Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jul 30.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2010 Winter;22(1):133–150. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990319

Predictors and Sequelae of Trajectories of Physical Aggression in School-Age Boys and Girls

Susan B Campbell 1, Susan Spieker 2, Nathan Vandergrift 3, Jay Belsky 4, Margaret Burchinal 3; the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network
PMCID: PMC4520547  NIHMSID: NIHMS525376  PMID: 20102652

Abstract

Teacher-rated trajectories of physical aggression in boys and girls from 1st through 6th grade were examined using data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. In separate analyses, four trajectories were identified in boys and three in girls. Higher levels of aggression in both boys and girls were related to greater sociodemographic risk and higher maternal harshness in the preschool years; lower levels of observed maternal sensitivity during early childhood also predicted higher trajectories of aggression among girls. Trajectory groups also differed on a range of social and academic adjustment outcomes in 6th grade, with the most aggressive children and even moderately aggressive children evidencing some difficulties in adjustment. Patterns and levels of aggression in boys and girls are discussed as are their predictors and consequences.

Keywords: trajectories of physical aggression, school-age boys and girls, preschool predictors, social adjustment, academic functioning


A growing body of evidence indicates that high and stable levels of physical aggression and related externalizing behavior problems in childhood are associated with a range of negative outcomes in adolescence (e.g., Broidy et al., 2003; Fergusson, Lynskey, & Horwood, 1996; Moffitt, Caspi, Dickson, Silva, & Stanton, 1996), including poor emotion regulation, school failure, peer problems, and adolescent delinquency (Fergusson et al., 1996; Loeber, Farrington, Stouthamer-Loeber, Moffitt, & Caspi, 1998; Moffitt et al., 1996). Interpretation of this work, however, is complicated by different definitions of aggression and related problems ranging from potentially normative aggression in young children’s peer groups (e.g., Hay, Castle, & Davies, 2000), to combinations of aggressive and noncompliant behavior (e.g., Côté, Tremblay, Nagin, Zoccolillo, & Vitaro, 2002; Schaeffer et al., 2006), to more serious aggression that occurs in the context of high levels of disruptive and antisocial behavior (e.g., Moffitt et al., 1996; Odgers et al., 2008). Furthermore, not all children who show early aggressive and disruptive behavior continue to have difficulties, raising important questions about the best way to identify the subgroup of children who are truly at risk, both to better delineate the pathways and processes leading to serious long-term difficulties and to inform prevention efforts.

Earlier studies of aggression and disruptive behavior relied on cut-off scores to identify longitudinal patterns (e.g., Aguilar, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 2000; Moffitt et al., 1996), whereas more recent research has used trajectory analysis or other statistical techniques that identify subgroups of children following relatively distinct developmental pathways (e.g., Broidy et al., 2003; Odgers et al., 2008; Schaeffer et al., 2006). Although most studies delineating developmental trajectories have specifically modeled ratings of physical aggression (Broidy et al., 2003), others have used more inclusive definitions that also encompass other disruptive or predelinquent behaviors (Odgers et al., 2008; Schaeffer et al., 2006; Shaw, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, & Nagin, 2003). In the current report, we focus specifically on teacher ratings of physical aggression, but our work is informed by studies that include broader definitions of disruptive behavior. Interest has also moved beyond the description of developmental trajectories per se to concerns about both the antecedents and sequelae of different longitudinal patterns of physical aggression. The risk of negative outcomes is clearly greatest for children showing high levels of stable aggression from childhood to adolescence, which in turn is most evident in the context of family adversity, but research nevertheless also indicates that such outcomes are not foreordained (Moffitt, 2003; Odgers et al., 2008; Rutter, 2003). Much of the research on predictors and consequences of physical aggression trajectories has focused on boys, although consideration of physical aggression in girls is also warranted, given questions about the age of onset and developmental course of aggression and conduct problems in girls (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003; Hay, 2007; Moffitt, Caspi, Rutter, & Silva, 2001; Silverthorn & Frick, 1999).

In this paper we use data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development to describe trajectories of physical aggression in school as rated by teachers, looking separately at patterns of aggression in boys and girls from 1st to 6th grade. We chose to model trajectories separately by child sex to address concerns about the lack of attention to differential patterns and processes in the emergence of problem behavior in girls (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003) and the possibility that physical aggression would be relatively rare and less predictive in girls (Silverthorn & Frick, 1999). We appreciate, however, the arguments of Moffitt et al. (2001) that developmental processes are more similar than different with respect to antisocial behavior in males and females, even if levels remain different. After modeling trajectories, we then utilize earlier child and maternal parenting measures, obtained when children were preschool age and selected because of their theoretical importance, to predict trajectory group membership; these include children’s language development and ability to delay gratification, and observed maternal sensitivity and self-reported harsh control. Consideration of these child and parenting measures improves upon prior research on precursors of aggression trajectories which primarily has emphasized sociodemographic indicators of risk (e.g., Côté, Vaillancourt, Barker, Nagin, & Tremblay, 2007; Nagin & Tremblay, 2001) or has relied on teacher or parent report measures of child characteristics that are concurrent with the initial assessment of aggression and often obtained from the same reporter (Côté et al., 2002; Schaeffer et al., 2006). Next, we examine a broad range of adjustment outcomes in early adolescence as a function of aggression trajectories including mother, teacher, and self-report measures of social adjustment, school functioning, and relationship quality. This is in contrast to most research on the sequelae of physical aggression trajectories which has tended to focus on more serious antisocial and delinquent outcomes, rather than on more subtle adjustment problems that may still confer risk in early adolescence and that may also be associated with low to moderate levels of aggression. In addition, given the relatively low prevalence of conduct problems in girls in middle childhood and early adolescence (e.g., Messer, Goodman, Rowe, Meltzer, & Maughan, 2006), examination of a wider range of outcomes may provide more insight into the sequelae of physical aggression in girls.

Trajectories of Aggression

Research on early-emerging physical aggression indicates that persistent aggression, rather than more age-related (Hartup, 1974; Tremblay, 2000) or time-limited aggression (Moffitt, 1993; 2003), predicts more problematic outcomes (Broidy et al., 2003; Moffitt et al., 1996; Patterson, Forgatch, Yoerger, & Stoolmiller, 1998). A particularly promising research strategy involves the identification of subgroups of children following different pathways or trajectories of physical aggression who theoretically should also vary in long-term risk. As already noted, much of the early research on physical aggression trajectories focused on boys, given their elevated levels of physical aggression relative to girls (in contrast to relational or social aggression which is equally elevated or higher in girls) (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Murray-Close, Ostrov, & Crick, 2007). Boys’ physical aggression trajectories are generally described by three or four patterns, when modeled using teacher ratings obtained from school entry to early adolescence. Broidy et al. (2003) ran parallel analyses on six cohorts of boys and reported that across samples, two each from the US, Canada, and New Zealand, most boys evinced either low or moderate levels of aggression. Small groups of boys showing either high increasing or high stable aggression also were identified consistently. A fourth moderate decreasing trajectory was also identified in the two Canadian samples. In these analyses, low to moderate levels of aggression appeared to be normative, whereas only 4 to 11% of boys were on a high aggression trajectory.

Despite the emphasis on trajectories of aggression in boys, Broidy et al. (2003) also presented data on longitudinal patterns of physical aggression in girls. Teacher-rated physical aggression was generally much lower in girls, but stable aggression groups also emerged. The vast majority of girls showed either no physical aggression or very low aggression, but even when aggression was stable and elevated relative to other girls, it was still much lower than the level of aggression reported for boys. Across four samples of girls, from two to four trajectories of physical aggression were identified, with over 80% in no aggression or very low aggression groups. In three samples, at most 10% of girls were in a higher aggression group and in one sample, no girls were actually elevated. More recently, Schaeffer et al. (2006) used growth mixture modeling instead of trajectory analysis to identify developmental patterns of aggressive and disruptive behaviors in a sample of inner city, mostly African-American children studied from 1st to 5th grade. Two fairly similar patterns of aggressive/disruptive behavior emerged for boys and girls (chronic high and low), but boys also showed an increasing trajectory in contrast to a moderate stable pattern for girls; on average, girls were also lower in aggression and disruptive behavior than boys.

These studies are important for several reasons. From a developmental psychopathology perspective, it is important to identify pathways and risk markers for later adjustment difficulties as well as for diagnosable disorders. The availability of large longitudinal data sets allows for the examination of different developmental pathways that facilitates the identification of subgroups of children who are truly at risk in contrast to those showing only age-related behavior problems (e.g., NICHD Early Child Care Research Network [ECCRN], 2004). Furthermore, there are continuing debates about the nature of aggression in girls and boys (e.g., Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003; Hay, 2007), calls for studies that model developmental trajectories separately by gender (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003), and concerns about whether the thresholds for problematic aggression and disruptive behavior are comparable across gender (e.g., Côté et al., 2002). For example, it has been argued that early onset and stable physical aggression is more difficult to identify in girls than in boys, and that more subtle indicators may be precursors of later behavior problems in girls (e.g., Côté, Zoccolillo, Tremblay, Nagin, & Vitaro, 2001). However, the findings of Broidy et al. (2003) suggest that stable physical aggression is both less common in girls and is less clearly a predictor of serious antisocial behavior in adolescence, in contrast to the findings for boys.

The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (NICHD ECCRN, 2004) examined mother reports of physical aggression from ages 2 to 9 in a large sample of children not considered to be at risk. Data for boys and girls were considered together. Five aggression trajectories were identified: very low, low stable, moderate stable, moderate decreasing, and chronic. In general, a linear relationship was found between the level of aggression and the proportion of boys in the group, with the chronic aggression trajectory group comprised almost entirely of boys (72%), thereby possibly obscuring unique patterns of physical aggression in girls. Indeed, when boys’ and girls’ aggression trajectories were modeled together in the same analysis, only 8 out of 585 girls (1.4%) were in the high and stable aggression group, precluding a clear evaluation of the meaning or sequelae of aggression in girls. In a recent paper, Joussemet et al. (2008) also reported that very few girls were represented in higher aggression trajectory groups and that when both boys’ and girls’ trajectories were modeled together, child sex was one of the main predictors of group assignment. Thus, it is prudent to examine trajectories separately, as some trajectories of physical aggression, although lower than in boys, may be non-normative in girls, thereby placing them at risk for adjustment difficulties both at home and in the peer group (Côté et al., 2007; Murray-Close et al., 2007).

In the current study, we seek to follow up on this work by identifying trajectories of physical aggression during the elementary school years, separately for boys and girls, as rated by teachers. Our large sample includes children recruited from 10 locations across the US and from urban, suburban, and rural areas, thereby providing a broader sample than utilized in prior studies of aggression trajectories. We utilize teacher ratings of aggression, rather than mother reports, partly because aggression that is obvious in school may be more indicative of adjustment problems than aggression identified by parents. Whereas strong peer group and social norms are likely to regulate (in either direction) the expression of aggression in the school setting, aggression toward siblings may be more normative.

Precursors of Aggression Trajectories

As is clear from the literature, high-stable aggression in boys is associated with sociodemographic and family risk (Nagin & Tremblay, 2001). This is not surprising in view of the wealth of data linking early-emerging conduct problems and delinquency to lower parental education and income and single-parent status (Campbell, Shaw, & Gilliom, 2000; Fergusson et al., 1996; Moffitt et al., 1996; Patterson et al., 1998). Although more proximal child characteristics and parenting processes may have dynamic explanatory value, illuminating how problem behavior develops, they have been examined only rarely in studies of trajectories of physical aggression. Shaw et al. (2003) reported that child fearlessness in toddlerhood distinguished higher from lower trajectory groups of conduct problem behaviors assessed by maternal report from ages 2 to 9 in a high risk sample of inner city boys. In the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD), membership in the five trajectory groups was predicted by sociodemographic risk, being a boy, and maternal stimulation and involvement observed at home (NICHD ECCRN, 2004). Taken together, these findings suggest that child characteristics, maternal parenting behavior, and sociodemographic risk predict trajectories of disruptive and aggressive behavior, as assessed primarily in boys. Clearly, then, there is reason, both theoretical (e.g., social learning theory, attachment theory) and empirical for including more process-oriented predictors in studies of the origins of trajectories of physical aggression in both boys and girls of elementary school age.

Toward this end, we examine two child characteristics and two aspects of maternal parenting that should predict differential levels of physical aggression. Moffitt (1993) and others (e.g., Lahey, Waldman, & McBurnett, 1999; Tremblay, 2000) implicate cognitive delays, especially in emergent language skills, as a possible risk factor for persistent aggressive behavior, particularly in boys. Early language development is examined in this inquiry as a potential predictor of aggression over time (Dionne, Tremblay, Boivin, Laplante, & Pérusse, 2003; Tremblay, 2000). Tremblay (2000) hypothesized that children’s physical aggression should decline as more sophisticated language and social cognitive skills emerge, thereby providing preschool children with alternative strategies such as negotiation and sharing to resolve disputes over toys. Empirical evidence confirms a link between poor language development and aggressive and disruptive behavior in young children (Cantwell & Baker, 1987; Dionne et al., 2003; Stevenson, Richman, & Graham, 1985). Thus, we expect that children showing poorer language development in the preschool period will be on a higher aggression trajectory.

During the preschool period, children also become more skilled at regulating their own behavior (Campbell, 2002; Kopp, 1989) and individual differences in the ability to delay gratification, an indicator of self-regulation, should also be linked to the ability to control aggression. Children with better self-regulatory strategies that are reflected in the ability to wait for a desired goal and that help them to regulate their impulses should be less likely to behave aggressively in school. Thus, we expect that more difficulty delaying gratification will predict a higher trajectory of physical aggression over the elementary school years.

Links between delayed language and poor regulation and trajectories of physical aggression also may be stronger in boys, given their higher levels of language difficulties (e.g., Baker & Cantwell, 1983) and problems with impulse control relative to girls during the preschool period (e.g., Bongers, Koot, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 2003; Maccoby, 1998). In support of this possibility, Messer et al. (2006) recently reported that boys who later developed conduct problems were exposed to more “neuro-developmental” risks in early childhood and Côté et al. (2002) found that the combination of hyperactivity and fearlessness in kindergarten, which may be construed as indicators of poor impulse control, predicted adolescent conduct disorder in boys, but not girls. Hay (2007) has also suggested, in a recent review, that the combination of language delays and regulatory difficulties may partly account for the observed sex differences in aggression that emerge by preschool age. However, it is also possible that both boys and girls with less proficient language and poorer ability to regulate impulses will evince more physical aggression in elementary school. That is, a similar process may be operative across sexes even though the levels of problem behavior may appear to be different (Moffitt et al., 2001).

Warm, supportive, and proactive parenting as well as limited use of physical punishment and negative, coercive child-rearing strategies should likewise be associated with less physical aggression in children (Campbell et al., 2000; Coie & Dodge, 1998; Kochanska, 2002; Patterson et al., 1998; Snyder, Reid, & Patterson, 2003). When parents are warm and supportive, they provide a basis for confidence in self and trust in others (Bowlby, 1969), model positive interaction strategies likely to carry over to interactions with peers (Snyder et al., 2003), and foster a willingness to comply with adult expectations (Kochanska, 2002). In contrast, negative, coercive exchanges are likely to foster anger, resentment, and insecurity in children as well as model power assertive and coercive ways of interacting with agemates (Miller & Sperry, 1987; Snyder et al., 2003). Thus, in addition to examining sociodemographic risk and child characteristics, in the current report we include a maternal report measure of harsh parenting and an observed measure of maternal sensitive responsiveness.

It is expected that higher levels of harsh control and lower levels of observed maternal sensitivity in early childhood will predict higher levels of physical aggression as observed by teachers in elementary school. There also is suggestive evidence that boys may be more vulnerable than girls to less positive parenting and associated risk factors (Cole, Teti, & Zahn-Waxler, 2003; McFadyen-Ketchum, Bates, Dodge, & Pettit, 1996). For example, McFadyen-Ketchum et al. (1996) reported that coercive responses to child misbehavior in the home and low warmth predicted increases in externalizing problems in boys, but decreases in girls, followed from kindergarten to 3rd grade. These gender differentiated findings suggested to them that boys were more likely to escalate negative behavior in the face of negative maternal control, whereas girls were more likely to comply. Cole et al. (2003) suggest that the dynamics of the mother-child relationship and mutual regulation of both positive and angry exchanges, along with girls’ better self-regulation skills may explain why they found more stable behavior problems in preschool boys in the context of angry interactions. These findings, paired with the general assumption that boys are more vulnerable to a range of early environmental stressors (Hay, 2007) may support an argument in favor of boys being more vulnerable to harsh control and low sensitivity. Nevertheless, only limited theory or data are available to support expectations that girls’ and boys’ trajectories of aggression will be differentially predicted by parenting measures. In summary, we propose that indicators of early language development, self-regulation, and maternal parenting assessed during the infancy and preschool years will differentially predict teacher-rated trajectories of physical aggression and that the links between these early measures and children’s aggression may be especially strong for boys.

Sequelae of Aggression Trajectories

Based on the theoretical perspectives of Moffitt (1993) and Patterson et al. (1998), early and persistent aggression, especially in the context of family adversity, should forecast a variety of negative outcomes, including not only more antisocial behavior, but also more family and peer relationship problems and academic difficulties. Research linking trajectory group membership with later outcomes has emphasized delinquency and antisocial behavior almost exclusively. In general, boys rated by teachers as more physically aggressive in elementary school were more likely to acknowledge engaging in delinquent behavior in adolescence, but girls, even relatively aggressive girls, did not report engaging in delinquent activities in adolescence (Broidy et al., 2003).

Importantly, however, more subtle adjustment problems were not examined in the multi-sample analyses reported by Broidy et al. (2003), although moderate aggression in both boys and girls may well have some negative sequelae. In earlier work using the NICHD SECCYD data set, we argued that aggression in early childhood may predict not only later aggression and antisocial behavior, but a range of social and academic outcomes, partly as a function of the level and stability of early aggression (Campbell et al., 2006; NICHD ECCRN, 2004), a point also made recently by Fergusson, Horwood, and Ridder (2005). In fact, we found that outcomes varied by trajectory group, with those on the moderate decreasing trajectory (i.e., those showing only transient and age-related aggression) showing good adjustment at age 9 (NICHD ECCRN, 2004) and across the transition to adolescence (Campbell et al., 2006). In contrast, children in the moderate stable group appeared to be showing signs of inattention and poor behavioral regulation, whereas those on the chronic aggression trajectory appeared to be showing early signs of conduct problems and worsening peer relations (Campbell et al., 2006; NICHD ECCRN, 2004). In addition, children in the low, stable aggression trajectory group (in contrast to the very low group) also showed signs of emerging peer problems in elementary school, and this was especially so for girls who reported higher levels of loneliness in third and fifth grade (Campbell et al., 2006), perhaps because levels of physical aggression, even quite low levels, are non-normative in school-age girls (Maccoby, 1998; Moffitt, 2003).

Based on the foregoing literature review, we expect to find that boys and girls on both high and moderate trajectories of aggression in school will show poorer adjustment outcomes on measures of internalizing and externalizing problems, academic competence, and social relationships, as reported by mothers, teachers, and the children themselves, relative to those who are on a very low aggression trajectory across middle childhood. We do not make explicit predictions about different patterns of outcome by child sex, although it is likely that girls on both higher and more moderate aggression trajectories will show a mix of internalizing, externalizing, and social problems at the transition to adolescence, whereas boys will show predominantly externalizing problems (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003).

Method

Participants

Families were recruited during hospital visits to mothers shortly after the birth of a child in 1991 at ten locations in the U.S. (Little Rock, AR; Irvine, CA; Lawrence, KS; Boston, MA; Philadelphia, PA; Pittsburgh, PA; Charlottesville, VA; Morganton, NC; Seattle, WA; Madison, WI). Recruitment and selection procedures are described in detail in several publications (see NICHD ECCRN, 1997; 2002) and at our web sites (http://secc.rti.org or www.nichd.nih.gov/crmc/secc). During selected 24-hour intervals, all women giving birth in study hospitals were screened for eligibility and willingness to be contacted again. Of the 8,986 mothers who gave birth during the sampling period, 5,416 (60%) agreed to be telephoned in two weeks and met the eligibility requirements (healthy mother over 18 who spoke English, singleton birth, live within an hour of research site, neighborhood not deemed too dangerous by police to visit). Of that group, a conditionally random sample of 3,015 was selected (56%) for a two-week phone call. The conditioning assured adequate representation (at least 10%) of single mothers, mothers without a high school degree, and ethnic minority mothers. At the 2-week call, families were excluded if the baby had been hospitalized for more than 7 days, they expected to move in the next three years, or they could not be reached in at least three attempts. A total of 1,525 families were selected for the call as eligible and agreed to an interview. Of these, 1,364 completed a home interview when the infant was 1 month old and became study participants.

The resulting sample was diverse: 24% were minority, 11% of the mothers had not completed high school, and 14% were single. Mothers had an average of 14.4 years of education. Average family income was 3.6 times the poverty threshold. The participating families were similar to the eligible hospital sample in terms of maternal education, percentage in different ethnic groups, and presence of a husband/partner in the household.

The trajectory analyses conducted for the current report are based on 1081 children (533 girls and 548 boys, 79% of initial sample) who had at least two out of a possible six teacher reports of aggressive behavior between first grade and sixth grade (see below). These children were compared with those not included in this analysis (n=283) on demographic indicators at one month and on one observational variable assessed at 6 months. Included and excluded children did not differ on child sex. However, included children and families were more likely to be white (82% included vs. 75% not included) and to have a higher income to needs ratio (M’s=2.91 vs. 2.10, F(1, 1272) = 20.96, p<.001). Mothers included in these analyses had a higher level of education (M’s=14.43 yrs vs. 13.5 yrs, F(1, 1361) = 31.32, p<.001) and were more likely to be living with a partner at one month (87% vs. 79.5%). Mothers included in these analyses were also higher in sensitivity when observed with their infants at 6 months (M=9.28) than were mothers who were not included (M=8.87, F(1, 1270) = 9.80, p<.01). Because the NICHD SECCYD sample excluded children of adolescent mothers, mothers who did not speak English, and children who were hospitalized at birth or who had a diagnosed disability, children experiencing the full range of potentially high risk conditions are not represented in the data set.

Overview of Data Collection

Children were followed from birth to sixth grade. Mothers were interviewed in person during home visits when infants were 1 month old. Detailed measures of the family and parenting behavior were obtained via interviews and observations when children were 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months old during home and laboratory visits. Children’s language development was assessed at 36 and 54 months and children’s academic achievement was assessed at fifth grade. Teachers also completed questionnaires on study children yearly from first through sixth grade. Mothers and children also provided data during home and laboratory visits across the elementary school years. The following sections describe the specific variables used in this paper and how they functioned in the analyses. Additional details about all data collection procedures, psychometric properties of the instruments, and descriptions of how composites were derived and constructed are documented in Manuals of Operation of the study (http://secc.rti.org).

Measures

Teacher Reports of Aggressive Behavior

At each assessment from first grade through sixth grade, teachers completed the Teacher Report Form of Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (TRF, Achenbach, 1991a). The TRF includes 113 items that describe typical, but potentially problematic child behaviors. Teachers rated how descriptive each item was of the child’s usual behavior in school over the last two months as “not true” (0), “somewhat or sometimes true” (1), or “very true or often true” (2).

For the current trajectory analyses, six items reflecting overt physical aggression to people and objects were selected: destroys own things, destroys others’ things, gets in many fights, threatens others, physically attacks people, and cruelty, bullying, or meanness to others. Raw scores for these items were summed at each age and could range from 0 to 12. This scale showed adequate internal consistency across age with coefficient alphas ranging from .81 to .86. Scores were moderately correlated over time (range = .24 – .55 for boys, mean = .38; range = .26–.62 for girls, mean = .38). Mean scores for boys on this scale ranged from a high of .76 (SD = 1.67) at 2nd grade to a low of .64 (SD= 1.54) at 6th grade; the average mean score for boys on the physical aggression composite was .68. Mean scores for girls were consistently lower ranging from a high of .34 at 3rd grade (SD = 1.21) to a low of .19 (SD = .74) at 6th grade; the average aggression score across grades for girls was .295.

Predictors of Trajectory Group Membership

Demographic Characteristics

During home interviews at one month, mothers reported their own education (in years) and the study children’s race/ethnicity. The presence of a husband or partner in the home was assessed during telephone and in-person interviews at regular intervals between 1 month and 54 months and was scored as “yes” or “no” at each assessment from one month to 54 months. Mothers also reported their family income at 1, 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months. Income-to-needs ratios were calculated from U.S. Census Bureau tables as the ratio of family income to the appropriate poverty threshold for each household size and these ratios were averaged from 1–54 months.

A sociodemographic risk factor score was derived to reduce the number of variables in the analyses and because demographic indicators were quite highly correlated. Maternal education, mean income-to-needs, child ethnicity, and whether the mother was partnered were included as early (birth to 54 months) demographic predictors. We first constructed a risk index based on four variables (0 or 1), with a 1 scored for the following: minority status; family with an average income-to-needs ratio of 2 or less (i.e., twice the poverty level or lower); child’s mother had only a high school education or less; child’s mother was unmarried at any time from the child’s birth through 54 months. The entire data set (n=1364) was used in this analysis. These four variables were then entered into a categorical confirmatory factor analysis to determine whether they formed a single factor. The model fit the data well (TLI =0.98; CFI= 0.99, RMSEA = 0.06), so the factor score estimate of risk was used as a covariate in subsequent analyses. Factor loadings ranged from .94 for income to .59 for ethnicity.

Child Characteristics

Language at 36 and 54 Months

At 36 months, children were administered the Reynell Developmental Language Scale (Reynell, 1991). The scale is composed of 67 items and yields two scores, a verbal comprehension score and an expressive language score. Both scales have adequate internal consistency (α= .93 and .86, respectively). At 54 months the Preschool Language Scale (PLS, α = .87, Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 1992) was administered and standard scores on the auditory comprehension and expressive communication scales were composited. Scores on the Reynell and PLS were then composited to create a measure of early language development (α = .83). Examiners went through a rigorous training and certification procedure before collecting data.

Delay of Gratification

At 54 months, children were observed in a standard delay of gratification task. In the middle of a laboratory visit, the child was offered a small immediate reward of candy (m and m’s or other preferred food, if the parent objected to candy) or a larger reward later. The child was taught to ring a bell to summon the experimenter who needed to leave the room and then was left to play the “waiting game.” The child was given two plates: one with a small amount of food that could be eaten right away (but only after the experimenter was summoned back by a ring of the bell) and another with a larger amount that could only be eaten at the end of the waiting game. Latency or length of time waited was used in the analysis.

Maternal Parenting

Maternal Sensitivity

Mother-child interactions were videotaped in semi-structured 15-minute observations at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months. The tasks provided a context for assessing age-appropriate qualities of maternal behavior. The tasks at all ages are described in the Manuals of Operation and involved a free play session at 6 months and developmentally appropriate play and problem-solving tasks at subsequent assessments.

Data were collected across the 10 sites by research assistants who attended centralized training sessions at each age and passed certification tests prior to data collection. Videotapes of mother-child interactions were sent to a central non-data collection site for coding. Coders were blind to other information about study families. At 6, 15, and 24 months, composite maternal sensitivity scores were created from the sums of three 4-point ratings (maternal sensitivity to child non-distress, intrusiveness [reversed], and positive regard). At 36 and 54 months, the maternal sensitivity composite was the sum of three 7-point ratings of supportive presence, respect for autonomy, and hostility (reversed). Cronbach’s alphas for the sensitivity composites ranged from .70 to .84. Inter-coder reliability was determined by assigning two coders to 19–20% of the tapes drawn randomly at each assessment period. Inter-coder reliability was calculated as the intra-class correlation coefficient. Inter-coder reliability coefficients for the composite maternal sensitivity scores used in the current report ranged from .83 to .87. For the current analyses, sensitivity scores were averaged from 6 months to 54 months to form a composite indicator of maternal sensitivity during infancy and early childhood. The coefficient alpha for this overall maternal sensitivity composite was .77.

Maternal Reports of Harsh Discipline

At 54 months, mothers completed a revised version of the Raising Children Checklist (Greenberger & Goldberg, 1989) that was modified specifically for use in the NICHD SECCYD. The current analyses focused on the 8-item harsh control scale. Mothers were asked to rate their attitudes and expectations about child behavior on a 4 –point scale (“definitely no” to “definitely yes”). Higher scores reflect stronger agreement with items assessing expectations for immediate compliance and respect for authority, as well as approval of the use of physical punishment (α = .71).

Child Outcomes

Maternal Reports of Adjustment

At sixth grade mothers completed standardized questionnaires to describe children’s behavior at home and with peers: the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS, Gresham & Elliott, 1990), the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL, Achenbach, 1991b), and the Adult-Child Relationship Scale (Pianta, 2001). The 38 social competence items from the SSRS assessed social skills with peers and adults (α=.90). Scores on the two broadband scales of the CBCL, Internalizing Problems (e.g., too fearful and anxious, 31 items, α = .87) and Externalizing Problems (e.g., argues a lot, 33 items, α =.89) were used to index behavior problems. Finally, mothers rated the degree of conflict they experienced with the study child on the 7-item Conflict scale of the Adult-Child Relationship Scale (Pianta, 2001; α = .85).

Measures of Academic Competence

Children were administered the Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Battery (Woodcock & Johnson, 1989, 1990) during laboratory visits at fifth grade. The achievement score was the composite of four subtests: letter-word identification, passage comprehension, calculation, and applied problems. Because this measure is known to be quite stable and was available at fifth rather than sixth grade, it was deemed appropriate to include as an outcome measure.

Teachers also rated children’s academic achievement in sixth grade. The composite rating of children’s current school performance was based on accomplishments in six subjects (reading, oral language, written language, math, social studies, and science) that were rated on a 5-point scale (1= very poor to 5= very good; six items, α =.95). Six items, rated on the same 5-point scale, were used to assess work habits (e.g., follows classroom procedures, works independently, α = .96).

Child Self-Reports of Adjustment and Relationships

Children completed three self-report measures at the sixth grade visit assessing relationships with mothers and friends as well as risk-taking behavior. The Friendship Quality Questionnaire (Parker & Asher, 1993), a 28-item scale, assesses children’s views of their relationship with their best friend. Items are rated on a 5-point scale (1=not at all true to 5= really true). Twenty items make up the total friendship quality score (α = .87) with higher scores reflecting a more positive (validation, companionship, disclosure) and less negative (conflict) relationship. Children were also asked how often they and their friends engaged in a number of moderately to highly risky behaviors (e.g., skipped school, gotten into a gang fight, smoked cigarettes, stolen something worth a lot), using a measure derived from the Fast Track project (Kaplow, Curran, Dodge, & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 2002). Total risk taking scores for self (α = .73) and friend (α= .82) were moderately correlated (r = .72). Scores were composited because we reasoned that study children might more easily acknowledge that their friends engaged in risky behaviors, than admit that they themselves did. Finally, children reported on their perceptions of maternal hostility. Children rated the frequency of negative interaction (8 items rated 1= “not at all” to 4= “a lot”; α = .75). For example, items assessed perceptions of maternal anger, yelling, and criticism.

Results

Overview of Data Analysis

First trajectories of physical aggression were described separately for boys and girls. We next used multinomial logistic regression to examine whether theoretically meaningful precursors predicted trajectory group membership. Two sets of models were fit separately for boys and girls. The first included the social risk index and two proximal child factors (early language and delay of gratification); the second included social risk and the two maternal parenting measures (observed sensitivity and mother-reported harsh control). When significant associations between early childhood predictors and trajectory group membership were detected, pairwise comparisons were conducted to identify which trajectory groups differed on that predictor. Effect sizes were computed as odds ratios. A value greater than one implies that that predictor is more strongly related to the first group and a value less than one implies that it is more strongly related to the second group. In the final set of analyses, trajectory group membership served as the independent variable in analyses of covariance that compared trajectory groups on maternal, teacher, and child reports of adjustment outcomes at 6th grade and a direct assessment of academic achievement at 5th grade, controlling for sociodemographic risk. Pairwise comparisons were conducted when significant differences emerged in the overall comparison of the trajectory groups and effect sizes were computed as the difference between the means divided by the standard deviation under the model (i.e., the root mean squared error). The Benjamini-Hochberg (1995) technique was used to adjust for multiple tests, keeping the nominal alpha at 0.05.

Missing Data

We estimated the aggression trajectories using only observed data, including cases with at least two teacher ratings (Nagin, 2005), but we conducted multiple imputations using all individuals included in the trajectory analyses for subsequent analyses on predictors and outcomes. Thus, the sample sizes for all analyses were constant. Thus, for the semiparametric group-based models, Full Information Maximum Likelihood was used, thereby including every case with at least two teacher ratings (Schafer & Graham, 2002). For the analyses involving groups, multiple imputation using PROC MI in SAS v9.1 was used. There were 20 imputations and all parameters were collapsed across the data sets in accordance with Rubin (1987, 1996). Both of these approaches to missing data are considered to be state of the art for use with the respective types of models (Schafer & Graham, 2002).

Trajectories of Physical Aggression

Nagin’s (1999, 2005) semiparametric, group-based modeling (SPGM) was used to describe boys’ and girls’ trajectories of physical aggression separately based on teacher ratings from grades one through six. As noted, children with at least two out of a possible six teacher ratings were included through the use of full information maximum likelihood estimations (Jones, Nagin, & Roeder, 2001; Schafer & Graham, 2002). Teacher ratings were available at least four out of six times for 89% of the analysis sample; only 4% of the sample had ratings only twice.

The analytic model assumes that the population of children is composed of distinct groups of individuals who show different developmental trajectories and individuals within each group are homogeneous in terms of their developmental patterns of aggression relative to those in other trajectory groups. Growth curves for each individual are estimated and then prototypic group curves are identified from these individual curves (Nagin, 1999, 2005). The degree to which each individual’s growth curve resembles each of the group curves is estimated, and individuals are typically classified into clusters accordingly. Each analysis specifies a distribution for the dependent variable over time, a polynomial of order k (e.g., linear, quadratic), and the number of groups to be extracted. Recommended practice involves fitting a series of analyses specifying different numbers of groups and selecting the solution that accounts for the most information in the data as indicated by the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC).

A zero-inflated Poisson distribution was used because the dependent variable could be viewed as a count of the number of aggressive problem behaviors, weighted by frequency of occurrence and there was an over-dispersion of zero scores. A cubic growth curve model was specified to describe the pattern of change over time. As recommended by Nagin (1999, 2005), we examined the BIC, the posterior probability score, and the trajectories themselves to decide which model best fit the data. It should be noted that SPGM, by forcing discrete group membership without allowing variability around those classifications, may compensate for the lack of variability by extracting more classes as noted in Bauer and Curran (2003).

For teacher ratings of aggression in boys, the BIC score for the 4-group model (−2821.23) was better than for the 3-group model (−2872.51) or the 5-group model (−2941.20). The mean posterior probability scores, indexing the degree to which each child fit his assigned trajectory, ranged from .87 to .96 for the 4-group model, with a mean of .91, indicating that most boys fit their assigned trajectory well. Predicted and observed trajectories are depicted in Figure 1. Most boys (62.2%, n= 351) were assigned to the no-aggression group (posterior probability: mean = .95; median =.99; min = .40; max=.99); they received consistently low ratings of aggression across elementary school. Another 16.7% (n = 88) showed a moderate-decreasing pattern of aggression according to teacher ratings, reflected in moderate aggression at first grade that increased from first to second grade and then gradually declined from third through sixth grade (posterior probability: mean = .875; median =.96; min = .46; max=1.00). In contrast, boys on the moderate-increasing aggression trajectory (14.5%, n= 74) were rated as moderately aggressive in first grade, with a decline at second grade, and then an increase to moderate aggression from fourth to sixth grade (posterior probability: mean = .87; median =.94; min = .44; max=1.00). Finally, another 6.6% (35 boys) were assigned to the high-stable trajectory group, described by the highest and most stable level of aggression, with scores elevated above the other three trajectories at each assessment from first to sixth grade (posterior probability: mean = .95; median =.99; min = .59; max=1.00).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Predicted and actual trajectory groups for males.

The trajectory analysis of girls’ aggression ratings indicated that a 3 group model best fit the data (BIC for 2-group model = −1703.93; for 3-group model = −1638.82; for 4-group model = −1692.31), as displayed in Figure 2. Posterior probabilities for the 3-group model were high (range = .91 to .96, mean = .94). The vast majority of girls (428 or 78%) were seen by teachers as not at all aggressive (no-aggression) (posterior probability: mean = .96; median =.996; min = .516; max=.997). Another 81 girls or 17.3% showed a low-stable level of aggression (posterior probability: mean = .91; median =.97; min = .50; max=1.00). Finally, a small group of girls (n = 24; 4.7%) showed a high-variable pattern of aggressive behavior that rose somewhat from first grade to third grade and then declined slightly, still remaining quite elevated relative to peers (posterior probability: mean = .956; median =.99; min = .53; max=1.00).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Predicted and actual trajectory groups for females.

Earlier Predictors of Aggression Trajectories

Multinomial logistic regression analyses were used to examine predictors of aggression trajectories. Two sets of analyses were conducted. The first set considered only the risk index and proximal child characteristics (early language and delay of gratification). Trajectory group membership was predicted by the social risk index, but neither early language nor delay of gratification was a significant predictor. For both boys (Wald χ2 = 3.34, p =.001) and girls (Wald χ2 = 2.11, p =.035) greater social risk was associated with higher teacher ratings of aggression. Boys who experienced higher levels of social risk in early childhood were more likely to be in the high (OR = 4.13, 95% CI: 2.15–7.93, p<.000), moderate-increasing (OR= 2.44, 95% CI: 1.60–3.73, p<.000), and moderate-decreasing (OR=2.11, 95% CI: 1.35–3.30, p=.001) groups relative to the no-aggression group. Similarly, girls who experienced higher levels of social risk were more likely to be in the high-variable (OR=2.12, 95% CI: 1.05–4.27, p=.037) or low-stable (OR=1.66, 95% CI: 1.09–2.53, p=.017) aggression group relative to the no-aggression group. Descriptive statistics on predictor variables by sex and trajectory group are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics on Predictor Variables by Sex and Trajectory Group

Boys’ Trajectories
No Aggression Mod-Decrease Mod-Increase High-Stable
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
n=351 n=88 n=74 N=35
Early Language 97.87 (14.15) 90.16 (16.17) 93.73 (16.17) 87.56 (14.77)
Delay of Gratification 4.47 (2.99) 3.77 (3.15) 3.75 (3.13) 2.47 (3.11)
Maternal Sensitivity 12.42 (1.63) 11.69 (1.66) 11.96 (1.90) 10.79 (2.19)
Harsh Control 20.50 (3.01) 21.87 (3.18) 22.08 (2.99) 23.42 (2.95)
Risk Factor −0.07 (0.56) 0.36 (0.71) 0.25 (0.68) 0.61 (0.64)
Girls’ Trajectories
No Aggression Low-Stable High Variable
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
n=428 n=81 N=24
Early Language 102.61 (13.58) 96.84 (15.75) 86.79 (16.78)
Delay of Gratification 4.79 (2.95) 4.21 (3.01) 2.34 (3.18)
Maternal Sensitivity 12.69 (1.49) 11.98 (1.79) 10.67 (2.36)
Harsh Control 20.86 (3.33) 22.91 (3.61) 23.18 (3.66)
Risk Factor −0.09 (0.58) 0.21 (0.76) 0.56 (0.74)

Note: Early language was assessed at 36 and 54 months; delay of gratification and harsh control were assessed at 54 months; maternal sensitivity was observed at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months; risk factor scores were based on a categorical confirmatory factor analysis that included maternal education at 1 month; child ethnicity; income and partner status assessed at 1, 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months.

The second model included the social risk index and the two proximal parenting measures, observed maternal sensitivity from 6–54 months and maternal reports of harsh control at 54 months. Over and above social risk, higher levels of harsh control (Wald χ2 = 4.70, p =.003) differentially predicted boys’ membership in trajectory groups. Boys whose mothers reported using more harsh control at 54 months were more likely to be in either the high-stable (OR=1.23, 95% CI: 1.06–1.44, p=.01) or moderate-increasing (OR=1.15, 95% CI: 1.04–1.26, p=.01) trajectory groups, relative to the no-aggression group. Maternal sensitivity did not add to the prediction of trajectory group membership.

In contrast, both maternal sensitivity (Wald χ2 = 4.75, p =.009) and harsh control (Wald χ2 = 5.74, p =.003) differentially predicted trajectory group membership for girls, whereas social risk was not a significant predictor with early maternal parenting measures in the model. Specifically, girls whose mothers were relatively lower in sensitivity were more likely to be in the high-variable trajectory group relative to either the low-stable (OR=.73, 95% CI: .55–.97, p=.031) or no-aggression (OR=.67, 95% CI: .52–.87, p=.002) trajectories. In addition, higher harsh control predicted membership in the low-stable relative to the no-aggression trajectory group (OR=1.15, CI: 1.06–1.26, p=.001).

Outcomes of Trajectory Group Membership at 6th grade

The final set of analyses focused on 6th grade outcomes. Analyses of variance, using social risk as the covariate and trajectory group as the independent variable, were conducted. Outcome measures were collected in 6th grade, except for vacademic achievement, which was assessed in 5th grade. Overall group comparisons and adjusted means that show the results of pairwise differences are presented in Table 3 for boys and Table 4 for girls. The outcomes included maternal reports of externalizing problems, internalizing problems, mother-child conflict, and social skills; teacher-reports of overall school performance and school work habits; Woodcock-Johnson academic achievement scores; and child reports of friendship quality, engagement in risky behavior, and maternal hostility.

Table 3.

Comparison of 6th grade outcomes by trajectory group for boys adjusting for social risk

Trajectory
Group
No
Aggression
N=351
Moderate
Decrease
N=88
Moderate
Increase
N=74
High
Stable
N=35
F Adj Mean1 Adj Mean Adj Mean Adj Mean
Maternal Reports
   Externalizing 15.16*** 43.73a 49.19b 47.40a,b 52.67c
   Internalizing 2.19 47.36 49.08 46.06 50.94
   Conflict 11.92*** 15.26a 17.88a 17.82a 20.37b
   Social Skills 6.86*** 109.12a 104.59 105.93a 99.09b
Teacher Reports
   School Perform 5.34** 3.40a 3.22b 3.32 2.79b
   Work Habits 27.31*** 3.61a 2.98b 2.92b 2.27c
Direct Assessment
   Achievement 2.04 109.82 108.55 107.59 104.48
Self Reports
   Risky Behavior 6.93*** 3.05 3.97a 4.20a 3.83b
   Friendship Quality 1.54 4.09 3.93 4.03 4.08
   Maternal Hostility 1.04 11.47 11.96 11.72 11.86

Note: Analysis of covariance with social risk as covariate. Adjusted means are reported. The Benjamini-Hochberg adjusted p values were used to determine significance. Means with different subscripts are statistically different (p<.05).

**

p <.001,

***

p <.0001

Table 4.

Comparison of 6th grade outcomes by trajectory group for girls adjusting for social risk

Trajectory
Group
No
Aggression
N=428
Low
Stable
N=81
High
Variable
N=24
F Adj Mean Adj Mean Adj Mean
Maternal Reports
   Externalizing 19.97**** 44.98a 50.90a 53.52b
   Internalizing 0.98 47.45 48.86 47.61
   Conflict 6.89** 16.72 19.33a 17.87b
   Social Skills 12.71**** 108.06a 102.78a 95.53b
Teacher Reports
   School Perform 7.68*** 3.61a 3.44a 3.37b
   Work Habits 28.00**** 4.15a 3.63a 3.33b
Direct Assessment
   Achievement 4.74** 110.11a 109.33 107.16b
Self Reports
   Risky Behavior 9.17**** 2.48a 2.97a 3.88b
   Friendship Quality 4.44* 4.38a 4.27 4.06b
   Maternal Hostility 5.81** 11.24 12.33a 11.27b

Note: Analysis of covariance with social risk as covariate. Adjusted means are reported. The Benjamini-Hochberg adjusted p values were used to determine significance. Means with different subscripts are statistically different (p<.05).

*

p <.05;

**

p <.01;

***

p <.001;

****

p <.0001

Boys

Trajectory Group Main Effects

Controlling for sociodemographic risk, the trajectory groups differed significantly on six of 10 outcome measures: maternal reports of externalizing problems (F(3, 543) = 15.16, p <.001), social skills (F(3, 543) = 6.86, p <.001), and mother-child conflict (F(3, 543) = 11.92, p <.001); child reports of risky behavior (F(3, 543) = 6.93, p <.001); and, teacher reports of overall school performance (F(3, 543) = 5.34, p =.001) and work habits (F(3, 543) = 27.31, p <.001). Boys in the four trajectory groups did not differ significantly on maternal ratings of internalizing problems (F(3, 543) = 2. 19, p= .09), on the Woodcock-Johnson (F(3, 543) = 2.04, p = .11), or on self-reports of friendship quality (F(3, 543) = 1.54, n.s.) or maternal hostility (F(3, 543) = 1.04, n.s.). In general, as would be expected, the no-aggression group showed the fewest problems and the best social skills relative to the other trajectory groups. Boys in the two moderate aggression trajectory groups did not differ from one another on any outcome measure, despite their different patterns of aggression over time, and they showed fewer differences from the no-aggression group than expected. Specific pairwise comparisons are discussed below with effect sizes included when differences are statistically significant.

High-stable versus moderate-decreasing and moderate-increasing aggression trajectories

Boys in the high-stable aggression trajectory group were rated as showing more externalizing problems than those in the moderate-decreasing (d = .35) and moderate-increasing (d = .54) trajectory groups. Mothers also indicated that they had more conflict-laden relationships with boys in the high-stable trajectory group than with boys in either the moderate-decreasing (d = .41) or moderate-increasing (d = .42) aggression groups. Finally, mothers rated boys in the high-stable group as significantly less socially skilled than boys in the moderate-increasing group (d = .46). Teachers likewise rated boys in the high-stable trajectory group as showing poorer work habits than boys in either the moderate-decreasing (d = .62) or moderate-increasing trajectory (d = .57) groups. Boys in the two moderate aggression groups, however, reported that they and their peers engaged in more risk-taking behavior than did boys in the high-stable trajectory group (d = .06 for moderate-decreasing; d = .15 for moderate-increasing), albeit with small effect sizes.

Moderate-increasing versus no-aggression trajectories

Boys in the moderate-increasing aggression trajectory group differed significantly from boys in the no-aggression group on only one outcome: Teachers rated boys showing increasing aggression as having poorer work habits in school than their non-aggressive peers (d = .61).

Moderate-decreasing versus no-aggression trajectories

Boys showing decreasing aggression differed from the boys in the no-aggression group on three adult report measures. They received higher ratings from their mothers on externalizing problems (d = .55), and teachers rated them as performing more poorly in school (d = .18) and as having poorer work habits (d = .56).

High-stable versus no-aggression trajectories

Not surprisingly, mothers rated boys in the high-stable group as showing significantly higher levels of externalizing problems (d = .91) and poorer social skills (d = .67), and as engaging in more mother-child conflict (d = .84) than boys showing no-aggression. Teachers also rated boys in the high-stable aggression group as poorer in overall school performance (d = .63) and work habits (d = 1.18) in comparison to boys in the no-aggression trajectory group.

Girls

Trajectory Group Main Effects

Trajectory group differences were evident across nine of the ten outcome measures: maternal reports of externalizing problems (F(2, 529) = 19.97, p<.001), mother-child conflict (F(2, 529) = 6.89, p<.001), and social skills (F(2, 529) = 12.71, p<.001); teacher reports of overall school performance (F(2, 529) = 7.68, p<.001) and work habits (F(2, 529) = 28.00, p<.001); child reports of maternal hostility (F(2, 529) = 5.81, p<.01), risky behavior (F(2, 529) = 9.17, p<.001), and friendship quality (F(2, 529) = 4.44, p = .012); and tested achievement on the Woodcock-Johnson assessment (F(2, 529) = 4.74, p<.01). Only maternal reports of internalizing problems did not show a significant group main effect.

High-variable aggression versus low-stable aggression trajectories

Seven variables differentiated between the two groups of girls showing relatively higher levels of aggression. Girls in the high-variable aggression group were rated by their mothers as having more externalizing problems (d = .25) and poorer social skills (d =.43) than were girls showing low-stable aggression. Teachers also rated girls in the high-variable aggression group as poorer in both overall school performance (d = .08) and work habits (d = .31) than their low-stable counterparts. Girls in the high-variable aggression group also reported engaging in more risk-taking behavior with their peers (d = .44). However, girls exhibiting low-stable levels of aggression were rated by their mothers as engaging in more conflict with them (d = .40), and the girls likewise perceived their mothers as more angry and hostile (d = .39) in comparison to girls in the high-variable aggression group.

Low-stable aggression versus no- aggression trajectories

Girls showing low-stable aggression did not differ from their no-aggression counterparts on any outcome measures.

High-variable aggression versus no-aggression trajectories

As would be expected, girls in the high-variable aggression trajectory group were rated by their mothers as showing more externalizing problems (d = .81) and poorer social skills (d = .74) than no-aggression girls. The high-variable aggression girls also reported engaging in more risky behavior (d = .69) and having poorer quality friendships (d = .61) than their no aggression counterparts. They were also rated by their teachers as showing poorer overall school performance (d = .26) and work habits (d = .85); this was confirmed by their poorer performance on the Woodcock-Johnson achievement measure (d = .21).

Discussion

We examined trajectories of physical aggression as rated by teachers from first to sixth grade separately by child sex in a large, though not nationally-representative sample of children from across the United States. We also examined a small set of antecedents of the trajectories and their developmental sequelae. The observed patterns of change in aggression over time were broadly consistent with those identified by Broidy et al. (2003) in their multi-study analysis. New empirical ground was broken, however, in two ways--by our consideration of child characteristics and parenting as antecedents of trajectory group membership and by our assessment of a broad range of child outcome measures in early adolescence. The former was accomplished by including measures of early language development and delay of gratification in the preschool period which are likely precursors of aggression. In terms of maternal parenting measures, in addition to the assessment of harsh and coercive parenting (e.g., Patterson et al., 1998; Snyder et al., 2003), we included a measure of positive supportive parenting derived from observations of maternal sensitivity across infancy and early childhood. Further, a range of adjustment outcomes in sixth grade, obtained from different sources, including tested academic achievement, parent- and teacher-reported social adjustment, and a child-reported friendship quality were examined, thereby moving beyond the antisocial and delinquent outcomes emphasized in most prior studies of aggression trajectories.

Trajectories of Aggression

Using a 6-item aggression scale derived from the TRF to model trajectories of aggression, we identified four trajectories in boys and three in girls. The data show good convergence with the patterns of aggression described by Broidy et al. (2003) who reported on six samples of boys rated by teachers on aggression scales comprised of from two to five items. Boys in these samples ranged in age from 6 to 15, and they varied in their degree of demographic risk as well as their country of origin (US, Canada, New Zealand). The majority of boys in the Broidy et al. (2003) analyses showed low levels of aggression whereas from 4% to 10% showed high levels of aggression that were either relatively stable or slightly increasing. Likewise, we found that 62% of the boys in NICHD Study sample were consistently very low in aggression and 6.6% were stably high. The boys studied by Broidy et al. (2003) varied in whether one or two intermediate groups were identified. Like the two Quebec samples, four trajectories that included two moderately aggressive groups emerged in the current inquiry, but unlike those described by Broidy et al. (2003) who identified primarily decreasing trajectories, both moderate increasing and moderate decreasing aggression trajectories emerged in this study. Differences in measurement and sample composition likely account for these slight discrepancies in results across investigations. Similarities clearly outweigh differences between trajectory patterns detected here and in the Broidy et al. (2003) multi-study analysis.

The three trajectories of physical aggression we identified in girls also converge with patterns of aggression described by Broidy et al. (2003). Most girls in the NICHD SECC sample were very low in aggression (78%); 17% of the girls showed a somewhat stable level of low aggression, a pattern similar to one identified in three of the four female samples studied by Broidy et al. (2003). Finally, a small proportion (4.7%), while elevated relative to the other girls in the sample, was still generally lower in aggression overall than boys. These girls, however, showed a variable pattern of aggression that appeared to peak at grades 2 and 3 and then decline somewhat, while still remaining elevated relative to same-sex peers. Taken together, these results, when considered along with those of others, indicate that most children do not show elevated aggression, but that both high and low to moderate levels can be identified in both boys and girls. Further, higher proportions of girls show low to no aggression, and even the most aggressive girls have generally lower scores than the most aggressive boys.

Predictors of Aggression Trajectories

Although research indicates that trajectories of aggression are predicted by high levels of family adversity (Joussemet et al., 2008; Nagin & Tremblay, 2001; NICHD ECCRN, 2004; Shaw et al., 2003), to our knowledge, this is the first study to examine both early child characteristics and maternal parenting as predictors of aggression trajectories identified from teacher reports, and also to examine these separately by child sex. Although we hypothesized—and found--that greater sociodemographic risk was associated with higher levels of aggression, our primary interest was in more proximal child characteristics that might predict poorer control of aggression in school and in aspects of maternal parenting that might also be associated with lower self-regulation and more problems controlling aggression at school. Recall that tests of these hypotheses were conducted after accounting for effects of sociodeomographic risk.

Contrary to expectation, early child language skills at 36 and 54 months and delay of gratification at 54 months did not predict membership in higher aggression trajectory groups in either boys or girls. This null finding was surprising, especially for boys, as the combination of poorer self-regulation and language development have been posited to account for the higher levels of aggression observed in boys relative to girls at preschool-age (Hay, 2007) and have been implicated in the emergence of stable aggression in other theoretical models of aggression and antisocial behavior (Moffitt, 1993). One possible explanation for these null findings is that these measures of language ability and self-regulation may prove to be better predictors of later academic achievement and impulse control and not specifically predictors of high levels of physical aggression. Alternatively, the explanatory power potentially attributable to these factors could have been accounted for by the sociodemographic risk variables which functioned as covariates in the analyses.

In both sexes, harsh control by mothers at 54 months predicted later teacher ratings of higher and more stable aggression in elementary school. Although this finding is consistent with the work of Patterson and colleagues (e.g., Patterson et al., 1998; Snyder et al., 2003) linking coercive control to later aggression and externalizing problems, to our knowledge this is among the first studies to link early harsh control with later trajectories of aggression as assessed by teachers, providing a confirmation of this link over time and across settings and informants. Joussemet et al. (2008) recently reported that controlling parenting predicted higher aggression in a sample of children in Quebec, although differential effects by child sex could not be determined. In the current analyses, maternal harsh control was a significant precursor of aggression trajectory for both boys and girls, with sociodemographic risk controlled, underscoring the robustness of this association. Mothers who endorsed more harsh and strict disciplinary practices, including approval of physical punishment and expectations for immediate compliance at 54 months, had children who teachers rated as elevated in aggression across middle childhood. Most studies linking coercive parenting during the preschool period to concurrent and later disruptive behavior have focused on boys (e.g., Belsky, Woodworth, & Crnic, 1996; Campbell, Pierce, Moore, Marakovitz, & Newby, 1996; Shaw, Winslow, Owens, Vondra, Cohn, & Bell, 1998). The association between early harsh maternal control and low, but stable and non-normative, physical aggression in girls across the elementary school years has implications for understanding social development in girls.

These findings on maternal harsh control are consistent with the argument that some young boys and girls may model their parents’ use of physically coercive behaviors in their interactions with peers and teachers. An alternative interpretation is that mothers use more harsh control with more aggressive and coercive children who also show these behaviors in school. Among other possibilities, the negative quality of the mother-child interaction may breed anger and resentment that is expressed in interactions outside the home or children who rely on external controls from power assertive adults may not learn to use internalized regulatory strategies in the school setting (Joussemet et al., 2008; Kochanska, 2002). These formulations all suggest the importance of modeling and other social learning and emotional development mechanisms that may play a role in the transmission of negative affect and angry engagement with teachers and peers (Chang, Schwartz, Dodge, & McBride-Chang, 2003; Lynch et al., 2006; Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007). Not to be discounted, of course, is the prospect that genetic factors may be operative, too. In the context of early harsh control, it is interesting that girls in the low-aggression trajectory group also perceived their mothers as more hostile when they were in 6th grade relative to girls who showed high-variable aggression, and their mothers likewise rated them as engaging in more mother-child conflict, suggesting that long-standing mother-child conflict may play a role in the emergence and stability of low, but non-normative, physical aggression in girls. This underscores the salient role the mother-child relationship may play in the emergence and stability of aggression in school-age girls.

For girls, the quality of the mother-child relationship as reflected in maternal sensitive responding from 6–54 months also predicted trajectory group membership above and beyond both sociodemographic risk and harsh control. When mothers were low in sensitivity, their daughters were more likely to display high-variable aggression in school, suggesting the importance of positive maternal engagement for girls. This finding is not surprising, given the importance of the quality of the mother-child relationship for children’s adjustment (e.g., Kochanska, 2002; Thompson, 1998). It is surprising, however, that the effect of maternal sensitivity emerged for girls, but not for boys, as other data suggest that boys may be especially vulnerable to the effects of insensitive parenting (McFadyen-Ketchum et al., 1996). This link between lower maternal sensitivity and higher aggression in girls is consistent with attachment theory, in that the quality of the earlier mother-child relationship is deemed important for the development of children’s self-regulatory skills and control of negative affect (Thompson, 1998). We are at a loss, however, to explain why this effect was found for girls, but not for boys.

Outcomes of Trajectory Groups in Early Adolescence

Fundamental to a developmental analysis of different longitudinal patterns of aggression, and complementing inquiry into their potential determinants, is the issue of their developmental sequelae. Do these trajectories matter for adjustment? With the exception of the earlier report on this sample focused on maternal rather than teacher reports of aggression, prior research on aggression trajectories has emphasized almost exclusively links between high levels of chronic aggression and later delinquent behavior. The focus in the present report was thus purposefully on a broader range of adjustment outcomes at 6th grade. In addition, in the prior work on this sample, trajectories and outcomes were examined with boys and girls combined (Campbell et al., 2006). The earlier work indicated that mothers’ reports of low, moderate, and high levels of persistent aggression predicted poorer school adjustment in 6th grade. For example, low but stable aggression predicted more self-reported peer problems and sad mood, whereas moderate and high stable aggression predicted more externalizing problems as rated by teachers (Campbell et al., 2006).

The current analyses support and extend these earlier findings. Of particular importance, even low to moderate levels of teacher-rated aggression proved to be associated with a few adjustment difficulties. In general, however, as expected, both boys and girls on the highest aggression trajectory differed from their same sex counterparts on the no-aggression trajectory in the expected direction on several maternal and teacher report measures, including externalizing problems, social skills, work habits, and overall school performance. Maternal reports of internalizing problems did not differentiate among trajectory groups, although we had raised the possibility that more aggressive girls would evidence some signs of internalizing problems. However, internalizing problems may be overlooked by adults who are often more aware of externalizing difficulties or they may appear later in development, well after the transition to adolescence.

Girls on the high-variable aggression trajectory, however, did acknowledge engaging in more risky behavior and reported lower quality friendships than their no-aggression counterparts, suggesting that girls showing a relatively higher level of aggression are at risk for a range of negative outcomes across settings and relationships. These results contrast with the more equivocal findings of Broidy et al. (2003) pertaining to delinquent behavior, but are consistent with those from the Dunedin and Christchurch studies suggesting that adolescent girls who show non-normative levels of aggression are likely to continue to have social and educational difficulties later on. In particular, concerns about substance use, early promiscuous sexuality, and potentially early pregnancy and childbearing merit attention, as aggressive girls are at greater risk for these outcomes (Bardone, Moffitt, Caspi, Dickson, & Silva, 1996; Miller-Johnson et al., 1999; Serbin, Peters, McAffer, & Schwartzman, 1991; Underwood, Kupersmidt, & Coie, 1996; Woodward & Fergusson, 1999). Perhaps of note in this regard is recent evidence from the NICHD Study indicating that early harsh maternal control predicts earlier age of menarche and, thereby, greater sexual and other risk taking at age 15 (Belsky, Steinberg, Houts, Halpern-Felsher & the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, submitted), findings in accord with Belsky, Steinberg and Draper’s (1991) evolutionary theory of socialization.

Lower perceived friendship quality during the transition to adolescence may be an especially important indicator of adjustment problems in girls, given the importance of close same-sex friendships for girls at this age (Maccoby, 1998); this particular finding is also intriguing in view of the fact that lower maternal sensitivity, potentially another indicator of relationship quality, predicted being in this high-variable trajectory group. Further research will be needed to clarify links among early experiences with parents and later friendship quality in the context of aggression, which in girls at the transition to adolescence is likely to consist not only of some physical aggression, but also relational aggression (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003; Murray-Close et al., 2007). In contrast, however, it is noteworthy that mother-child conflict as perceived by both mothers and the girls themselves was not elevated in this group relative to no-aggression girls, possibly providing some degree of protection from more serious problems in middle adolescence.

Although girls in the low-stable aggression group did not differ from the no-aggression girls, it is noteworthy that they and their mothers acknowledged more mother-child conflict relative to the high-variable aggression girls, suggesting that these girls also may be at risk for additional adjustment difficulties later on as they move through adolescence. Alternatively, given adequate school performance and social skills, these girls may fare well as they become more independent from family and more invested in peers. Boys in the moderate-decreasing and increasing aggression trajectory groups appear to be showing some school adjustment problems, as reflected in poor work habits in school, and they report engaging with peers in more risky behavior than boys in the highest aggression group. Taken together, these subtle indicators of problem behavior in the moderately aggressive boys may become more salient as they move through puberty and the temptations of the peer group become more difficult to resist; alternatively, these may prove to be of limited predictive validity, given the apparently positive relationships these boys have with mothers and with peers.

However, even if these youngsters are not showing high levels of problem behavior or school failure at 6th grade, it is likely that some of them will manifest more social problems by middle adolescence in contrast to children who were not at all aggressive. For example, at least a few of these low to moderately aggressive youngsters may seek out a more antisocial peer group and/or evidence lower academic achievement as they move through middle school to high school. The poorer work habits and higher levels of risky behavior of the moderately aggressive boys and low aggressive girls especially bear watching. It will be important to follow this sample into adolescence and adulthood, given the intriguing findings from the Dunedin study of male and female antisocial trajectories to age 32 in adulthood. Odgers et al. (2008) reported adverse adjustment in adulthood for males, but not females with childhood limited antisocial behavior, and for both males and females with adolescent onset anti-social behavior. Boys on the two moderate trajectories and girls on the low-stable trajectory may continue on a relatively elevated trajectory or even show an increase in aggression by middle adolescence.

In summary, the results of this inquiry suggest that qualities of the mother-child relationship predict longitudinal patterns of aggressive behavior in boys and girls as assessed by teachers in school. Moreover, these aggression trajectories have implications for children’s social functioning at home, at school, and in the peer group at 6th grade, as well as their academic achievement. Data from the current study also highlight the importance of even moderate to low levels of stable or variable aggression in elementary school as possibly forecasting adjustment difficulties in early adolescence, thereby suggesting that school personnel and guidance counsellors should pay serious attention to children who as early as first or second grade are showing somewhat elevated levels of physically aggressive behavior. Preventive interventions at home and school could potentially be warranted, even in such obviously non-clinical cases.

Despite the use of a rich longitudinal data set, the direction of effects cannot be determined in these analyses, nor does this design permit the disentangling of genetic predispositions toward aggressive behavior in children and parents from influences that may emerge from coercive and insensitive parent-child interactions (Lynch et al., 2006; Rutter, 2003; Snyder et al., 2003). In fact, because observations did not focus on coercive processes per se, these can only be inferred from maternal and child reports of harshness, conflict, and hostility. Furthermore, we did not include data on fathers’ parenting behaviors in these analyses, thereby overlooking an important aspect of socialization and family functioning. The use of teacher reports of aggressive behavior is both a strength of this study, in that teachers are independent informants in an important context, and a limitation, because teachers may be more likely to observe aggression in boys than in girls. Girls are more likely to use physical aggression in close relationships (Moffitt et al., 2001), with siblings (Martin & Ross, 2005), and on the far side of the playground (Pepler & Craig, 1995), all situations which are not observable by teachers. Ideally, it would have been beneficial to have obtained peer reports of aggression. Interpersonal aggression by women and girls may be less salient than male aggression but have adverse consequences just the same (Hay, 2007). Nevertheless, converging evidence across time, reporters, measures, and settings, coupled with long-standing developmental theory, adds credence to the conclusion that relationship quality is an important contributor to the emergence of problematic -- albeit low levels of -- physical aggression in middle childhood. Future work will examine how well these trajectories predict adjustment outcomes in mid-adolescence, as well as whether some girls and boys showing low to moderate levels of aggression evidence escalating difficulties including drug use, sexual promiscuity, and school failure by age 15.

Table 2.

Multinomial regressions predicting trajectory group membership from earlier measures separately by gender

Boys Girls

Model 1
Wald χ2
Model 2
Wald χ2
Model 1
Wald χ2
Model 2
Wald χ2
Early language 1.10 --- 1.57 ---
Delay of gratification 0.77 --- 0.91 ---
Maternal harsh control --- 4.70** --- 5.74**
No<MI,HS No < Lo
Maternal sensitivity --- 1.03 --- 4.75**
No, Lo>Hi
Risk factor score 3.34** 8.19*** 2.11* 2.28
No<MD, MI,
HS
No<MD,MI,
HS
No<Lo, Hi

Note – Model I includes early language, delay of gratification and risk index; model 2 includes maternal harsh control, maternal sensitivity, and risk index. Results from pairwise comparisons are shown under chi-square values.

For Boys: HS = High-stable (n= 35); MI = Moderate Increasing (n= 74); MD= Moderate Decreasing (n= 88); No= No Aggression Trajectory Group (n= 351).

For Girls: Hi = High-Variable (n= 24); Lo = Low-Stable (n= 81); No= No Aggression Trajectory Group (n= 428).

*

p<.05,

**

p <.01,

***

p<.001.

Acknowledgments

This study is directed by a Steering Committee and supported by NICHD through a cooperative agreement (U10), which calls for scientific collaboration between the grantees and the NICHD staff. The content is solely the responsibility of the named authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the EKS National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, the National Institutes of Health, or individual members of the Network. Current members of the Steering Committee of the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, listed in alphabetical order, are: Jay Belsky (Birkbeck University of London), Cathryn Booth-LaForce (University of Washington), Robert H. Bradley (University of Arkansas at Little Rock), Celia A. Brownell (University of Pittsburgh), Margaret Burchinal (University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill), Susan B. Campbell (University of Pittsburgh), Elizabeth Cauffman (University of California, Irvine), Alison Clarke-Stewart (University of California, Irvine), Martha Cox (University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill), Robert Crosnoe (University of Texas, Austin), James A. Griffin (NICHD Project Scientist and Scientific Coordinator), Bonnie Halpern-Felsher (University of California, San Francisco), Willard Hartup (University of Minnesota), Kathryn Hirsh-Pasek (Temple University), Daniel Keating (University of Michigan, Ann Arbor), Bonnie Knoke (RTI International), Tama Leventhal (Tufts University), Kathleen McCartney (Harvard University), Vonnie C. McLoyd (University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill), Fred Morrison (University of Michigan, Ann Arbor), Philip Nader (University of California, San Diego), Marion O'Brien (University of North Carolina, Greensboro), Margaret Tresch Owen (University of Texas, Dallas), Ross Parke (University of California, Riverside), Robert Pianta (University of Virginia), Kim M. Pierce (University of Wisconsin-Madison), A. Vijaya Rao (RTI International), Glenn I. Roisman (University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign), Susan Spieker (University of Washington), Laurence Steinberg (Temple University), Elizabeth Susman (Pennsylvania State University), Deborah Lowe Vandell (University of California, Irvine), and Marsha Weinraub (Temple University).

References

  1. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Teacher’s Report Form and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991a. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991b. [Google Scholar]
  3. Aguilar B, Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson E. Distinguishing the early-onset/persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior types: From birth to 16 years. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:109–132. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400002017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker L, Cantwell DP. Developmental, social, and behavioral characteristics of speech and language disordered children. In: Chess S, Thomas A, editors. Annual progress in child psychiatry and child development. New York: Brunner/Mazel; 1983. pp. 205–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bardone AM, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Dickson N, Silva PA. Adult mental health and social outcomes of adolescent girls with depression and conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:811–829. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bauer DJ, Curran PJ. Distributional assumptions of growth mixture models: Implications for overextraction of latent trajectory classes. Psychological Methods. 2003;8:338–363. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Belsky J, Woodworth S, Crnic K. Trouble in the second year: Three questions about family interaction. Child Development. 1996;67:556–578. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P. Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development. 1991;62:647–670. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01558.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Belsky J, Steinberg L, Houts RM, Halpern-Felsher BL the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. The Development of Reproductive Strategy in Females: Earlier Maternal Harshness→Earlier Menarche→Increased Sexual Risk Taking. Birkbeck University of London; (Submitted). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bongers IL, Koot HM, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. The normative development of child and adolescent problem behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:179–192. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss. Vol. I. Attachment. New York: Basic Books; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  13. Broidy LM, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, Bates JE, Brame B, Dodge KA, Fergusson D, Horwood JL, Loeber R, Laird R, Lynam DR, Moffitt TE, Pettit GS, Vitaro F. Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:222–245. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Campbell SB. Behavior problems in preschool children: Clinical and developmental issues. 2nd edition. New York: Guilford Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  15. Campbell SB, Pierce EW, Moore G, Marakovitz S, Newby K. Boys' externalizing problems at elementary school: Pathways from early behavior problems, maternal control, and family stress. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:701–720. [Google Scholar]
  16. Campbell SB, Shaw DS, Gilliom M. Early externalizing behavior problems: Toddlers and preschoolers at risk for later maladjustment. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:467–488. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Campbell SB, Spieker S, Burchinal M, Poe M the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Trajectories of aggression from toddlerhood to age 9 predict academic and social functioning through age 12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:791–800. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cantwell DP, Baker L. Prevalence and type of psychiatric disorder and developmental disorders in three speech and language groups. Journal of Communication Disorders. 1987;20:151–160. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(87)90006-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chang L, Schwartz D, Dodge KA, McBride-Chang K. Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology. 2003;17:598–606. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Coie JD, Dodge KA. Aggression and antisocial behavior. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development. 5th ed. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 779–862. (Series Ed.) (Vol. Ed.) [Google Scholar]
  21. Cole PM, Teti LO, Zahn-Waxler C. Mutual emotion regulation and the stability of conduct problems between preschool and early school age. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:1–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Côté S, Tremblay RE, Nagin DS, Zoccolillo M, Vitaro F. Childhood behavioral profiles leading to adolescent conduct disorder: Risk trajectories for boys and girls. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:1086–1094. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200209000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Côté S, Vaillancourt T, Barker ED, Nagin D, Tremblay RE. The joint development of physical and indirect aggression: Predictors of continuity and change during childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:37–55. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Côté S, Zoccolillo M, Tremblay RE, Nagin D, Vitaro F. Predicting girls’ conduct disorder in adolescence from childhood trajectories of disruptive behaviors. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:678–684. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200106000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development. 1995;66:710–722. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00900.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Crick NR, Zahn-Waxler C. The development of psychopathology in females and males: Current progress and future challenges. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:719–742. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dionne G, Tremblay R, Boivin M, Laplante D, Pérusse D. Physical aggression and expressive vocabulary in 19-month-old twins. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:261–273. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fergusson DM, Horwood JL, Ridder E. Show me the child at seven: the consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:837–849. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00387.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fergusson DM, Lynskey MT, Horwood LJ. Factors associated with continuity and changes in disruptive behavior patterns between childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1996;24:533–553. doi: 10.1007/BF01670099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Greenberger E, Goldberg WA. Work, parenting, and the socialization of children. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25:22–35. [Google Scholar]
  31. Gresham FM, Elliot SN. Social skills rating system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hartup WW. Aggression in childhood: Developmental perspectives. American Psychologist. 1974;29:337–341. doi: 10.1037/h0037622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hay DF. The gradual emergence of sex differences in aggression: alternative hypotheses. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:1527–1537. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hay DF, Castle J, Davies L. Toddlers’ use of force against familiar peers: A precursor of serious aggression? Child Development. 2000;71:457–467. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jones BL, Nagin DS, Roeder K. A SAS procedure based on mixture models for estimating developmental trajectories. Sociological Methods and Research. 2001;29:374–393. [Google Scholar]
  36. Joussemet M, Vitaro F, Barker ED, Côté S, Nagin DS, Zoccolillo M, Tremblay RE. Controlling parenting and physical aggression during elementary school. Child Development. 2008;79:411–425. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01133.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kaplow J, Curran PJ, Dodge K the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Child, parent, and peer predictors of early-onset substance use: A multisite longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30:199–216. doi: 10.1023/a:1015183927979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kochanska G. Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11:191–195. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kopp CB. Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25:343–354. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lahey BB, Waldman ID, McBurnett K. The development of antisocial behavior: An integrative causal model. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40:669–682. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Loeber R, Farrington DP, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. The development of male offending: Key findings from the first decade of the Pittsburgh Youth Study. Studies on Crime and Prevention. 1998;7:141–171. [Google Scholar]
  42. Lynch SK, Turkheimer E, D’Onofrio BM, Mendle J, Emery RE, Slutske W, Martin NG. A genetically informed study of the association between harsh punishment and offspring behavioral problems. Journal of Family Psychology. 2006;20:190–198. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Maccoby EE. The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  44. Martin JL, Ross HS. Sibling aggression: sex differences and parents’ reactions. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2005;29:129–138. [Google Scholar]
  45. McFadyen-Ketchum SA, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS. Patterns of change in early childhood aggressive-disruptive behavior: Gender differences in predictions from early coercive and affectionate mother-child interactions. Child Development. 1996;67:2417–2433. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Messer J, Goodman R, Rowe R, Meltzer H, Maughan B. Preadolescent conduct problems in girls and boys. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:184–191. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000186403.13088.d8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Miller P, Sperry LL. The socialization of anger and aggression. Merrill-Palmer-Quarterly. 1987;33:1–33. [Google Scholar]
  48. Miller-Johnson S, Winn DM, Coie J, Maumary-Gremaud A, Hyman C, Terry R, Lochman J. Motherhood during the teen years: A developmental perspective on risk factors for childbearing. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:85–100. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Moffitt TE. Adolescent-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100:674–701. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Moffitt TE. Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review and a research agenda. In: Lahey BB, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, editors. Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 49–75. [Google Scholar]
  51. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Dickson N, Silva P, Stanton W. Childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset antisocial conduct problems in males: Natural history from ages 3 to 18. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:399–424. [Google Scholar]
  52. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M, Silva P. Sex differences in antisocial behavior: Conduct disorder, delinquency and violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  53. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development. 2007;16:361–388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Murray-Close D, Ostrov JM, Crick NR. A short-term longitudinal study of growth or relational aggression during middle childhood: Associations with gender, friendship intimacy, and internalizing problems. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:187–203. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nagin DS. Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semiparametric, group-based approach. Psychological Methods. 1999;4:139–157. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.6.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nagin DS. Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  57. Nagin DS, Tremblay RE. Parental and early childhood predictors of persistent physical aggression in boys from kindergarten to high school. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:389–394. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Familial factors associated with characteristics of non-maternal care for infants. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1997;59:389–408. [Google Scholar]
  59. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Non-maternal care and family factors in early development: An Overview of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2002;22:457–492. [Google Scholar]
  60. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2004;69 doi: 10.1111/j.0037-976x.2004.00312.x. (Whole No.4, Serial No. 278). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Poulton R, Sears MR, Thomson WM, Caspi A. Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:673–716. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Parker JG, Asher SR. Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology. 1993;29:611–621. [Google Scholar]
  63. Patterson GR, Forgatch M, Yoerger K, Stoolmiller M. Variables that initiate and maintain an early-onset trajectory for juvenile offending. Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10:531–547. doi: 10.1017/s0954579498001734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pepler DJ, Craig W. A peek behind the fence: naturalistic observations of aggressive children with remote audiovisual recording. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31:548–553. [Google Scholar]
  65. Pianta RC. Student-Teacher Relationship Scale. Odessa, FL: PAR; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  66. Reynell J. Reynell Developmental Language Scales (U.S. Edition) Los Angeles: Western Psychological Service; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  67. Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York: Wiley; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  68. Rubin DB. Multiple imputation after 18+ years. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1996;91:473–489. [Google Scholar]
  69. Rutter M. Crucial paths from risk indicator to causal mechanism. In: Lahey BB, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, editors. Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 3–25. [Google Scholar]
  70. Schaeffer CM, Petras H, Ialongo N, Masyn KE, Hubbard S, Poduska J, Kellam S. A comparison of girls’ and boys’ aggressive-disruptive behavior trajectories across elementary school: Prediction to young adult antisocial outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:500–510. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:147–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Serbin LA, Peters PL, McAffer VJ, Schwartzman AE. Childhood aggression and withdrawal as predictors of adolescent pregnancy, early parenthood, and environmental risk for the next generation. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 1991;23:318–331. [Google Scholar]
  73. Shaw DS, Gilliom M, Ingoldsby EM, Nagin DS. Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Shaw DS, Winslow EB, Owens EB, Vondra JI, Cohn JF, Bell RQ. The development of early externalizing problems among children from low-income families: A transformational perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998;26:95–107. doi: 10.1023/a:1022665704584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Silverthorn P, Frick PJ. Developmental pathways to antisocial behavior: the delayed onset pathway in girls. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:101–126. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Snyder J, Reid J, Patterson GR. A social learning model of child and adolescent antisocial behavior. In: Lahey BB, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, editors. Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 27–48. [Google Scholar]
  77. Stevenson J, Richman N, Graham PJ. Behaviour problems and language abilities at three years and behavioural deviance at eight years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1985;26:215–230. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1985.tb02261.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Thompson R. Early socio-personality development. In: Eisenberg N, editor. Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development. 5th edition. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 25–65. [Google Scholar]
  79. Tremblay RE. The development of aggressive behavior during childhood: What have we learned in the past century? International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2000;24:129–141. [Google Scholar]
  80. Underwood MK, Kupersmidt JB, Coie JD. Childhood peer sociometric status and aggression as predictors of adolescent childbearing. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 1996;6:201–223. [Google Scholar]
  81. Woodcock RW, Johnson MB. Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  82. Woodcock RW, Johnson MB. Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  83. Woodward LJ, Fergusson DM. Early conduct problems and later risk of teenage pregnancy in girls. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:127–142. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Zimmerman IL, Steiner VG, Pond RE. Preschool Language Scale-3. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1992. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES