Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Neurosci. 2015 Aug;129(4):512–522. doi: 10.1037/bne0000061

The Impact of Hippocampal Lesions on Trace Eyeblink Conditioning and Forebrain-Cerebellar Interactions

Craig Weiss 1, John F Disterhoft 1
PMCID: PMC4518454  NIHMSID: NIHMS705807  PMID: 26214216

Abstract

Twenty-five years ago Behavioral Neuroscience published a pivotal paper by Moyer, Deyo and Disterhoft (1990) that described the impaired acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioning in rabbits with complete removal of the hippocampus. As part of the Behavioral Neuroscience celebration commemorating the 30th anniversary of the Journal, we reflect upon the impact of that study on understanding the role of the hippocampus, forebrain, and forebrain-cerebellar interactions that mediate acquisition and retention of trace conditioned responses, and of declarative memory more globally. We discuss the expansion of the conditioning paradigm to species other than the rabbit, the heterogeneity of responses among hippocampal neurons during trace conditioning, the responsivity of hippocampal neurons following consolidation of conditioning, the role of awareness in conditioning, how blink conditioning can be used as a translational tool by assaying potential therapeutics for cognitive enhancement, how trace and delay classical conditioning may be used to investigate neurological disorders including Alzheimer's Disease and schizophrenia, and how the two paradigms may be used to understand the relationship between declarative and nondeclarative memory systems.

Keywords: Alzheimer's Disease, Declarative Memory, Hippocampus, Prefrontal cortex, Schizophrenia

Eyeblink Conditioning in the Rabbit

Eyeblink conditioning became one of the more widely used paradigms to study learning and memory during the 1970s as the rabbit model of the paradigm was developed and analyzed using behavioral psychophysics to optimize the paradigm (Schneiderman, Fuentes, Gormezano, 1962; Disterhoft, Kwan, Lo, 1977). A neurobiological analysis of the learned behavior began with an examination of the hippocampus because of the impact that hippocampal ablation had on acquisition of new declarative memories in humans that had temporal lobe resections for the treatment of intractable epilepsy, e.g. patient H.M. (Scoville & Milner, 1957). Consistent with the results of the lesion, neurophysiological recordings from rabbit hippocampus during delay eyeblink conditioning (where the unconditioned stimulus overlaps and coterminates with the conditioning stimulus) revealed learning-specific increases in the amplitude and time-course of extracellularly recorded activity from CA1 pyramidal neurons (Berger, Alger, Thompson, 1976), as expected for a structure mediating memory.

However, since others had shown that neither the dorsal hippocampus nor cortex were required for acquisition of simple delay conditioning (Oakley & Russell, 1972; Norman, Buchwald & Villablanca, 1977), the investigation of the essential pathway mediating blink conditioning extended beyond the hippocampus. The essential pathway was examined from the periphery into the central nervous system (CNS), i.e. from the retractor bulbi (RB) muscle (which pulls the eye back into the socket and causes extension of the nictitating membrane) to the accessory abducens nucleus (which has the motor neurons that innervates RB; Disterhoft, Quinn, Weiss, Shipley, 1985), and beyond (Gonzalez-Joekes & Schreurs, 2012). The essential pathway was also examined by recording activity from several sites within the central nervous system (CNS) while looking for neuronal activity that mirrored the amplitude and time course of the conditioned response (CR). The cerebellum was one site that revealed “neuronal models” of CRs (McCormick et al., 1981, 1982).

The findings that ablation of the lateral cerebellum abolished behavioral CRs and that activity in the cerebellar nucleus developed concomitantly with expression of CRs (McCormick et al., 1982) led to an intensive investigation during most of the 1980s on the role of the cerebellum in mediating delay EBC. However, the scientific wind turned back to the hippocampus in 1986 when three papers examined the role of the hippocampus in trace conditioning, where a stimulus free gap separates the two conditioning stimuli. The decade concluded with the submission of the Moyer et al. paper which was published in 1990.

The three papers examining the role of the hippocampus were by Port et al. (1986), Solomon et al. (1986), and James et al. (1987). All of them examined the effect of dorsal hippocampal lesions upon trace conditioning of the blink response in rabbit. The reports by Port and by James failed to show a significant deficit in conditioning. In fact, the learning curves and peak amplitudes of the CR presented by James et al. were almost identical between rabbits that had received the dorsal hippocampal lesion and those that had received a cortical control lesion. Their rabbits were conditioned with a 90 dB burst of white noise as the conditioning stimulus (CS) and an AC periorbital shock of 2.5 mA for 50 ms as the unconditioned stimulus (US). The study by Solomon et al. conditioned rabbits with a 1kHz, 85 dB tone and a 3 psi puff of air to the cornea after the rabbits received an aspiration lesion of the dorsal hippocampus. Their results showed a severe impairment for acquiring trace CRs. The different outcomes of the two studies may have been due to partial lesion effects, i.e. the ventral hippocampus was still intact (Weiss, Bouwmeester, et al. 1999), or a fortuitous consequence of the US modality, i.e. years later it was shown that lesions of the prefrontal cortex impaired trace conditioning when the US was an airpuff to the cornea, but not when the US was an electric shock to the periorbital region (Oswald, Maddox, Tisdale, Powell, 2010).

Given the different results of those three reports, Moyer et al. (1990) examined the effect of hippocampal lesions that included the dorsal and ventral regions on acquisition of trace CRs using a tone conditioning stimulus and an airpuff unconditioned stimulus. Importantly, they also compared 300 ms and 500 ms trace intervals upon acquisition rates, to determine if the imposed memory load affected hippocampal dependence. The results indicated that hippocampal dependence increased with memory load, i.e. lesions severely impaired acquisition of trace CRs when the trace interval was 500 ms, and had variable results when a 300 ms interval was used. The “trace 500” paradigm appears to have become the standard for examining hippocampal-dependent learning in the rabbit, and delay conditioning is often used after trace conditioning to demonstrate that the brainstem and cerebellum are intact and functioning (Weiss, Bouwmeester, Power & Disterhoft, 1999; Tseng, Guan, Disterhoft, Weiss, 2004) suggesting that higher brain centers, e.g. hippocampus, prefrontal cortex (PFC), and thalamus (Halverson, Poremba & Freeman, 2008; Steinmetz, Buss, Freeman, 2013) are critical regions for mediating the trace CR.

The modality of the US is less standard among laboratories. Some laboratories use a puff of air (or nitrogen) to the cornea, and some laboratories use a periorbital shock. The shock US appears to be more aversive and likely activates neuromodulators more so than does an airpuff. The neuromodulators may compensate for the loss of hippocampal-prefrontal cortical activation in lesion studies by either modulating the PFC directly, or modulating direct projections from the ventral hippocampus to PFC (Rosene & Van Hoesen, 1977; Witter et al., 1989). The loss of this modulation might explain why complete hippocampal lesions (those that include the ventral hippocampus) or lesions of the prefrontal cortex, particularly the caudal anterior cingulate cortex (cACC), prevent acquisition of trace EBC with an airpuff US (Kronforst-Collins & Disterhoft, 1998; Weible, McEchron, Disterhoft, 2000).

CA1 Responsivity During Delay and Trace Conditioning

Neuronal responses from CA1 hippocampus during delay conditioning are quite remarkable (Berger & Thompson, 1978; Berger, Rinaldi, Weisz & Thompson, 1983). Neurons from rabbits presented with pairs of tones and airpuffs exhibited increased activity that developed first in response to the airpuff and then gradually increased activity during the CS-US interval over the course of the session, as CRs were developing, and the increased activity preceded the behavioral response in time. This increase began within the first few trials of stimulus presentations. The correlation of neuronal activity and behavior changed with learning such that early in training neuronal activity correlated more with the unconditioned response, and later in training the activity correlated more with the conditioned response. These changes did not occur in rabbits given explicitly unpaired presentations of tones and puffs in random order, even on trials with a large unconditioned response, and not when a spontaneous blink was present. These data are strong evidence for a learning-specific change in the activity of CA1 neurons recorded from conditioned rabbits.

However, since the hippocampus is not required for delay conditioning, as previously mentioned, recordings were taken from CA1 during hippocampally-dependent trace conditioning in search for activity patterns that might reveal the nature of the role that the hippocampus plays in conditioning and memory consolidation (Solomon et al., 1986; Weiss, Kronforst-Collins & Disterhoft, 1996; Weible, O’Reilly, Weiss & Disterhoft, 2006). The results of Solomon et al. were based on multiunit data and suggested that there was more diversity in the response profiles than had been found with delay conditioning. The report by Weiss et al. analyzed single neuron data from rabbits that achieved at least 60% CRs, i.e. the rabbits had already passed the initial acquisition phase. Those data confirmed and catalogued the wide variety of responses that were found in CA1 during trace conditioning, suggesting that memory demanding trace conditioning requires the hippocampus to do critical computations that are not necessary for simple delay conditioning, and that inhibitory processes (reflected as a rate-decreasing response) are just as involved as excitatory processes (McKay, Oh, Disterhoft, 2013). The report by Weible et al. examined the hippocampus more thoroughly during acquisition. They recorded from the dorsal and ventral hippocampus of both sides of the brain (relative to the eye being stimulated). A variety of responses was again found, but this study also reported that the ipsilateral hippocampus exhibited significant increases in activity early in training while the contralateral hippocampus exhibited greater response magnitudes later in training. They also reported that the ventral hippocampus was less involved in terms of the percentage of responsive neurons (8% vs 69%), and more involved during the post-trial period than during the CS-US interval. This may reflect anatomical connections with the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis (Jacobson & Sapolsky, 1991; Herman et al., 1992) and the amygdala (Petrovich, Canteras & Swanson, 2001).

An examination of the diversity of responses was taken a step further by McEchron, Weible and Disterhoft (2001). They used a hierarchical clustering analysis to segregate neurons by differences in their response profiles and found six different categories. Interestingly, the two most frequent profiles were classified as rate-decreasing activity and accounted for 52% of the profiles. More recently we have begun to use support vector machine (SVM) and general linear modeling (GLM) approaches to classify neurons according to their response profiles and have found that the SVM approach can predict conditioned responses and their onset latency based on neuronal activity (Lawlor, Hattori, Weiss, Disterhoft & Kording, 2013). We expect that these statistical approaches will reveal interesting changes in network properties that were previously unrecognized.

Hippocampus and Memory Consolidation

The hippocampus is critically involved in acquisition of new memories, as was exemplified most frequently by the case of patient H.M. However, given that HM had intact episodic memories for events prior to his surgery, the hippocampus has been considered to act as a temporary memory buffer with permanent memory residing elsewhere, most likely in the cerebral cortex (Squire & Alvarez, 1995). The effect of this consolidation process was tested with trace EBC in the rabbit after Kim and Fanselow (1992) had demonstrated a time-limited role for the hippocampus in contextual fear conditioning. Kim, Clark, and Thompson (1995) lesioned the hippocampus either one day or one month after rabbits had attained a criterion level of performance on trace EBC. The rabbits lesioned one day after criterion were severely impaired when tested after a seven day recovery period (they did not exhibit more than about 20% of trials with conditioned responses), but rabbits that were not lesioned until one month after attaining criterion returned to pre-lesion performance levels after a slight decrease in performance on the first session following the one month hiatus. This is strong evidence that the hippocampus is not required for memory recall after consolidation has occurred, at least as measured by performance rates during trace EBC.

The biophysical basis for the hippocampus not being required following memory consolidation is likely due to the transient nature of the learning specific reduction in the amplitude of the post-burst afterhyperpolarization (AHP) of CA1 and CA3 pyramidal neurons (Moyer, Thompson, Disterhoft, 1996; Thompson, Moyer, Disterhoft, 1996a), i.e. the amplitude of the post-burst AHP returned to a baseline level within seven days after rabbits achieved the learning criterion of at least 80% CRs within one session. Apparently, this is a sufficient period of hippocampal activation to support the formation of consolidated memory representation in other regions such as the neocortex. The transient nature of hippocampal involvement in eyeblink conditioning has also been demonstrated in rats (Takehara, Kawahara, Kirino, 2003) and mice (Takehara, Kawahara, Takatsuki, Kirino, 2002) that received aspiration lesions of the hippocampus at various times after learning was achieved.

Although the hippocampus is not required to express CRs in the consolidated state, it may still be involved in expressing the CR. For example, even though HM’s semantic memory was spared, he exhibited severe impairments in remote autobiographical memory (Steinvorth, Levine & Corkin, 2005). This suggests that the hippocampus may be doing important processing during recall of consolidated memory. To address this question directly we adopted a paradigm of training our animals to criterion (plus 3–5 days) and then giving them a 30 day training hiatus before testing (and recording single neuron activity) for retention of remotely acquired memory. We examined this activity in the hippocampus after consolidation (Hattori, Chen, Weiss & Disterhoft, 2014) and also inferred hippocampal involvement by looking at the prefrontal cortex after consolidation (Hattori, Yoon, Disterhoft & Weiss, 2014). The results were dramatic and surprising, i.e., hippocampal pyramidal neurons were found to be even more responsive after the training hiatus than before the hiatus. This activity may reflect reactivation or reconsolidation processes. Strong reactivation would be needed if the memory had degraded over time (Rudy, Biedenkapp, O’Reilly, 2005). Furthermore, activity in the prelimbic cortex was also found to be significantly greater after consolidation than before consolidation and may reflect hippocampal-prefrontal interactions (Hattori, Yoon, et al., 2014) and entorhinal-prefrontal interactions (Takehara-Nishiuchi, Bared, Morrissey, 2012).

Eyeblink Conditioning in the Rat

The rabbit is an excellent animal model system for studying blink conditioning since it tolerates restraint well, has large eyes with few spontaneous eye movements, and it has a relatively large head that allows implants to be secured to the skull for studies using chronic recording and stimulation techniques. However, since other behavioral tasks are necessary to examine the more global nature of hippocampal-dependent memory, species that have more spontaneous behaviors than a rabbit would be useful. The rat is ideal for examining such behaviors, and since EBC, the hippocampus, and memory are very sensitive to the effects of age (Thompson, Moyer & Disterhoft, 1996b; Oh et al., 2010; Matthews et al., 2009; Gant & Thibault, 2009), we and others adapted the method of Skelton (1988) to condition rats while they were tethered and allowed to move about freely within a conditioning chamber. The freely moving rat model was used to examine the effects of aging (Weiss & Thompson, 1992; Knuttinen et al., 2001), the interaction of two hippocampally-dependent tasks (Kuo et al., 2006; Curlik et al., 2014), the effect of varying the trace interval (Weiss, Knuttinen et al., 1999; Bangasser et al., 2006; Walker & Steinmetz, 2008), and the effect of potential therapeutics to ameliorate age-related cognitive impairments (Burgdorf et al., 2011). Importantly, we tweaked the paradigm for trace conditioning in the rat (Weiss et al., 1999) and then showed that complete lesions of the hippocampus were necessary to prevent acquisition of trace EBC (Weiss, Bouwmeester, Power, & Disterhoft, 1999), just as Moyer et al. had found for the rabbit. Interestingly, the critical trace interval for hippocampal dependence was found to be as short as 250ms; an interval of 500ms did not support conditioning when the US was a puff of air to the cornea. In summary, the rat is an excellent experimental subject and when combined with protein knockdown techniques (short hairpin RNAs, small interfering RNAs, or microRNA) the rat model may be as good a model system as the mouse (see next section) to examine the molecular mechanisms of memory.

Eyeblink Conditioning in the Mouse

In order to understand molecular mechanisms mediating hippocampal function and conditioning, we and others adapted the freely moving rat methodology to the mouse so that we could use transgenic and knockout technologies to analyze the systems and cellular mechanism of learning and memory storage. This occurred at a time prior to the proliferation of gene interfering techniques. Our initial study examined one of the murine models for Alzheimer’s disease and the effect on hippocampal volume (Weiss et al., 2002). We found that the V717F transgene impaired acquisition and decreased hippocampal volume, as measured with magnetic resonance imaging. We also showed in C57Bl6 mice that ibotenic acid lesions of the hippocampus impaired acquisition in proportion to the volume of tissue destroyed, and that the critical trace interval was at least 250 ms, as also found for the rat (Tseng, Guan, Disterhoft & Weiss, 2004), i.e., the mouse would not acquire conditioned responses when the trace interval was 500 ms (as used with rabbits), but they were able to be conditioned with delay procedures. Others have used the mouse paradigm to examine deletion of hippocampal monoamine oxidase (Singh et al., 2013), involvement of CA3-CA1 synapses (Gruart, Munoz & Delgado-Garcia, 2006), the role of the CA3 NMDA receptors (Kishimoto et al., 2006), adaptive timing in cerebellum (Chettih et al., 2011), the role of nucleus reuniens as a common input to both hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex (Eleore et al., 2011), and the role of cerebellar Purkinje neurons (Brown, Agelan & Woodruff-Pak, 2010; Koekkoek et al., 2005).

The tethered preparation was critical to using the mouse since our pilot work indicated that whole-body restraint was stressful for the mouse. However, Chettih, McDougle, Ruffolo, and Medina (2011) demonstrated good conditioning in the head-fixed mouse when the legs of the mouse were allowed to ambulate on top of a freely moving cylindrical treadmill. This simple modification appears to reduce the stress of restraint and allows conditioning to occur. The restrained mouse preparation designed by Medina and his colleagues is now being used in our laboratory, and can be used with genetically modified mice, especially mice engineered with the inducible Cre system that allows specific gene deletion, a technique more suited for mice at this time.

Eyeblink Conditioning in Humans

The declarative nature of memory can be inferred from experiments with non-human animal subjects, but it can only be examined directly in human subjects. The first report of eyeblink conditioning was done in humans by Cason (1922), two years before Pavlov’s lectures about conditioned reflexes to the Military Medical Academy in St Petersburg, Russia. Cason was attempting to dissociate the timing of voluntary responses from the timing of conditioned responses. His results indicated that the conditioned response occurred with a latency that was significantly shorter than the latency for the same person instructed to blink as quickly as possible when the sound was heard. This suggested that different circuitry was mediating the conditioned reflex, a result that we and others are still investigating to this day.

It was not until Clark and Squire (1998) interviewed their subjects about the stimulus contingencies in a conditioning session that the full potential of using human subjects was realized. They asked about the temporal relationship of the conditioning stimuli that was used in either delay or trace blink conditioning. Their results revealed that trace, but not delay conditioning required awareness of the temporal relationship between the conditioning stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus, i.e. amnesic patients acquired delay conditioning but not trace conditioning. Knuttinen et al. (2001) found similar effects due to age-related impairments in awareness. While there are some methodological concerns with the Clark and Squire study, i.e. the effects of aging and attention (LaBar & Disterhoft, 1998), the face validity of eyeblink conditioning as an assay for declarative memory and hippocampal function increased after seeing that the percent of trials with CRs and the participants’ correct predictions of when a US would be delivered. Their work also suggested that promoting awareness, by attempting to predict the onset of the US, facilitated trace conditioning (Manns, Clark, Squire, 2000), possibly due to activation of the hippocampus. In fact, greater hippocampal activation was found during trace eyeblink conditioning as compared to concurrently learned delay conditioning when assayed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans (Cheng et al., 2008). The mechanisms underlying facilitation by awareness may be related to the mechanisms mediating facilitated acquisition when trials are specifically presented only when rabbits are exhibiting hippocampal theta activity (Griffin et al., 2004; Asaka et al., 2005; Berry & Hoffmann 2011).

Trace Eyeblink Conditioning as a Tool for Translational Neuroscience

EBC as an Assay for Therapeutics

The hippocampus is essential for declarative memory and its function is impaired by aging and Alzheimer’s Disease. The hippocampal dependence of trace eyeblink conditioning, and its dependence on awareness of stimulus contingencies has made the paradigm useful for studying cognitive deficits associated with normal aging and age-related impairments, and the amelioration of those deficits by potential therapeutics. Several classes of therapeutics have been tested with blink conditioning: calcium channel blockers, inhibitors of acetylcholinesterase, allosteric potentiating ligands for nicotinic receptors, muscarinic agonists, and allosteric modulators of the NMDA receptor. Calcium channel blockers were examined since an imbalance in calcium homeostasis has been hypothesized to underlie age-related cognitive impairments (Landfield, 1987; Oh, Oliveira, Waters & Disterhoft, 2013). Nimodipine, a dihydropyridine, blocks L-type calcium channels and was found to facilitate learning in aging rabbits (Deyo, Straube, Disterhoft, 1989). Metrifonate, a cholinesterase inhibitor improves learning and retention in aging rabbits (Kronforst-Collins et al., 1997; Oh et al. 1999). Galantamine, a potent allosteric potentiating ligand of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (and a weak competitive and reversible cholinesterase inhibitor) increases acetylcholine release, facilitated acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioning in aging rabbits, and increased the excitability of CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons (Weible, Oh, Lee, Disterhoft, 2004; Oh, Wu, Power, Disterhoft, 2006;). CI-1017, an agonist of the M1 muscarinic receptor also facilitated acquisition of trace EBC in aging rabbits and increased the excitability of CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons (Weiss, Preston, Oh, Schwarz, Welty & Disterhoft, 2000). The theoretical rationale for these studies was the proven effect of each of the compounds mentioned to increase excitability of CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons by reduction of the size of the slow, post-burst afterhyperpolarization, mediated by a calcium-activated outward potassium current which is increased in aging neurons, leading to age-associated impairments in cognition (Oh and Disterhoft, 2006). Finally, GLYX-13, an allosteric modulator of the NMDA receptor, was derived from a monoclonal antibody against NMDA receptors from the dentate gyrus (Moskal et al., 2005). Both the antibody (B6B21) and GLYX-13 were found to facilitate trace EBC in aging animals when administered into the ventricle or intravenously, respectively (Thompson, Moskal, Disterhoft, 1992; Moskal et al., 2005; Burgdorf et al., 2011).

EBC as an Assay for Neurological Disease

Aside from using blink conditioning to study learning and memory impairments due to normal aging and the effects of different therapeutics, the paradigm has been used to study disease related impairments in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and schizophrenia. AD shares some overlap with the effects of age (since age is a major contributing factor for the disease), but Woodruff-Pak (1990, 2001, 2007) reports that individuals with AD are more impaired on EBC (especially delay EBC, due to degeneration of cerebellar Pukinje neurons) than are age-matched control subjects.

Factors other than aging that influence AD include high cholesterol and diabetes. Bernard Schreurs and the late David Sparks published several reports regarding the role of cholesterol, copper, and dietary treatments on AD-like pathology and behavioral conditioning in the rabbit (Sparks & Schreurs, 2003; Schreurs et al., 2007; Wang & Schreurs, 2010; Schreurs, 2013). The effects of copper and cholesterol are complicated and are affected by the timing of the treatment in relation to training and retention testing. However, further research with this dietary paradigm is important since rabbits fed a cholesterol enriched diet exhibit at least 12 pathological markers of AD (Sparks & Schreurs, 2003), and because rabbits have the same amino acid sequence for amyloid as do humans (Davidson et al., 1992). This makes the rabbit a non-transgenic model system for AD that excludes the side-effects of foreign DNA, as present in murine models of AD.

Spontaneous Alzheimer’s Disease, the most prevalent form of the disease, is considered by some to be due to a form of diabetes (“Type 3 Diabetes”) or an “insulin-resistant brain state” (de la Monte & Wands, 2008; Correia et al., 2011; Talbot et al., 2012). Several studies by William Klein and his colleagues support the hypothesis that defective insulin signaling is a major contributor to the pathology associated with AD (Zhao et al., 2009; De Felice et al.,2009; Bomfim et al., 2012), including the pathology found in rabbits that develop diabetes following an injection of Alloxan. Alloxan kills islet cells in the pancreas, induces a diabetic state due to the lack of insulin, and after an incubation period of about 15 weeks several pathological markers of AD are apparent including, a five-fold increase in Aβ40/Aβ42 in cortex and hippocampus, the generation of Aβ oligomers, and elevation of phospho-tau along neuronal tracts (Bitel et al., 2010, 2012). These pathologies in the rabbit suggest that hippocampal-dependent trace eyeblink conditioning should be a sensitive behavioral assay for the changes induced by a diabetic state in the rabbit.

Both trace and delay eyeblink conditioning are dependent on the cerebellum (Woodruff-Pak, Lavond & Thompson, 1985), but only trace conditioning is dependent on the hippocampus for acquisition of CRs. However, if the hippocampus malfunctions, as in AD or following an injection of scopolamine (a cholinergic antagonist), delay conditioning is also impaired, unless conditioning is done in an experimental animal in which the hippocampus was removed (Solomon et al., 1983). These data suggest that cerebellar-hippocampal interactions (and cerebellar-prefrontal interactions) are also important for delay conditioning and probably other cognitive tasks.

Schizophrenia is a disorder that Nancy Andreasen and colleagues termed a “cognitive dysmetria” (Andreasen, Paradiso & O’Leary, 1998) and is thought to be due to disturbances in a cortico-cerebellar-thalamic-cortical circuit (Sears, Andreasen & O’Leary, 2000). This circuit is expected to be active during EBC and could affect both trace and delay conditioning, if a malfunctioning circuit impairs delay conditioning as does a malfunctioning hippocampus. Brown et al. (2005) and Bolbecker et al. (2009) each examined the effect of schizophrenia in delay EBC and found that affected individuals exhibited lower rates of conditioning due to an increase in maladaptive short latency responses, an effect also seen following the hippocampal lesions of Moyer et al.

Hierarchical Memory Systems

Squire and Zola-Morgan (1991) proposed a hierarchical scheme for organizing memory into declarative (explicit) and nondeclarative (implicit) components. They included simple (delay) classical conditioning at the bottom of the tree, as part of the nondeclarative system and had it nestled in between the functional components of nonassociative learning and priming; the more complex trace conditioning paradigm was not included in their scheme. We propose instead that classical conditioning be placed in a more central position, next to procedural learning, and to bridge the divide between declarative and nondeclarative memory (Fig. 1). This scheme can be used to frame hippocampal and prefrontal interactions with the cerebellum as proposed by Weiss and Disterhoft (1996) and consistent with results using electrical stimulation of the prefrontal cortex (Kalmbach, Ohyama and Mauk, 2010) and recording data from the prelimbic cortex (Hattori, Yoon et al., 2014). The basic premise is that the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex have an innate property to form an initial association between the CS and US when they are presented with a trace interval separating them. The projection from prefrontal cortex to pons then facilitates the effectiveness of the CS into the cerebellum to facilitate conditioning that would otherwise be outside of the optimal timing for long term depression (LTD) to occur at the parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapse. The cerebellar network eventually projects to the red nucleus and subsequently motor neurons mediating the blink while the hippocampal-reuniens-prefrontal network (Cassel et al., 2013; Hallock, Wang, Shaw & Griffin, 2013; Varela, Kumar, Yang, Wilson, 2014) activates the prelimbic cortex where working memory, awareness, and the declarative nature of the experience may be instantiated, just as neuronal activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of monkeys appears to reflect the memory for a cue that has been presented and removed during delayed matching to sample tasks (Bauer & Fuster, 1976; Quintana, Yajeya & Fuster, 1988; Fuster 2001).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Domains of memory. This adaptation of a figure from Larry Squire and Stuart Zola-Morgan (1991) shows that trace conditioning can be positioned to act as a Rosetta stone (Thompson, 1988) for understanding hippocampal-cerebellar interactions that mediate conditioned adaptations of somatomotor responses.

Lesion/Inactivation Studies

The preceding hierarchy is based largely on functional considerations. A more mechanistic view requires anatomical localization, and cell type specificity would greatly add to an understanding of the signal processing that occurs along the pathways. Anatomical localization is often determined by removing a structure from the circuit. In regards to the circuitry mediating blink conditioning, the deep cerebellar nuclei appear to be essential for mediating both delay and trace conditioning (Woodruff-Pak & Disterhoft, 2008 for review). However, the complete essential forebrain circuitry that contributes to hippocampal-cerebellar interactions mediating trace conditioning is still unknown and is actively being investigated by us and others (Siegel et al., 2012). Aspiration, electrolytic, and neurotoxic lesions have been classical ways to remove a structure and determine the necessity of a region. Ibotenic acid lesions of primary somatosensory cortex were found to prevent and abolish whisker-signaled trace eyeblink conditioning (Galvez, Weible, Disterhoft, 2007), muscimol induced inactivation of secondary somatosensory cortex was found to abolish conditioning in rabbits that had consolidated their conditioning for one month (Ward, Flores, Weiss, Disterhoft, 2011), electrolytic lesions of the caudate were found to prevent acquisition and impaired improvements in responding among rabbits that had been allowed to consolidate their learning for a period of one month (Flores & Disterhoft, 2009, 2013), aspiration lesions of the caudal anterior cingulate region prevented acquisition of trace conditioning (Weible, McEchron, Disterhoft, 2000), and electrolytic lesions of the perirhinal or postrhinal corticies (but not lateral entorhinal cortex) significantly impaired acquisition of trace blink conditioning in rats (Suter, Weiss & Disterhoft, 2013). Inactivation of accessory thalamic nuclei were found to impair acquisition and retention of delay blink conditioning to either a visual or auditory conditioning stimulus and would be expected to have similar effects on trace conditioning (Halverson & Freeman, 2010a,b; Halverson, Lee, Freeman, 2010; Steinmetz, Buss, Freeman, 2013). More modern techniques include the cre-lox system (Nagy, 2000) which allows conditional gene deletion in mice, chemogenetic inactivation which uses a peripheral injection to activate a virus expressing designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs (DREADDs, Alexander et al., 2009), and optogenetic techniques (Lee et al., 2012) which allow precise temporal control over inactivation (which can prevent complications due to functional reorganization following prolonged inactivation, Goshen et al., 2011). We are currently investigating the effects of chemogenetic and optogenetic inactivation in rabbit and will be examining cell type specific inactivation in the mouse.

Eyeblink Conditioning: Past its prime, or primed for the future?

Some may wonder if eyeblink conditioning has passed its prime after nearly four decades of research. We argue instead that the paradigm is primed for the future, especially in regards to translational neuroscience research and treatments for normal age-related memory impairments (Cheng, Faulkner, Disterhoft & Desmond, 2010; Burgdorf et al., 2011; Bellebaum & Daum, 2004; Weible, Oh, Lee & Disterhoft, 2004), Alzheimer’s Disease (Woodruff-Pak, 2001), psychiatric diseases, e.g. schizophrenia and autism (Parker, Narayanan & Andreasen, 2014) and addiction. Nancy Andreasen has proposed that disrupted cerebellar-forebrain input to both the prefrontal cortex and ventral tegmental area contributes to a dysmetria of thought and schizophrenic behavior, and to autistic behavior. Thomas Gould has examined the differential effects of the addictive compound nicotine on delay and trace conditioning, albeit using fear conditioning instead of blink conditioning (Kenney & Gould, 2008; Gould, Feiro, Moore, 2004) and finds that trace conditioning is affected more than delay conditioning. Given our ability to use individually adjustable tetrodes to record many neurons simultaneously throughout the rabbit brain during conditioning (Hattori, Chen, et al., 2014; Hattori, Yoon, et al., 2014), and our ability to image the rabbit brain longitudinally without anesthesia or sedation (Miller et al., 2003, 2008; Schroeder, Weiss, Procissi, Disterhoft, 2012) puts the field in a very good position to understand the mechanisms underlying conditioning and memory for the associated stimuli, and the problems that arise to impair or prevent the creation, storage, or retrieval of memories.

Robert “Bob” Clark (2011) pointed out the irony that a simple reflex system like delay eyeblink conditioning can inform us so much about declarative memory by simply inserting a stimulus free trace interval between the conditioning stimuli. We agree with him and continue to build upon a solid foundation of neurobiological research that followed the Moyer et al. report demonstrating that acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioning requires the hippocampus.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NINDS RO1 NS059879 (CW), NIMH RO1 MH47340 (JFD), and NIA R37 AG008796 (JFD).

Footnotes

Dedication

This review is dedicated to the memory of Professor Richard F. Thompson, inaugural Editor of Behavioral Neuroscience and our long-time mentor and colleague in the use of eyeblink conditioning as a behavioral paradigm to understand the brain mechanisms of associative learning.

References

  1. Alexander GM, Rogan SC, Abbas AI, Armbruster BN, Pei Y, Allen JA, Nonneman RJ, Hartmann J, Moy SS, Nicolelis MA, McNamara JO, Roth BL. Remote control of neuronal activity in transgenic mice expressing evolved G protein-coupled receptors. Neuron. 2009;63(1):27–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andreasen NC, Paradiso S, O'Leary DS. “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry? Schizophr Bull. 1998;24(2):203–18. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Asaka Y, Mauldin KN, Griffin AL, Seager MA, Shurell E, Berry SD. Nonpharmacological amelioration of age-related learning deficits: the impact of hippocampal theta-triggered training. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2005;102(37):13284–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506515102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bangasser DA, Waxler DE, Santollo J, Shors TJ. Trace conditioning and the hippocampus: the importance of contiguity. J Neurosci. 2006;26(34):8702–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bauer RH, Fuster JM. Delayed-matching and delayed-response deficit from cooling dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1976;90(3):293–302. doi: 10.1037/h0087996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bellebaum C, Daum I. Effects of age and awareness on eyeblink conditional discrimination learning. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118(6):1157–65. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.6.1157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berger TW, Alger B, Thompson RF. Neuronal substrate of classical conditioning in the hippocampus. Science. 1976;192(4238):483–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1257783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berger TW, Thompson RF. Identification of pyramidal cells as the critical elements in hippocampal neuronal plasticity during learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1978;75(3):1572–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.75.3.1572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Berger TW, Rinaldi PC, Weisz DJ, Thompson RF. Single-unit analysis of different hippocampal cell types during classical conditioning of rabbit nictitating membrane response. J Neurophysiol. 1983;50(5):1197–219. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.50.5.1197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berry SD, Hoffmann LC. Hippocampal theta-dependent eyeblink classical conditioning: coordination of a distributed learning system. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;95(2):185–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bitel CL, Feng Y, Souayah N, Frederikse PH. Increased expression and local accumulation of the prion protein, Alzheimer Aβ peptides, superoxide dismutase 1, and nitric oxide synthases 1 & 2 in muscle in a rabbit model of diabetes. BMC Physiol. 2010;10:18. doi: 10.1186/1472-6793-10-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bitel CL, Kasinathan C, Kaswala RH, Klein WL, Frederikse PH. Amyloid-β and tau pathology of Alzheimer's disease induced by diabetes in a rabbit animal model. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;32(2):291–305. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-120571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bolbecker AR, Mehta CS, Edwards CR, Steinmetz JE, O'Donnell BF, Hetrick WP. Eye-blink conditioning deficits indicate temporal processing abnormalities in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2009;111(1–3):182–91. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bomfim TR, Klein WL, et al. An anti-diabetes agent protects the mouse brain from defective insulin signaling caused by Alzheimer's disease-associated Aβ oligomers. J Clin Invest. 2012;122(4):1339–53. doi: 10.1172/JCI57256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown KL, Agelan A, Woodruff-Pak DS. Unimpaired trace classical eyeblink conditioning in Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) mutant mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2010;93(3):303–11. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brown SM, Kieffaber PD, Carroll CA, Vohs JL, Tracy JA, Shekhar A, O'Donnell BF, Steinmetz JE, Hetrick WP. Eyeblink conditioning deficits indicate timing and cerebellar abnormalities in schizophrenia. Brain Cogn. 2005;58(1):94–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Burgdorf J, Zhang XL, Weiss C, Matthews E, Disterhoft JF, Stanton PK, Moskal JR. The N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor modulator GLYX-13 enhances learning and memory, in young adult and learning impaired aging rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(4):698–706. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cason H. The conditioned eyelid reaction. J Exper Psychol. 1922;5:153–196. [Google Scholar]
  19. Cassel JC, Pereira de Vasconcelos A, Loureiro M, Cholvin T, Dalrymple-Alford JC, Vertes RP. The reuniens and rhomboid nuclei: neuroanatomy, electrophysiological characteristics and behavioral implications. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;111:34–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cheng DT, Disterhoft JF, Power JM, Ellis DA, Desmond JE. Neural substrates underlying human delay and trace eyeblink conditioning. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105(23):8108–13. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800374105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cheng DT, Faulkner ML, Disterhoft JF, Desmond JE. The effects of aging in delay and trace human eyeblink conditioning. Psychol Aging. 2010;25(3):684–90. doi: 10.1037/a0017978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chettih SN, McDougle SD, Ruffolo LI, Medina JF. Adaptive timing of motor output in the mouse: the role of movement oscillations in eyelid conditioning. Front Integr Neurosci. 2011;5:72. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Clark RE. Eyeblink conditioning and systems consolidation: an ironic yet powerful pairing. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;95(2):118–24. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Clark RE, Squire LR. Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. Science. 1998;280(5360):77–81. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5360.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Correia SC, Santos RX, Perry G, Zhu X, Moreira PI, Smith MA. Insulin-resistant brain state: the culprit in sporadic Alzheimer's disease? Ageing Res Rev. 2011;10(2):264–73. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2011.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Curlik DM, Weiss C, Nicholson DA, Disterhoft JF. Age-related impairments on one hippocampal-dependent task predict impairments on a subsequent hippocampal-dependent task. Behav Neurosci. 2014;128(6):676–88. doi: 10.1037/bne0000018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Davidson JS, West RL, Kotikalapudi P, Maroun LE. Sequence and methylation in the beta/A4 region of the rabbit amyloid precursor protein gene. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 1992;188(2):905–11. doi: 10.1016/0006-291x(92)91141-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. De Felice FG, Vieira MN, Bomfim TR, Decker H, Velasco PT, Lambert MP, Viola KL, Zhao WQ, Ferreira ST, Klein WL. Protection of synapses against Alzheimer's-linked toxins: insulin signaling prevents the pathogenic binding of Abeta oligomers. PNAS. 2009;106(6):1971–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809158106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. de la Monte SM, Wands JR. Alzheimer's disease is type 3 diabetes-evidence reviewed. J Diabetes Sci Technol. 2008;2(6):1101–13. doi: 10.1177/193229680800200619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Deyo RA, Straube KT, Disterhoft JF. Nimodipine facilitates associative learning in aging rabbits. Science. 1989;243(4892):809–11. doi: 10.1126/science.2916127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Disterhoft JF, Kwan HH, Lo WD. Nictitating membrane conditioning to tone in the immobilized albino rabbit. Brain Res. 1977;137(1):127–43. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)91016-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Disterhoft JF, Quinn KJ, Weiss C, Shipley MT. Accessory abducens nucleus and conditioned eye retraction/nictitating membrane extension in rabbit. J Neurosci. 1985;5(4):941–50. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-04-00941.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Eleore L, López-Ramos JC, Guerra-Narbona R, Delgado-García JM. Role of reuniens nucleus projections to the medial prefrontal cortex and to the hippocampal pyramidal CA1 area in associative learning. PLoS One. 2011;6(8):e23538. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Flores LC, Disterhoft JF. Caudate nucleus is critically involved in trace eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci. 2009;29(46):14511–20. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3119-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Flores LC, Disterhoft JF. Caudate nucleus in retrieval of trace eyeblink conditioning after consolidation. J Neurosci. 2013;33(7):2828–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2326-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex--an update: time is of the essence. Neuron. 2001;30(2):319–33. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00285-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Galvez R, Weible AP, Disterhoft JF. Cortical barrel lesions impair whisker-CS trace eyeblink conditioning. Learn Mem. 2007;14(1):94–100. doi: 10.1101/lm.418407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gant JC, Thibault O. Action potential throughput in aged rat hippocampal neurons: regulation by selective forms of hyperpolarization. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30(12):2053–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gonzalez-Joekes J, Schreurs BG. Anatomical characterization of a rabbit cerebellar eyeblink premotor pathway using pseudorabies and identification of a local modulatory network in anterior interpositus. J Neurosci. 2012;32(36):12472–87. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2088-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Goshen I, Brodsky M, Prakash R, Wallace J, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K. Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories. Cell. 2011;147(3):678–89. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gould TJ, Feiro O, Moore D. Nicotine enhances trace cued fear conditioning but not delay cued fear conditioning in C57BL/6 mice. Behav Brain Res. 2004;155(1):167–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Griffin AL, Asaka Y, Darling RD, Berry SD. Theta-contingent trial presentation accelerates learning rate and enhances hippocampal plasticity during trace eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118(2):403–11. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gruart A, Muñoz MD, Delgado-García JM. Involvement of the CA3-CA1 synapse in the acquisition of associative learning in behaving mice. J Neurosci. 2006;26(4):1077–87. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2834-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hallock HL, Wang A, Shaw CL, Griffin AL. Transient inactivation of the thalamic nucleus reuniens and rhomboid nucleus produces deficits of a working-memory dependent tactile-visual conditional discrimination task. Behav Neurosci. 2013;127(6):860–6. doi: 10.1037/a0034653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Halverson HE, Freeman JH. Ventral lateral geniculate input to the medial pons is necessary for visual eyeblink conditioning in rats. Learn Mem. 2010a;17(2):80–5. doi: 10.1101/lm.1572710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Halverson HE, Freeman JH. Medial auditory thalamic input to the lateral pontine nuclei is necessary for auditory eyeblink conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2010b;93(1):92–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Halverson HE, Lee I, Freeman JH. Associative plasticity in the medial auditory thalamus and cerebellar interpositus nucleus during eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci. 2010;30(26):8787–96. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0208-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Halverson HE, Poremba A, Freeman JH. Medial auditory thalamus inactivation prevents acquisition and retention of eyeblink conditioning. Learn Mem. 2008;15(7):532–8. doi: 10.1101/lm.1002508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hattori S, Chen L, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Robust hippocampal responsivity during retrieval of consolidated associative memory. Hippocampus. 2014 Dec 16; doi: 10.1002/hipo.22401. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hattori S, Yoon T, Disterhoft JF, Weiss C. Functional Reorganization of a Prefrontal Cortical Network Mediating Acquisition and Retention of the Conditioned Blink Reflex. J Neurosci. 2014;34(4):1432–45. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4428-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Herman JP, Cullinan WE, Young EA, Akil H, Watson SJ. Selective forebrain fiber tract lesions implicate ventral hippocampal structures in tonic regulation of paraventricular nucleus corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) mRNA expression. Brain Res. 1992;592(1–2):228–38. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91680-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jacobson L, Sapolsky R. The role of the hippocampus in feedback regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Endocr Rev. 1991;12(2):118–34. doi: 10.1210/edrv-12-2-118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. James GO, Hardiman MJ, Yeo CH. Hippocampal lesions and trace conditioning in the rabbit. Behav Brain Res. 1987;23(2):109–16. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(87)90048-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kalmbach BE, Ohyama T, Mauk MD. Temporal patterns of inputs to cerebellum necessary and sufficient for trace eyelid conditioning. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):627–40. doi: 10.1152/jn.00169.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kenney JW, Gould TJ. Modulation of hippocampus-dependent learning and synaptic plasticity by nicotine. Mol Neurobiol. 2008;38(1):101–21. doi: 10.1007/s12035-008-8037-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kim JJ, Clark RE, Thompson RF. Hippocampectomy impairs the memory of recently, but not remotely, acquired trace eyeblink conditioned responses. Behav Neurosci. 1995;109(2):195–203. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kim JJ, Fanselow MS. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science. 1992;256(5057):675–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1585183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kishimoto Y, Nakazawa K, Tonegawa S, Kirino Y, Kano M. Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are crucial for adaptive timing of trace eyeblink conditioned response. J Neurosci. 2006;26(5):1562–70. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4142-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Knuttinen MG, Gamelli AE, Weiss C, Power JM, Disterhoft JF. Age-related effects on eyeblink conditioning in the F344 x BN F1 hybrid rat. Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00194-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Koekkoek SK, Yamaguchi K, Milojkovic BA, Dortland BR, Ruigrok TJ, Maex R, De Graaf W, Smit AE, VanderWerf F, Bakker CE, Willemsen R, Ikeda T, Kakizawa S, Onodera K, Nelson DL, Mientjes E, Joosten M, De Schutter E, Oostra BA, Ito M, De Zeeuw CI. Deletion of FMR1 in Purkinje cells enhances parallel fiber LTD, enlarges spines, and attenuates cerebellar eyelid conditioning in Fragile X syndrome. Neuron. 2005;47(3):339–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kronforst-Collins MA, Disterhoft JF. Lesions of the caudal area of rabbit medial prefrontal cortex impair trace eyeblink conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998;69(2):147–62. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kronforst-Collins MA, Moriearty PL, Schmidt B, Disterhoft JF. Metrifonate improves associative learning and retention in aging rabbits. Behav Neurosci. 1997;111(5):1031–40. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.111.5.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kuo AG, Lee G, Disterhoft JF. Simultaneous training on two hippocampus-dependent tasks facilitates acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioning. Learn Mem. 2006;13(2):201–7. doi: 10.1101/lm.98406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. LaBar KS, Disterhoft JF. Conditioning, awareness, and the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1998;8(6):620–6. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:6<620::AID-HIPO4>3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Landfield PW. “Increased calcium-current” hypothesis of brain aging. Neurobiol Aging. 1987;8:346–347. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(87)90074-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Lawlor PN, Hattori S, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF, Kording K. Decoding techniques predict conditioned blink responses, revealing dynamics of associative learning. Soc Neurosci Abs. 2013 2013-S-9687. [Google Scholar]
  67. Lee SH, Kwan AC, Zhang S, Phoumthipphavong V, Flannery JG, Masmanidis SC, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Zhang F, Boyden ES, Deisseroth K, Dan Y. Activation of specific interneurons improves V1 feature selectivity and visual perception. Nature. 2012;488(7411):379–83. doi: 10.1038/nature11312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Manns JR, Clark RE, Squire LR. Parallel acquisition of awareness and trace eyeblink classical conditioning. Learn Mem. 2000;7(5):267–72. doi: 10.1101/lm.33400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Matthews EA, Linardakis JM, Disterhoft JF. The fast and slow afterhyperpolarizations are differentially modulated in hippocampal neurons by aging and learning. J Neurosci. 2009;29(15):4750–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0384-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. McCormick DA, Lavond DG, Clark GA, Kettner RE, Rising CE, Thompson RF. The engram found? Role of the cerebellum in classical conditioning of nictitating membrane and eyelid responses, Bull. Psychon Soc. 1981;18:103–105. [Google Scholar]
  71. McCormick DA, Clark GA, Lavond DG, Thompson RF. Initial localization of the memory trace for a basic form of learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1982;79(8):2731–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. McEchron MD, Weible AP, Disterhoft JF. Aging and learning-specific changes in single-neuron activity in CA1 hippocampus during rabbit trace eyeblink conditioning. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86(4):1839–57. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. McKay BM, Oh MM, Disterhoft JF. Learning increases intrinsic excitability of hippocampal interneurons. J Neurosci. 2013;33(13):5499–506. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4068-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Miller MJ, Chen N-K, Li L, Tom B, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF, Wyrwicz AM. fMRI of the conscious rabbit during unilateral classical eyeblink conditioning reveals bilateral cerebellar activation. J Neurosci. 2003;23(37):11753–11758. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-37-11753.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Miller MJ, Weiss C, Song X, Iordanescu G, Disterhoft JF, Wyrwicz AM. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of delay and trace eyeblink conditioning in the primary visual cortex of the rabbit. J Neuroscience. 2008;28 (19):4974–4981. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5622-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Moskal JR, Kuo AG, Weiss C, Wood PL, O'Connor Hanson A, Kelso S, Harris RB, Disterhoft JF. GLYX-13: a monoclonal antibody-derived peptide that acts as an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor modulator. Neuropharmacology. 2005;49(7):1077–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2005.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Moyer JR, Jr, Deyo RA, Disterhoft JF. Hippocampectomy disrupts trace eye-blink conditioning in rabbits. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104(2):243–52. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.104.2.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Moyer JR, Jr, Thompson LT, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning increases CA1 excitability in a transient and learning-specific manner. J Neurosci. 1996;16(17):5536–46. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-17-05536.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Nagy A. Cre recombinase: the universal reagent for genome tailoring. Genesis. 2000;26:99–109. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Norman RJ, Buchwald JS, Villablanca JR. Classical conditioning with auditory discrimination of the eye blink in decerebrate cats. Science. 1977;196(4289):551–3. doi: 10.1126/science.850800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Oakley DA, Russell IS. Neocortical lesions and Pavlovian conditioning. Physiol Behav. 1972;8(5):915–26. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(72)90305-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Oh MM, Oliveira FA, Disterhoft JF. Learning and aging related changes in intrinsic neuronal excitability. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.24.002.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Oh MM, Oliveira FA, Waters J, Disterhoft JF. Altered calcium metabolism in aging CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2013;33(18):7905–11. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5457-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Oh MM, Power JM, Thompson LT, Moriearty PL, Disterhoft JF. Metrifonate increases neuronal excitability in CA1 pyramidal neurons from both young and aging rabbit hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1999;19(5):1814–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-05-01814.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Oh MM, Wu WW, Power JM, Disterhoft JF. Galantamine increases excitability of CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Neuroscience. 2006;137(1):113–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Oswald BB, Maddox SA, Tisdale N, Powell DA. Encoding and retrieval are differentially processed by the anterior cingulate and prelimbic cortices: a study based on trace eyeblink conditioning in the rabbit. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2010;93(1):37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Parker KL, Narayanan NS, Andreasen NC. The therapeutic potential of the cerebellum in schizophrenia. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:163. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Petrovich GD, Canteras NS, Swanson LW. Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains and hypothalamic behavior systems. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2001;38(1–2):247–89. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00080-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Port RL, Romano AG, Steinmetz JE, Mikhail AA, Patterson MM. Retention and acquisition of classical trace conditioned responses by rabbits with hippocampal lesions. Behav Neurosci. 1986;100(5):745–52. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.100.5.745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Quintana J, Yajeya J, Fuster JM. Prefrontal representation of stimulus attributes during delay tasks. I. Unit activity in cross-temporal integration of sensory and sensory-motor information. Brain Res. 1988;474(2):211–21. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90436-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Rogers RF, Britton GB, Steinmetz JE. Learning-related interpositus activity is conserved across species as studied during eyeblink conditioning in the rat. Brain Res. 2001;905(1–2):171–7. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02532-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Rosene DL, Van Hoesen GW. Hippocampal efferents reach widespread areas of cerebral cortex and amygdala in the rhesus monkey. Science. 1977;198(4314):315–7. doi: 10.1126/science.410102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Schroeder MP, Weiss C, Procissi D, Disterhoft J. Functional connectivity of consolidated memory circuits in rabbit after trace eyeblink conditioning. 2012 doi: 10.1101/lm.040220.115. SFN Abs 598.02. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Schneiderman N, Fuentes I, Gormezano I. Acquisition and extinction of the classically conditioned eyelid response in the albino rabbit. Science. 1962;136(3516):650–2. doi: 10.1126/science.136.3516.650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1957;20(1):11–21. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Schreurs BG. Cholesterol and copper affect learning and memory in the rabbit. Int J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;2013:518780. doi: 10.1155/2013/518780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Schreurs BG, Smith-Bell CA, Darwish DS, Stankovic G, Sparks DL. High dietary cholesterol facilitates classical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response. Nutr Neurosci. 2007;10(1–2):31–43. doi: 10.1080/10284150701232034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Sears LL, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS. Cerebellar functional abnormalities in schizophrenia are suggested by classical eyeblink conditioning. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48(3):204–9. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00247-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Siegel JJ, Kalmbach B, Chitwood RA, Mauk MD. Persistent activity in a cortical-to-subcortical circuit: bridging the temporal gap in trace eyelid conditioning. J Neurophysiol. 2012 Jan;107(1):50–64. doi: 10.1152/jn.00689.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Singh C, Bortolato M, Bali N, Godar SC, Scott AL, Chen K, Thompson RF, Shih JC. Cognitive abnormalities and hippocampal alterations in monoamine oxidase A and B knockout mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110(31):12816–21. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308037110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Solomon PR, Solomon SD, Schaaf EV, Perry HE. Altered activity in the hippocampus is more detrimental to classical conditioning than removing the structure. Science. 1983;220(4594):329–31. doi: 10.1126/science.6836277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Solomon PR, Vander Schaaf ER, Thompson RF, Weisz DJ. Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit's classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. Behav Neurosci. 1986;100(5):729–44. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.100.5.729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Sparks DL, Schreurs BG. Trace amounts of copper in water induce beta-amyloid plaques and learning deficits in a rabbit model of Alzheimer's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2003;100(19):11065–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1832769100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Squire LR, Alvarez P. Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1995;5(2):169–77. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80023-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science. 1991;253(5026):1380–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1896849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Steinmetz AB, Buss EW, Freeman JH. Inactivation of the ventral lateral geniculate and nucleus of the optic tract impairs retention of visual eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 2013;127(5):690–3. doi: 10.1037/a0033729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Steinvorth S, Levine B, Corkin S. Medial temporal lobe structures are needed to re-experience remote autobiographical memories: evidence from H.M. and W.R. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(4):479–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Suter EE, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Perirhinal and postrhinal, but not lateral entorhinal, cortices are essential for acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioning. Learn Mem. 2013;20(2):80–4. doi: 10.1101/lm.028894.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Takehara K, Kawahara S, Kirino Y. rats) Time-dependent reorganization of the brain components underlying memory retention in trace eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9897–9905. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-30-09897.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Takehara K, Kawahara S, Takatsuki K, Kirino Y. Time-limited role of the hippocampus in the memory for trace eyeblink conditioning in mice. Brain Res. 2002;951:183–190. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03159-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Takehara-Nishiuchi K, Maal-Bared G, Morrissey MD. Increased Entorhinal-Prefrontal Theta Synchronization Parallels Decreased Entorhinal-Hippocampal Theta Synchronization during Learning and Consolidation of Associative Memory. Front Behav Neurosci. 2012;5:90. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Talbot K, Trojanowski JQ, Arnold SE, et al. Demonstrated brain insulin resistance in Alzheimer's disease patients is associated with IGF-1 resistance, IRS-1 dysregulation, and cognitive decline. J Clin Invest. 2012;122(4):1316–38. doi: 10.1172/JCI59903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Thompson LT, Moskal JR, Disterhoft JF. Hippocampus-dependent learning facilitated by a monoclonal antibody or D-cycloserine. Nature. 1992;359(6396):638–41. doi: 10.1038/359638a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Thompson LT, Moyer JR, Jr, Disterhoft JF. Transient changes in excitability of rabbit CA3 neurons with a time course appropriate to support memory consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 1996a;76(3):1836–49. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.1836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Thompson LT, Moyer JR, Jr, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning in rabbits demonstrates heterogeneity of learning ability both between and within age groups. Neurobiol Aging. 1996b;17(4):619–29. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(96)00026-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Thompson RF. Classical conditioning: the Rosetta stone for brain substrates of age-related deficits in learning and memory. Neurobiol Aging. 1988;9(5–6):547–8. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(88)80111-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Tseng W, Guan R, Disterhoft JF, Weiss C. Trace eyeblink conditioning is hippocampally dependent in mice. Hippocampus. 2004;14:58–65. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Varela C, Kumar S, Yang JY, Wilson MA. Anatomical substrates for direct interactions between hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the thalamic nucleus reuniens. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;219(3):911–29. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0543-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Walker AG, Steinmetz JE. Hippocampal lesions in rats differentially affect long- and short-trace eyeblink conditioning. Physiol Behav. 2008;93(3):570–8. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Wang D, Schreurs BG. Dietary cholesterol modulates the excitability of rabbit hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neurosci Lett. 2010;479(3):327–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Ward RL, Flores LC, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Disruption of primary and secondary somatosensory cortex impairs expression of a previously learned whisker-signaled trace conditioned response. 2011 SFN Abs 836.02. [Google Scholar]
  122. Weible AP, McEchron MD, Disterhoft JF. Cortical involvement in acquisition and extinction of trace eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114(6):1058–67. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.6.1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Weible AP, Oh MM, Lee G, Disterhoft JF. Galantamine facilitates acquisition of hippocampus-dependent trace eyeblink conditioning in aged rabbits. Learn Mem. 2004;11(1):108–15. doi: 10.1101/lm.69804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Weible AP, O’Reilly JA, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Comparisons of Dorsal and Ventral Hippocampus CA1 Pyramidal Neuron Activity During Trace Eye-blink Conditioning in the Rabbit. Neuroscience. 2006;141 (3):1123–1137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Weiss C, Bouwmeester H, Power JM, Disterhoft JF. Hippocampal lesions prevent trace eyeblink conditioning in the freely moving rat. Behav Brain Res. 1999;99(2):123–32. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00096-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Weiss C, Knuttinen M-G, Power JM, Patel RI, O’Connor MS, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning in the freely moving rat: Optimizing the conditioning parameters. Behav Neurosci. 1999;113 (5):1–6. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.113.5.1100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Eyeblink conditioning, motor control, and the analysis of limbic-cerebellar interactions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1996;19 (3):479–481. [Google Scholar]
  128. Weiss C, Kronforst-Collins MA, Disterhoft JF. Activity of hippocampal pyramidal neurons during trace eyeblink conditioning. Hippocampus. 1996;6(2):192–209. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:2<192::AID-HIPO9>3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Weiss C, Preston AR, Oh MM, Schwarz RD, Welty D, Disterhoft JF. The M1 muscarinic agonist CI-1017 facilitates trace eyeblink conditioning in aging rabbits and increases the excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2000;20(2):783–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00783.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Weiss C, Thompson RF. Delayed acquisition of eyeblink conditioning in aged F1 hybrid (F-344 x Brown Norway) rats. Neurobiol Aging. 1992;13:319–323. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(92)90045-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Weiss C, Venkatasubramanian PN, Aguado AS, Power JM, Tom BC, Li L, Chen KS, Disterhoft JF, Wyrwicz AM. Impaired eyeblink conditioning and decreased hippocampal volume in PDAPP V717F mice. Neurobiol Disease. 2002;11:425–433. doi: 10.1006/nbdi.2002.0555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Witter MP, Groenewegen HJ, Lopes da Silva FH, Lohman AH. Functional organization of the extrinsic and intrinsic circuitry of the parahippocampal region. Prog Neurobiol. 1989;33(3):161–253. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(89)90009-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Woodruff-Pak DS. Eyeblink classical conditioning differentiates normal aging from Alzheimer's disease. Integr Physiol Behav Sci. 2001;36(2):87–108. doi: 10.1007/BF02734044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Woodruff-Pak DS, Agelan A, Del Valle L. A rabbit model of Alzheimer's disease: valid at neuropathological, cognitive, and therapeutic levels. J Alzheimers Dis. 2007;11(3):371–83. doi: 10.3233/jad-2007-11313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Woodruff-Pak DS, Disterhoft JF. Where is the trace in trace conditioning? Trends Neurosci. 2008;31(2):105–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Woodruff-Pak DS, Finkbiner RG, Sasse DK. Eyeblink conditioning discriminates Alzheimer's patients from non-demented aged. Neuroreport. 1990;1(1):45–8. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199009000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Woodruff-Pak DS, Lavond DG, Thompson RF. Trace conditioning: abolished by cerebellar nuclear lesions but not lateral cerebellar cortex aspirations. Brain Res. 1985;348(2):249–60. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90443-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Zhao WQ, Lacor PN, Chen H, Lambert MP, Quon MJ, Krafft GA, Klein WL. Insulin receptor dysfunction impairs cellular clearance of neurotoxic oligomeric a{beta} J Biol Chem. 2009;284(28):18742–53. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M109.011015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES