Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2015 Jul 29;10(7):e0133213. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133213

Viability of Booby Offspring is Maximized by Having One Young Parent and One Old Parent

Hugh Drummond 1,*, Cristina Rodríguez 1
Editor: Anja Widdig2
PMCID: PMC4519181  PMID: 26221951

Abstract

It is widely expected that the quality of offspring will vary with the age of their parents and that this variation should influence animals’ choice of mates. However, theoretical predictions for age effects are contradictory and, to our knowledge, we do not know for any wild animal how the quality of offspring is affected by both parents’ ages across their lifespans, or whether mothers’ and fathers’ ages interact. We tackled this question using long-term data on a highly philopatric, insular population of the blue-footed booby (Sula nebouxii). In this species extra-pair paternity is most common in pairs of two young parents or two old parents, implying that these age combinations might prejudice offspring quality. Analysis of the viability of 3,361 offspring of parents up to 21 years old revealed that fledglings with two young parents or two old parents were least likely to become breeders, whereas fledglings with one young parent and one old parent were most likely to do so. For young parents of either sex, offspring viability increased with age of the other parent; for very old parents, it decreased. These effects could be mediated by parents flexibly modifying their investment in offspring in response to their own and their partners´ ages, but evidence for this was lacking. In 5,343 breeding attempts, although mothers’ and fathers’ ages independently affected four heavily care-dependent breeding traits at the clutch and nestling stages, their interaction did not affect any trait. The effects of parental age combinations on viability could also be mediated by genes: fledglings with one young parent and one old parent could benefit from greater heterozygosity or better genes.

Introduction

It is now well documented that individual animals’ reproductive performance and breeding success commonly improve in early life and decline in late life [13]. We have less of an idea about how the viability of an animal’s progeny varies over its lifespan, even though this issue is fundamental for understanding mate choice and modeling life histories. The last 15 years have been characterized by increasing consensus around each of two contrary but non-exclusive theoretical predictions for genetically mediated effects of breeder age on offspring viability [46]. On the one hand, differential survival of high quality males and females in wild populations is widely expected to generate a positive association between breeder age and the quality of genes passed to progeny [79], although there are also life history arguments for expecting this association to be negative [10,11]. On the other hand, decline over the lifetime is expected in sperm traits of males and in the germline DNA of both sexes, especially males; and these declines should lead to deficits, defects and diseases in offspring [12,13]. Studies have confirmed deterioration in sperm traits [12] and in germline DNA of males and females [1417]; how these changes affect fertility and offspring viability is largely unresolved [6] but one study has shown that deterioration in male germline DNA after maturity is associated with reduced growth rates in offspring [18]. Furthermore, the viability of offspring can potentially vary with age-related variation in parental investment and capacity for parental care. For example, parental care sometimes improves with age and experience in early life and declines with age and senescence in later life [1921], raising the possibility of reduced viability in offspring nurtured by very young or very old parents.

Laboratory and field studies focused on either the mother´s age or the father´s age have turned up a mixed bag of positive and negative effects on offspring quality. Advanced male age, even in species with no parental care, is associated with reduced hatching success [11] and poor larval and infant body mass and survival [22,23], as well as diminished activity and learning during infancy and adulthood [24]. However, in some cases superior phenotypes have been observed in the progeny of males that are older [25,26] or middle-aged [2729]. In invertebrates greater female age is associated with a decline in numerous preadult and adult traits of progeny ([30,31] but see [32]). In birds it is associated with a decline in egg size and in the growth, development and immunity of chicks [21,33], and with accelerated offspring reproductive senescence [34]. In mammals it is associated with negative effects on juvenile survival and daughters’ milk yield [35,36]. However, improvement with age has been documented early in females’ lives too: older mothers produced larger hatchlings in an arachnid [37], higher quality or larger eggs in birds [21,38,39] and heavier offspring with faster reproductive development in mammals [40,41]; and offspring of middle-aged lizards were most likely to survive the juvenile period [28].

Recognition of parental age effects on offspring quality may have been hampered by two nearly universal shortcomings of this literature. First, as noted by other authors [10,11], analyses of offspring quality have largely failed to sample comprehensively across the natural age-span, resulting in frequent confusion between middle age and old age. Often just two age categories are used (young and old) and their relationship to natural age-spans is seldom clear. Second, possible interactions between male age and female age are virtually unexplored. In humans such interactions can affect the probability of birth defects [42,43] and the genetic quality of progeny [44]. In non-human animals, even long-term field studies are generally unable to test for interactions because of the difficulty of obtaining adequate samples of pairs of known-age parents encompassing the age-span, and because partners’ ages are often correlated. In view of recently documented widespread effects of senescence on diverse reproductive traits of wild animals of both sexes [45,34], inclusion of whole natural age-spans and male-female age interactions is required to document age effects more comprehensively.

We analysed effects of male and female ages and interactions between them across their age-spans on offspring viability in the blue-footed booby (Sula nebouxii), a socially monogamous species with roughly symmetrical biparental care. The pattern of age-related sexual infidelity observed in these boobies was interpreted as possibly enabling young females to avoid producing offspring that have two young biological parents, and old females to avoid producing offspring that have two old biological parents, because these two parental age combinations might generate poor quality offspring [46]. Extra-pair chicks occur in 11% of broods and the tendency to produce extra-pair chicks increases in very young females (≤4 years old) with decreasing age of social partners, and in old females (≥8 years old) with increasing age of social partners. It is likely that female boobies exert considerable control over extra-pair paternity because they are larger than males and control sexual access, and because extra-pair copulations follow reciprocal courtship and are consensual [47]. Also, female birds can control fertilization through postcopulatory mechanisms [48]. Effects of combined parental ages on offspring have not been explored in this species, but reproductive traits of long-lived male and female boobies improve in early life and show a senescent decline in later life. Their breeding success peaks at ~10 years [4951] and the viability of their fledglings (probability they will recruit) plateaus when parents are 5–12 years old [52]. Documented traits that may partially explain these patterns include an age-related decline in the quality of eggs and parental care of females [21] and in the germ-line DNA of males [17].

Our test used a 25-year reproductive database for the insular booby population in which parental ages interact to affect extra-pair paternity [46]. In this population annual survival of male and female adults is roughly 80% [53] and average pair bond duration is only 1.7 years [54,55], allowing pairing of different-aged males and females. Adults experience minimal extrinsic mortality (negligible predation [56]) and, for recent fledgling cohorts, most parents’ exact ages are known [57]. Considering the age-related patterns of extra-pair paternity in this population, we predicted that the probability of fledglings recruiting into the breeding population should increase with age of the father for young females but decrease for old females. After confirming this pattern, we tested whether it is due to age combination-related variation in parental investment by asking whether combined parental ages similarly affected any of six breeding traits that are heavily dependent on bi-parental care: clutch size, hatching success, brood size, number of fledglings, fledging success and fledgling body condition.

Methods

Ethics Statement

This observational field study was carried out following the Animal Behavior Society's Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research and in accordance with all Mexican legal requirements for research in national parks and animal conservation and welfare. The study species is not classified as endangered or protected by norm 059–2010 of the Secretaría del Medioambiente y Recursos Naturales (SEMARNAT). The methods of monitoring of boobies in Parque Nacional Isla Isabel were approved by the Dirección General de Vida Silvestre, Secretaría de Gestión para la Protección Ambiental (SEMARNAT permits 517, 574, 5664, 10470, SGPA/DGVS/01323, SGPA/DGVS/3152, SGPA/DGVS/1543, SGPA/DGVS/0491, SGPA/DGVS/1547, SGPA/DGVS/10832, SGPA/DGVS/01916,SGPA/DGVS/00733, SGPA/DGVS/00357, SGPA/DGVS/00505, SGPA/DGVS/00091). During monitoring, adults were not captured or manipulated; chicks were captured by hand after hatching and at fledging for banding and measuring, which was carried out in the shade and took less than 4 min, then immediately returned to their nests, where parents always immediately accepted them.

Recruitment

We monitored reproduction in a study area on Isla Isabel, off the Pacific coast of Mexico (21°52'N, 105°54'W), during 5 months of each year between 1988 and 2012. Breeding of all pairs of boobies in the study area was monitored by recording nest contents every 3–6 days, banding chicks with plastic bands at hatching and steel bands at fledging (age 70 days), reading bands of caretaking adults at each nest three times (without capturing them), and measuring body mass and ulna length at fledging (details in [57]). Sex of fledglings was unknown. We are confident that this monitoring allowed detection of nearly all recruitment (first appearance with a partner and clutch) of the focal fledglings through 2012. First, over a 19-year period when 10,839 fledglings and 3,160 adults were banded, 40% of fledglings recruited into the study area; and an intermittent study of booby populations in the Islas Marietas, Isla San Pedro Martir and El Rancho island (130–903 km distant) provided resightings of only seven male and six female fledglings breeding elsewhere than Isla Isabel [54]. Further, boobies that recruit into the natal colony breed close to their natal site and then remain permanently faithful to their first breeding site [54]. Second, unless they are abandoned, nests in the study area and within 20 m of its borders cannot escape detection because every year two observers inspected the whole of this greater area every 3–6 days, and in only one direction (South) is there any scope for boobies to nest beyond its limits (and only within a 10-m corridor along the shoreline).

For the first sample comprising seven cohorts (2002–2009 except 2003, from which no fledglings recruited), we scored recruitment (1–0 variable) during the first six years of life up to 2012, and included the number of years during which each cohort was monitored (3–6 years) as a covariate. More than 95% of fledglings recruit by an age of 6 years [57]. We complemented this analysis with an analysis of early recruitment (during the first 4 years of life) in which cohorts 2002–2008 (except 2003) were all monitored for the same number of years. The first sample included 3,361 fledglings with mothers 2–20 years old (mean±SD = 8.28±3.19 years) and fathers 1–21 years old (8.59±3.11 years); the second included 2,819 fledglings with mothers 2–20 years old (8.10±3.11 years) and fathers 1–20 years old (8.32±2.92 years). The first sample was based on 915 individual mothers and 945 individual fathers; the second on 904 individual mothers and 872 individual fathers. Parentage was known from routine monitoring of fledglings´ natal nests and associated caretaking adults.

Breeding traits

Males and females share all aspects of parental care, including territory defence, incubation and brood care. For analysing the effects of fathers’ and mothers’ ages on clutch size, brood size, number of fledglings, hatching success, fledging success and fledgling body condition, the monitoring described above yielded a sample of 5,343 breeding events of known-age pairs in the study area in the 11 years from 2002 to 2012. Mothers and fathers in this sample were 1–23 years old (8.05±3.87 years and 8.39±3.72 years, respectively).

Statistical analyses

We analysed effects of fathers’ and mothers’ ages on recruitment and breeding traits using generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) including fathers’ and mothers’ ages, their quadratic terms and the interaction between the mother’s and father’s linear ages; fledglings’ hatch dates; and hatch order in the natal brood of 1–4 chicks. Mothers’ and fathers’ recruitment ages were not included because these are not related to fledgling recruitment [52], and their lifespans were not included because for most young parents these were not known. To implement a maximal model and reduce Type 1 error, we fitted the intercepts and slopes of the three random variables: mother’s ID, father’s ID and cohort [58]. However, maximal models failed to converge, even when the predictor variables were centered. This failure could not be satisfactorily resolved by deleting males and females that were observed at only a single age because these represented roughly 40% of individuals. Therefore for each model we removed, successively, the slopes for cohort, male ID and female ID until convergence was achieved. For analyses of recruitment all resulting random structures included only the intercepts of random variables; for breeding traits all resulting structures included the intercepts for cohort, male ID and female ID and also the slope for female ID. For hatching success and fledging success, the additional random variable, nest ID, was represented only by its intercept.

To analyse the recruitment of fledglings, we used GLMMs with binomial error distribution and a logit link, including, for the first sample of breeding pairs only, number of years of monitoring as an additional covariate. Ages of male and female partners in the first and the second samples were weakly correlated (R = 0.34, P = 0.01, n = 3,361; R = 0.31, P = 0.01, n = 2,819; respectively), but the Variance Inflation Factor showed no collinearity (GVIF: females,1.12; males,1.13; GVIF: females,1.08; males,1.07; respectively).

To analyse clutch size, brood size and number of fledglings (count data), we used GLMMs with negative binomial error distributions to account for overdispersion [59]. For hatching success and fledging success we used GLMMs with binomial error distribution and a logit link, in which each egg (or chick) was scored as hatching (or fledging) or not and nest ID was an additional random term. For fledgling body condition index, we used a GLMM with normal error distribution and an identity link function; normality was confirmed by graphical analysis of residuals (normal Q-Q plots [60]). For the index we used the residuals of the regression of log body mass on log ulna length at 70 d (linear regression: F 1,4871 = 1946.23, P<0.0001, R 2 = 0.28, P<0.001) [61].

In each case, the full model was compared with a model lacking the fixed terms of interest (Mother´s age, Father´s age, Mother´s age2, Father´s age2, Mother´s age*Father´s age) using a Likelihood Ratio Test. If significant (all were, except the models for clutch size and fledgling body condition), we obtained a final model by deleting the non-significant quadratic and interaction terms. We used version 3.1.0 of R [62] with the glmmADMB library for count data [63] and 3.1.5 of R with the lme4 library for proportions and for index of body condition [64].

Results

Recruitment

For the first sample (scored over 6 years), the probability of recruitment of fledglings was affected by the interaction of the mother’s and father’s linear ages (X 2 1 = 5.96, P = 0.015; Table 1). The interaction was due to offspring with two young parents or two old parents being least likely to recruit, while those with one young parent plus one old parent were most likely to recruit (Fig 1A). Offspring with a young or old parent of either sex were more likely to recruit if their other parent was of the opposite age extreme. For offspring of very young mothers (≤4 years), probability of recruitment roughly doubled when their fathers were very old (≥13 years) rather than very young, whereas for offspring of very old mothers it increased by more than half when their fathers were very young rather than very old (Fig 1B). Recruitment prospects of the offspring of young versus very old fathers were affected by partner age in a similarly contrasting pattern.

Table 1. Final models for effects of parents' ages and interactions on the probability of recruitment of fledglings.

P-values for terms comprised in significant interactions are not shown because they are meaningless. For random factors we show variances (and SD) of intercepts.

Fixed effects Estimate (SE) X2 P
First sample (scored over 6 years)
Intercept -2.7778 (1.7936)
Mother´s age 0.1403 (0.0417)
Father´s age 0.1145 (0.0405)
Laying date -1.4780 (0.1914) 59.07 <0.001
Status 17.20 <0.001
(a) Second chick -0.3465 (0.0927)
(b) Third chick -0.4894 (0.1918)
Years of monitoring 0.1226(0.3326) 0.14 0.714
Mother´s age * Father´s age -0.0108 (0.0044) 5.97 0.015
σ2 father ID = 0.0610 (0.2470); σ2 mother ID = 0.0740 (0.2720); σ2 cohort = 0.9480(0.9736); σ2 residual = 1
Second sample (scored over 4 years)
Intercept -2.1032 (0.5582)
Mother´s age 0.1399 (0.0436)
Father´s age 0.1065 (0.0423)
Laying date -1.4817 (0.1971) 55.64 <0.001
Status 16.47 <0.001
(a) Second chick -0.3489 (0.0956)
(b) Third chick -0.4727 (0.1941)
Mother´s age * Father´s age -0.0102 (0.0047) 4.68 0.030
σ2 father ID = 0.0047 (0.0686); σ2 mother ID = 0.1500 (0.3873); σ2 cohort = 1.0860 (1.0400); σ2 residual = 1

Fig 1. Effect of combined parental ages on estimated probability of recruiting into the breeding population.

Fig 1

(a) The surface, generated from a GLMM, is based on 3,361 fledglings (P = 0.015). (b) For illustration, predictive curves were generated by GLMM estimates for very young mothers (≤4 years; solid line) and very old mothers (≥13 years; dashed line).

Neither the significant interaction nor the form of the surface in Fig 1 was due to small unrepresentative samples for extreme parental ages: significance of the interaction of parental linear ages was little affected by deleting the offspring of the 7 very old mothers paired to very young fathers (β = -0.0125, SE = 0.0045, X 2 1 = 7.79, P = 0.005) or the offspring of the 23 young mothers paired to very old fathers (β = -0.0107, SE = 0.0047, X 2 1 = 5.23, P = 0.022), the two parental age combinations least represented in the sample. Sample sizes for parental age combinations are shown in S1 Table.

Results for the second sample (scored over 4 years) were similar in every respect. Probability of early recruitment was affected by the interaction of the mother’s and the father’s linear ages (X 2 1 = 4.68, P = 0.030, Table 1), and this effect remained significant after deleting the offspring of very old mothers paired to very young fathers (n = 7) or very young mothers paired to very old fathers (n = 22) (respectively: β = -0.0102, SE = 0.0050, X 2 1 = 4.682, P = 0.031; β = -0.0102, SE = 0.0050, X 2 1 = 4.07, P = 0.043). Sample sizes for parental age combinations are shown in S1 Table.

Breeding traits

None of the six breeding traits was affected by the interaction of parents’ linear ages (Table 2) although positive and quadratic effects of either male age or female age were common: hatching success, brood size and number of fledglings were affected by linear and quadratic terms for female age, and fledging success was affected by the linear and quadratic terms for both female and male age (Table 2, Fig 2).

Table 2. Final models for effects of parents' ages and interactions between them on breeding traits.

P-values for terms comprised in significant interactions are not shown because they are meaningless. For random factors we show variances (and SD) for slopes (S) and intercepts (I). Piecewise regression on significant quadratic effects showed that all initial increases and final decreases with age were significant (P<0.05). Effects of ID may have been nonsignificant because individuals were observed with few repetitions; e.g., 2.87±2.37 repetitions for the 1,860 fathers and 3.00±2.58 repetitions for the 1,789 mothers analysed for fixed effects of models 3 and 4.

Fixed effects n Estimate (SE) Deviance X 2 P
1. Clutch size 5,343 ns
2. Hatching success 10,428
Intercept 1.6884 (0.4798)
Father´s age -0.0080 (0.0095) 0.63 0.426
Mother´s age 0.2049 (0.0375) 27.42 <0.001
Mother´s age2 -0.0106 (0.0020) 25.38 <0.001
Laying date -4.9944 (0.1404) 1481.70 <0.001
σ2 father ID,I = 0.1165 (0.3413); σ2 mother ID,I (mother´s age) = 0.5820 (0.7630); σ2 mother ID,S (mother´s age) = 0.0052 (0.0722);
σ2 mother ID,I (father´s age) = 0.3584 (0.5987); σ2 mother ID,S (father´s age) = 0.0024 (0.0488);
σ2 cohort,I = 2.1928 (1.4810); σ2nest ID,I = 0.876 (0.9361); σ2 residual = 1
3. Brood size 5,343
Intercept 0.0732 (0.1984)
Father´s age -0.0014 (0.0378) 0.12 0.729
Mother´s age 0.0740 (0.0156) 26.16 <0.001
Mother´s age2 -0.0035 (0.0008) 18.36 <0.001
Laying date -1.1813 (0.0510) 555.82 <0.001
σ2 father ID,I = 2.069e-09 (4.549e-05); σ2 mother ID,I (mother´s age) = 2.065e-09 (4.544e-05);
σ2 mother ID,S (mother´s age) = 2.061e-09 (4.54e-05); σ2 mother ID,I (father´s age) = 2.065e-09 (4.544e-05);
σ2 mother ID,S (father´s age) = 2.061e-09 (5.540-e-05); σ2 cohort,I = 0.3725 (0.6104)
4. No. fledglings 5,343
Intercept -0.3035 (0.2627)
Father´s age 0.0010 (0.0042) 0.66 0.817
Mother´s age 0.1131 (0.0179) 46.62 <0.001
Mother´s age2 -0.0054 (0.0010) 34.26 <0.001
Laying date -1.4539 (0.0567) 686.06 <0.001
σ2 father ID,I = 2.157e-09 (4.644e-05); σ2 mother ID,I (mother´s age) = 2.109e-09 (4.592e-05);
σ2 mother ID,S (mother´s age) = 2.061e-09 (4.540e-05); σ2 mother ID,I (father´s age) = 2.209e-09 (4.70e-05);
σ2 mother ID,S (father´s age) = 2.209e-09 (4.70e-05); σ2 cohort,I = 0.6799 (0.8245)
5. Fledging success 6,125
Intercept 1.2456 (0.4610)
Father´s age 0.1651 (0.0495) 8.89 0.011
Father´s age2 -0.0077 (0.0024) 8.78 0.003
Mother´s age 0.1524 (0.0528) 6.19 0.045
Mother´s age2 -0.0080 (0.0030) 6.01 0.014
Laying date -2.9791 (0.1828) 267.97 <0.001
σ2 father ID,I = 0.0818 (0.2860); σ2 mother ID,I (mother´s age) = 0.8284 (0.9101); σ2 mother ID,S (mother´s age) = 0.0104 (0.1020);
σ2 mother ID,I (father´s age) = 0.0000 (0.0000); σ2 mother ID,S (father´s age) = 0.0011 (0.0327);
σ2 cohort,I = 1.4612 (1.2198); σ2nest ID,I = 0.3086 (0.5555); σ2 residual = 1
6. Fledgling body condition  4,873 ns

Fig 2. Significant quadratic effects of mothers’ and fathers’ ages (years) on breeding traits.

Fig 2

Discussion

In the blue-footed booby, offspring viability, measured as probability of recruitment or early recruitment into the breeding population, depends strongly on the combination of parental ages. Fledglings with two very young parents (≤4 years) or two very old parents (≥13 years) were the most likely to fail, whereas fledglings with one very young parent and one very old parent were the most likely to succeed. For a young parent of either sex offspring viability increased with age of the partner, while for a very old parent of either sex offspring viability decreased with age of the partner. Failure to recruit could occur because fledglings died before the age of recruitment or because they were insufficiently competitive adults to acquire a territory and partner and care for a clutch. The phenotypic traits of fledglings associated with this pattern were not identified; they may not include body condition at fledging because this variable was not affected by a parental age interaction. Effects of combined parental ages across the age-span on offspring quality have not, to our knowledge, been investigated in other non-human species except in a study of semi-captive common lizards (Lacerta vivipara), where parental ages did not interact to affect first-year survival [28].

Variation in viability of fledglings could be due to variation in parental investment. At the oocyte/egg stage, female birds can influence diverse aspects of offspring development both epigenetically and by modifying hormone, carotenoid and antibody contents of yolk [6567], and both sexes can also influence development by adjusting their care of clutch and brood. Furthermore, in some circumstances female and male parents are expected to modify their investment in progeny in response to the characteristics of their mates [68,69], including age [70]. According to one model, adjustments made by female animals in response to male quality can depend on the female’s own age or body condition, for example with young females increasing investment when the male is of high quality and old females doing so when he is of low quality [71]. Consistent with this expectation, young female mallards (Anas platyrhynchos) laid larger eggs when their partner was attractive [72] whereas old female mallards in another study laid larger eggs when their partner was unattractive [73]. In theory then, the high viability of fledgling boobies with dissimilar-aged parents could be explained by young mothers increasing investment in eggs or care of clutch and brood when paired with old males, and old mothers doing so when paired with young males; or similar adjustments by fathers.

However, scrutiny of thousands of breeding attempts failed to provide any evidence that parental ages interact to affect parental investment. Four of six breeding traits measured during the period of parental care showed highly significant quadratic and linear effects of female age, and fledging success also showed highly significant quadratic and linear effects of male age, but none of the six was affected by the interaction of male and female ages (Fig 2, Table 2).Thus the pattern of parental age effects at the egg and nestling stages did not anticipate the pattern of their effects on fledgling recruitment. This implies that effects of parental age combination on recruitment may not be mediated by their effects on parental investment. Further, although age-sensitive adjustments in investment can potentially explain pairs of young or old breeders producing low-viability fledglings, they cannot explain them producing a high proportion of extra-pair chicks [46]. The latter finding calls for an explanation by a genetic mechanism because extra-pair fathers contribute no parental investment to offspring, only genes.

Identification of the benefits of extra-pair behaviour to female birds has been troublesome [74], but there are good grounds for believing that they sometimes accrue through pairing females’ genes with male genes that complement them better than those of their social partners [48]. It is possible that very old female boobies most frequently produce extra-pair chicks when paired to very old males, and young females when paired to young males, in order to avoid saddling their progeny with the gene combinations brought about by those age pairings [46]. If we assume that a genetic mechanism underlies the pattern of parental age-related fledgling viability, then by obtaining extra-pair sires for their offspring that are an average of 6.3 years older than their social partners [46], young females could increase their fledglings’ chances of recruiting by roughly 50% (estimated from Fig 1B).

Interacting effects of parental ages on offspring quality have been reported in humans, and genetic or gene-environment causes are suspected. Offspring of old fathers are more likely to have diseases of genetic origin [75], and the probability of birth defects of known or suspected genetic origin varies in complex ways with the combination of maternal and paternal ages [42,44]. We speculate that the high viability of fledglings with dissimilar-aged parents could derive from increased heterozygosity or high quality genes. In populations with overlapping generations, great environmental fluctuations such as those of El Niño Southern Oscillation can maintain variation in genes, including genes of large effect [76,77], and nestlings whose parents belong to temporally distant birth cohorts exposed to different selection regimes over different periods of time may be more heterozygous. Alternatively, high genetic quality could be favored in offspring of dissimilar-aged parents by the two age-related mechanisms most widely expected to affect viability of animal offspring: increase in the average genetic quality of surviving breeders [79] and decrease in the integrity of adult germ line DNA [12,13]. Assuming damage to DNA is greater in the germ line of males than females [78,79], and that the capacity of oocytes to repair DNA damage in male sperm declines with female age [13,80], superior viability could be shown by (1) offspring of young female/old male matings because of old males’ high genetic quality coupled with young females’ ability to repair their damaged DNA, and (2) offspring of old female/young male matings because of old females’ high genetic quality and relatively undamaged DNA. Fledglings derived from young/old parent matings could be the most viable because they are the only ones to receive high quality genes with relatively undamaged or substantially repaired DNA from one parent along with undamaged DNA from the other parent.

If the observed pattern of variation in offspring viability is due to genetic effects, then our analyses may well underestimate the impact of parental age combinations on offspring viability, because our sample of fledglings surely included a proportion of individuals sired by females’ extra-pair partners. Based on rates of extra-pair paternity observed in this population [46], an estimated 20.3% of fledglings with two very young parents and 20.8% of fledglings with an old mother (≥8 years) and a very old father were probably sired by males of less extreme age than the social father. Such extra-pair fledglings should inflate the model’s estimates for recruitment of offspring from those two parental age combinations because they derive from more favorable combinations. A quite different interpretation is that the low average viability of offspring of two old parents or two young parents is due to the relatively high incidence of extra-pair individuals, if we assume that extra-pair individuals have low viability. However, it is implausible that female boobies would allow, let alone foster, extra-pair fertilizations that greatly reduce the viability of their progeny.

We should seek parental age combination effects in other species and explore their consequences for mating systems. Although they can be large (Fig 1), combination effects may have gone undetected because of the difficulty of obtaining adequate samples, particularly of old individuals paired with variably aged partners. Wherever there is an effect of parental age combination on progeny, whether due to variation in parental investment or in offspring genomes, females’ and males’ own ages are likely to affect their preferences among breeding partners of different ages. Where combination effects are due to offspring genomes, the particular age combination of a breeding pair could potentially affect both partners’ extra-pair behaviour and their willingness to invest in their shared offspring. For example, suboptimal age combinations could favor females seeking extra-pair sires of ages that combine better with their own ages (cf. [46]), and males increasing their relative investment in extra-pair versus within-pair reproduction.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Sample sizes for analysis of recruitment of fledglings with different combinations of mothers’ and fathers’ ages.

(XLS)

Acknowledgments

For dedicated work in the field we thank José Luis Osorno, Roxana Torres and generations of student volunteers; for statistical advice we thank Alejandra Ramos, Lynna Kiere, Oscar Sánchez and, especially, an anonymous reviewer and Anja Widdig. For comments on the manuscript we thank Peter Becker, Sandra Bouwhuis, Sin-Yeon Kim, Sue Lewis and Dani Oro. Annual fieldwork on Isla Isabel was supported logistically by many generous fishermen, the Comisión Nacional de Areas Naturales Protegidas and the Mexican Navy. Data are available from: https://zenodo.org/record/19165.

Data Availability

All relevant data are available on Zenodo at https://zenodo.org/record/19165.

Funding Statement

Finance was provided by the Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (4722-N9407, C01-47599, D112-903581, PCCNCNA-031528, 31973-H, 34500V, 104313), the Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México (PAPIIT, IN211491, IN-200702-3, IN206610-3, IN205313) and the National Geographic Society (1992, 1996).

References

  • 1. Balbontin J, Hermosell IG, Marzal A, Reviriego M, Lope F de, Moller AP. Age-related change in breeding performance in early life is associated with an increase in competence in the migratory barn swallow Hirundo rustica J Anim Ecol. 2007; 76: 915–925. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01269.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. McCleery RH, Perrins CM, Sheldon BC, Charmantier A. Age-specific reproduction in a long-lived species: the combined effects of senescence and individual quality. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008; 275:963 970. 10.1098/rspb.2007.1418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Rebke M, Coulson T, Becker PH, Vaupel W. Reproductive improvement and senescence in a long-lived bird. PNAS. 2010; 107: 7841–7846. 10.1073/pnas.1002645107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Beck CW, Powell LA. Evolution of female mate choice based on male age: Are older males better mates? Evol Ecol Res. 2000; 2: 107–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Brooks R, Kemp DJ. Can older males deliver the good genes? Trends Ecol Evol. 2001; 16: 308–313. 10.1016/S0169-5347(01)02147-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Johnson SL, Gemmell NJ. Are old males still good males and can females tell the difference? Bioessays 2014; 34: 609–619. 10.1002/bies.201100157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Manning JT. Choosy females and correlates of male age. J Theoret Biol. 1985; 116: 349–354. 10.1016/S0022-5193(85)80273-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kokko H, Lindstrom J. Evolution of female preference for old mates. Proc R Soc B. 1996; 263: 1533–1538. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0224 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Kokko H. Good genes, old age and life-history trade-offs. Evol Ecol. 1998; 12: 739–750. 10.1023/A:1006541701002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Hansen TF, Price DK. Good genes and old age: Do old mates provide superior genes? J Evol Biol. 1995; 8: 759–778. 10.1046/j.1420-9101.1995.8060759.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Jones TM, Balmford A, Quinnell RJ. Adaptive female choice for middle–aged mates in a lekking sandfly. Proc R Soc B. 2000; 267: 681–686. 10.1098/rspb.2000.1056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Carmichael AN, Fridolfsson AK, Halverson J, Ellegren H. Male-biased mutation rates revealed from Z and W chromosome-linked ATP synthase α-subunit (ATP5A1) sequences in birds. J Mol Evol. 2000; 50: 443–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Velando A, Torres R, Alonso-Alvarez C. Avoiding bad genes: oxidatively damaged DNA in germ line and mate choice. Bioessays. 2008; 30: 1212–1219. 10.1002/bies.20838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Jones TM, Featherston R, Paris DB, Elgar MA. Age-related sperm transfer and sperm competitive ability in the male hide beetle. Behav Ecol. 2007; 18: 251–258. 10.1093/beheco/arl077 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Møller AP, Mousseau TA, Rudolfsen G, Balbontin J, Marzal A, Hermosell I, et al. Senescent sperm performance in old male birds. J Evol Biol. 2009; 22: 334–344. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01650.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Pizzari T, Dean R, Pacey A, Moore H, Bonsall MB. The evolutionary ecology of pre-and post-meiotic sperm senescence. Trends Ecol Evol. 2008; 23: 131–140. 10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Velando A, Noguera JC, Drummond H, Torres R. Senescent males carry premutagenic lesions in sperm. J Evol Biol. 2011; 24: 693–697. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.02201.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Preston BT, Saint Jalme M, Hingrat Y, Lacroix F, Sorci G. The sperm of aging male bustards retards their offspring’s development. Nature Communications. 2015; 6: 6146 10.1038/ncomms7146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Cichoñ M. Does prior breeding experience improve reproductive success in collared flycatcher females. Oecologia. 2003; 134: 78–81. 10.1007/s00442-002-1099-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Limmer B, Becker PH. Improvement in chick provisioning with parental experience in a seabird. Anim Behav. 2009; 77: 1095–1101. 10.1093/beheco/arr019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Beamonte-Barrientos R, Velando A, Drummond H, Torres R. Senescence of maternal effects: aging influences egg quality and rearing capacities of a long-lived bird. Am Nat. 2010; 175: 469–480. 10.1086/650726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Price DK, Hansen TF. How does offspring quality change with age in male Drosophila melanogaster? Behav Genetics. 1998; 28: 395–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Serre V, Robaire B. Paternal age affects fertility and progeny outcome in the Brown Norway rat. Fertil Steril. 1998; 70: 625–631. 10.1098/rspb.2000.1056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. García-Palomares S, Pertusa JF, Miñarro J, García-Pérez MA, Hermenegildo C, Rausell F, et al. Long-Term Effects of Delayed Fatherhood in Mice on Postnatal Development and Behavioral Traits of Offspring. Biol Reprod. 2009; 80: 337–342 10.1095/biolreprod.108.072066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Krishna MS, Santhosh HT, Hegde SN. Offspring of older males are superior in Drosophila bipectinata . Zool Stud. 2012; 51: 72–84. 10.1002/bies.201100157 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Hegyi G, Rosivall B, Török J. Paternal age and offspring growth: separating the intrinsic quality of young from rearing effects. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006; 60: 672–682. 10.1007/s00265-006-0211-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Jones TM, Elgar MA. The role of male age, sperm age and mating history on fecundity and fertilization success in the hide beetle. Proc R Soc B. 2004; 271: 1311–1318. 10.1098/rspb.2004.2723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Richard M, Lecomte L, De Fraipont M, Clobert J. Age-specific mating strategies and reproductive senescence. Mol Ecol. 2005; 14: 3147–3155. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02662.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Liu Y, Zhi M, Li X. Parental age and characteristics of the offspring. Ageing Res Rev.2011; 10: 115–123. 10.1371/journal.pone.0090097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Nikola T, Darka Š, Vesna S. The short-term and long-term effects of parental age in the bean weevil (Acanthoscelides obtectus). Evol Ecol. 2004; 18: 187–201. 10.1023/B:EVEC.0000021090.51851.bd [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Priest NK, Mackowiak B, Promislow DE. The role of parental age effects on the evolution of aging. Evolution. 2002; 56: 927–935. 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb01405.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Moore PJ, Harris WE. Is a decline in offspring quality a necessary consequence of maternal age? Proc R Soc B. 2003; 270: S192–S194. 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Saino N, Ambrosini R, Martinelli R, Møller AP. Mate fidelity, senescence in breeding performance and reproductive trade-offs in the barn swallow. J Anim Ecol. 2002; 71: 309–319. 10.1046/j.1365-2656.2002.00600.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Bouwhuis S, Charmantier A, Verhulst S, Sheldon BC. Trans-generational effects on ageing in a wild bird population. J Evol Biol. 2010; 23: 636–642. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01929.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Fuerst-Waltl B, Reichl A, Fuerst C, Baumung R, Sölkner J. Effect of maternal age on milk production traits, fertility, and longevity in cattle. J Dairy Sci. 2004; 87: 2293–2298. 10.3168/jds.S0022-030204)70050-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Descamps S, Boutin S, Berteaux D, Gaillard JM. Age-specific variation in survival, reproductive success and offspring quality in red squirrels: evidence of senescence. Oikos. 2008;117: 1406–1416. 10.1098/rspb.2008.1401 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Benton TG, St Clair JJH, Plaistow SJ. Maternal effects mediated by maternal age: from life histories to population dynamics. J Anim Ecol. 2008; 77: 1038–1046. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01434.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. González-Solis J, Becker PH, Jover L, Ruiz X. Individual changes underlie age-specific pattern of laying date and egg-size in female common terns (Sterna hirundo). J Ornithol. 2004; 145: 129–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Bogdanova MI, Nager RG, Monaghan P. Does parental age affect offspring performance through differences in egg quality? Funct Ecol. 2006; 20: 132–141. 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0573 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Ming-Hseng W, Frederick S, vom Saal. Maternal age and traits in offspring. Nature. 2000; 407: 469–470. 10.1038/35035156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Postma M, Bester MN, de Bruyn PN. Age-related reproductive variation in a wild marine mammal population. Polar Biol. 2013; 36: 719–729. 10.1007/s00300-013-1298-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Bille C, Skytthe A, Vach W, Knudsen LB, Andersen A-MY, Murray JC, et al. Parent's age and risk of oral clefts. Epidemiology. 2005; 16: 311–316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Green RF, Devine O, Crider KS, Olney RS, Archer N, Olshan AF, et al. The National Birth Defects Prevention Study. Association of paternal age and risk for major congenital anomalies from the national birth deffects prevention study, from 1997 to 2004. Ann Epidemiol. 2010; 20: 241–249. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2009.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Fisch H, Hyun G, Golden R, Hensle TW, Olsson CA, Liberson GL. The influence of paternal age on Down syndrome. J Urology. 2003; 169: 2275–2278. 10.1097/01.ju.0000067958.36077.d8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Nussey DH, Froy H, Lemaitre J-F, Gaillard J-M, Austad SN. Senescence in natural populations of animals: widespread evidence and its implications for bio-gerontology. Ageing Res Rev. 2013; 12: 214–225. 10.1016/j.exger.2012.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Ramos AG, Nunziata SO, Lance SL, Rodríguez C, Faircloth BC, Gowaty PA, et al. Interactive effects of male and female age on extra-pair paternity in a socially monogamous seabird. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2014; 68: 1603–1609. 10.1007/s00265-014-1769-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Osorio-Beristain M, Drummond H. Non-aggressive mate-guarding by the blue-footed booby: a balance of female and male control. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1998; 43: 307–315. 10.1007/s002650050496 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Griffith SC, Immler S. Female infidelity and genetic compatibility in birds: the role of the genetically loaded raffle in understanding the function of extrapair paternity. J Avian Biol. 2009; 2: 97–101. 10.1046/j.1365-294X.2002.01613.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Velando A, Drummond H, Torres R. Senescent birds redouble reproductive effort when ill: confirmation of the terminal investment hypothesis. Proc R. Soc B. 2006; 273: 1443–1448. 10.1098/rspb.2004.2678 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Torres R, Velando A. Male reproductive senescence: the price of immune-induced oxidative damage on sexual attractiveness in the blue-footed booby. J Anim Ecol. 2007; 76: 1161–1168. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01282.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Kim S-Y, Velando A, Torres R, Drummond H. Effects of recruiting age on senescence, lifespan and lifetime reproductive success in a long-lived seabird. Oecologia. 2011; 166: 615–626. 10.1007/s00442-011-1914-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Torres R, Drummond H, Velando A. Parental age and lifespan influence offspring recruitment: a long-term study in a seabird. PloS ONE. 2011; 6: 11 10.1371/journal.pone.0027245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Oro D, Torres R, Rodríguez Juárez C, Drummond H. Climatic influence on demographic parameters of a tropical seabird varies with age and sex. Ecology. 2010; 91: 1205–1214. 10.1098/rspb.2010.2569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Kim S-Y, Torres R, Dominguez CA, Drummond H. Lifetime philopatry in the blue-footed booby. Behav Ecol. 2007; 18: 1132–1138. 10.1093/beheco/arm091 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Sánchez-Macouzet O, Rodríguez C, Drummond H. Better stay together: pair bond duration increases individual fitness independent of age-related variation. Proc Royal Soc B. 2014; 281: 20132843 10.1098/rspb.2013.2843 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Nelson JB. The Sulidae: gannets and boobies Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Drummond H, Torres R, Krishnan VV. Buffered development: resilience after aggressive subordination in infancy. Am. Nat. 2003; 161: 794–807. 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0461 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing. J Memory and Language. 2013; 68:255–278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Crawley MJ. The R book. Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Wilk MB, Gnanadesikan R. Probability plotting methods for the analysis of data. Biometrika. 1968; 55: 1–17 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Torres R, Velando A. A dynamic trait affects continuous pair assessment in the blue-footed booby Sula nebouxii . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2003; 55: 65–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 62. R Development Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. http://www.R-project.org [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Skaug H, Fournier D, Nielsen A, Magnusson A, Bolker B. Generalized linear mixed models using AD Model Builder. 2011; R package version 0.6 5, r143.
  • 64.Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. 2014; R package version 1.1–7, http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
  • 65. Mousseau TA, Fox CW. Maternal effects as adaptations UK: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Groothuis TGG, Schwable H. Hormone-mediated maternal effects in birds: mechanisms matter but what do we know of them? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2008; 363: 1647–1661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Hasselquist D, Nilsson J- A. Maternal transfer of antibodies in vertebrates: transgenerational effects on offspring immunity. Phil Trans Royal Soc. 2009; 364: 51–60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Sheldon BC. Differential allocation: tests mechanisms and implications. Trends Ecol Evol. 2000; 15: 397–402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Ratikainen II, Kokko H. Differential allocation and compensation: who deserves the silver spoon? Behav Ecol. 2009; 21: 195–200 [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Michl G, Torok J, Peczely P, Garamszegi LZ, Schwabl H. Female collared flycatchers adjust yolk testosterone to male age, but not to attractiveness. Behav Ecol. 2005; 16: 383–388 [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Harris WE, Uller T. Reproductive investment when mate quality varies: differential allocation versus reproductive compensation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009; 364: 1039–1048 10.1098/rstb.2008.0299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Cunningham EJ, Russell AF. Egg investment in influenced by male attractiveness in the mallard. Nature. 2000; 404: 74–77. 10.1038/35003565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Bluhm CK, Gowaty PA. Reproductive compensation for offspring viability deficits by female mallards, Anas platyrhynchos . Anim Behav. 2004; 68: 985–992. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.01.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Forstmeier W, Nakagawa S, Griffith SC, Kempenaers B. Female extra-pair mating: adaptation or genetic constraint? Trends Ecol Evol. 2014; 29: 456–464. 10.1016/j.tree.2014.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Sartorius GA, Nieschlag E. Paternal age and reproduction. Hum Reprod Update. 2010; 16: 65–79. 10.1093/humupd/dmp027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Ellner S, Hairston NG. Role of overlapping generations in maintaining genetic variation in a fluctuating environment. Am Nat. 1994; 143: 403–417 [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Ellner S, Sasaki A. Patterns of genetic polymorphism maintained by fluctuating selection with overlapping generations. Theor Popul Biol. 1996; 50: 31–65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Ellegren H. Characteristics, causes and evolutionary consequences of male-biased mutation. Proc R Soc B. 2007; 274: 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Sayres MAW, Makova KD. Genome analyses substantiate male mutation bias in many species. BioEssays. 2011; 33: 938–945. 10.1002/bies.201100091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Hamatani T, Falco G, Carter MG, Ajutsu H, Stagg CA, Sharov AA, et al. Age-associated alteration of gene expression patterns in mouse oocytes. Hum Mol Genet. 2004; 13: 2263–2278. 10.1093/hmg/ddh241 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Table. Sample sizes for analysis of recruitment of fledglings with different combinations of mothers’ and fathers’ ages.

(XLS)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are available on Zenodo at https://zenodo.org/record/19165.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES