Abstract
Acetaminophen, an effective and popular over-the-counter pain reliever (e.g., the active ingredient in Tylenol), has recently been shown to blunt individuals’ reactivity to a range of negative stimuli in addition to physical pain. Because accumulating research has shown that individuals’ reactivity to both negative and positive stimuli can be influenced by a single factor (an idea known as differential susceptibility), we conducted two experiments testing whether acetaminophen blunted individuals’ evaluations of and emotional reactions to both negative and positive images from the International Affective Picture System. Participants who took acetaminophen evaluated unpleasant stimuli less negatively and pleasant stimuli less positively, compared with participants who took a placebo. Participants in the acetaminophen condition also rated both negative and positive stimuli as less emotionally arousing than did participants in the placebo condition (Studies 1 and 2), whereas nonevaluative ratings (extent of color saturation in each image; Study 2) were not affected by drug condition. These findings suggest that acetaminophen has a general blunting effect on individuals’ evaluative and emotional processing, irrespective of negative or positive valence.
Keywords: acetaminophen, evaluation, attitudes, affect, emotion, differential susceptibility
When people experience pain, one of the most common treatments they reach for is acetaminophen (the active ingredient in Tylenol). From dental pains to ankle sprains, acetaminophen is an effective pain reliever for a wide variety of physical ailments. It is thus unsurprising that acetaminophen is the most popular over-the-counter means of pain relief in the United States, being taken by an estimated 50 million Americans each week (Kaufman, Kelly, Rosenberg, Anderson, & Mitchell, 2002).
Recent psychological evidence has provocatively demonstrated that acetaminophen reduces individuals’ sensitivity to a range of negative stimuli beyond bodily aches and pains. When taken over the course of 3 weeks, for instance, acetaminophen (compared with a placebo) blunts hurt feelings that individuals report experiencing in their social relationships (DeWall et al., 2010). This finding is consistent with prior notions that pains originating from both physical and social sources share common neurochemical underpinnings (Panksepp, 1998).
A one-time dose of acetaminophen likewise attenuates individuals’ negative reactions to emotionally evocative stimuli. Specifically, work by Randles, Heine, and Santos (2013) suggested that participants who received an acute dose of acetaminophen experienced less negativity when thinking about their own mortality than did participants who received a placebo. Recent findings have likewise shown that acetaminophen can reduce the discomfort that people experience when making difficult decisions (DeWall, Chester, & White, 2015).
Acetaminophen: Psychological Mechanisms of Action
These intriguing findings raise a pertinent question: How can a pain reliever have an ameliorating effect on such a wide range of negative experiences, painful or otherwise? Though which specific neurochemical pathways are affected by acetaminophen is not entirely clear (Anderson, 2008; Pini, Sandrini, & Vitale, 1996; Smith, 2009), accumulating evidence indicates that the brain is a critical site (Graham, Davies, Day, Mohamudally, & Scott, 2013). Therefore, one potential mechanism explaining these effects is that acetaminophen alters one or more psychological processes, which in turn affects the magnitude of individuals’ evaluations of any negative stimulus. In this sense, the process of evaluation—that is, associating a stimulus with some degree of negativity, positivity, or both (Cacioppo & Berntson, 1994)—may be affected by the neurochemical effects of acetaminophen in the brain.
Indeed, a wide variety of existing theories and empirical research suggest that if acetaminophen attenuates negative evaluations, it may likewise blunt positive evaluations. For instance, individuals scoring lower in emotional expressivity (Gross & John, 1997, 1998), affect intensity (Larsen, 2009), or their need for affect (Maio & Esses, 2001) will react less extremely to both negative and positive experiences (e.g., Britt, Millard, Sundareswaran, & Moore, 2009; Schimmack & Diener, 1997; Suh, Diener, & Fujita, 1996). These findings are broadly consistent with data from neuroscience showing that negative and positive reactions are each associated with shared brain networks that are critical for evaluative and emotional processing (Berntson et al., 2011; Craig, 2009; Lindquist & Barrett, 2012). For example, Berntson and colleagues (2011) examined how insula damage (relative to damage to the amygdala or control regions) affected individuals’ evaluations of negative and positive images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2008). Results indicated that insula damage led to reduced negative evaluations of unpleasant stimuli and reduced positive evaluations of pleasant stimuli. This reduction in the extremity of both negative and positive evaluations following insula damage is intriguing in light of evidence indicating that acetaminophen attenuates activity in the insula and other brain regions (DeWall et al., 2010).
We propose that an idea originally arising from developmental psychology—specifically, the theory of differential susceptibility (Belsky & Pluess, 2009)—provides a parsimonious theoretical framework for understanding these diverse but consistent observations of individuals’ evaluative sensitivity to both negative and positive stimuli. The theory of differential susceptibility predicts that the same individuals who are more likely to struggle in negative, stressful environments might also be more likely to thrive in positive, nurturing environments (see Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007). It therefore follows that individuals who are less sensitive to negative experiences are likewise less responsive to positive experiences. As a result—and consistent with individual differences in emotional expressivity, affect intensity, and need for affect—past research that has examined psychological processes that lead people to be more or less sensitive to negative information may in fact be describing factors that cause individuals to be differentially sensitive to both negative and positive influences.
These observations suggest that the previously observed blunting effects of acetaminophen on various negative evaluations may represent only half the story. Specifically, acetaminophen may be exerting a broader attenuating effect on individuals’ evaluative and emotional processing of stimuli presented in their environment, be they painful or pleasant in nature. Therefore, we propose that this drug may more generally blunt the extremity of all evaluations, especially toward increasingly emotionally evocative stimuli. That is, contrary to existing assumptions, acetaminophen may actually reduce positive reactions as well as negative ones.
Overview of the Present Research
On the basis of existing theories and evidence of broader evaluative and emotional sensitivity, we predicted that people taking acetaminophen would experience blunted negative reactions to unpleasant stimuli and blunted positive reactions to pleasant stimuli, compared with people taking a placebo. Our first study was designed as an initial test of this hypothesis and was modeled on the aforementioned study of patients with insula lesions (Berntson et al., 2011). A second, essentially identical study was conducted not only to replicate these findings, but also to test whether the effects of acetaminophen were unique to evaluative judgments or whether acetaminophen might affect any and all judgments of relative magnitude (e.g., the degree of color saturation). In accord with recently recommended approaches to presenting the results of multiple studies through combined analyses (Eich, 2014), we collapsed the results of the two studies and submitted participants’ evaluations, self-reports of emotional arousal (Studies 1 and 2), and judgments about color saturation (Study 2) to the same between-within-participants mixed-model analyses.
Method
Eighty-two participants in Study 1 and 85 participants in Study 2 were recruited to participate in an experiment on “Tylenol and social cognition” in exchange for course credit. Our stopping rule of at least 80 participants per study was based on previously published research on acetaminophen (DeWall et al., 2010; Randles et al., 2013), in which 30 to 50 participants were recruited per condition (i.e., acetaminophen vs. a placebo). All procedures were identical across the two studies except where noted. The analyses reported here for the combined studies are reported for each study separately in the Supplemental Material available online.
Stimuli
Forty pictures from the IAPS were used as stimuli. These pictures were selected a priori from five categories based on normative evaluations (Berntson et al., 2011; Lang et al., 2008). These consisted of 10 extremely unpleasant stimuli (IAPS IDs: 2205, 2683, 2730, 2800, 3301, 3530, 6350, 9040, 9300, 9571), 5 moderately unpleasant stimuli (IAPS IDs: 1270, 2590, 2694, 5971, 9001), 10 neutral stimuli (IAPS IDs: 1670, 2372, 2570, 5395, 5520, 7000, 7041, 7175, 7186, 7224), 5 moderately pleasant stimuli (IAPS IDs: 1450, 1602, 2510, 2791, 5711), and 10 extremely pleasant stimuli (IAPS IDs: 2040, 2091, 4626, 4660, 5470, 7502, 8185, 8190, 8200, 8501). These stimuli depict a variety of social and nonsocial contexts and experiences.
Procedure
Participants were randomly assigned to take either an acute dose of 1,000 mg of acetaminophen or a placebo, both in a liquid vehicle. Experimenters and participants were unaware of participants’ assignment to condition. After a 60-min waiting period to allow acetaminophen to enter the brain (Anderson, 2008; Singla et al., 2012; Smith, 2009), participants completed all relevant measures on computers within individual cubicles.
First, the 40 pictures from the IAPS were presented in a random order, and participants evaluated each stimulus by responding to the question, “To what extent is this picture positive or negative?” using an 11-point scale from −5 (extremely negative) to 5 (extremely positive). Then, participants saw all 40 images once more in a different random order and rated their level of emotional arousal by responding to the question, “To what extent does this picture make you feel an emotional reaction?” using an 11-point scale from 0 (I feel little to no emotion) to 10 (I feel an extreme amount of emotion; Berntson et al., 2011). For both measures, each stimulus was present on screen until participants responded. Eight participants in Study 1 (3 in the acetaminophen condition, 5 in the placebo condition) and 6 participants in Study 2 (2 in the acetaminophen condition, 4 in the placebo condition) responded to at least one of the measures in less than 500 ms and were therefore excluded from analyses, as they appeared not to be taking the study seriously.
In Study 2, after evaluating and indicating their emotional arousal toward each stimulus, participants saw all images one last time, in a different randomized order. During this phase, participants were asked to respond to the question, “To what extent is the color blue represented in this picture?” using an 11-point scale from 0 (The picture has zero blue color) to 10 (This picture is 100% the color blue). This measure was designed to be structurally similar to the other measures while having participants focus on a dimension of judgment that would be minimally influenced by evaluative aspects, in order to test whether acetaminophen affects evaluations specifically or whether it blunts any and all judgments of magnitude.
From participants’ responses, we computed three measures of evaluation and emotional arousal. First, participants’ evaluation extremity (distance from the scale midpoint of 0; Abelson, 1995) and emotional arousal toward all 40 stimuli were averaged to create a global score for each measure. Second, participants’ evaluation extremity and emotional arousal toward neutral, moderate (both positive and negative), and extreme (both positive and negative) stimuli were computed in order to analyze how stimulus extremity, regardless of its negative or positive normative rating, might be affected as a function of treatment. Finally, participants’ raw evaluations and emotional arousal toward the stimuli were averaged within each of the five normative stimulus categories (extremely unpleasant, moderately unpleasant, neutral, moderately pleasant, extremely pleasant) to analyze how these measures might be affected by treatment in varying directions.
Results
At the end of Study 1, participants indicated whether they thought they took acetaminophen or a placebo, or whether they had no idea. Forty-two percent of participants responded that they did not know. Among participants who responded that they had taken one treatment over another, a chi-square test of independence was performed to examine whether actual treatment predicted participants’ perceived treatment. This test yielded a marginally significant result, χ2(1, N = 43) = 3.41, p = .065. Specifically, 67% of participants who guessed that they took acetaminophen were actually in the placebo condition. In Study 2, participants were asked to indicate whether they thought they took acetaminophen or a placebo without the option to say explicitly that they did not know. A chi-square test of independence yielded a non-significant result, χ2(1, N = 79) = 0.00, p = .950. Specifically, 54% of participants who guessed that they took acetaminophen were actually in the placebo condition. These results suggest that expectancy effects of treatment were not driving the findings.
Evaluations
Participants’ overall evaluation extremity to all stimuli across the two studies was submitted to an independent-samples t test, with treatment as the between-participants factor. As expected, participants who took acetaminophen evaluated the stimuli as less extreme (M = 1.96, SD = 0.56) than participants who received a placebo (M = 2.25, SD = 0.49), t(151) = 3.40, p = .001, ηp2 = .071 (Fig. 1).
Next, we accounted for differences between stimulus categories by submitting participants’ evaluation extremity across the two studies to a 2 (treatment: acetaminophen, placebo) × 3 (image category: neutral, moderate, extreme) mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA), with treatment as a between-participants factor and image category as a within-participants factor. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ2(2) = 16.92, p < .001. Because the epsilon value was greater than 0.75 (ε = 0.92), degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity. A main effect of image category was found, F(1.8, 277.8) = 1,145.33, p < .001, ηp2 = .884. Specifically, participants rated neutral stimuli least extremely (in either a positive or negative direction), moderate stimuli relatively more extremely than neutral stimuli, and extreme stimuli relatively more extremely than moderate stimuli. These results reflected normative ratings of the stimuli.
As expected, however, this analysis yielded a main effect of treatment, F(1, 151) = 10.43, p = .002, ηp2 = .065, and the predicted interaction of treatment and image category, F(1.8, 277.8) = 5.32, p = .007, ηp2 = .034 (Fig. 2). Contrast analyses within each category revealed that participants who took acetaminophen evaluated extreme stimuli (M = 3.01, SD = 0.89) as significantly less extreme (in either a positive or negative direction) than did participants who received a placebo (M = 3.46, SD = 0.71), t(151) = 3.38, p = .001. Likewise, participants who took acetaminophen evaluated moderate stimuli (M = 1.53, SD = 0.68) as significantly less extreme than did participants who received a placebo (M = 1.76, SD = 0.62), t(151) = 2.18, p = .030. Evaluation extremity toward neutral stimuli did not differ as a function of treatment, p = .422.
Finally, participants’ raw evaluations across studies were submitted to a 2 (treatment: acetaminophen, placebo) × 5 (normative rating: extremely unpleasant, moderately unpleasant, neutral, moderately pleasant, extremely pleasant) mixed-model ANOVA. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ2(9) = 185.78, p < .001. Because the epsilon value was less than or equal to 0.75 (ε = 0.58), degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity. A main effect of normative rating was found, F(2.3, 347.1) = 1,302.46, p < .001, ηp2 = .896. Specifically, participants rated extremely unpleasant pictures more negatively and extremely pleasant pictures more positively in a linear fashion, consistent with normative ratings of the stimuli. There was also a marginally significant main effect of treatment on evaluations, F(1, 151) = 3.42, p = .066, ηp2 = .022, which indicated that participants who took acetaminophen tended to rate the images more negatively overall (M = −0.25) than did participants who took a placebo (M = −0.17).
These main effects were qualified by a significant interaction of treatment and normative rating, F(2.3, 347.1) = 6.19, p = .001, ηp2 = .039 (Fig. 3), consistent with our hypothesis. Contrast analyses revealed that participants who took acetaminophen rated extremely unpleasant stimuli (M = −3.40, SD = 1.00) significantly less negatively than did participants who received a placebo (M = −3.73, SD = 0.78), t(151) = 2.28, p = .024. Likewise, participants who took acetaminophen also rated extremely pleasant stimuli (M = 2.62, SD = 1.01) significantly less positively than did participants who received a placebo (M = 3.18, SD = 0.89), t(151) = 3.60, p < .001.
Participants who took acetaminophen also rated moderately pleasant stimuli (M = 1.24, SD = 0.93) significantly less positively than participants who received a placebo (M = 1.57, SD = 0.92), t(151) = 2.18, p = .031. Finally, participants who took acetaminophen additionally tended to rate moderately unpleasant stimuli less negatively (M = −1.72, SD = 0.99) and neutral stimuli less positively (M = 0.01, SD = 0.43), compared with participants who received a placebo (Ms = −1.90, 0.12, SDs = 0.97, 0.49), ts(151) = 0.75, 1.73, ps = .456, .085, respectively, although these differences were not statistically significant. Entering study as a between-participants factor in each of the above analyses yielded no significant interactions, ps > .6, which indicates that the results were similar across experiments. Thus, these findings illustrate that acetaminophen blunted participants’ evaluations toward both unpleasant and pleasant stimuli, and this effect was most pronounced for stimuli that were the most extreme in either a negative or positive direction.
Emotional arousal
For emotional arousal, we first submitted participants’ overall emotional arousal to all stimuli across the two studies to an independent-samples t test, with treatment as the between-participants factor. As expected, participants who took acetaminophen were overall less emotionally aroused by the stimuli (M = 4.21, SD = 1.23) than participants who received a placebo (M = 4.88, SD = 1.29), t(151) = 3.28, p = .001, ηp2 = .067 (Fig. 4).
Next, we submitted participants’ emotional arousal to the stimuli across studies as categorized by their neutral, moderate, or extreme normative ratings to a 2 (treatment: acetaminophen, placebo) × 3 (image category: neutral, moderate, or extreme) mixed-model ANOVA, with treatment as a between-participants factor and image category as a within-participants factor. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ2(2) = 57.41, p < .001, so degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.77). A main effect of image category was found, F(1.5, 232.5) = 856.60, p < .001, ηp2 = .850. Specifically, participants were least emotionally aroused by neutral stimuli, were relatively more emotionally aroused by moderately pleasant and unpleasant stimuli, and were most emotionally aroused by extremely pleasant and extremely unpleasant stimuli, which reflected normative ratings of the stimuli.
As expected, however, this analysis yielded a significant main effect of treatment, F(1, 151) = 9.18, p = .003, ηp2 = .057, and a significant interaction of treatment and image category, F(1.5, 232.5) = 4.08, p = .027, ηp2 = .026 (Fig. 5). Contrast analyses within each category of stimuli revealed that participants who took acetaminophen were significantly less emotionally aroused by extreme stimuli (M = 5.85, SD = 1.67) than were participants who received a placebo (M = 6.76, SD = 1.53), t(151) = 3.52, p = .001. Likewise, participants who took acetaminophen were significantly less emotionally aroused by moderate stimuli (M = 3.83, SD = 1.53) relative to participants who received a placebo (M = 4.44, SD = 1.57), t(151) = 2.43, p = .016. Participants’ emotional arousal toward neutral stimuli did not differ as a function of treatment, p = .224.
Finally, we submitted participants’ emotional-arousal ratings within each of the five normative categories across studies to the same 2 × 5 mixed-model ANOVA used to analyze their evaluations. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ2(9) = 101.64, p < .001, so degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.72). A main effect of normative rating was obtained, F(2.9, 436.7) = 489.47, p < .001, ηp2 = .764. Specifically, participants expressed higher emotional arousal toward stimuli that were normatively more extreme in valence in a quadratic fashion, with the highest arousal toward extremely unpleasant and extremely pleasant stimuli, and the lowest arousal toward neutral stimuli.
As expected, however, a significant main effect of treatment was obtained, F(1, 151) = 9.80, p = .002, ηp2 = .061, as was the predicted interaction, F(2.9, 436.7) = 2.67, p = .049, ηp2 = .017 (Fig. 6). Contrast analyses indicated that participants who took acetaminophen were significantly less emotionally aroused by extremely pleasant stimuli (M = 4.89, SD = 1.84) than were participants who took a placebo (M = 6.04, SD = 1.60), t(151) = 4.10, p < .001. Similarly, participants who received acetaminophen were significantly less emotionally aroused by extremely unpleasant stimuli (M = 6.81, SD = 1.91) and moderately unpleasant stimuli (M = 4.88, SD = 1.83) than were participants assigned to the placebo condition (Ms = 7.49, 6.42, SDs = 1.91, 1.96), ts(151) = 2.20, 1.77, ps = .030, .078, respectively. Furthermore, participants who took acetaminophen were significantly less emotionally aroused by moderately pleasant stimuli (M = 2.79, SD = 1.71) than participants who took a placebo (M = 3.47, SD = 1.66), t(151) = 2.46, p = .015. Participants did not differ in their emotional arousal toward neutral stimuli as a function of treatment, p = .224. Entering study as a between-participants factor in each of the analyses on emotional arousal yielded no significant interactions, ps > .17, which indicates that the results were similar across experiments. In all, compared with a placebo, acetaminophen attenuated participants’ emotional reactivity in general, and it did so more potently for participants’ emotional reactions toward stimuli that were increasingly extreme, regardless of their negative or positive valence.
Nonevaluative judgments (ratings of color saturation)
Finally, to test whether acetaminophen specifically affected evaluative judgments or whether its effects generalize to any and all judgments of magnitude, we analyzed participants’ ratings of how much the color blue was represented in each image (Study 2). To mirror the analytical approach for the other ratings, we computed an objective measure of how much blue was represented in each image akin to the normative ratings of valence and arousal of the images. For each image, we computed the average red, green, and blue levels (i.e., primary colors in the digital color space) across all pixels. Images were divided into five categories by computing the quintiles for levels of the blue component.
We first submitted participants’ overall blue ratings to all stimuli to an independent-samples t test, with treatment as the between-participants factor. This analysis yielded a nonsignificant result, t(77) = 0.33, p = .744. Specifically, participants who took acetaminophen did not significantly differ in their blueness ratings of stimuli overall (M = 3.45, SD = 0.98) compared with participants who received a placebo (M = 3.38, SD = 0.83).
We further submitted participants’ blue ratings across the five levels of the images’ blue content to a 2 (treatment: acetaminophen, placebo) × 5 (objective blue content: bottom quintile, second quintile, third quintile, fourth quintile, top quintile) mixed-model ANOVA. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ2(9) = 40.28, p < .001, so degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.82). Objective blue content was a significant predictor of participants’ blue ratings, F(3.1, 253.7) = 269.05, p < .001, ηp2 = .777. Specifically, images in the top quintile of objective blue content were perceived to contain more blue than images in the bottom quintile, with the middle quintiles following in linear fashion, which reflected prior computations of objective color content.
However, there was no effect of treatment, F(1, 77) = 0.11, p = .744, ηp2 = .001, and no interaction of treatment and objective blue content, F(3.1, 253.7) = 0.11, p = .955, ηp2 = .001. Contrast analyses corroborated these findings, revealing that treatment did not significantly affect judgments of color saturation within any individual quintile, ps > .7. Thus, we obtained no evidence that the effects of acetaminophen generalize beyond judgments specific to evaluative processes.
Discussion
Across two studies, we demonstrated that acetaminophen attenuates individuals’ evaluations and emotional reactions to negative and positive stimuli alike. These results build on recent psychological research illustrating that acetaminophen can blunt the intensity with which individuals experience negative events that originate from physical, social, or cognitive sources (DeWall et al., 2015; DeWall et al., 2010; Randles et al., 2013). Further, these findings expand on the research to date to show that acetaminophen blunts positive evaluations in like fashion. Critically, this observed effect of acetaminophen was unique to judgments of valence and their related degree of emotional arousal, as the effect did not appear for other nonevaluative judgments of magnitude toward the same stimuli. In all, rather than being labeled as merely a pain reliever, acetaminophen might be better described as an all-purpose emotion reliever.
These effects of acetaminophen on evaluations of both negative and positive stimuli indicate that the neurochemical changes elicited by acetaminophen affect one or more related psychological evaluative processes. This mechanism of reducing valence sensitivity may be relevant for the theory of differential susceptibility (Belsky & Pluess, 2009), which is built on the premise that a common factor (e.g., genetic variation) affects reactivity to both negative and positive events. For example, individuals with one form of a gene (short/short serotonin-transporter-linked polymorphic region, or 5-HTTLPR, genotype) had the highest levels of depressive symptomatology when experiencing negative life events but also the lowest depressive symptomatology when experiencing positive life events. For individuals with another form of the gene (long/long genotype), life events had little relationship to depressive symptomatology (Taylor et al., 2006). Such differential-susceptibility effects of the 5-HTTLPR genotype have been supported by a recent meta-analysis (van IJzendoorn, Belsky, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2012).
The effects of the 5-HTTLPR genotype are thought to be due to effects on serotoninergic neurotransmission (Lesch et al., 1996; Way & Taylor, 2010). Notably, acetaminophen also affects serotonin signaling in the brain, which is necessary for the analgesic effects of the drug (Pini et al., 1996). Because acetaminophen and 5-HTTLPR both apparently affect serotonin signaling, they may be influencing a common neurochemical-psychological process that influences individuals’ evaluations more broadly. In other words, the blunting effect of acetaminophen on evaluations of both negative and positive stimuli may lead to psychological reactions akin to those expected for individuals with the 5-HTTLPR long/long genotype. This suggests that modulation of the extremity of evaluation is a possible psychological mechanism that gives rise to differential susceptibility. If so, these findings suggest that the neurochemical substrates contributing to differential susceptibility can be experimentally manipulated in the laboratory. Future research will be needed to determine whether serotoninergic signaling or some other neurochemical process, such as inflammatory signaling (Graham et al., 2013), is responsible for reducing evaluation extremity.
These findings additionally advance understanding of the psychological process of evaluation. Whether forming attitudes, pursuing goals, or experiencing emotions, people are constantly evaluating themselves, others, and their environment in degrees of negativity, positivity, or both. That a drug purported to relieve negative evaluations of pain also reduces positive evaluations of pleasant stimuli suggests the existence of a common evaluative psychological process that influences a wide range of thoughts and behaviors. This might mean, for instance, that certain methods designed to specifically alter individuals’ reactivity to negative stimuli (e.g., treatment of phobias) could, if too broadly applied, potentially change their sensitivity to emotionally evocative stimuli more generally, including positive events (e.g., causing them to feel less joy at a wedding). It is interesting that such diminished evaluation sensitivity could also presumably cause people to feel less conflicted, indecisive, or uncomfortable when they experience ambivalence toward individuals or experiences that elicit both negative and positive reactions (e.g., Priester & Petty, 1996; Rydell & Durso, 2012). Identifying the stage in evaluative processing that acetaminophen is affecting will be an important area for investigation in order to determine whether acetaminophen is altering evaluative processing itself or the perceived inputs or outputs to this process.
Some limitations of our work should be noted. Specifically, we cannot ascertain from the current studies whether acetaminophen might blunt individuals’ attention or motivation to process emotionally evocative stimuli instead of (or in addition to) their evaluative processing of these stimuli. Future research might shed more light on whether acetaminophen also alters attentional or motivational mechanisms related to the processing of valenced stimuli, as noted in previous studies of acetaminophen and evaluation (Randles et al., 2013).
In all, it is apparent that using acetaminophen for the treatment of pain might have broader consequences than previously thought. These findings call for additional research at both psychological and neurochemical levels to expand on and better understand the mechanisms through which acetaminophen influences evaluation. Acetaminophen and other pharmacological treatments are likely to be valuable tools for understanding the basic mechanisms of evaluation and emotional reactivity, which are critical for many aspects of life studied across the different fields of psychology.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Funding
This material is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship under Grant No. DGE-0822215 (to G. R. O. Durso), and Grant 8KL2TR000112-05 from the National Center For Advancing Translational Sciences (to B. M. Way).
We appreciate the assistance of William Malarkey in running each study. We additionally thank Hanah Chapman, Kristen Dunfield, Kyle Ratner, and the Social Neurochemistry Lab, along with Rich Petty, Russ Fazio, Duane Wegener, Pablo Briñol, the Attitudes and Persuasion Lab, and the Group for Attitudes and Persuasion at Ohio State from 2012 to 2014 for helpful comments. Finally, we thank Emily Hellwig, Elleni Avila, and Huidong Yang for their assistance with data collection.
Footnotes
Author Contributions
G. R. O. Durso developed the study concept. All authors contributed to the study design. Data were collected and analyzed by G. R. O. Durso and A. Luttrell. G. R. O. Durso drafted the manuscript, and A. Luttrell and B. M. Way provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Open Practices
The consent form completed by participants in these studies stated, “Only the investigators and their research assistants will have access to the original research data.” As a consequence, we cannot share the original data publicly at this time. However, for anyone interested in accessing the data, we would gladly add you as a co-investigator to our current research protocol. The complete Open Practices Disclosure for this article can be found at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information can be found at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
References
- Abelson RP. Attitude extremity. In: Petty RE, Krosnick JA, editors. Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1995. pp. 25–42. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson BJ. Paracetamol (acetaminophen): Mechanisms of action. Pediatric Anesthesia. 2008;18:915–921. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9592.2008.02764.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:300–304. [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson GG, Norman GJ, Bechara A, Bruss J, Tranel D, Cacioppo JT. The insula and evaluative processes. Psychological Science. 2011;22:80–86. doi: 10.1177/0956797610391097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britt TW, Millard MR, Sundareswaran PT, Moore D. Personality variables predict strength-related attitude dimensions across objects. Journal of Personality. 2009;77:859–882. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00567.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG. Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:401–423. [Google Scholar]
- Craig AD. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeWall CN, Chester DS, White DS. Can acetaminophen reduce the pain of decision-making? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2015;56:117–120. [Google Scholar]
- DeWall CN, MacDonald G, Webster GD, Masten CL, Baumeister RF, Powell C, Eisenberger NI. Acetaminophen reduces social pain: Behavioral and neural evidence. Psychological Science. 2010;21:931–937. doi: 10.1177/0956797610374741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eich E. Business not as usual. Psychological Science. 2014;25:3–6. doi: 10.1177/0956797613512465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham GG, Davies MJ, Day RO, Mohamudally A, Scott KF. The modern pharmacology of paracetamol: Therapeutic actions, mechanism of action, metabolism, toxicity and recent pharmacological findings. Inflammopharmacology. 2013;21:201–232. doi: 10.1007/s10787-013-0172-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, John OP. Revealing feelings: Facets of emotional expressivity in self-reports, peer ratings, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72:435–448. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.2.435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, John OP. Mapping the domain of expressivity: Multimethod evidence for a hierarchical model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:170–191. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman DW, Kelly JP, Rosenberg L, Anderson TE, Mitchell AA. Recent patterns of medication use in the ambulatory adult population of the United States: The Slone survey. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2002;287:337–344. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.3.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville: University of Florida; 2008. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. [Google Scholar]
- Larsen RJ. Affect intensity. In: Leary MR, Hoyle RH, editors. Handbook of individual differences in social behavior. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 241–254. [Google Scholar]
- Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Murphy DL. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindquist KA, Barrett LF. A functional architecture of the human brain: Emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:533–540. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maio GR, Esses VM. The need for affect: Individual differences in the motivation to approach or avoid emotions. Journal of Personality. 2001;69:583–615. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.694156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Pini LA, Sandrini M, Vitale G. The antinociceptive action of paracetamol is associated with changes in the serotonergic system in the rat brain. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1996;308:31–40. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(96)00261-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Priester JM, Petty RE. The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: Relating the positive and negative bases of attitudes to subjective ambivalence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71:431–449. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.3.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randles D, Heine SJ, Santos N. The common pain of surrealism and death: Acetaminophen reduces compensatory affirmation following meaning threats. Psychological Science. 2013;24:966–973. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rydell RJ, Durso GRO. Can I borrow a feeling? Spillover of negative arousal from inconsistent information during attitude formation diminishes perceptions of well-being. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2012;48:575–578. [Google Scholar]
- Schimmack U, Diener E. Affect intensity: Separating intensity and frequency in repeatedly measured affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;73:1313–1329. [Google Scholar]
- Singla NK, Parulan C, Samson R, Hutchinson J, Bushnell R, Beja EG, Royal MA. Plasma and cerebrospinal fluid pharmacokinetic parameters after single-dose administration of intravenous, oral, or rectal acetaminophen. Pain Practice. 2012;12:523–532. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-2500.2012.00556.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith HS. Potential analgesic mechanisms of acetaminophen. Pain Physician. 2009;12:269–280. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suh E, Diener E, Fujita F. Events and subjective well-being: Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;70:1091–1102. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.5.1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, Way BM, Welch WT, Hilmert CJ, Lehman BJ, Eisenberger NI. Early family environment, current adversity, the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism, and depressive symptomatology. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:671–676. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van IJzendoorn MH, Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Serotonin transporter genotype 5HTTLPR as a marker of differential susceptibility? A meta-analysis of child and adolescent gene-by-environment studies. Translational Psychiatry. 2012;2:Article e147. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.73. Retrieved from http://www.nature.com/tp/journal/v2/n8/full/tp201273a.html. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Way BM, Taylor SE. Social influences on health: Is serotonin a critical mediator? Psychosomatic Medicine. 2010;72:107–112. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181ce6a7d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.