Abstract
Background
In resourced-constrained settings, daily cooking practices are still the norm. Replacing sodium in regular salt to produce potassium-enriched salts are potential alternative routes to reduce sodium intake, paired with the benefit associated with potassium intake. This change would likely have effects on palatability and taste of prepared foods, yet a threshold to discriminate sensorial changes can be determined. The main goal of this study was to assess if the use of potassium-enriched salt substitutes lead to perceived differences in taste utilizing a sensory discrimination test.
Methods and Results
A triangle taste test was conducted and participants were offered samples of cooked rice prepared with different salts. The only ingredient that differed in the preparation was the salt used: 100%NaCl (regular salt) and salts where sodium was replaced by 50%, 33% or 25% KCl (potassium-enriched salt). Comparisons were carried out according to the minimum number of correct judgments. A total of 156 subjects, 49% males, mean age 41.0 years (SD±15.5) years, participated in the study. Samples using 25% potassium-enrichment were indistinguishable in terms of taste from regular salt, whereas samples with 33% and 50% potassium-enrichment were distinguishable. Results were consistent when stratified by sex and age. Less than 10% of participants attributed the differences to bitterness or metallic flavor.
Conclusions
The 25% potassium-enriched salt is indistinguishable from regular salt. These findings suggest a potential to achieve sodium intake reduction strategies in cooking practices by substituting regular salt with potassium-enriched salt without affecting palatability.
Introduction
Worldwide, hypertension is considered a major risk factor for cardiovascular disease, especially heart attack and stroke [1]. A recent analysis of regional and global trends of systolic blood pressure found that though there was a decreasing trend observed globally since 1980, the decline per decade was higher in high-income settings (Australasia, North America and Western Europe), it actually rose in other regions, and systolic blood pressure is currently highest in low-income and middle-income countries [2]. In these resource-constrained settings there are few strategies to maintain blood pressure within appropriate ranges, reflecting the challenge of affordable implementation in health care systems.
Increased sodium consumption is associated with increased blood pressure, whereas lower sodium intake does decrease blood pressure in adults [3–7]. Therefore, one of the strategies largely advocated worldwide to reduce the burden of high blood pressure emphasizes the implementation of salt reduction strategies [8–10]. Additionally, low potassium intake is associated with an increased risk of hypertension, and a high ratio of sodium/potassium intake has been proposed as a potent risk factor for hypertension and cardiovascular disease than each factor alone [11, 12]. Thus, the increase of dietary potassium might reduce mean systolic and diastolic blood pressure levels, and could also contribute to the prevention of hypertension, especially in populations with elevated blood pressure [13, 14].
The most recent WHO guidelines strongly recommend a reduction to less than 5 grams per day among adults with or without hypertension [15]. Yet, the successful implementation of this recommendation, especially in developing countries where day-to-day food preparation and cooking practices are substantial, might be difficult to achieve without taking into account the role of salt on palatability of foods associated with flavor and taste [16, 17].
Replacing the sodium in the regular salt to produce potassium-enriched salts are potential alternative routes to reduce sodium intake, paired with the increased benefit associated with higher potassium intake. However, increasing potassium in the salt used for food cooking could introduce noticeable changes in flavor and/or palatability such as saltiness, bitterness, or metallic taste [18], with potential impact on consumer liking [19]. The main goal of this study was to assess if the use of potassium-enriched salt substitutes lead to perceived differences in taste utilizing a triangle taste test, a type of sensory discrimination test. This technique is employed to determine if a sensory difference between two products can be detected by consumers; for instance, assuming participants cannot perceive a different taste, then liking will not be affected.
Materials and Methods
Study design
A cross-sectional study using a triangle taste test technique, utilized in sensory analysis, was conducted to evaluate whether using salts containing varying proportions of sodium chloride (NaCl) and potassium chloride (KCl) in rice preparation could be detected.
Setting and participants
This study was part of the initial exploratory phase of a larger pragmatic randomized community-based intervention to introduce a low-sodium potassium-enriched salt substitute to reduce blood pressure at the population level (ClinicalTrials.gov number: NCT01960972). The protocol of this umbrella study has been reported and is available elsewhere [20].
Participants from the six villages of Tumbes to be enrolled in the larger umbrella study were randomly selected from 2010 census, the most updated available. Males or females aged 18 years and over, capable of understanding study procedures, capable of providing informed consent, and who were full-time residents in the area were considered eligible.
Procedures
For the triangle taste test, regular salt (100% NaCl) and potassium-enriched alternative salts were used in the preparation of rice, Peru’s most common staple food. In Peru, we had access to regular salt and a 50% NaCl/50% KCl potassium-enriched salt, both containing small amounts of iodine as per local regulations following international standards [21]. By combining appropriate proportions of these two salts, we generated salts containing 33% and 25% KCl concentration. Thus, in addition to regular salt, three types of potassium-enriched salts were used in the preparations: (i) 50% NaCl + 50% KCl, (ii) 67% NaCl + 33% KCl, and (iii) 75% NaCl + 25% KCl.
A single trained person did all the preparations using standardized techniques. To ensure consistency in food preparation, a simple formula yielded for 10 portions was cooked with the following ingredients: rice (150 grams), salt (5 grams), and water (400 grams). All the ingredients were combined in a pot and then boiled for a period of 30 minutes. At the end of this period, heat was turned off and rice was let cool for 5 minutes. Finally, rice was served in plastic recipients. In this way, the only element that differed in the food preparation was the type of salt used and, in doing so; it was expected to obtain rice preparations that might differ only on taste (unobserved) but were otherwise similar across a range of other factors, such as appearance, size, color and smell. No additional seasonings were utilized in the process.
Experiment
We were interested if participants were able to identify the odd sample when exposed and after eating a set of three food samples cooked with different ratio of sodium/potassium salts. All samples were coded with a three-digit random number and participants were presented to three sets of food. Each set had two identical samples and one odd sample, and they were presented simultaneously in a predetermined counterbalanced order. The samples were randomly presented to avoid positional bias since the middle sample is usually chosen as odd. Possible combinations of samples were: AAB, ABA, BAA, BBA, BAB, and ABB. An example of the distribution of sets and samples used, including the variation of potassium-enriched concentration, is shown in Fig 1.
The ingredients of the food samples and the experiment procedures were explained to the participants and they were asked to identify the odd sample. If the participant was unable to determine which sample was different, they were asked to guess. Between tasting samples and sets, a glass of water was offered to the participants to avoid sensory fatigue [22].
After completing the experiment, participants were asked to complete a short questionnaire to identify the sample they believed to be the odd one as well as to indicate the degree of difference between samples (slight, moderate, or high). At the end of the experiment, demographic data, including sex, age, education level, marital status, and currently working status, was also collected.
Sample size
With a power over 90% at the 5% level of significance, the triangle taste test was planned to include146 participants. As the probability of correctly respond by chance is 33% in a triangle taste test, this sample size provided us with enough power to state that there is not a detectable difference between different samples [23]. In addition, at least 53 participants in each subgroup were needed to guarantee a power of at least 80% to state that there is not a detectable difference between different samples in the subgroup analysis [24].
Statistical analysis
After double data entry, the information was transferred to STATA 11 (STATA Corp, College Station TX, US) for analysis. Initially, description of demographic characteristics of participants was shown by using proportions.
To determine if the total number of correct responses for the total number of participants was statistically significant, we used the critical number of correct responses in a triangle taste test. Thus, we computed a chi-squared based on the following formula:
In this formula, “a” is the proportion of participants who answered correctly, “f” is the proportion of people who did not answer correctly, and “n” is the number of participants. Results were also performed stratified by sex, age and education level. Additional comparisons were performed using Chi-squared test or Fisher exact test as needed.
Ethical issues
Oral informed consent was obtained from all participants before enrollment. Oral consent was used due to low rates of literacy in the study setting, and the interviewer signed the form to document participant’s consent. This project and its procedures were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Boards (IRBs) at Universidad Peruana Cayetano Heredia (Peru) and Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health (US).
Results
Baseline characteristics
A total of 156 participants, 77 (49%) were males, mean age 41.0 (SD15.5) years, were enrolled in the study. Characteristics of the study population are detailed in the Table 1.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the study population.
Variables | N (%) |
---|---|
Sex | |
Female | 79 (50.6%) |
Male | 77 (49.4%) |
Age | |
< 40 years | 81 (51.9%) |
≥ 40 years | 75 (48.1%) |
Education level | |
< 12 years | 129 (82.7%) |
≥ 12 years | 27 (17.3%) |
Civil status | |
Never married | 42 (26.9%) |
Ever married | 114 (73.1%) |
Currently working | |
No | 86 (55.1%) |
Yes | 70 (44.9%) |
Correct identification of the odd sample
Samples using 25% potassium-enrichment were indistinguishable from regular salt. Participants were able to distinguish the odd sample in preparations that contained 50% and 33% of KCl (Table 2).
Table 2. Results of the triangle taste test: Detection of difference in rice flavor affected by partial sodium chloride replacement a .
Number of judgments for rice flavor with replacement of NaCl by | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex | Age | Years of education | |||||
Replacement level (%) b | Total | Males | Females | < 40 years | ≥ 40 years | < 12 years | ≥ 12 years |
(n = 77) | (n = 79) | (n = 81) | (n = 75) | (n = 129) | (n = 27) | ||
50% KCl | 82/156*** | 44/77*** | 38/79* | 43/81*** | 39/75** | 64/129*** | 18/27** |
33% KCl | 69/156* | 38/77* | 31/79 | 36/81* | 33/75 | 60/129** | 9/27 |
25% KCl | 63/156 | 32/77 | 31/79 | 31/81 | 32/75 | 54/129 | 9/27 |
a Number of judgments correct divided by total in triangle tastes.
b The proportion of sodium of the reference rice replaced by equivalent amounts of the potassium salt. Significant level:
* = significant at P = 0.05,
** = significant at p = 0.01,
*** = significant at p = 0.001
When the analyses were stratified by sex, age, and education level, the result did not differ markedly from the main findings: participants were not able to distinguish samples with 25% KCl; however, the odd sample was distinguishable when salt with 50% KCl (Table 3). Of note, females and those aged 40 years and above were not able to appropriately distinguish samples prepared with 33% KCl. Although the same trend was observable for those with ≥12 years of education, power to detect differences can be an issue in this case.
Table 3. Results of the triangle taste test: Reported differences according to participants’ responses.
50% NaCl + 50% KCl | 67% NaCl + 33% KCl | 75% NaCl + 25% KCl | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Incorrect | Correct | p-value* | Incorrect | Correct | p-value* | Incorrect | Correct | p-value* | |
Reported difference (%) | |||||||||
Slightly | 33.8% | 40.2% | 0.58 | 39.1% | 42.0% | 0.26 | 37.6% | 17.5% | 0.03 |
Moderate | 56.8% | 53.7% | 47.1% | 52.2% | 52.7% | 69.8% | |||
High | 9.4% | 6.1% | 13.8% | 5.8% | 9.7% | 12.7% | |||
Reported taste (%) | |||||||||
Bitter taste | 4.1% | 2.4% | 0.58 | 0.0% | 4.4% | 0.05 | 2.2% | 3.2% | 0.69 |
Metallic taste | 10.8% | 7.3% | 0.45 | 8.1% | 10.1% | 0.65 | 12.9% | 9.5% | 0.52 |
*P-values were calculated using Chi-squared tests.
Difference and reported taste of the odd sample
The degree of difference between samples that most participants identified was moderate (Table 3). In addition, when asked about the flavor of the reported odd sample tasted very few acknowledged them as bitter or metallic taste. There was no evidence of a difference in reported taste between those who did and did not identify correctly the odd sample.
Discussion
Main findings
Palatability is one of the main concerns, if not the main one, when considering substituting NaCl present in salt by KCl. The findings of this study indicate that changes in taste by using a 75%NaCl/25%KCl salt substitute were not discernible when compared to common salt. These results were consistent even when the analysis was performed stratified by sex and age implying that palatability was similar between products independently of sex and age. Moreover, these sub-group analyses show that especially women and those aged 40 years and above were not able to discern samples with 33% KCl. Henceforth, replacing sodium for potassium in daily cooking practices can be a potential avenue to introduce prevention initiatives aimed at improving cardiovascular health.
It is possible to replace NaCl with KCl in food preparations, because of both its saltiness and its comparable physical properties [25]. Both salts are colorless crystals and soluble in water, but unlike sodium, potassium has not been associated with progression towards hypertension and subsequent cardiovascular diseases [26]. The two salts could be used in a salt substitute either alone or in combination, though some may find KCl to have a bitter, chemical, or metallic taste [18]. The excess of potassium in diet is not a common problem: hyperkalemia by potassium intake would be seen with large infusions or oral doses of several grams of potassium chloride. However, in patients with severe and terminal chronic kidney disease, as well as those using some medication, accumulation of potassium can cause hyperkalemia. Thus, the safety profile of the salt substitute will be guaranteed, because the changes will be relatively minor compared to the levels when potassium does produce some negative health effects. Based on our study, although a small proportion of participants detected some bitterness or metallic taste, using an appropriate combination of KCl and NaCl appears to minimize these unpleasant potassium-related tastes.
Comparisons with other studies
One of the main barriers in reducing salt intake is changes in palatability. However some studies have shown that it is possible to reduce gradually the amount of NaCl in the main sources of salt without affecting taste, such as the case of low-salt bread [27, 28]; but there is an additional benefit obtained when potassium is added, as previously detailed.
Our results agree with previous studies in that the greater the concentration of KCl in the substitute, the greater chance that it will be distinguishable in sensory discrimination tests. For example, several years ago, Salovaara showed it was possible to reduce the salt content of bread in about one third with no significantly detectable disadvantage in bread taste [29]. In addition, this report found that potassium chloride may replace 20% of the sodium equivalent normally added to bread without causing difference in flavor. Recently, Li et al compared a salt substitute with 65% NaCl, 25% KCl, and 10% magnesium sulfate to common salt. Changes in saltiness, flavor and acceptability of both home-cooked foods and a standard salt-seasoned soup were not different between study groups [30]. Another study replaced a fraction of the NaCl in white bread with KCl, and found that up to 30% of the NaCl could be replaced by KCl and have similar acceptability scores to the standard white bread [31].
Some data suggests that salt preference can be learned [30]. In this regard, it is possible that among participants with more frequent exposure to KCl, their concentration threshold of detection of KCl gradually decreased over time. This, in turn, provides an additional major opportunity to transit, gradually, towards higher NaCl substitution practices that contribute to accrue longer-term health-related benefits. Nevertheless, information regarding the effectiveness of changes in salt preference due to gradually reduction of salt is scarce [32].
Public health impact
The potential expected benefit of the use of potassium-enriched salt at the population level lies on its demonstrated effects to reduce blood pressure through two different mechanisms: moderately reducing sodium intake and increasing potassium in the diet. One US-based study enrolled people with mildly elevated blood pressure and replaced their common salt with Smart Salt (50% NaCl, 25% KCl, and 25% magnesium ammonium KCl hydrate) over a period of eight weeks, which led to a significant reduction in systolic blood pressure [33]. A potassium-enriched salt substitute might have also a benefit effect on normotensive subjects: for example, a randomized controlled trial in a rural Chinese population showed an overall difference in systolic and diastolic blood pressure of 2 mmHg between two groups consuming either the salt substitute or normal salt after 24 months of follow-up [34]. Finally, in a study of five kitchens of a veteran retirement home in which groups received either potassium-enriched salt (experimental group) or regular salt (control group), the experimental group had better cardiovascular disease survivorship than the control group [35]. Moreover, according to our findings, it is possible to implement salt substitutes containing 33% KCl in some sub-groups of the population. Reasons for reported differences need to be further studied.
Based on this data, potassium-enriched salt substitutes may be a feasible approach to reducing sodium intake at the population level. The survey presented here was performed as part of the exploratory phase of a research study with the goal of implementing a potassium-enrichedsalt substitute to reduce blood pressure at the population level. Because randomly-selected people in this Peruvian population were not able to differentiate between a salt substitute with 25% and common salt, there is a good possibility that this implementation can be successful using the 25% potassium-enriched salt as a starting point for its implementation.
Strength and limitations
The main strengths of this study included the use of a random sample of participants to evaluate different concentrations of KCl in the substitutes, including the random presentation of samples. However, this study has also some limitations. First, we only used rice in our experiments as we needed to standardize techniques including cooking procedures. Further studies are suggested to assess the potential impact of salt substitutes in food preparations. Second, some participants could have achieved sensory fatigue because of repeated assessments despite of using a glass of water to reduce this trouble. Finally, we could not find differences in comparisons according to demographic variables because of low number of participants in subgroups, especially in the case of education level (≥12 years of education). However, there is a clear trend in correct responses when the concentration of potassium in the sample is decreasing.
Conclusions
Samples using 25% potassium-enriched salt was indistinguishable from regular salt. These findings suggest a potential to achieve sodium intake reduction strategies in cooking practices by substituting regular salt with potassium-enriched salt without affecting palatability.
Acknowledgments
This study is supported by a grant from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health, USA (Project 1 U01 HL114180-01) under the Global Alliance for Chronic Diseases programme. All authors and the CRONICAS Center of Excellence in Chronic Diseases are supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Global Health Initiative under the contract Global Health Activities in Developing Countries to Combat Non-Communicable Chronic Diseases (Project Number 268200900033C-1-0-1). The funders had no role on study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This study is supported by a grant from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health, United States of America (Project 1 U01 HL114180-01) under the Global Alliance for Chronic Diseases programme. All authors and the CRONICAS Center of Excellence in Chronic Diseases are supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Global Health Initiative under the contract Global Health Activities in Developing Countries to Combat Non-Communicable Chronic Diseases (Project Number 268200900033C-1-0-1). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Anand SS, Yusuf S. Stemming the global tsunami of cardiovascular disease. Lancet 2011;377(9765):529–32. 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)62346-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Danaei G, Finucane MM, Lin JK, Singh GM, Paciorek CJ, Cowan MJ, et al. National, regional, and global trends in systolic blood pressure since 1980: systematic analysis of health examination surveys and epidemiological studies with 786 country-years and 5.4 million participants. Lancet 2011;377(9765):568–77. 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)62036-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. He FJ, Li J, Macgregor GA. Effect of longer term modest salt reduction on blood pressure: Cochrane systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised trials. BMJ 2013;346:f1325 10.1136/bmj.f1325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Graudal NA, Hubeck-Graudal T, Jurgens G. Effects of low sodium diet versus high sodium diet on blood pressure, renin, aldosterone, catecholamines, cholesterol, and triglyceride. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011(11):CD004022 10.1002/14651858.CD004022.pub3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Dickinson HO, Mason JM, Nicolson DJ, Campbell F, Beyer FR, Cook JV, et al. Lifestyle interventions to reduce raised blood pressure: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. J Hypertens 2006;24(2):215–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Hooper L, Bartlett C, Davey SG, Ebrahim S. Advice to reduce dietary salt for prevention of cardiovascular disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2004(1):CD003656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Cutler JA, Follmann D, Allender PS. Randomized trials of sodium reduction: an overview. Am J Clin Nutr 1997;65(2 Suppl):643S–51S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. World Health Organization. Global status report on noncommunicable diseases 2010. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Beaglehole R, Bonita R, Horton R, Adams C, Alleyne G, Asaria P, et al. Priority actions for the non-communicable disease crisis. Lancet 2011;377(9775):1438–47. 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60393-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. World Health Organization. Global Action Plan for the Prevention and Control of Noncommunicable Diseases. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Yang Q, Liu T, Kuklina EV, Flanders WD, Hong Y, Gillespie C, et al. Sodium and potassium intake and mortality among US adults: prospective data from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Arch Intern Med 2011;171(13):1183–91. 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Umesawa M, Iso H, Date C, Yamamoto A, Toyoshima H, Watanabe Y, et al. Relations between dietary sodium and potassium intakes and mortality from cardiovascular disease: the Japan Collaborative Cohort Study for Evaluation of Cancer Risks. Am J Clin Nutr 2008;88(1):195–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Dickinson HO, Nicolson DJ, Campbell F, Beyer FR, Mason J. Potassium supplementation for the management of primary hypertension in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2006;3:CD004641 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Whelton PK, He J, Cutler JA, Brancati FL, Appel LJ, Follmann D, et al. Effects of oral potassium on blood pressure. Meta-analysis of randomized controlled clinical trials. JAMA 1997;277(20):1624–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. World Health Organization. Guideline: Sodium intake for adults and children. Geneva: World Health Organization (WHO); 2012. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Liem DG, Miremadi F, Keast RS. Reducing sodium in foods: the effect on flavor. Nutrients 2011;3(6):694–711. 10.3390/nu3060694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. DeSimone JA, Beauchamp GK, Drewnowski A, Johnson GH. Sodium in the food supply: challenges and opportunities. Nutr Rev 2013;71(1):52–9. 10.1111/nure.12006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Sinopoli DA, Lawless HT. Taste properties of potassium chloride alone and in mixtures with sodium chloride using a check-all-that-apply method. J Food Sci 2012;77(9):S319–22. 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2012.02862.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Grummer J, Bobowski N, Karalus M, Vickers Z, Schoenfuss T. Use of potassium chloride and flavor enhancers in low sodium Cheddar cheese. J Dairy Sci 2013;96(3):1401–18. 10.3168/jds.2012-6057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Bernabe-Ortiz A, Diez-Canseco F, Gilman RH, Cardenas MK, Sacksteder KA, Miranda JJ. Launching a salt substitute to reduce blood pressure at the population level: a cluster randomized stepped wedge trial in Peru. Trials 2014;15:93 10.1186/1745-6215-15-93 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Pretell EA, Delange F, Hostalek U, Corigliano S, Barreda L, Higa AM, et al. Iodine nutrition improves in Latin America. Thyroid 2004;14(8):590–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Hetherington MM. Sensory-specific satiety and its importance in meal termination. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1996;20(1):113–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Bi J. Sensory discrimination tests and measurements: statistical principles, procedures and tables. Iowa: Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 24. Meilgaard M, Civille GV, Carr BT. Sensory Evaluation Techniques, Fourth Edition NY: CRC Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 25. Armenteros M, Aristoy MC, Barat JM, Toldra F. Biochemical and sensory changes in dry-cured ham salted with partial replacements of NaCl by other chloride salts. Meat Sci 2012;90(2):361–7. 10.1016/j.meatsci.2011.07.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Geleijnse JM, Witteman JC, Stijnen T, Kloos MW, Hofman A, Grobbee DE. Sodium and potassium intake and risk of cardiovascular events and all-cause mortality: the Rotterdam Study. Eur J Epidemiol 2007;22(11):763–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Girgis S, Neal B, Prescott J, Prendergast J, Dumbrell S, Turner C, et al. A one-quarter reduction in the salt content of bread can be made without detection. Eur J Clin Nutr 2003;57(4):616–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Saavedra-Garcia L, Sosa-Zevallos V, Diez-Canseco F, Miranda JJ, Bernabe-Ortiz A. Reducing salt in bread: a quasi-experimental feasibility study in a bakery in Lima, Peru. Public Health Nutr 2015:1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Salovaara H. Sensory limitations to replacement of sodium with potassum and magnessium in bread. Cereal Chem 1982;59(5):427–30. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Li N, Prescott J, Wu Y, Barzi F, Yu X, Zhao L, et al. The effects of a reduced-sodium, high-potassium salt substitute on food taste and acceptability in rural northern China. Br J Nutr 2009;101(7):1088–93. 10.1017/S0007114508042360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Braschi A, Gill L, Naismith DJ. Partial substitution of sodium with potassium in white bread: feasibility and bioavailability. Int J Food Sci Nutr 2009;60(6):507–21. 10.1080/09637480701782118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bobowski N. Shifting Human Salty Taste Preference: Potential Opportunities and Challenges in Reducing Dietary Salt Intake of Americans. Chem Percept 2015; 10.1007/s12078-015-9179-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Sarkkinen ES, Kastarinen MJ, Niskanen TH, Karjalainen PH, Venalainen TM, Udani JK, et al. Feasibility and antihypertensive effect of replacing regular salt with mineral salt-rich in magnesium and potassium- in subjects with mildly elevated blood pressure. Nutr J 2011;10:88 10.1186/1475-2891-10-88 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Zhou B, Wang HL, Wang WL, Wu XM, Fu LY, Shi JP. Long-term effects of salt substitution on blood pressure in a rural north Chinese population. J Hum Hypertens 2013;27(7):427–33. 10.1038/jhh.2012.63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Chang HY, Hu YW, Yue CS, Wen YW, Yeh WT, Hsu LS, et al. Effect of potassium-enriched salt on cardiovascular mortality and medical expenses of elderly men. Am J Clin Nutr 2006;83(6):1289–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.