Skip to main content
Public Health Nutrition logoLink to Public Health Nutrition
. 2014 Sep 19;18(8):1389–1396. doi: 10.1017/S1368980014002079

Predicting use of effective vegetable parenting practices with the Model of Goal Directed Behavior

Cassandra S Diep 1,2,*, Alicia Beltran 1, Tzu-An Chen 1, Debbe Thompson 1, Teresia O’Connor 1, Sheryl Hughes 1, Janice Baranowski 1, Tom Baranowski 1
PMCID: PMC4505808  NIHMSID: NIHMS706631  PMID: 25234656

Abstract

Objective

To model effective vegetable parenting practices using the Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices construct scales.

Design

An Internet survey was conducted with parents of pre-school children to assess their agreement with effective vegetable parenting practices and Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices items. Block regression modelling was conducted using the composite score of effective vegetable parenting practices scales as the outcome variable and the Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices constructs as predictors in separate and sequential blocks: demographics, intention, desire (intrinsic motivation), perceived barriers, autonomy, relatedness, self-efficacy, habit, anticipated emotions, perceived behavioural control, attitudes and lastly norms. Backward deletion was employed at the end for any variable not significant at P<0·05.

Setting

Houston, TX, USA.

Subjects

Three hundred and seven parents (mostly mothers) of pre-school children.

Results

Significant predictors in the final model in order of relationship strength included habit of active child involvement in vegetable selection, habit of positive vegetable communications, respondent not liking vegetables, habit of keeping a positive vegetable environment and perceived behavioural control of having a positive influence on child’s vegetable consumption. The final model’s adjusted R 2 was 0·486.

Conclusions

This was the first study to test scales from a behavioural model to predict effective vegetable parenting practices. Further research needs to assess these Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices scales for their (i) predictiveness of child consumption of vegetables in longitudinal samples and (ii) utility in guiding design of vegetable parenting practices interventions.

Keywords: Vegetables, Parenting practices, Theory, Pre-school child


Vegetables are rich in nutrients, dietary fibre and phytochemicals, and likely have protective effects against obesity, CVD, multiple cancers and other chronic diseases among children and adults( 1 8 ). In addition, higher diet quality, including increased consumption of vegetables, has been associated with improved academic performance in children( 9 ). Consuming vegetables is an important part of a healthy diet and lifestyle, yet levels of vegetable consumption are generally low, below dietary recommendations( 10 , 11 ). The estimated percentages of young children not meeting recommended levels of total vegetable intake were high according to 2001–2004 national data: 80·3 % of children aged 2–3 years and 92·0 % of children aged 4–8 years had inadequate vegetable intake( 11 ).

Establishing vegetable intake early in life is important, especially because vegetable and other food consumption patterns in childhood track into adolescence and adulthood( 12 14 ). Development of young children’s dietary behaviours is partly influenced by parents and their parenting practices( 15 17 ), yet many parents of pre-school children report challenges to getting their child to eat vegetables. Some challenges include availability, cost barriers and negative parenting practices( 18 ). Specifically, behaviours that parents use to influence a child’s long-term vegetable intake are vegetable parenting practices and have been classified as effective or ineffective based on professional judgement of long-term vegetable consumption( 16 ). Recent research confirmed separate vegetable parenting practices dimensions for effective and ineffective vegetable parenting practices( 19 ), suggesting the importance of promoting parents’ use of effective vegetable parenting practices (EVPP) while reducing use of ineffective vegetable parenting practices( 20 , 21 ). EVPP are vegetable intake-related parenting practices that provide structure, non-directive control and are responsive; examples include praising a child when he/she eats vegetables (responsiveness), eating together as a family (structure) and allowing a child to self-serve vegetables (non-directive control)( 16 ). Parental feeding structure, control and responsiveness categories were conceptualized based on the general parenting style paradigm( 22 24 ) to provide a comprehensive taxonomy of parents’ role in children’s eating( 25 ). Understanding EVPP use could inform future interventions and research.

Behavioural theories provide a framework for understanding and systematically studying feeding behaviours( 26 , 27 ). Specifically, the Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices (MGDVPP) provides a useful model for understanding and predicting parenting practices shown to encourage pre-school children’s vegetable intake( 17 , 20 , 21 , 28 , 29 ). Based on the Model of Goal Directed Behavior and Self-Determination Theory, MGDVPP was adapted for vegetable parenting practices (see Fig. 1)( 29 ). The Model of Goal Directed Behavior is an expansion of the Theory of Planned Behavior, adding ‘anticipated emotions’ as a psychosocial predictor and inserting ‘desire’ between the psychosocial predictors (i.e. attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioural control and anticipated emotions) and ‘intention’( 30 33 ). As originally in the Theory of Planned Behavior, there is then a possible direct effect of perceived behavioural control on behaviour( 34 ). Intention is hypothesized to also lead to actual behavioural performance( 35 ), although there has been evidence of an intention–behaviour gap( 36 38 ). With the extra constructs, the Model of Goal Directed Behavior has shown enhanced behaviour predictiveness over the Theory of Planned Behavior( 30 33 ). In addition, because desires embody intrinsic motivation, Self-Determination Theory constructs that contribute to intrinsic motivation (i.e. autonomy, competence/self-efficacy and relatedness)( 39 ) were added to the Model of Goal Directed Behavior to directly predict desire. Lastly, habit (or automated behaviour) and barriers to performing a behaviour are related to behaviour( 40 , 41 ), and thus were added to enhance predictiveness of EVPP.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Hypothesized path model of goal-directed vegetable parenting practices

The present study explored and reported the modelling of EVPP using MGDVPP constructs: intention, desire (intrinsic motivation), perceived barriers, autonomy, relatedness, self-efficacy, habit, anticipated emotions, perceived behavioural control, attitudes and norms. The authors hypothesized MGDVPP constructs to be predictive of EVPP and thereby provide a good model for understanding and predicting parenting practices that are believed to effectively encourage a pre-school child’s vegetable intake. There is no other known model to predict EVPP. If shown to be predictive, the MGDVPP may inform future interventions that help parents adopt effective behaviours for encouraging their child’s vegetable intake.

Methods

Study sample

In 2009, 406 parents initiated an Internet survey assessing their level of agreement with the EVPP and MGDVPP items. Respondents were parents of a child of pre-school age and were recruited through multiple methods, including a notice posted in the Children’s Nutrition Research Center newsletter and the Baylor College of Medicine volunteer website; fliers posted at the Texas Medical Center, public libraries and YMCA; and personal emails to Children’s Nutrition Research Center volunteers. A more detailed description of recruitment methods is available elsewhere( 19 21 , 28 ).

For the present study, only respondents with complete surveys were included in the analyses. Six participants with any missing demographic information were also excluded from the final analyses, making the final sample size n 307. Because demographic questions were included at the end of the survey, no information was available to compare the participants who completed the survey with those who did not. In addition, the authors assumed that (i) if a respondent had more than one child, he/she chose one child and answered questions accordingly and (ii) multiple respondents did not answer separate surveys for the same child. The authors removed duplicates based on IP and email addresses listed, but it is possible that more than one parent completed surveys for the same child from different IP addresses or using different emails.

The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Baylor College of Medicine Institutional Review Board. All participants provided informed consent and were offered a $US 20 gift card upon completion of the survey.

Measures

Respondents completed 227 items related to demographics, MGDVPP scales (i.e. intention, desire (intrinsic motivation), perceived barriers, autonomy, relatedness, self-efficacy, habit, anticipated emotions, perceived behavioural control, attitudes and norms) and EVPP scales. Demographic questions included child age, child gender, parent gender, parent race/ethnicity, highest household educational attainment and annual household income (Table 1). MGDVPP items were generated from intensive qualitative interviews with parents of pre-school children about their motivations to use vegetable parenting practices; details of these interviews have been published( 29 ). Table 2 provides a listing of MGDVPP and EVPP scales, subscales, number of items, means and standard deviations, range of possible scores, Cronbach’s α values and bivariate correlations with EVPP.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of the study participants: parents (n 307) of pre-school chldren, Houston, TX, USA

Characteristic n %
Total 307 100·0
Gender of parent
Male 33 10·7
Female 274 89·3
Gender of child
Male 163 53·1
Female 144 46·9
Race/ethnicity of parent
Black/African American 60 19·5
White 114 37·1
Hispanic 31 10·1
Asian 43 14·0
Other 59 19·2
Highest household educational attainment
High-school graduate or less 30 9·7
Technical school 11 3·6
Some college 67 21·8
College graduate 96 31·3
Postgraduate study 102 33·2
Missing 1 0·3
Annual household income (2009)
<$US 10 000 11 3·6
$US 10 000–19 999 16 5·2
$US 20 000–39 999 56 18·2
$US 40 000–59 999 58 18·9
≥$US 60 000 166 54·1

Table 2.

Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices scales and subscales

Scale Subscale Mean sd Range Cronbach’s α Number of items Correlation with EVPP
EVPP Effective vegetable parenting practices 23·00 3·61 15–34 0·69 14
Intentions Authoritative parenting intentions 17·50 1·31 11–18 0·83 6 −0·14*
Active child involvement intentions 16·05 2·41 6–18 0·84 6 −0·33***
Controlling parenting intentions 9·54 2·59 5–15 0·71 5 −0·01
Permissive parenting intentions 3·66 1·28 2–6 0·61 2 0·01
Desire Intrinsic motivation 9·01 2·27 4–12 0·78 4 0·23***
Perceived barriers Child doesn’t like vegetables 14·69 4·88 8–24 0·88 8 −0·35***
Respondent doesn’t like vegetables 11·14 3·30 9–26 0·85 9 0·39***
Cost of vegetables 7·53 2·34 5–15 0·67 5 0·32***
Autonomy Choice 7·92 1·06 4–9 0·31 3 −0·23***
Relatedness Parent values 7·72 2·16 4–12 0·81 4 −0·13*
Child wellness 8·26 1·15 3–9 0·61 3 −0·08
Competence/self-efficacy Strong competence/self-efficacy 19·27 3·87 8–24 0·85 8 −0·38***
Weak competence/self-efficacy 27·99 2·50 19–30 0·76 10 −0·28***
Habit Active child involvement in vegetable selection 10·98 3·04 6–18 0·83 6 0·60***
Controlling vegetable practices 11·80 2·13 5–15 0·68 5 0·11
Positive vegetable environment 3·59 0·95 3–8 0·67 3 0·44***
Positive vegetable communications 6·92 1·74 5–13 0·60 5 0·44***
Anticipated emotions Negative child behaviour with positive emotional response 9·69 2·84 8–23 0·92 8 −0·08
Positive child behaviour with negative emotional response 4·82 1·50 4–11 0·83 4 0·02
Negative child behaviour with negative emotional response 17·90 3·87 8–24 0·79 8 0·13*
Positive child behaviour with positive emotional response 11·38 1·17 4–12 0·66 4 −0·05
Perceived behavioural control Control of positive influences on vegetable consumption 34·46 4·37 17–39 0·85 13 −0·37***
Control of negative influences on vegetable consumption 16·93 4·29 11–32 0·82 11 0·05
Control of negative parenting practices 7·55 1·80 4–12 0·54 4 −0·06
Attitudes Health benefits of vegetables 16·14 2·03 9–18 0·72 6 −0·08
Negative effects of vegetables 7·42 1·73 6–15 0·66 6 0·08
Benefits of vegetables other than health 11·58 0·94 7–12 0·66 4 −0·07
Norms Descriptive norms 3·86 0·83 2–6 0·13 3 −0·10
Normative expectations 11·86 5·17 1–18 0·71 2 −0·08

EVPP, effective vegetable parenting practices.

*P<0·05, ***P<0·001.

The dependent variable was a composite EVPP scale, obtained by summing the values of fourteen items that loaded on to a second-level EVPP factor in a confirmatory factor analysis described elsewhere( 19 ). Items were generated from health professionals’ and dietetics practitioners’ responses to an Internet survey( 16 ). Acceptable fit was obtained for the EVPP scale( 19 ).

Details about the EVPP and MGDVPP scales, including item generation and psychometric properties of the scales, have been published( 19 21 , 28 ). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses revealed multiple dimensions were necessary to fit most MGDVPP scales’ items( 28 ). Tests of construct validity showed that almost all MGDVPP scales (86·2 %) bivariately correlated with composite EVPP or ineffective vegetable parenting practices scales; specifically, fifteen of twenty-nine scales (or 51·7 %) correlated with EVPP. In addition, internal consistency reliability coefficients for MGDVPP subscales ranged from 0·13 to 0·92, with a majority being 0·70 or higher (see Table 2). Many of the subscales with lower reliability coefficients (i.e. below 0·70) comprised only three or four items( 42 ). However, these shorter subscales had acceptable average inter-item correlations above 0·20( 43 ), an alternative reliability indicator for scales with few items( 28 , 43 ).

Analytical methods

The present study involved descriptive statistics and block regression modelling, using the composite score of the EVPP scales as the dependent variable and MGDVPP subscales as predictor variables in separate and sequential blocks. Variance inflation factor and tolerance for each predictor were used to test potential collinearity. Predictors with a variance inflation factor above 10 or a tolerance below 0·10 may merit further investigation( 44 ).

Modelling started with demographics (i.e. child age, child gender, highest household educational attainment, annual household income and parent race/ethnicity), followed by the intention subscales, desire (intrinsic motivation), perceived barriers, autonomy, relatedness, self-efficacy, habit, anticipated emotions, perceived behavioural control, attitudes and lastly norms. Demographic variables were entered first to understand the extent of their influence without any MGDVPP subscales; thus, the variance due to subsequent MGDVPP subscales would predict variance beyond demographics. Because, theoretically, intentions are the most proximal to behaviour and should be the strongest predictor, they were added next. Variables were then added in order of distance from behaviour – from most proximal to least proximal. Habit subscales were entered relatively late in the testing sequence to prevent drowning out other predictors. All demographic variables remained in all models, but backward deletion was employed at the end of each block entry for any other variables not related to the outcome variable at P<0·10. For the final model, any variables not related to the outcome variable at P<0·05 were deleted. All analyses were performed using the statistical software package SAS version 9·3.

Results

Study sample

Most respondents were female (89·3 %), had a male child (53·1 %), were a college graduate (64·5 %) and had an annual household income of at least $US 60 000 (54·1 %; Table 1). There was representation from the major racial and ethnic groups in Houston, TX, including White (37·1 %), Black/African American (19·5 %), other (19·2 %), Asian (14·0 %) and Hispanic (10·1 %). Most respondents (91·5 %) classified as ‘other’ were of multiple races/ethnicities.

Predictors of effective vegetable parenting practices

All tolerance values were greater than 0·10 and variance inflation factor values were less than 10, so there was no problem with collinearity in the data. The strongest predictor of EVPP use was the habit of active child involvement in vegetable selection (β=0·430, P<0·001), followed by habit of positive vegetable communications (β=0·217, P<0·001), barrier of respondent not liking vegetables (β=0·173, P<0·001), perceived behavioural control of positive influence on vegetable consumption (β=–0·147, P=0·001) and habit of positive vegetable environment (β=0·118, P=0·020; Table 3). Of all these predictors, perceived behavioural control of positive influence on vegetable consumption was the only variable negatively related to EVPP. None of the demographic characteristics, intention subscales, intrinsic motivation subscales, autonomy subscales, relatedness subscales, self-efficacy subscales, anticipated emotions subscales, attitudes subscales or norms subscales was significantly related to EVPP use. All related variables produced a final model with an adjusted R 2 of 0·486.

Table 3.

Block regression modeling results using the Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices to predict effective vegetable parenting practices

Parameter estimates
Predictor variable Standardized estimate se
Child age −0·051 0·189
Child gender 0·013 0·310
Highest household educational attainment
6th grade or less −0·037 2·625
Attended some high school 0·028 2·671
High-school graduate or General Educational Development 0·082 0·618
Technical school −0·026 0·869
Some college 0·018 0·457
College graduate −0·003 0·392
Annual household income
<$US 10 000 −0·035 0·857
$US 10 000–19 999 −0·030 0·730
$US 20 000–39 999 0·039 0·471
$US 40 000–59 999 0·004 0·418
Parent race/ethnicity
Black/African American 0·068 0·463
Hispanic/Latino 0·035 0·555
Other 0·017 0·383
Barrier of respondent doesn’t like vegetables 0·173*** 0·051
Habit of active child involvement in vegetable selection 0·430*** 0·058
Habit of positive vegetable environment 0·118* 0·191
Habit of positive vegetable communications 0·217*** 0·103
Perceived behavioural control of positive influence on vegetable consumption −0·147** 0·037
R 2 (adjusted) 0·486

The reference category for child gender is girl; for highest household educational attainment is postgraduate study; for annual household income is ≥$US 60 000; and for parent race/ethnicity is White.

*P<0·05, **P<0·01, ***P<0·001.

Discussion

The current study was the first to test scales to predict EVPP use. The R 2 for the final model (0·486) indicated almost half the variability in EVPP use was explained by the demographic variables, intention subscales, desire (intrinsic motivation), perceived barriers, autonomy, relatedness, self-efficacy, habit, anticipated emotions, perceived behavioural control, attitudes and norms. This suggests the final model used constructs important in predicting EVPP use, even though some of these constructs were from subscales with low internal consistency reliabilities.

The strongest predictor of EVPP use was the parent’s habit of active child involvement in vegetable selection (e.g. automatically asking the child to help select vegetables at the grocery store), followed by habit of positive vegetable communications (e.g. automatically praising the child when the parent sees him/her eating vegetables) and habit of positive vegetable environment (e.g. automatically including vegetables with most meals)( 28 ). These three habit subscales are considered effective practices themselves because they provide structure, non-directive control and are responsive( 16 ). If parents had effective vegetable-related habits around their children, they reasonably would also employ effective practices to encourage their child’s vegetable consumption. Thus consistent with research on habit( 45 ), effective parenting habits( 19 ) strongly predicted use of EVPP.

Perceived behavioural control of positive influence on vegetable consumption had a negative relationship with EVPP. Thus, the easier parents perceived it was to have a positive influence on their child’s vegetable consumption, the less likely the parents were to use EVPP, contradicting what might be expected. The items that highly loaded on this perceived behavioural control subscale identified parent’s perception of how easy it is to get the child to eat more vegetables by asking the child to select vegetables at the grocery store, showing the child they (the parents) enjoy eating vegetables or asking the child to help with vegetable preparations. These items themselves are considered effective parenting practices( 19 ), yet were negatively related to EVPP use in the present study. Perhaps, if a parent finds these parenting practices too easy, he/she may believe they are not effective and therefore not employ them.

The last subscale to be related to EVPP use was the barrier of the respondent (parent) not liking vegetables: the higher the barrier score, the more likely a parent was to use EVPP. The three most highly loaded items on this barrier subscale included the parent not liking vegetables, no one in the family eating vegetables and the parent not liking the taste of vegetables( 28 ). While this is contrary to expectations, perhaps parents who did not like vegetables themselves still appreciated the benefits of vegetables and were motivated to obtain the benefits for their child, using effective practices to encourage their child’s vegetable consumption.

Many of the subscales were not statistically significant predictors of EVPP use. For example, intention did not predict EVPP use, as might have been expected based on past literature( 46 ), but this finding supports other research suggesting an intention–behaviour gap and the weak (or possibly missing) relationships between intention and behaviour( 36 38 ). It is also possible that respondents interpreted the intention of future vegetable parenting practices behaviour to indicate a change in their current practices, and if they were already performing EVPP, the lack of relationship could be understood. In addition, no demographic variables were related to EVPP. Past research showed parents in racial/ethnic minority subgroups, with less than high-school education and with low household income used more controlling food-related parenting practices( 46 ). Another study found contradicting results regarding education levels: higher levels of maternal education were associated with higher use of controlling feeding practices( 47 ). These control scales included primarily the ineffective control vegetable parenting practices, such as restriction or pressure to eat. The current composite indicators of EVPP in the present study included non-directive control (an effective form of control), responsiveness and structure items, suggesting there may be no relationship of socio-economic status to EVPP. The generally higher socio-economic status of the current sample may also have influenced the lack of relationships. Future research will need to assess when and how socio-economic status influences parenting practices.

Although not investigated in the current study, reducing ineffective vegetable parenting practices is also important to consider for a child’s long-term vegetable consumption. Described elsewhere( 20 , 21 ), ineffective practices include exerting dominance or restrictive feeding, and may be related to obesity( 48 ). The predictors of ineffective vegetable parenting practices( 20 , 21 ) differed from predictors of EVPP, so both may be important in future research and practice.

Despite using a broad theoretical framework and validated variables, there are some limitations of the current study. First, the study had a cross-sectional design, which precludes inferences of causality and temporal relationships( 49 , 50 ). Although there were relationships among MGDVPP constructs and EVPP use, the authors could not conclude that certain constructs caused others. Second, data were not collected on actual vegetable consumption, so the authors could not assess relationships between EVPP and vegetable intake and could not verify findings from another study on parenting practices and vegetable consumption( 17 ). Third, because data came from a survey using self-reported measures, they were subject to self-report bias. Relatedly, there may have been self-selection bias of those who agreed to participate. Compared with the population of Houston( 51 ), the current sample included families with higher educational attainment, higher household income than Houston’s median household income and less Hispanic representation; thus, findings cannot be generalized to the population of Houston or to other populations. Lastly, in the current study the authors removed duplicates based on IP and email addresses, and thus assumed that a child was represented only once in the data set, but it is possible that multiple respondents answered separate surveys for the same child. These limitations in design may have contributed to some of the unexpected study findings, such as the higher socio-economic status of participants masking relationships between demographics and EVPP or the cross-sectional nature of the study precluding detection of relationships with future parenting practices. Future research should obtain more diverse samples and use objective measures in a longitudinal design.

Conclusions

The present study contributes to current research regarding vegetable parenting practices using a theoretical framework by testing the MGDVPP and its scales (i.e. intention, desire (intrinsic motivation), perceived barriers, autonomy, relatedness, self-efficacy, habit, anticipated emotions, perceived behavioural control, attitudes and norms) to predict EVPP use. Further research is needed to expand the items in some of the scales to increase the internal consistency reliabilities and assess their predictiveness in longitudinal designs with more diverse samples.

Second, findings have implications for future interventions. Basing dietary interventions on behavioural theory may enhance outcome effectiveness for vegetable consumption( 52 ). To increase EVPP use, an intervention would need to target increasing habit( 53 ). Future research should employ these scales to assess their utility for documenting parenting and psychosocial change in evaluating EVPP interventions. Innovative interventions targeting these scales may lead to increased EVPP use and ultimately may lead to increased vegetable intake among children.

Acknowledgements

Financial support: This research was funded by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (grant number HD058175). This work is a publication of the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA/ARS) Children's Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, and was funded in part with federal funds from the USDA/ARS under Cooperative Agreement No. 58-6250-6001. The contents of this publication do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the USDA, nor does mention of trade names, commercial products or organizations imply endorsement from the US government. C.S.D. was supported fully by a Primary Care Research Training Grant from National Research Service Award (#T32 HP10031). No funder had a role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: C.S.D. participated in review of analyses and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. A.B. was the project manager. T.-A.C. was the data manager and statistician. D.T., T.O. and S.H. contributed conceptually to the measures. J.B. was the project coordinator. T.B. was principal investigator for the overall project. All authors reviewed, critiqued and approved this manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Baylor College of Medicine Institutional Review Board.

References

  • 1. Boeing H, Bechthold A, Bub A et al. (2012) Critical review: vegetables and fruit in the prevention of chronic diseases. Eur J Nutr 51, 637–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Hu FB (2003) Plant-based foods and prevention of cardiovascular disease: an overview. Am J Clin Nutr 78, 3 Suppl., 544S–551S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Ledoux TA, Hingle MD & Baranowski T (2011) Relationship of fruit and vegetable intake with adiposity: a systematic review. Obes Rev 12, e143–e150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Liu RH (2004) Potential synergy of phytochemicals in cancer prevention: mechanism of action. J Nutr 134, 12 Suppl., 3479S–3485S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Key TJ, Schatzkin A, Willett WC et al. (2004) Diet, nutrition and the prevention of cancer. Public Health Nutr 7, 187–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Riboli E & Norat T (2003) Epidemiologic evidence of the protective effect of fruit and vegetables on cancer risk. Am J Clin Nutr 78, 3 Suppl., 559S–569S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. World Cancer Research Fund (1997) Food, Nutrition and the Prevention of Cancer. A Global Perspective. Washington, DC: American Institute for Cancer Research. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. World Health Organization (2003) Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases: Report of a Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation. WHO Technical Report Series no. 916. Geneva: WHO. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Florence MD, Asbridge M & Veugelers PJ (2008) Diet quality and academic performance. J Sch Health 78, 209–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2010) State-specific trends in fruit and vegetable consumption among adults – United States, 2000–2009. MMWR Morb Mortal Weekly Rep 59, 1125–1130. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Krebs-Smith SM, Guenther PM, Subar AF et al. (2010) Americans do not meet federal dietary recommendations. J Nutr 140, 1832–1838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Cutler GJ, Flood A, Hannan P et al. (2009) Major patterns of dietary intake in adolescents and their stability over time. J Nutr 139, 323–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Resnicow K, Smith M, Baranowski T et al. (1998) 2-year tracking of children’s fruit and vegetable intake. J Am Diet Assoc 98, 785–789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Singer MR, Moore LL, Garrahie EJ et al. (1995) The tracking of nutrient intake in young children: the Framingham Children’s Study. Am J Public Health 85, 1673–1677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Nicklas TA, Baranowski T, Baranowski JC et al. (2001) Family and child-care provider influences on preschool children’s fruit, juice, and vegetable consumption. Nutr Rev 59, 224–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. O’Connor T, Watson K, Hughes S et al. (2010) Health professionals’ and dietetics practitioners’ perceived effectiveness of fruit and vegetable parenting practices across six countries. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 1065–1071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. O’Connor TM, Hughes SO, Watson KB et al. (2010) Parenting practices are associated with fruit and vegetable consumption in pre-school children. Public Health Nutr 13, 91–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Cullen KW, Baranowski T, Rittenberry L et al. (2000) Socioenvironmental influences on children’s fruit, juice and vegetable consumption as reported by parents: reliability and validity of measures. Public Health Nutr 3, 345–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Baranowski T, Chen TA, O’Connor T et al. (2013) Dimensions of vegetable parenting practices among preschoolers. Appetite 69, 89–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Baranowski T, Beltran A, Chen TA et al. (2014) Predicting use of ineffective vegetable parenting practices with the Model of Goal Directed Behavior. Public Health Nutr (Epublication ahead of print version). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Baranowski T, Beltran A, Chen TA et al. (2013) Predicting use of ineffective responsive, structure and control vegetable parenting practices with the model of goal directed behavior. J Food Res 6, 80–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Darling N & Steinberg L (1993) Parenting style as context: an integrative model. Psychol Bull 113, 487–496. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Maccoby E & Martin J (1983) Socialization in the context of the family: parent–child interaction. In Handbook of Child Psychology, 4th ed., pp. 1–101 [PH Mussen, editor]. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Slater MA & Power TG (1987) Multidimensional assessment of parenting in single-parent families. In Advances in Family Intervention, Assessment and Theory, pp. 197–228 [JP Vincent, editor]. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Hughes SO, O’Connor TM & Power TG (2008) Parenting and children’s eating patterns: examining control in a broader context. Int J Child Adolesc Health 1, 323–329. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Cerin E, Barnett A & Baranowski T (2009) Testing theories of dietary behavior change in youth using the mediating variable model with intervention programs. J Nutr Educ Behav 41, 309–318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Shaikh AR, Yaroch AL, Nebeling L et al. (2008) Psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption in adults: a review of the literature. Am J Prev Med 34, 535–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Baranowski T, Beltran A, Chen TA et al. (2013) Psychometric assessment of scales for a Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices (MGDVPP). Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 10, 110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Hingle M, Beltran A, O’Connor T et al. (2012) A model of goal directed vegetable parenting practices. Appetite 58, 444–449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Bagozzi RP, Baumgartner H & Pieters R (1998) Goal-directed emotions. Cogn Emot 12, 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Leone L, Perugini M & Ercolani AP (2004) Studying, practicing, and mastering: a test of the model of goal-directed behavior (MGB) in the software learning domain. J Appl Soc Psychol 34, 1945–1973. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Perugini M & Bagozzi RP (2001) The role of desires and anticipated emotions in goal-directed behaviours: broadening and deepening the theory of planned behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol 40, 79–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Taylor SD, Bagozzi RP & Gaither CA (2005) Decision making and effort in the self-regulation of hypertension: testing two competing theories. Br J Health Psychol 10, 505–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Ajzen I (2002) Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. J Appl Soc Psychol 32, 665–683. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Ajzen I (1991) The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Rhodes R & Dickau L (2012) Experimental evidence for the intention–behavior relationship in the physical activity domain: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol 31, 724–727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Sheeran P (2002) Intention–behavior relations: a conceptual and empirical review. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 12, 1–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Sniehotta FF, Scholz U & Schwarzer R (2005) Bridging the intention–behaviour gap: planning, self-efficacy, and action control in the adoption and maintenance of physical exercise. Psychol Health 20, 143–160. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Ryan RM & Deci EL (2000) Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol 55, 68–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. De Bruijn GJ, Keer M, Conner M et al. (2012) Using implicit associations towards fruit consumption to understand fruit consumption behaviour and habit strength relationships. J Health Psychol 17, 479–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Lytle LA, Varnell S, Murray DM et al. (2003) Predicting adolescents’ intake of fruits and vegetables. J Nutr Educ Behav 35, 170–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Hinkin TR (1995) A review of scale development practices in the study of organization. J Manage 21, 967–988. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Clark LA & Watson D (1995) Construct validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psychol Assess 7, 309–319. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Kutner MH, Nachtsheim CJ, Neter J et al. (2005) Applied Linear Statistical Models, 5th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Gardner B, Bruijn G & Lally P (2011) A systematic review and meta-analysis of applications of the Self-Report Habit Index to nutrition and physical activity behaviours. Ann Behav Med 42, 174–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Loth KA, MacLehose RF, Fulkerson JA et al. (2013) Eat this, not that! Parental demographic correlates of food-related parenting practices. Appetite 60, 140–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Saxton J, Carnell S, van Jaarsveld CHM et al. (2009) Maternal education is associated with feeding style. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 894–898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Morawska A & West F (2013) Do parents of obese children use ineffective parenting strategies? J Child Health Care 17, 375–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Carlson M & Morrison RS (2009) Study design, precision, and validity in observational studies. J Palliat Med 12, 77–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Noordzij M, Dekker FW, Zoccali C et al. (2009) Study designs in clinical research. Nephron Clin Pract 113, c218–c221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. US Census Bureau (2014) State and County QuickFacts: Houston, TX. http://quickfacts.census.gov (accessed May 2014).
  • 52. Diep CS, Chen TA, Davies VF et al. (2014) Influence of behavioral theory on fruit and vegetable intervention effectiveness among children: a meta-analysis. J Nutr Educ Behav (In the Press). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Baranowski T (2011) Understanding the behavioral linkages needed for designing effective interventions to increase fruit and vegetable intake in diverse populations. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 1472–1475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Public Health Nutrition are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES