlled oral administration. Anal Bioanal Chem. 15 November 2013. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huizink A., Ferdinand R., Van Der Ende J., Verhulst F. (2006) Symptoms of anxiety and depression in childhood and use of MDMA: prospective, population based study. Br Med J 332: 825–828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hysek C., Domes G., Liechti M. (2012) MDMA enhances ‘mind reading’ of positive emotions and impairs ‘mind reading’ of negative emotions. Psychopharmacology 222: 293–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hysek C., Simmler L., Schillinger N., Meyer N., Schmid Y., Donzelli M., et al. (2014) Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic effects of methylphenidate and MDMA administered alone or in combination. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 17: 371–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston T., Millar Z., Huot P., Wagg K., Thiele S., Salomonczyk D., et al. (2012) A novel MDMA analogue, UWA-101, that lacks psychoactivity and cytotoxicity, enhances L-DOPA benefit in Parkinsonian primates. FASEB J 26: 2154–2163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones L., Bates G., Bellis M., Beynon C., Duffy P., Evans-Brown M., et al. (2011) A summary of the health harms of drugs. London: Department of Health. [Google Scholar]
- Kaye S., Darke S., Duflou J. (2009) Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA)-related fatalities in Australia: demographics, circumstances, toxicology and major organ pathology. Drug Alcohol Depend 104: 254–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick M., Baggott M., Mendelson J., Galloway G., Liechti M., Hysek C., et al. (2014a) MDMA effects consistent across laboratories. Psychopharmacology 231: 3899–3905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick M., Francis S., Lee R., De Wit H., Jacob S. (2014b) Plasma oxytocin concentrations following MDMA or intranasal oxytocin in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology 46: 23–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick M., Lee R., Wardle M., Jacob S., De Wit H. (2014c) Effects of MDMA and intranasal oxytocin on social and emotional processing. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 1654–1663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kish S., Fitzmaurice P., Chang L., Furukawa Y., Tong J. (2010) Low striatal serotonin transporter protein in a human polydrug MDMA (ecstasy) user: a case study. J Psychopharmacol 24: 281–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kish S., Lerch J., Furukawa Y., Tong J., McCluskey T., Wilkins D., et al. (2010) Decreased cerebral cortical serotonin transporter binding in ecstasy users: a positron emission tomography/[(11)C]DASB and structural brain imaging study. Brain 133: 1779–1797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolyaduke O., Hughes R. (2013) Increased anxiety-related behavior in male and female adult rats following early and late adolescent exposure to 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA). Pharmacol Biochem Behav 103: 742–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan V., Nestler E. (2008) The molecular neurobiology of depression. Nature 455: 894–902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuypers K., De La Torre R., Farre M., Yubero-Lahoz S., Dziobek I., Van Den Bos W., et al. (2014) No evidence that MDMA-induced enhancement of emotional empathy is related to peripheral oxytocin levels or 5-HT1a receptor activation. PLoS One 9: e100719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lieb R., Schuetz C., Pfister H., Von Sydow K., Wittchen H. (2002) Mental disorders in ecstasy users: a prospective-longitudinal investigation. Drug Alcohol Depend 68: 195–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liechti M., Vollenweider F. (2000) The serotonin uptake inhibitor citalopram reduces acute cardiovascular and vegetative effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (‘ecstasy’) in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 14: 269–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majumder I., White J., Irvine R. (2011) Antidepressant-like effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine in an animal model of depression. Behav Pharmacol 22: 758–765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majumder I., White J., Irvine R. (2012) antidepressant-like effects of ecstasy in subjects with a predisposition to depression. Addict Behav 37: 1189–1192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MAPS (2015) 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) Annual Report IND# 63,384. Multi-disciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies.
- McCann U., Ridenour A., Shaham Y., Ricaurte G. (1994) Serotonin neurotoxicity after (+/-)3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA; ‘ecstasy’): a controlled study in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 10: 129–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCann U., Szabo Z., Seckin E., Rosenblatt P., Mathews W., Ravert H., et al. (2005) Quantitative PET studies of the serotonin transporter in MDMA users and controls using [11C]McN5652 and [11C]DASB. Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 1741–1750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mithoefer M., Wagner M., Mithoefer A., Jerome L., Doblin R. (2011) The safety and efficacy of {+/-}3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine-assisted psychotherapy in subjects with chronic, treatment-resistant posttraumatic stress disorder: the first randomized controlled pilot study. J Psychopharmacol 25: 439–452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mithoefer M., Wagner M., Mithoefer A., Jerome L., Martin S., Yazar-Klosinski B., et al. (2013) Durability of improvement in post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms and absence of harmful effects or drug dependency after 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine-assisted psychotherapy: a prospective long-term follow-up study. J Psychopharmacol 27: 28–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murrough J., Charney D. (2012) Is there anything really novel on the antidepressant horizon? Curr Psychiatry Rep 14: 643–649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naranjo C., Shulgin A., Sargent T. (1967) Evaluation of 3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine (MDA) as an adjunct to psychotherapy. Med Pharmacol Exp Int J Exp Med 17: 359–364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestler E., Barrot M., Dileone R., Eisch A., Gold S., Monteggia L. (2002) Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 34: 13–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oehen P., Traber R., Widmer V., Schnyder U. (2013) A randomized, controlled pilot study of MDMA (+/- 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine)-assisted psychotherapy for treatment of resistant, chronic post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). J Psychopharmacol 27: 40–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen N. (2008) On Speed: The Many Lives of Amphetamine. New York: New York University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ricaurte G., Finnegan K., Irwin I., Langston J. (1990) Aminergic metabolites in cerebrospinal fluid of humans previously exposed to MDMA: preliminary observations. Ann N Y Acad Sci 600: 699–708; discussion 708–610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riedlinger J., Montagne M. (2001) Potential clinical uses for MDMA. In: Holland J. (ed.), Ecstasy: The Complete Guide: A Comprehensive Review of the Risks and Benefits of MDMA. Rochester, VT: Park Street Press, pp. 243–247. [Google Scholar]
- Rothman R., Baumann M. (2002) Serotonin releasing agents. Neurochemical, therapeutic and adverse effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71: 825–836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudnick G., Wall S. (1992) The molecular mechanism of ‘ecstasy’ [3,4-methylenedioxy-methamphetamine (MDMA)]: serotonin transporters are targets for MDMA-induced serotonin release. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 89: 1817–1821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schenk S., Abraham B., Aronsen D., Colussi-Mas J., Do J. (2013) Effects of repeated exposure to MDMA on 5HT1a autoreceptor function: behavioral and neurochemical responses to 8-OHDPAT. Psychopharmacology 227: 355–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmid Y., Hysek C., Preller K., Bosch O., Bilderbeck A., Rogers R., et al. (2015) Effects of methylphenidate and MDMA on appraisal of erotic stimuli and intimate relationships. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 25: 17–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmid Y., Hysek C., Simmler L., Crockett M., Quednow B., Liechti M. (2014) Differential effects of MDMA and methylphenidate on social cognition. J Psychopharmacol 28: 847–856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt C. (1987) Acute administration of methylenedioxymethamphetamine: comparison with the neurochemical effects of its N-desmethyl and N-ethyl analogs. Eur J Pharmacol 136: 81–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt C., Taylor V. (1987) Depression of rat brain tryptophan hydroxylase activity following the acute administration of methylenedioxymethamphetamine. Biochem Pharmacol 36: 4095–4102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt C., Wu L., Lovenberg W. (1986) Methylenedioxymethamphetamine: a potentially neurotoxic amphetamine analogue. Eur J Pharmacol 124: 175–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott R., Hides L., Allen J., Lubman D. (2013) Subacute effects of ecstasy on mood: an exploration of associated risk factors. J Psychopharmacol 27: 53–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seibert J., Hysek C., Penno C., Schmid Y., Kratschmar D., Liechti M., et al. (2014) Acute effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine and methylphenidate on circulating steroid levels in healthy subjects. Neuroendocrinology 100: 17–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sessa B., Nutt D. (2007) MDMA, politics and medical research: have we thrown the baby out with the bathwater? J Psychopharmacol 21: 787–791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spronk D., Dumont G., Verkes R., De Bruijn E. (2014) The acute effects of MDMA and ethanol administration on electrophysiological correlates of performance monitoring in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 231: 2877–2888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele T., Nichols D., Yim G. (1987) Stereochemical effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and related amphetamine derivatives on inhibition of uptake of [3H]monoamines into synaptosomes from different regions of rat brain. Biochem Pharmacol 36: 2297–2303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taffe M., Huitron-Resendiz S., Schroeder R., Parsons L., Henriksen S., Gold L. (2003) MDMA exposure alters cognitive and electrophysiological sensitivity to rapid tryptophan depletion in Rhesus monkeys. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 76: 141–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson M., Li K., Clemens K., Gurtman C., Hunt G., Cornish J., et al. (2004) Chronic fluoxetine treatment partly attenuates the long-term anxiety and depressive symptoms induced by MDMA (‘ecstasy’) in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 694–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urban N., Girgis R., Talbot P., Kegeles L., Xu X., Frankle W., et al. (2012) Sustained recreational use of ecstasy is associated with altered pre and postsynaptic markers of serotonin transmission in neocortical areas: a PET study with [(1)(1)C]DASB and [(1)(1)C]MDL 100907. Neuropsychopharmacology 37: 1465–1473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Eijndhoven P., Van Wingen G., Fernandez G., Rijpkema M., Verkes R., Buitelaar J., et al. (2011) Amygdala responsivity related to memory of emotionally neutral stimuli constitutes a trait factor for depression. Neuroimage 54: 1677–1684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider F., Gamma A., Liechti M., Huber T. (1998) Psychological and cardiovascular effects and short-term sequelae of MDMA (‘ecstasy’) in MDMA-naive healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 19: 241–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallinga A., Grahlmann C., Granneman R., Koolhaas J., Buwalda B. (2011) Gender Differences in hyperthermia and regional 5-HT and 5-HIAA Depletion in the brain following MDMA administration in rats. Brain Res 1398: 13–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle M., De Wit H. (2014) MDMA alters emotional processing and facilitates positive social interaction. Psychopharmacology 231: 4219–4229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yubero-Lahoz S., Kuypers K., Ramaekers J., Langohr K., Farre, Farre M., De La Torre R. (2014) Changes in serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene expression in peripheral blood cells after MDMA intake. Psychopharmacology. 20 December 2014. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]