Abstract
Objectives
Mobile phone texting is a common daily occurrence with a paucity of research examining corresponding gait characteristics. To date, most studies have participants walk in a straight line vs. overcoming barriers and obstacles that occur during regular walking. The aim of our study is to examine the effect of mobile phone texting during periods of cognitive distraction while walking and negotiating barriers synonymous with pedestrian traffic.
Methods
Thirty participants (18-50y) completed three randomized, counter-balanced walking tasks over a course during: (1) normal walking (control), (2) texting and walking, and (3) texting and walking whilst being cognitively distraction via a standard mathematical test performed while negotiating the obstacle course. We analyzed gait characteristics during course negotiation using a 3-dimensional motion analysis system and a general linear model and Dunnet-Hsu post-hoc procedure the normal walking condition to assess gait characteristic differences. Primary outcomes included the overall time to complete the course time and barrier contact. Secondary outcomes included obstacle clearance height, step frequency, step time, double support phase and lateral deviation.
Results
Participants took significantly longer (mean ± SD) to complete the course while texting (24.96±4.20 sec) and during cognitive distraction COG (24.09±3.36 sec) vs. normal walking (19.32±2.28 sec; all, P<0.001). No significant differences were noted for barrier contacts (P = 0.28). Step frequency, step time, double support phase and lateral deviation all increased in duration during the texting and cognitive distraction trial. Texting and being cognitively distracted also increased obstacle clearance versus the walking condition (all, P<0.02).
Conclusions
Texting while walking and/or being cognitively distracted significantly affect gait characteristics concordant to mobile phone usage resulting in a more cautious gate pattern. Future research should also examine a similar study in older participants who may be at a greater risk of tripping with such walking deviations.
Introduction
Mobile and Smartphone ownership increased internationally as has the individual use of texting, SMS messaging, Internet use, social media access and emailing. For simplicity we will use “text” to denote the various forms of Internet communication functions. While text growth facilitates communication it also increases the risk of distraction during walking, which may in turn carry with it an increased risk for tripping, collision or secondary injuries to other pedestrians attempting to avoid those who are texting and deviating from a normal path of ambulation [1–3]. It has been proffered that these phenomena are due to inattentional blindness and situational awareness [4]. While the risk associated with texting has been documented for during driving, the potential hazards associated with walking are not as well established [5–7].
Few studies have investigated walking and texting and fewer yet the simultaneous negotiation of an obstacle course [8]. This is an important consideration as “real life” requires the user to look away from pathway obstacles in order to text [9]. As such, texting while walking disrupts gait speed, potentially increasing road crossing time and riskier road crossing behaviors, possibly increasing tripping and accident risk [10–13]. While most studies use fairly simplistic models, we are unaware of research using more circuitous obstacle courses designed and built to represent common pedestrian obstacles such as curbs, people, steps, etc. The primary aim of our study is to examine the effect texting on gait characteristics while negotiating common pedestrian obstacles. We hypothesize that texting will affect normal gait characteristics and increase barrier contacts, a surrogate for tripping, while negotiating steps, ramps and obstacles representing pedestrian traffic.
Materials and Methods
Participants
We recruited thirty participants (18 females; 18–50 years) to take part in our study approved by the University of Bath Department for Health Ethics Advisory Panel. We included only participants who owned their own mobile phone for more than one month and excluded candidates taking medications that may cause dizziness. All participants signed an informed consent outlining the study aims and procedures. Subsequently, participants completed questionnaires regarding their current mobile phone use and a physical-activity readiness questionnaire [14].
Experimental Procedures
Before initiating formal testing procedures, participants completed a familiarization session of the obstacle course under each testing condition: (1) walking with no distraction (WLK, control), (2) responding to standardized texting questions on their own phone (TXT) and (3) completing a mental mathematics quiz (AB Math Lite 5.3) on an iPhone (Apple, Cupertino, CA US phone (COG). All participants were instructed not to look at either the messages or the mental mathematics quiz until they began walking. All walking conditions were administered in a randomized, counter-balanced manner.
The obstacle course was designed to mimic obstacles one would encounter in everyday life (Fig 1) and consisted of seven obstacles designed and based on fieldwork within the City Centre of Bath, UK: (1) An obstacle resembling a curb to step over, (2) A platform to step-on, step-off platform, (3) A set of uneven steps, (4–5) Two traffic bollards to step around and, (6–7) Two dummies of sufficient height representing model people for participants to step around.
We used a 3D optical motion analysis system (Qualisys, Sweden), with 13 Oqus 4 cameras set-up to collect kinematic data whilst the participants negotiated the obstacle course in each of the conditions. Six reflective markers were attached to each participant using double sided tape: two on each shoe (n = 4), one approximately over the 5th metatarsal-phalangeal (MTP) joint and one on the back of the heel, and two on an elasticated band worn around the pelvis. The two pelvis markers were positioned on either side of the spine, approximately over the posterior superior iliac spines. The elasticated band was used to provide a tight fitting surface to which to attach the pelvis markers as the participants undertook the testing in their everyday clothes. We also placed photocell light gates (Powertimer 1.0, Newtest, Finland) at the start and finish of the course to measure time to complete the course. Reflective markers were placed on every obstacle on the corners of the front edge of each obstacle except for the two model people. The 3D positions of the markers on the participant and the markers on the obstacles were determined at 100Hz. From these data we calculated the velocity of the participant and various step characteristics as they negotiated the course (Fig 1). Data from Qualisys was imported into an Excel spreadsheet in order to perform requisite calculations. No filtering was applied or data adjusted we only looked at marker displacement of the markers and did not investigate velocity or acceleration.
Step Characteristics
To calculate step characteristics we determined the timings of heel strike and toe-off using an algorithm developed by Zeni Jr. et al. (2008). [15] This entailed finding the coordinates of the mid-point of the pelvis markers and subtracting this from the coordinates of heel and 5th MTP markers. Heel strike was defined as the point at which the heel was at a maximum distance in front of the hip and toe-off as the point at which the 5th MTP was at a maximum distance behind the hip. Based upon the timings and positions of heel strike and toe off of each step the following parameters were determined and where appropriate a mean value calculated over the course:
Step count: the number of steps taken for the obstacle course
Step time: the time between the heel strike of one foot and the heel strike of the contralateral foot
Step frequency: the number of steps per second
Step length: the distance between the position of the heel marker at heel strike of one foot and the position of the heel marker at heel strike of the contralateral foot
Double support phase: the time between second foot heel strike and first foot toe off or the time during which both feet were in contact with the ground
Barrier clearance: the difference between the 5th MTP marker vertical coordinate and the barrier vertical coordinate at the time the 5th MTP joint arrived at the obstacle.
We also performed a sub-analysis to examine some of these gait characteristics during the approach and negotiation of the obstacles. Stair approach is defined as each respective gait characteristic observed in the 1.5 m immediate prior to negotiating the stairs. Stair ascent is defined as each respective gait characteristic observed in the ascending the stairs. Platform traverse is each respective gait characteristic observed while traversing the platform.
In addition to the outcome measures defined above, the deviation from a straight path was investigated during the first two sides of the obstacle course. This part of the course was chosen as it involved two clearly defined straight channels with a 90° turn between them (Fig 1). The length of the straight-line path of the first and second sides of the course was determined from the co-ordinates of the centre of the beginning and the end of the path for these two sides (as defined by cones and markers on the ground). The length of the path travelled by the participants was defined as the cumulative displacement of the centre of the pelvis markers along the first and second sides of the course. This was calculated by determining the centre point of the two pelvis markers at each time point, calculating the displacement of this point between each time point and summing these from the beginning of the course until the participant reached the end of the second side. The deviation from walking a straight path was determined by subtracting the straight path distance from the actual path travelled.
Statistical Analysis
We examined our data using a general linear model covaried for the control/WLK condition using SPSS (v21). Data sphericity was accounted for via a Greenhouse-Geisser correction. Post-hoc analyses were performed using a Dunnett-HSU analysis to compare TXT and COG to the WLK condition. We also performed Pearson correlations between various gait characteristics and barrier contact in an effort to associate potential gait characteristics and tripping risk. All data is presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or 95% confidence intervals where appropriate. Statistical significance was established at P < 0.05. The effect size is presented as the partial eta squared for each analysis.
Results and Discussion
Course Performance Characteristics
We have presented the overall findings for negotiating the complete course in Table 1. Overall, we found that it took significantly longer to complete the course due to slower walking speeds under the TXT and COG conditions vs. WLK (all, P<0.001, Fig 2A). In support of these observations, participants exhibited significantly shorter step lengths (Fig 2B), lower step frequencies, longer double support phases (Fig 2C), and greater obstacle clearance heights while TXT and COG vs. WLK (all, P<0.001, Fig 2D). While the COG condition was significantly different to TXT for walking speed (P<0.02) and step frequency (P<0.04), no other significant differences were noted between the TXT and COG conditions. Despite these alterations in gait characteristics we did not observe any changes in barrier contacts during any treatment condition (P = 0.10).
Table 1. Course performance characteristics of participants negotiating obstacle course.
Distraction Challenge | Mean | SD | Significance vs. WLK | Partial Eta Squared | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Course Time (sec) | Walk | 19.32 | 2.3 | ||
Text | 24.96 | 4.2 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 24.09 | 3.4 | < 0.001 | 0.79 | |
Walk Speed (m/sec) | Walk | 0.78 | 0.1 | ||
Text | 0.61 | 0.1 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive a | 0.63 | 0.1 | < 0.001 | 0.88 | |
Lateral Deviation | Walk | 2.71 | 0.3 | ||
from Straight Path | Text | 2.76 | 0.3 | NS | |
Cognitive | 2.77 | 0.6 | NS | 0.04 | |
Barrier Contact (n) | Walk | 0.13 | 0.4 | ||
Text | 0.23 | 0.4 | NS | ||
Cognitive | 0.10 | 0.3 | NS | 0.10 | |
Step Length (cm) | Walk | 42.00 | 30.0 | ||
Text | 36.00 | 20.0 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 37.00 | 20.0 | < 0.001 | 0.87 | |
Step Frequency (steps/sec) | Walk | 1.84 | 0.2 | ||
Text | 1.67 | 0.2 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive b | 1.71 | 0.2 | < 0.001 | 0.62 | |
Double Support Phase (sec) | Walk | 170.00 | 30.0 | ||
Text | 200.00 | 30.0 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 200.00 | 40.0 | < 0.001 | 0.26 | |
Obstacle Clearance Height (mm) | Walk | 94.00 | 13.0 | ||
Text | 111.00 | 19.0 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 110.00 | 23.0 | <0.02 | 0.40 |
a = Significant vs. Text = 0.015.
b = Significant vs. Text = 0.035.
Performance Characteristics for Individual Obstacles
We have presented the findings for negotiating each individual obstacle in Table 2.
Table 2. Performance characteristics for individual obstacles.
Mean | SD | Significance vs. WLK | Partial Eta Squared | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Time (ms) | Stair | Walk | 550.00 | 60.0 | ||
Approach | Text | 580.00 | 90.0 | 0.074 | ||
Cognitive | 590.00 | 70.0 | 0.004 | 0.267 | ||
Platform | Walk | 630.00 | 60.0 | |||
Traverse | Text | 710.00 | 110.0 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 710.00 | 100.0 | < 0.001 | 0.453 | ||
Stair | Walk | 480.00 | 130.0 | |||
Ascent | Text | 570.00 | 90.0 | 0.000 | ||
Cognitive | 550.00 | 70.0 | 0.008 | 0.414 | ||
Step count (n) | Stair | Walk | 1.24 | 0.4 | ||
Approach | Text | 1.43 | 0.5 | |||
Cognitive | 1.38 | 0.5 | 0.140 | |||
Platform | Walk | 1.07 | 0.3 | |||
Traverse | Text | 1.40 | 0.5 | 0.004 | ||
Cognitive | 1.24 | 0.4 | 0.060 | 0.270 | ||
Stair | Walk | 1.10 | 0.3 | |||
Ascent | Text | 1.53 | 0.5 | < 0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 1.34 | 0.5 | < 0.001 | 0.400 | ||
Step Frequency (steps/sec) | Stair | Walk | 1.83 | 0.2 | ||
Approach | Text | 1.89 | 0.5 | NS | ||
Cognitive | 1.83 | 0.5 | NS | 0.001 | ||
Platform | Walk | 1.61 | 0.2 | |||
Traverse | Text | 1.55 | 0.5 | NS | ||
Cognitive | 1.44 | 0.2 | < 0.001 | 0.556 | ||
Stair | Walk | 2.17 | 0.6 | |||
Ascent | Text | 1.80 | 0.4 | 0.005 | ||
Cognitive | 1.87 | 0.3 | 0.014 | 0.266 | ||
Step Length (cm) | Stair | Walk | 69.70 | 17.1 | ||
Approach | Text | 61.20 | 7.7 | 0.011 | ||
Cognitive | 59.90 | 10.7 | 0.004 | 0.268 | ||
Platform | Walk | 72.40 | 7.0 | |||
Traverse | Text | 58.40 | 15.7 | 0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 64.60 | 9.4 | 0.001 | 0.486 | ||
Stair | Walk | 54.40 | 23.8 | |||
Ascent | Text | 48.70 | 18.2 | NS | ||
Cognitive | 50.80 | 14.6 | NS | 0.087 | ||
Double Support Phase (ms) | Stair | Walk | 150.00 | 30.0 | ||
Approach | Text | 170.00 | 40.0 | 0.004 | ||
Cognitive | 170.00 | 20.0 | < 0.001 | 0.483 | ||
Platform | Walk | 170.00 | 70.0 | |||
Traverse | Text | 190.00 | 40.0 | NS | ||
Cognitive | 200.00 | 80.0 | NS | 0.097 | ||
Stair | Walk | 220.00 | 120.0 | |||
Ascent | Text | 290.00 | 150.0 | NS | ||
Cognitive | 280.00 | 160.0 | <0.001 | 0.440 | ||
Clearance Height (mm) | Stair | Walk | 135.00 | 26.0 | ||
Approach | Text | 159.00 | 41.0 | <0.001 | ||
Cognitive | 148.00 | 51.0 | NS | 0.365 | ||
Platform | Walk | 65.00 | 26.0 | |||
Traverse | Text | 65.00 | 22.0 | NS | ||
Cognitive | 65.00 | 32.0 | NS | 0.000 | ||
Stair | Walk | 73.00 | 14.0 | |||
Ascent | Text | 97.00 | 37.0 | 0.003 | ||
Cognitive | 99.00 | 22.0 | <0.001 | 0.615 |
Step Approach
When negotiating the 1.5 m approach to the steps, we did not observe a significant difference to the step count or step frequency on the approach. However, we did observe a significantly greater length of time to the approach for the COG condition and a strong trend for significance during the TXT condition (P = 0.07) due to a significant reduction in step length and double support phase for the TXT (P = 0.011) and COG conditions (P = 0.004). While we also observed a greater step clearance height for the TXT condition (P<0.001), we did no observe a significant for the COG condition.
Platform Traverse
Overall, it took longer to negotiate the platform during the TXT and COG conditions (all, P<0.001) vs. WLK with no distraction. This was due, in part, to a greater step count for the TXT condition (P = 0.004) and strong trend for the COG condition (P = 0.06). Other contributing factors for this observation included a shorter step frequency for the COG condition (P<0.001) and shorter step lengths for the TXT and COG condition (both, P<0.001). No significant differences were noted for the double support phase or clearance height.
Stairs Ascent
Negotiating the stairs took significantly longer during the TXT (P<0.001) and COG conditions P = 0.008). Contributing factors for this observation included a greater step count during the TXT and COG conditions (P<0.001), a greater step frequency during the TXT (P = 0.005) and COG conditions (P = 0.014), and greater clearance heights whilst negotiating the stairs for the TXT (P = 0.003) and COG conditions (P<0.001). No significant differences were noted for step length or the double support phase of stair ascent.
Discussion
The primary aim of our study was to examine the effect of a TXT and COG challenge on gait characteristics during concomitant walking and mobile phone use. Our primary findings show that TXT and COG significantly shorten step length, reduce step frequency, lengthen double phase support and increase obstacle clearance height. Our secondary outcome analysis also shows a similar pattern alterations relative to each respective obstacle encountered over the course. Specifically, participants altered their approach to stairs, subsequent stair ascent obstacle step clearance height and the negotiation of platforms under the TXT and COG conditions. Thus, we accept our first research hypothesis that TXT and COG significantly alters gait characteristics during walking. While one might infer that these alterations in gait might increase the risk for tripping, our surrogate analysis (i.e., barrier contacts) showed no significant differences between any treatment conditions. Therefore, we reject our hypothesis that TXT and COG would increase the occurrence barrier contact and that the connection between gait alterations, deviations from walking a straight path and tripping becomes tenuous. Our results, in conjunction with others [16–18], suggest that those who walk and text adopt a “protective” gait pattern alteration in order to minimize the risk of potential accidents.
Overall, the adoption of a more conservative locomotion strategy involves a decreased walking velocity and foot placement adjustment before encountering an obstacle [16–18]. Some hypothesize that when faced with a cognitive challenge, posture and gait function are prioritized over cognitive demands, as posture and gait operate at a more subconscious level [19, 20]. Whether the change in gait pattern in our study is conscious or sub-conscious is currently indeterminable; however, the decrease in walking velocity associated with a TXT and COG challenge in our study coupled with previous literature examining other populations examining walking support this hypothesis [11, 16–18, 21, 22]. In essence, when faced with the dual task challenge of walking and texting while undergoing a cognitive challenge, participants decrease their walking speed to avoid accidents [23]. The results from the present study support this mechanism given that participants displayed adaptive changes in gait patterns during the TXT and COG conditions. As it pertains to texting our findings differ to those recently published by Plummer et al (2015) who demonstrated no significant gait alterations using a single and dual task assessment of walking and texting in 32 young adults (18–30 y) [20]. We propose several reasons for these differences.
First, Plummer et al (2015) used a straight course pathway free of obstacles, while our course was circuitous and incorporated common pedestrian obstacles. Second, we used a broader age range (18–50 y), which may have introduced a “familiarity” component as younger individuals are more likely to be familiar with the dual tasking associated with walking whilst texting. Specifically, those individuals under the age of 30, such as those in the Plummer study, likely started using mobile phones. While we attempted to explore an ageing component within our study generally feel that the upper age limit within our study was not sufficient to separate out more robust ageing differences. However, our findings are generally consistent with previous research showing that when time constraints are imposed on participants, older individuals typically take longer or exhibit more cautious gate characteristics than their younger counter parts [24–26]. Though currently absent from the literature a more thorough investigation into the effects of simultaneous texting and walking, as older individuals are more susceptible to tripping (12, 13). While our study does support a greater risk for tripping for the age group we examined, other areas of walking behaviours that warrant attention include slower road crossing time and riskier road crossing behaviors, all of which are associated with an increased tripping and accident risk [10–13]. Others have shown that walking increases ones time spent looking away from obstacles by as much as 400% [9] leading to a phenomenon a known as “inattentional blindness” [27–29]. Citing a more humorous example, Hyman et al. (2010) showed that people using mobile phones were less likely to notice a unicycling clown performing along their walking route [4].
Our study presents for the first time gait characteristics associated with TXT and COG challenges while negotiating a serpentine course designed to emulate common outdoor walking tasks. Though we attempted to examine gender and aging affects by recruiting a more mature population, the age gap was likely insufficient to distinguish more robust obstacle course differences and we are therefore unable to generalize our findings beyond the age limits associated within our study cohort. We also cannot generalize to the potential effects of TXT and COG distractions associated with mobile phone use beyond our laboratory conditions. Our study is strengthened by our use of a crossover design, as well as our incorporation of an unfamiliar phone to decrease any phone-familiarity bias. In retrospect, our study would be enhanced by the use of eye-tracking technology, which would allow for more conclusions to be drawn regarding the attention prioritization encountered during course negotiation.
The present study supports the premise that gait is not a completely automated function and is altered by the addition of TXT and COG tasks. The minimal differences between the TXT and COG conditions in the present study potentially show that even though the conditions could require different cognitive responses, those under the age of 50 y are used to interacting with mobile technology whilst walking. Based on our findings, participants display a more conservative and hesitant gait in response to the dual-tasking situation of COG and TXT. The present study has, however, advanced the relevant field of research via the use of a more sophisticated methodology, which has in turn provided greater insight into the relationship between mobile phone usage gait alterations.
Supporting Information
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files
Funding Statement
Funding for this project was obtained from internal monies within the Dept. of Health
References
- 1. Nasar J, Hecht P, Wener R. Mobile telephones, distracted attention, and pedestrian safety. Accident; analysis and prevention. 2008;40(1):69–75. 10.1016/j.aap.2007.04.005 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Nasar JL, Troyer D. Pedestrian injuries due to mobile phone use in public places. Accident; analysis and prevention. 2013;57:91–5. 10.1016/j.aap.2013.03.021 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Schwebel DC, Stavrinos D, Byington KW, Davis T, O'Neal EE, de Jong D. Distraction and pedestrian safety: how talking on the phone, texting, and listening to music impact crossing the street. Accident; analysis and prevention. 2012;45:266–71. 10.1016/j.aap.2011.07.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Hyman IE, Boss SM, Wise BM, McKenzie KE, Caggiano JM. Did you see the unicycling clown? Inattentional blindness while walking and talking on a cell phone. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2010;24(5):597–607. 10.1002/acp.1638 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Strayer DL, Johnston WA. Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone. Psychological Science 2001;12(6):462 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Ishigami Y, Klein RM. Is a hands-free phone safer than a handheld phone? J Safety Res. 2009;40(2):157–64. 10.1016/j.jsr.2009.02.006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Patten CJ, Kircher A, Ostlund J, Nilsson L. Using mobile telephones: cognitive workload and attention resource allocation. Accident; analysis and prevention. 2004;36(3):341–50. 10.1016/S0001-4575(03)00014-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Demura S, Uchiyama M. Influence of cell phone email use on characteristics of gait. European Journal of Sport Science. 2009;9(5):303–9. 10.1080/17461390902853069 . [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Hosking SG, Young KL, Regan MA. The effects of text messaging on young drivers. Human factors. 2009;51(4):582–92. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Thompson LL, Rivara FP, Ayyagari RC, Ebel BE. Impact of social and technological distraction on pedestrian crossing behaviour: an observational study. Injury prevention: journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention. 2013;19(4):232–7. 10.1136/injuryprev-2012-040601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Lamberg EM, Muratori LM. Cell phones change the way we walk. Gait Posture. 2012;35(4):688–90. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.12.005 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Pijnappels M, Reeves ND, Maganaris CN, van Dieen JH. Tripping without falling; lower limb strength, a limitation for balance recovery and a target for training in the elderly. Journal of electromyography and kinesiology: official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology. 2008;18(2):188–96. 10.1016/j.jelekin.2007.06.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Owen DH. Maintaining posture and avoiding tripping. Optical information for detecting and controlling orientation and locomotion. Clinics in geriatric medicine. 1985;1(3):581–99. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Thomas S, Reading J, Shephard RJ. Revision of the Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q). Canadian journal of sport sciences = Journal canadien des sciences du sport. 1992;17(4):338–45. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Zeni JA Jr, Richards JG, Higginson JS. Two simple methods for determining gait events during treadmill and overground walking using kinematic data. Gait & Posture. 2008;27(4):710–4. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Chen H-C, Ashton-Miller JA, Alexander NB, Schultz AB. Stepping over obstacles: gait patterns of healthy young and old adults. Journal of Gerontology. 1991;46(6):M196–M203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Lowrey CR, Watson A, Vallis LA. Age-related changes in avoidance strategies when negotiating single and multiple obstacles. Experimental brain research. 2007;182(3):289–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. McKenzie NC, Brown LA. Obstacle negotiation kinematics: age-dependent effects of postural threat. Gait & posture. 2004;19(3):226–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M, Kerns KA, Baldwin M. The effects of two types of cognitive tasks on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of falls. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 1997;52(4):M232–M40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Plummer P, Apple S, Dowd C, Keith E. Texting and walking: effect of environmental setting and task prioritization on dual-task interference in healthy young adults. Gait Posture. 2015;41(1):46–51. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.08.007 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Burns P, Parkes A, Burton S, Smith R, Burch D. How dangerous is driving with a mobile phone? Benchmarking the impairment to alcohol. TRL Report 2002.
- 22. Schabrun SM, van den Hoorn W, Moorcroft A, Greenland C, Hodges PW. Texting and Walking: Strategies for Postural Control and Implications for Safety. PLOS ONE. 2014;9(1):e84312 10.1371/journal.pone.0084312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Ebersbach G, Dimitrijevic MR, Poewe W. Influence of concurrent tasks on gait: a dual-task approach. Perceptual and motor skills. 1995;81(1):107–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Chapman G, Hollands MA. Evidence that older adult fallers prioritise the planning of future stepping actions over the accurate execution of ongoing steps during complex locomotor tasks. Gait & posture. 2007;26(1):59–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Di Fabio RP, Greany JF, Zampieri C. Saccade-stepping interactions revise the motor plan for obstacle avoidance. Journal of motor behavior. 2003;35(4):383–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. McFadyen BJ, Prince F. Avoidance and accommodation of surface height changes by healthy, community-dwelling, young, and elderly men. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2002;57(4):B166–B74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Becklen R, Cervone D. Selective looking and the noticing of unexpected events. Memory & cognition. 1983;11(6):601–8. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Mack A. Inattentional blindness: Looking without seeing. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2003;12:180–4. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Lamberg EM, Muratori LM. Cell phones change the way we walk. Gait & Posture. 2012;35(4):688–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files