Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Public Policy Law. 2015 Mar 23;21(2):134–144. doi: 10.1037/law0000039

Effects of Behavioral Genetic Evidence on Perceptions of Criminal Responsibility and Appropriate Punishment

Paul S Appelbaum 1, Nicholas Scurich 2, Raymond Raad 3
PMCID: PMC4521637  NIHMSID: NIHMS672233  PMID: 26240516

Abstract

Demonstrations of a link between genetic variants and criminal behavior have stimulated increasing use of genetic evidence to reduce perceptions of defendants’ responsibility for criminal behavior and to mitigate punishment. However, because only limited data exist regarding the impact of such evidence on decision makers and the public at large, we recruited a representative sample of the U.S. adult population (n=960) for a web-based survey. Participants were presented with descriptions of three legal cases and were asked to: determine the length of incarceration for a convicted murderer; adjudicate an insanity defense; and decide whether a defendant should receive the death penalty. A fully crossed, between-participants, factorial design was used, varying the type of evidence (none, genetic, neuroimaging, both), heinousness of the crime, and past criminal record, with sentence or verdict as the primary outcome. Also assessed were participants’ apprehension of the defendant, belief in free will, political ideology, and genetic knowledge. Across all three cases, genetic evidence had no significant effects on outcomes. Neuroimaging data showed an inconsistent effect in one of the two cases in which it was introduced. In contrast, heinousness of the offense and past criminal record were strongly related to participants’ decisions. Moreover, participants’ beliefs about the controllability of criminal behavior and political orientations were significantly associated with their choices. Our findings suggest that neither hopes that genetic evidence will modify judgments of culpability and punishment nor fears about the impact of genetic evidence on decision makers are likely to come to fruition.

Keywords: genetic evidence, mitigation, insanity defense, neuroimaging evidence, sentencing


A variety of data supports the likely influence of genetic variables on criminal and other anti-social behaviors. Sibling and twin studies have provided evidence of familial aggregation of criminal activity, with Rhee and Waldman's (2002) much-cited meta-analysis of 51 studies showing that additive genetic influences account for 32% of the variance, and interactions among genes for another 9%. Two large twin studies estimated heritabilities of 37-57% for 5 kinds of aggressive behaviors (Yeh, Coccaro, & Jacobson, 2010), and 67% for antisocial behavior (Tuvblad, Narusyte, Grann, Sarnecki, & Lichtenstein, 2011). Work is ongoing to identify genes that may be linked to anti-social behavior, including monoamine oxidase A (MAOA), catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), dopamine transporter (DAT1), dopamine receptor (DRD2 and DRD 4), and serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) (Ferguson & Beaver, 2009; Iofrida, Plaumbo, & Pellegrini, 2014), although many of the findings remain a subject of contention (Vassos, Collier, & Fazel, 2014). It seems likely that data regarding genes associated with increased risk for criminal behavior will continue to appear (Tiihonen et al., 2014).

However, particular attention has been paid to monoamine oxidase A (MAOA), an enzyme that degrades monoamine neurotransmitters. After suggestive evidence of a link between absent MAOA activity and criminal behavior (Brunner, Nelen, Breakefield, Ropers, & van Oost, 1993), Caspi et al. (2002) took advantage of a longitudinal epidemiologic study of a birth cohort in Dunedin, NZ to explore the phenomenon further. Examining high- and low-activity polymorphisms in the promoter region of the gene on the X chromosome in males, they found no effect of MAOA genotype per se, but did find evidence for an interaction between a history of childhood maltreatment and MAOA status – a gene-environment interaction. Subjects with an allele associated with reduced MAOA production who had a history of childhood maltreatment comprised only 12% of the sample, but accounted for 44% of convictions for violent crime. Subsequent studies in males largely have confirmed (Aslund et al., 2011; Choe, Shaw, Hyde & Forbes, 2014; Derringer, Krueger, Irons, & Iacono, 2010; Ducci et al., 2008; Fergusson, Boden, Horwood, Miller, & Kennedy, 2011; Foley et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2004; Kim-Cohen et al., 2006; Nilsson et al., 2006; Reif et al., 2007; Widom & Brzustowicz, 2006), but in some cases failed to confirm (Haberstick et al., 2005; 2014; Huizinga et al., 2006; Prichard, Mackinnon, Jorm, & Easteal, 2008; Reti et al., 2011; Tiihonen et al., 2014; Vanyukov et al., 2007; Young et al., 2006), these findings. Integration of these results is complicated by variation in methods and measures across groups; however, three meta-analyses—the most recent and largest of which included 20 studies involving males with over 5800 participants—support the association between MAOA and anti-social behavior by maltreated boys (Byrd & Manuck, 2014; Kim-Cohen et al., 2006; Taylor & Kim-Cohen, 2007).4 Further supporting this finding, the same gene-environment interaction was found to be associated with symptoms of antisocial personality disorder (Beach et al., 2010). Mechanisms for the differential effects of MAOA alleles are currently being elucidated (Alia-Klein et al., 2008; Buckholtz & Meyer-Lindenberg, 2014).

Within only a few years of the initial report of a link between MAOA and antisocial behavior in humans, the legal profession was aware of the data and beginning to explore their implications for the criminal justice system. Hope was expressed that such data could support claims for exculpation or leniency in sentencing on the grounds that the defendant was less able than most people to control his behavior (Johnson, 1998), perhaps undermining traditional legal notions of free will (Jones, 2003) and contributing to a reorientation of the criminal justice system from punishment to rehabilitation (Friedland, 1997). By 1995, a convicted murderer sought unsuccessfully to have his death sentence overturned on the basis that the trial court had refused to authorize payment for a test of his MAOA allele (Mobley v. State, 1995). Soon some forensic evaluators began to obtain MAOA data routinely in serious criminal cases (Bernet, Vnencak-Jones, Farahany, & Montgomery, 2007), and reports appeared of sentence reductions on the basis of genetic evidence in Italy (Feresin, 2009; Greely, 2011) and the U.S. (Denno, 2011). A lively debate continues in the legal and philosophical literatures on the relevance of genetic data to claims for exculpation and mitigation, centered on whether the genetic findings are closely enough linked to excusing or mitigating conditions traditionally recognized by the law (Baum, 2011; Farahany, 2009; Morse, 2011; Slobogin, 2014).

Surveys of legal cases suggest that courts are more receptive to genetic data than had been thought, although most such data currently are derived from family histories rather than genetic tests. Genetic information is being introduced for a wide range of purposes, including support for diagnostic conclusions, as well as arguments for mitigation (Denno, 2011; Farahany, 2011; Stix, 2014). A study of published cases between 1994 and 2011 found some evidence that the introduction of behavioral genetic evidence in courts is becoming more frequent, and is being met with less resistance, at least at the sentencing phase of trials (Denno, 2011).

The effects that behavioral genetic data will have on criminal proceedings depend on the attitudes and beliefs of the parties involved in criminal court proceedings (judges, juries, and others), and ultimately on public beliefs. Not only does the public constitute the pool from which jurors are drawn, but even for those determinations that are not made by jurors in most states (e.g., sentencing [King & Noble, 2004]), public views may guide the development of rules for the application of behavioral genetic data. However, prior to our pilot study (described in the following paragraph), there were no empirical data on public perceptions of behavioral genetics and its impact on criminal responsibility. One study had focused on potential jurors’ comprehension of genetic information, and was encouraging in that regard (Hans et al, 2011). Another study looked at state trial court judges, and found that after hearing a description of a case of aggravated battery by a psychopathic defendant, those judges who were exposed to genetic and other biological data to support the diagnosis of psychopathy indicated that they would impose less severe sentences on the perpetrator. The difference was significant; however, the absolute magnitude of the effect—one less year in prison, on average—was small (12.83 years [SEM 1.104] vs. 13.93 years [SEM 1.039]) (Aspinwall, Brown, & Tabery, 2012), and the methods and conclusions of the study have not gone unchallenged (Denno, 2013).

In our pilot study, a representative sample of the U.S. adult population (n=250) was given a vignette of an apparently impulsive homicide, and received one of four explanations for the perpetrator's behavior: simple impulsivity; a history of child abuse; a gene that alters brain function; and a combination of the abuse and genetic explanations. The genetic explanations did not lead to a significant change in the seriousness of the crime of which participants would convict the defendant, or the length of sentence imposed. However, genetic explanations did have the apparently paradoxical effect of making defendants seem more dangerous and therefore perhaps more deserving of longer sentences (Appelbaum & Scurich, 2014).

There have also been efforts to study the impact of other kinds of neuroscientific information, and they have led to conflicting results. One widely cited study suggested that neuroscientific information, even when irrelevant to the issue in question, lent credibility to poor explanations of behavioral phenomena among non-experts (Weisberg, Keil, Goodstein, Rawson, & Gray, 2008). Using a somewhat different paradigm, another research group showed that brain images, but not other sorts of illustrations, increased perceptions of the quality of scientific reasoning in articles about scientific advances (McCabe & Castel, 2008). More recently, however, several studies with more precise methodologies have failed to replicate those findings (Farah & Hook, 2013; Gruber & Dickerson, 2012; Michael, Newman, Vuorre, Cumming, & Garry, 2013). Two studies of mock jurors being asked to make decisions about mens rea (culpable mental states) and sentencing have shown no incremental effect of brain images beyond the impact of verbal testimony regarding neuropsychological impairment (Greene & Cahill, 2012; Schweitzer et al., 2011). However, a more recent study of sentencing in capital trials found that neuroimages reduced perceptions of responsibility and sentences of death for defendants diagnosed with psychopathy, but increased responsibility in defendants with schizophrenia (Saks, Schweitzer, Aharoni, & Kiehl, 2014). Hence, the likely effect of neuroscientific evidence in legal settings is still unclear.

The Present Study

The growing attention to genetic evidence in the legal system calls for a more sophisticated examination of its potential impact. In the present paper we report the results of a large-scale study that used a representative sample of the U.S. population to look at the effect of behavioral genetic information. Participants were exposed to three different legal cases, in which we varied the ways such evidence may be used, the types of scientific evidence presented, and the characteristics of the offender and of the crime.

Two of the cases in this study deal with sentencing contexts (capital and non-capital), given suggestions that genetic evidence is most likely to be used at that phase of trial for purposes of mitigation (Denno, 2011). The third case is an insanity defense, with the goal of exploring the potential impact of genetic data in a traditional justification defense, including their use to support diagnostic claims, which appears to be common (Farahany, 2011). Since consideration of genetic evidence is likely to be integrated by decision makers into a larger “story,” including other characteristics of the defendant and the crime (Pennington & Hastie, 1986), we examined the interactions of such characteristics with the scientific evidence. In particular, because the impact of evidence presented in mitigation or justification of an offense may be muted by the heinousness of the offense and the defendant's previous criminal record, we systematically varied these characteristics. (There are reasons to believe that a history of prior violence, in particular, may be likely to affect sentencing decisions, given that it may be perceived as a proxy for future dangerousness (Blume, Johnson, & Sundby, 2008; Garvey, 1998)). Finally, given that evolving practice appears to include presentation of both genetic and neuroimaging data (Bernet et al., 2007), we explored the impact of these types of evidence separately and jointly. To highlight the scientific evidence for our participants, we used several types of images: neuroimages showing impairment in the frontal lobes, which may be associated with increased impulsivity; images of genetic test results (i.e., a “Manhattan plot” and a figure showing a deletion in one of the defendant's chromosomes), which we explained as indicating the presence of variants associated with heightened impulsivity or a psychiatric disorder respectively; and a genogram, which graphically displays family relationships and the penetrance of the condition in question.

As potential correlates of participants’ decisions, in addition to their demographic characteristics, we used measures of knowledge and attitudes that might explain their choices: measures of belief in free will, political ideology, and genetic knowledge. Although the literature on free will is complex, data suggest that greater belief in free will predicts more punitive approaches to sentencing (Sharif et al., 2014). Similarly, conservative political views, at least at the aggregate level, appear to be associated with longer prison sentences (Bowers, 1998) and greater use of the death penalty (McCann, 2008). Lastly, we hypothesized that greater familiarity with genetic concepts would make respondents more likely to see genetic testimony as mitigating or justifying criminal conduct.

Methods

Participants

Identification and recruitment of participants was conducted by YouGov, a survey research company that maintains a web-based panel of respondents. YouGov constructed a sample representative of the adult U.S. population with a two-stage sampling design. First, a sampling frame was constructed from the American Community Study (U.S. Census Bureau, 2010), with additional data from the Current Population voter supplement (U.S. Census Bureau, 2008) and the Pew Religious Life Study (Pew Research, 2007). From the frame, a stratified random sample was drawn similar in size to the desired study sample. At the second stage, the sampling algorithm behind the proprietary sampling system searched the opt-in panel (i.e., respondents to a generic invitation to participate in a survey) for participants who most closely matched the individuals in the randomly drawn target sample. The algorithm invites 2-3 matches for every respondent in the target frame. The matching criteria include age, race, gender, and education. The final sample (n=960) has the characteristics of the adult U.S. population.

Sample size was determined by an a priori power analysis. Assuming a medium effect size of Delta = 0.75 (Cohen, 1988; Delta, sometimes referred to as d, is the difference between the largest mean and the smallest mean, in units of the within-cell standard deviation) and Type I error of 5%, each case required more than 30 participants per condition to obtain power greater than 0.80. Based on these assumptions and given a maximum of 24 cells per case, the minimum-required sample size was 720; however, we opted to have 40 participants per cell, for a total of 960 participants. The demographic characteristics of the final sample are provided in Table 1. In addition, participants were asked to respond to an 18-item questionnaire testing their knowledge of basic genetics; mean and median percentage correct were both 83% (s.d.=10.9), indicating a good level of genetic knowledge among most participants. Participants also completed the free will subscale of the FAD-plus scale (Cronbach's alpha = .866), which measures beliefs about free will and related concepts (Paulhas & Carey, 2011). Higher scores indicate greater beliefs in free will. YouGov provided data on respondents’ self-described political orientation (from very liberal to very conservative) based on a standard question they ask of their panel members.

Table 1.

Characteristics of Participants (n=960)

Characteristic N %
Sex
    Male 462 48.1%
    Female 498 51.9%
Race
    White 694 72.3%
    Black 103 10.7%
    Hispanic 97 10.1%
    Asian 17 2.0%
    Native American 11 1.1%
    Mixed 23 2.4%
    Other 15 1.6%
Education
    < High school graduate 38 4.0%
    High school graduate 382 39.8%
Some college 222 23.1%
    Two-year college degree 72 7.5%
    Four-year college degree 157 16.4%
    Post-college education 89 9.3%
Marital status
    Married 458 47.7%
    Domestic partnership 42 4.4%
    Separated 13 1.4%
    Divorced 94 9.8%
    Widowed 46 4.8%
    Never married 307 32.0%
Employment status
    Full-time 342 35.6%
    Part-time 99 10.3%
    Currently unemployed 119 12.4%
    Retired 164 17.1%
    Permanently disabled 70 7.3%
    Homemaker 72 7.5%
    Student 67 7.0%
    Other 27 2.8%
Political orientation
    Very liberal or liberal 244 25.4%
    Moderate 280 29.2%
    Very conservative or conservative 335 34.9%
    Not sure 101 10.5%

Drafts of the case vignettes described below were reviewed for the realism of their portrayals of legal and mental health issues and for clarity by a panel of reviewers that included 5 attorneys (4 of them legal academics with relevant expertise), an experienced academic forensic psychiatrist, and an academic psychologist with expertise in issues related to evidence law. The final versions of the case vignettes were modified per their comments.

General Procedure

In this online study, participants were presented with descriptions of three different legal cases and asked to render a decision. One case asked participants to determine the length of incarceration for a convicted murderer (case 1); the second case had participants adjudicate whether a defendant should be found not guilty by reason of insanity (case 2); and the final case asked whether a defendant convicted of capital murder should receive the death penalty or life in prison without the possibility of parole (case 3). The approximate length of the stimulus for each case was 700 words. Whenever genetic or neuroimaging findings were introduced, they were accompanied by an image to enhance the likelihood that participants would attend to the key manipulations. The order in which the three cases were presented was randomized, and participants were randomly assigned to one experimental condition within each case. After rendering a decision in each case, participants answered eight questions about their reactions to the defendant. For example, participants were asked, “to what extent is [the defendant] an immoral person?” and “how dangerous is [the defendant] to the public?” All ratings were made on a 9-point Likert scale. Finally, participants provided general background and demographic information.

Case 1

Design

The experimental design was a 4 (explanation of behavior: impulsive; genetic; neuro; both) x 2 (heinousness of crime: low; high) x 3 (criminal history: none; non-violent; violent) fully crossed, between-participants factorial design. Participants read about a fracas that occurred outside a bar when the defendant provoked a fight because the victim had flirted with his girlfriend. During the course of the altercation, the defendant stabbed the victim, who was pronounced dead on the scene by the emergency response team. The defendant unsuccessfully claimed that the stabbing was done in self-defense. Participants were told that the defendant had been convicted of murder and was now awaiting sentencing.

There were three experimental manipulations in this case. First, the defense's proffered explanation of the murder was varied. Participants were randomly assigned to vignettes in which: the defense attorney claimed that it was simply an impulsive act (impulsive); a psychiatrist testified that the defendant had a rare gene that predisposed him to impulsive and violent behavior (genetic explanation); a psychiatrist testified that the results of brain imaging revealed that the defendant had abnormal activity in the front part of the brain that predisposed him to impulsive and violent behavior (neuro explanation); or a psychiatrist testified about both the genetic and neuro explanations (both). The genetic and neuro explanations were accompanied by images of genetic test results and MRI scans, respectively.

Two additional variables in the vignettes that were hypothesized to have an effect on participants’ sentencing decisions were also manipulated. The heinousness of the murder was varied so that there was either one stab wound that killed the victim instantly (low) or 17 stab wounds all over the victim's body including one in the eye (high). Second, in his remarks at the sentencing hearing, the prosecutor noted the defendant's history of criminal conduct: a previous conviction for shoplifting (non-violent); a previous conviction for assault (violent), or no previous convictions (none). In addition, the prosecutor argued that the defendant's actions proved that he is a “bad actor” who should be punished for his behavior regardless of the explanations offered.

After reading the materials, participants were asked to indicate the appropriate prison sentence for the defendant on a 1-60 year scale. Participants then completed the 8 reaction questions.

Results

The median prison sentence selected by our respondents was 24 years with an interquartile range (IQR) of 27. Figure 1, a histogram of participants’ responses to the prison sentence question, demonstrates substantial variation in the length of confinement chosen.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Prison sentences imposed by participants (n = 960) in case 1.

A 3-way ANOVA was conducted to examine if prison sentences vary as a function of the experimental manipulations. The ANOVA detected a main effect for heinousness of crime (F(1, 960) = 37.23, p < .001, ηp2 = .038) and criminal history (F(2, 960) = 3.56, p < .05, ηp2 = .008). The main effect for explanation of behavior (i.e., impulsive, genetic, neuro, or both) was not significant (F(3, 960) < 1), nor were any of the interactions (all ps > .10). The heinousness of the crime affected prison sentences, such that the high heinous killing resulted in significantly longer sentences (mean = 29.82, 95% CI = [28.2, 31.4]) than the low heinous killing (mean = 22.94, 95%CI = [21.4, 24.5]). Compared to no criminal history, both non-violent and violent criminal histories resulted in longer prison sentences (means = 24.33, 27.07, and 27.75, respectively). A post-hoc Bonferroni test indicated that the only significant contrast is between the no criminal history and the violent criminal history (p < .05); the difference between no criminal history and a non-violent criminal history, as well as between non-violent and violent criminal histories, is not significant (p > .10).

Several of the subject variables correlated with the length of prison sentences. Belief in free will as measured by the FAD-plus was correlated .237 (p < .001) with the sentence, indicating that the length of confinement imposed increased as participants’ belief in free will increased. Genetic knowledge was negatively, but weakly, correlated (r = -.082, p < .01) with prison sentences, with greater knowledge about genetics associated with shorter prison sentences; and political ideology was positively, albeit weakly, correlated with prison sentences (r = .095, p < .001), with higher levels of conservatism related to longer prison sentences.

Combining the responses to the eight reaction questions into a single composite score resulted in a Cronbach's alpha of .761, indicating the presence of a latent construct (see DeVellis, 2003). We refer to this construct as ‘apprehension of defendant,’ where higher scores equal greater apprehension of the defendant. A scale reliability analysis indicated that removing any particular item would not significantly increase alpha. The Pearson correlation of apprehension of defendant and the prison sentence was .615 (p < .001).

A 3-way ANOVA with heinousness of crime, explanation of behavior, and criminal history as the independent variables and apprehension of the defendant as the dependent variable detected a significant main effect only for heinousness of crime (F(1, 960) = 22.79, p < .001, ηp2 = .024). The main effects for explanation of behavior, criminal history and all the interactions were not statistically significant (all ps > .10).

Discussion

There was substantial variation in the sentences selected by our participants in response to this vignette describing an unpremeditated murder, but the explanation of the defendant's behavior that was offered by the defense had no significant effect on their choices. However, although respondents were not swayed by the scientific evidence of genetic or brain-related influences on the defendant's behavior, they were impacted by more traditional types of evidence. Specifically, heinousness of the offense and past criminal history were significant predictors of the length of sentence, and heinousness also predicted the degree of apprehension of the defendant manifested by respondents. Despite the concern sometimes expressed that genetic and neuroimaging evidence might be a “double-edged sword” (Aspinwall, Brown & Tabery, 2012) that could be turned against the defendant who introduce them, we found neither a positive nor negative effect of such evidence. Although jurors in most states are not asked to make sentencing determinations, these findings suggest that the general public is not inclined to see genetic or neuroimaging evidence that the defendant is at heightened risk of impulsive behavior as warranting modification of sentences.

Case 2

Design

The experimental design was a 2 (heinousness of crime: low; high) x 2 (type of test: medical or family tree) x 2 (purpose of test: diagnosis or impulsivity) x 3 (history of mental illness) fully crossed, between-participants factorial design. Participants read about a case in which a 32-year old man made several affectionate advances towards a female co-worker. All the advances were rejected. The final rejection sent the defendant into a spiral of uncharacteristic behavior, including extreme anxiety, sleep disturbance, disorganized speech, and profligacy with money. One night the defendant spotted the co-worker at a local gas station. He approached and tried to hug her but she pushed him away. He grabbed her by the hair, dragged her into his truck, and drove off. The entire event was captured by surveillance cameras and viewed by three eyewitnesses. The victim's body was discovered two days later dumped by the side of a road. The defendant was arrested and charged with kidnapping and murder, to which he pled not guilty by reason of insanity (NGRI). A psychiatrist called by the defense testified that the defendant met the criteria for NGRI under a modified version of the American Legal Institute's formulation for insanity (i.e., a cognitive or volitional impairment claim).

There were four experimental manipulations in this study. First, the heinousness of the crime was manipulated: the victim was either shot once in the head (low) or shot once each in the head and the genitals (high). Second, the psychiatrist conducted one of two types of genetic investigations, either a laboratory test for genetic abnormalities (lab test) or an analysis of the defendant's family tree (family tree); surveys of reported cases suggest that genetic evidence is introduced in both forms in criminal trials (Denno, 2011; 2013). In both conditions, an image of the genetic test was presented. Third, the purpose of the test was varied: the test was either conducted to buttress a diagnosis of bipolar disorder (diagnosis) or to explain directly the defendant's impulsive behavior (impulsivity). Finally, the defendant's history of mental illness was described as consisting of: no prior episode (none); a similar but less severe episode (mild); or a very similar episode (severe).

After reading the case materials, participants were given judicial instructions regarding the test of insanity to be applied. They were also told that if the defendant was found NGRI he would be sent to a maximum security psychiatric hospital. Participants then indicated whether they would find the defendant guilty of murder or NGRI. On a separate screen, participants who chose a guilty verdict were asked for how many years the defendant should be sentenced to prison, while those who chose NGRI were asked the length of time he should be confined to the hospital, both on a 1-60 year scale. Finally, participants answered the 8 reaction questions.

Results

A majority of participants (62%, n = 593) found the defendant guilty. With regard to the prison sentence if the defendant were convicted, the median response was 45 years (IQR = 30, range 1-60), whereas the median length of hospital incarceration if the defendant were found NGRI was 20 years (IQR = 35, range 1-60).

A binary logistic regression with type of test, purpose of test, heinousness of crime, and history of mental illness as the independent variables and verdict (i.e., guilty or NGRI) as the dependent variable was not statistically significant (χ2= 9.37, df = 7, p = .23). This indicates that the experimental manipulations cannot explain variance in participants’ verdicts. In other words, the manipulations did not affect the likelihood that participants would find the defendant NGRI.

Participants who found the defendant guilty had significantly greater apprehension of the defendant (mean = 6.26, 95% CI [6.17, 6.34]) than participants who chose a NGRI verdict (mean = 4.52, 95% CI [4.41, 4.63]), t(958) = 23.89, p < .001). Additionally, participants who found the defendant guilty compared with those who found him NGRI had stronger beliefs in free will (mean = 4.17, 95% CI [4.11, 4.22] vs. 3.58, 95% CI [3.51, 3.66], t(958) = 12.37, p < .001) and conservative political ideology (mean = 3.63, 95% CI [3.52, 3.75] vs. 3.17, 95% CI [3.02, 3.31], t(958) = 5.01, p < .001)), but lower levels of genetic knowledge (mean = .82, 95% CI [.81, .83] vs. .84, 95% CI [.83, .85], t(958) = -3.16, p = .002).

Combining the responses to the eight reaction questions into a single composite score resulted in a Cronbach's alpha of .789. A scale reliability analysis indicated that removing any particular item would not significantly increase alpha. An ANOVA with the experimental manipulations as the independent variables and ‘apprehension of defendant’ as the dependent variable detected a significant main effect for the purpose of the test (F(1, 960) = 3.78, p < .05, ηp2 = .004). In general, when the test was conducted to buttress a diagnosis, the apprehension of defendant scores were lower (mean = 5.5), compared with when the test was conducted to explain impulsivity (mean = 5.7). A 2-way interaction between the heinousness of the crime and the type of evidence (i.e., laboratory vs. family history) was also detected (F(1, 960) = 5.14, p < .05, ηp2 = .005) (Figure 2). None of the other main effects or interactions was significant (all ps > .10).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The interactive effect of the type of genetic test and the heinousness of crime on apprehension of the defendant. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

As is apparent, the type of genetic evidence has an effect but only when the crime is low in heinousness. With low heinousness, the laboratory test engendered more apprehension of the defendant than the family tree. However, when the crime was high in heinousness, the type of genetic evidence had no effect. Note that the only difference between the heinousness conditions is an extra gunshot wound to the victim's genital area.

Discussion

Empirical data on the use of the insanity defense indicate that most cases that go to trial end with a guilty verdict (74% in one large study; Silver, Cirincione, & Steadman, 1994), which was the outcome for the majority of respondents here. That outcome was not affected by the introduction of genetic evidence either to support the diagnosis of bipolar disorder or to speak directly to the defendant's ability to control his behavior. Nor did it matter whether the testimony about the defendant's genetic predisposition was based on genetic testing conducted in a laboratory or on his family history. In contrast to the previous case, the degree of heinousness of the offense had no impact on the verdict, perhaps because some respondents took it as evidence that the crime was driven by mental illness while others saw it as indicating vengefulness. Neither did heinousness directly affect respondents’ apprehension of the defendant, although it did show an effect in modifying the impact of the type of genetic evidence on apprehension of the defendant. Once more, there was no evidence of a paradoxical effect of genetic evidence, i.e., its introduction did not result in higher rates of guilty verdicts—although evidence of a genetic basis for increased impulsivity did lead to heightened apprehension of the defendant.

Case 3

Design

The experimental design was a 2 (heinousness of crime: low; high) x 3 (criminal history: none; non-violent; violent) x 4 (explanation of behavior: impulsive; genetic; neuro; both) fully crossed, between-participants factorial design. Participants were told that they would be reading about a case in which they would be asked to consider the death penalty for the defendant. Consistent with federal guidelines for death qualification, participants were asked, “Do you have a conviction against the death penalty so strong that you could not take an oath to fairly try a death penalty case and follow the law?” and were asked to respond “yes” or “no.” They then read a scenario in which three individuals robbed a bank at gun point. Responding to a silent alarm, a police officer entered the bank and demanded that the suspects “drop their weapons.” The defendant fatally shot the police officer from behind. The trio was apprehended as they fled, and the defendant was subsequently convicted of first-degree murder. Participants were asked to determine whether the defendant should receive the death penalty or whether there were mitigating factors that called for leniency, in which case he would be sentenced to life in prison without parole (LWOP).

There were three manipulations in this study. First, the heinousness of the murder was manipulated: the victim was either shot once, fell to the ground and died (low), or he was shot once and fell to the ground, at which time the defendant stood over him and fired until the weapon was empty (high). Second, a presentencing report indicated that the defendant's criminal history included: no previous convictions; convictions for disorderly conduct and graffiti (non-violent); or convictions for assault and rape (violent). Finally, the proffered explanation and putative mitigation for the defendant's conduct that was offered by his attorney was varied: the attorney claimed it was simply an impulsive act (impulsive explanation); a psychiatrist testified that the defendant had a rare gene that “…led the defendant to act on impulses without thinking them through, and this is certainly a result of his biological makeup” (genetic explanation); a psychiatrist testified that the results of a brain imaging scan found an abnormality with the same effect (neuro explanation); or the psychiatrist offered both the genetic and neuro explanations (both). The genetic and neuro explanations were accompanied by images.

After reading the case materials, participants were given sentencing instructions and asked to determine a sentence, which was either LWOP or the death penalty. Finally, participants answered the eight reaction questions.

Results

A large majority of participants (80%, n = 763) indicated that they did not have a principled objection to the use of the death penalty that would affect their decision making in the case. In other words, 80% of the participants were qualified to adjudicate a death penalty case, commonly referred to as “death-qualified jurors.” Since non-death-qualified jurors who are identified during voir dire are typically excluded from death penalty hearings (Witherspoon v. Illinois, 1968), only death-qualified participants are included in the analyses reported below.

58% (n = 448) of participants chose to impose the death penalty. A binary logistic regression was conducted with heinousness of murder, criminal history, and explanation of behavior as the independent variables and sentence (i.e., LWOP or death) as the dependent variable. The model was significant (χ2= 41.03, df = 12, p < .001). There was a significant main effect for heinousness of the crime, with participants in the high heinousness condition being over 2.5 times more likely to impose death (exp(B) = 2.52, 95% CI [1.65, 2.86], Wald = 18.07, df = 1, p < .001). There was also a main effect for previous convictions (Wald = 9.31, p < .01), with participants in the non-violent and violent conditions being nearly 2 times more likely to impose the death penalty than participants in the no-previous-convictions condition (exp(B) = 1.85, 95% CI [1.17, 2.91], Wald = 6.90, df = 1, p < .01; exp(B) = 1.79, 95% CI [1.15, 2.72], Wald = 6.64, df = 1, p < .01, respectively). There was also a main effect for explanation (Wald = 8.53, p < .05); the only significant effect pertained to the neuro explanation, with participants in this condition being less likely to impose death than participants in the impulsive condition (exp(B) = .56, 95% CI [.35, .87], Wald = 6.47, df = 1, p < .05).

Combining the responses to the eight reaction questions into a single composite score resulted in a Cronbach's alpha of .769. A scale reliability analysis indicated that removing any particular item would not significantly increase alpha. Participants who voted to impose the death penalty had significantly greater apprehension of the defendant (mean = 7.72, 95% CI [6.54, 6.72]) than participants who chose LWOP (mean = 6.66, 95% CI [6.54, 6.77]), t(761) = 13.66, p < .001, as well as greater beliefs in free will (mean = 4.15, 95% CI [4.09, 4.22] vs. 3.84, 95% CI [3.77, 3.92], t(761) = 6.215, p < .001). They also showed higher levels of conservatism (mean = 3.80, 95% CI [3.68, 3.93] vs. 3.324, 95% CI [3.17, 3.47), t(761) = 4.82, p < .001). There were no differences in genetic knowledge between participants who imposed the death penalty and those who voted for LWOP (t(761) = .355, p = .723).

An ANOVA with the experimental manipulations as the independent variables and ‘apprehension of defendant’ as the dependent variable detected a significant main effect for the heinousness of the crime (F(1, 763) = 7.28, p < .01, ηp2 = .01) and for the explanation of behavior (F(1, 763) = 6.50, p < .001, ηp2 = .037), and a 2-way interaction between the heinousness of murder and explanation of behavior (F(1, 763) = 2.63, p < .05, ηp2 = .011) (Figure 3). None of the other main effects or interactions were significant (all ps > .10).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Apprehension of the defendant as a function of the heinousness of the crime and the proffered explanation of his behavior. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

The interaction results from the impulsive explanation and high heinousness condition, which engendered inordinate apprehension of the defendant. That is, there are no differences in apprehension (ps > .10 for all orthogonal contrasts) among the various explanations in the low heinousness condition (the left column). Nor are there differences in the high heinousness condition (the right column) among the genetic, neuro and both explanation conditions (ps > .10); the only difference is in the impulsive explanation and the other explanations (p < .05) in the high heinousness condition.

Discussion

Death penalty hearings appear to be the context in which behavioral genetic data are most likely to be introduced in court (Denno, 2011), hence the results of this vignette have direct relevance to a common, high-stakes scenario. It is of considerable interest, therefore, that the results of introducing genetic evidence resemble those seen in the non-capital-sentencing context of Case 1. Genetic evidence did not affect the decision regarding imposition of the death penalty or LWOP, but again both the heinousness of the crime and past criminal history had significant effects. For the first time, however, we also identified an effect of neuroimaging evidence, albeit a limited one: introduction of neuroimaging evidence led to a reduction in the likelihood of the death penalty compared with an argument about impulsivity in the absence of scientific evidence; however, the effect of neuroimaging evidence was not significantly different from the effect of genetic evidence alone or genetic and neuroimaging evidence combined. Neither type of scientific evidence enhanced apprehension of the defendant, although an argument that the defendant's behavior represented impulsive (and by implication, unpremeditated) behavior did increase apprehension when the crime was particularly heinous.

General Discussion

Given the growing literature about genetic influences on violent and other anti-social behaviors, and the increasing use of genetic evidence in court, this study examined the likely impact of such evidence on sentencing and insanity defense determinations by a representative sample of the U.S. adult population. Across all three cases, genetic evidence had no significant effects on the outcomes of the case—length of prison sentence, findings of insanity, or imposition of the death penalty. This finding is consistent with the results of our earlier, pilot study, which similarly found no impact of genetic evidence on determinations of a defendant's degree of culpability or the sentence imposed in a non-capital case of homicide (Appelbaum & Scurich, 2014). In contrast, heinousness of the offense and past criminal record were strongly related to the participants’ decisions. Taken together our data suggest that the impact of genetic evidence may have been considerably overestimated, especially when compared with characteristics of the crime and the offender's history, at least given current public knowledge of and attitudes towards genetics.

Because we included conditions involving neuroimaging evidence in two of the cases, both alone and in combination with genetic evidence, we were able to compare the effect of another type of scientific evidence, one more commonly introduced in court than genetic data (Farahany, 2011). In addition, especially in death penalty cases, it appears increasingly common for genetic and neuroimaging data to be used in tandem (Bernet et al., 2007), which we explored in these cases as well. The previous literature shows varying and inconsistent effects of neuroimaging evidence in criminal cases (Greene & Cahill, 2012; Schweitzer et al., 2011; Saks et al., 2014), resembling our findings here. Neuroimaging evidence did not affect the outcome in the non-capital sentencing case and had an inconsistent effect in the death penalty case, where it reduced the likelihood of a death penalty compared with an impulsivity claim, but not when neuroimaging evidence was paired with genetic evidence. Here too it appears that fears that neuroimages, with their graphic portrayal of brain function, would come to dominate jurors’ decisions were considerably overblown (Farah & Hook, 2013).

In addition to genetic evidence failing to alter the legal outcomes in these cases, it appeared to have little impact on participants’ perceptions of the defendants involved. This is in contrast to our previous study, in which genetic evidence of a propensity to impulsive and violent behavior increased fear of the defendant (Appelbaum & Scurich, 2014). In two of our three cases here, however, there was no significant effect of genetic evidence on respondents’ scores on the apprehension scale. The exception was the insanity defense case, which showed a complex pattern, with genetic data suggesting a propensity to behave impulsively inducing greater apprehension, but not genetic data supporting the defendant's diagnosis of bipolar disorder. In addition, in that case genetic test data were more fear-inducing than family history data. Thus, at most we can describe an inconsistent effect on perceptions of the defendant, suggesting that our sample of the general population does not hold the kind of beliefs in genetic determinism that are sometimes attributed to ordinary people, a finding consistent with other studies of popular views (Condit, 1999).

As expected, participants with higher levels of belief in free will and more conservative political views consistently chose more punitive options: longer prison sentences, fewer NGRI verdicts, more death sentences. Higher levels of genetic knowledge, however, were associated with shorter sentences and more NGRI findings, although not with fewer death penalty verdicts. Thus, participants’ commitments to beliefs about the controllability of criminal behavior and political orientations had a much greater impact on the outcome of these cases than did either genetic or neuroimaging evidence.

The limitations of this study include those commonly associated with online surveys. The identity of the person completing the survey can never be known with certainty, and may not be the same as the person to whom the invitation was sent. In addition, in the absence of direct observation, the care with which the survey is completed is unknown. However, research suggests that Internet survey data tend to be of high quality, with good internal consistency and high test-retest reliability, yielding results similar to those obtained by traditional methods (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & John, 2004).

Moreover, the YouGov panel in this study provides a statistically representative sample of the general population—albeit not a true random sample—that would not otherwise be easy to obtain. With regard to methodologic concerns, the use of written vignettes is quite different from the extended oral presentations that would take place at trial. Whether our participants would have responded differently to realistic trial testimony or even to vignettes with somewhat different content (e.g., varying the characteristics of the perpetrators, victims, or experts) is unknown; we cannot rule out the possibility that our manipulations were less impactful than trial testimony or alternatively designed vignettes might have been. The concomitant use of genetic/neuroimaging findings and images makes it impossible for us to assess the distinct effects, if any, of either the findings or the images alone. Finally, we should note that although jurors are usually called upon for NGRI and death sentence determinations, in most states (with 6 exceptions (Hoffman, 2003)) jurors do not make non-capital sentencing decisions. Thus, with regard to Case 1 or the sentencing/hospitalization decision in Case 2, sentencing or reviewing judges may have responded differently to the situation presented than did our respondents. However, respondents’ views provide an exploratory perspective on public attitudes regarding the question of what impact genetic and neuroimaging data should have on such decisions, and arguably are a more sensitive measure of the impact of such evidence than dichotomous determinations of guilty/NGRI verdicts or sentences of death/life in prison.

What are the implications of our data for the use of psychiatric and behavioral genetic evidence in criminal trials? Assuming that our findings are replicated in future work, they suggest that the increasing resort to genetic evidence by criminal defense lawyers (Denno, 2011) may be unlikely to yield the desired results. Our sample of the general population—from which juries are drawn—declined to rely on genetic (or for the most part, neuroimaging) evidence in their determinations of culpability and sentencing. However, there was also no evidence of an adverse effect on defendants of genetic arguments (the so-called “double-edged sword”). Resort to psychiatric and behavioral genetic evidence seems to be most common in death-penalty cases (Denno, 2011), perhaps in part because sentencing rules may limit the introduction of evidence demonstrating an impaired capacity to control behavior in some other cases (e.g., U.S. Sentencing Commission, 2014). Moreover, in capital cases defense lawyers might correctly perceive that their clients have little to lose from putting their genetic propensities into evidence; the chance that some jurors may be influenced towards mitigation may be sufficient for the use of such evidence to continue. These data, though, suggest that prospects may be dim for the successful extension of this practice into other areas of the criminal law.

Future research on the use of genetic and neuroscientific data in court might build on these findings by using methods that more closely resemble actual trials and jury deliberations. That could include live presentations of arguments by advocates for each side or videotaped testimony by real or simulated witnesses. Allowing groups of participants to deliberate jointly would also yield a more ecologically valid context for the examination of the impact of genetic information in the criminal courts. Although we used static images of genetic test results and neuroimages to highlight the scientific evidence being presented, there may be more impactful ways of presenting the data. In addition, the effect of genetic data might be enhanced by quantifying its effect, which we did not do in this study; that is, the increase in the propensity for violent, anti-social or impulsive behavior could be presented in numerical terms (e.g., “a doubling of the risk of violence”) to see if concretizing the effect in that way yields a different outcome. Finally, the next wave of our work will explore the use of genetic evidence in other adjudicatory contexts, including juvenile court settings and disciplinary hearings. Perhaps in settings in which criminal punishment is not the prime consideration, mitigating arguments that rely on genetic data will have greater effect. At present, though, our data are fairly consistent in suggesting that the hopes for data on genetic propensities to impulsive violence to modify judgments of culpability and appropriate punishment appear to be unfulfilled.

Supplementary Material

S1

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the following colleagues for their review of the case vignettes used in this study: Richard Bonnie, JD; Deborah Denno, JD, PhD; Harold Edgar, JD; Carolyn Genovesi, JD; Steven K. Hoge, MD; John Monahan, PhD; Daniel Richman, JD.

This work was supported by funding from the National Human Genome Research Institute, P50HG007257.

Footnotes

4

Another recent meta-analysis of studies of the relationship between MAOA alleles and anti-social behavior, in contrast to the original findings of Caspi et al. (2002) but like some other recent studies (Tiihonen et al., 2014), demonstrated a positive main effect of the low activity alleles (Ficks & Waldman, 2014). In this meta-analysis, studies’ inclusion of a measure of childhood adversity to explore gene-environment interactions did not affect the significance or magnitude of the main effect. However, Byrd & Manuck (2014) in their meta-analysis showed that not all measures of childhood adversity were associated with the posited gene-environment interaction, only actual childhood maltreatment. Hence, Ficks & Waldman's failure to find an effect of including a gene-environment interaction is not necessarily in conflict with the conclusion that studies of maltreatment per se demonstrate such an effect. These dueling meta-analyses demonstrate the importance of attending to the details of study design, including the definition of variables, in assessing the evidence for gene -environment interaction.

References

  1. Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ, Kriplani A, Logan J, Tomasi D, Williams B, Fowler JS. Brain monoamine oxidase A activity predicts trait aggression. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28(19):5099–104. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0925-08.2008. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0925-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Appelbaum PS, Scurich N. Impact of behavioral genetic evidence on the adjudication of criminal behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law. 2014;42(1):91–100. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aslund C, Nordquist N, Comasco E, Leppert J, Oreland L, Nilsson KW. Maltreatment, MAOA, and delinquency: sex differences in gene-environment interaction in a large population-based cohort of adolescents. Behavior Genetics. 2011;41(2):262–72. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9356-y. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9356-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aspinwall LG, Brown TR, Tabery J. The double-edged sword: does biomechanism increase or decrease judges’ sentencing of psychopaths? Science. 2012;337(6096):846–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1219569. doi:10.1126/science.1219569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baum ML. The monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) genetic predisposition to impulsive violence: is it relevant to criminal trials? Neuroethics. 2013;6(2):287–306. doi:10.1007/s12152-011-9108-6. [Google Scholar]
  6. Beach SRH, Brody GH, Gunter TD, Packer H, Wernett P, Philibert RA. Child maltreatment moderates the association of MAOA with symptoms of depression and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Family Psychology. 2010;24(1):12–20. doi: 10.1037/a0018074. doi:10.1037/a0018074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bernet W, Vnencak-Jones CL, Farahany N, Montgomery SA. Bad nature, bad nurture, and testimony regarding MAOA and SLC6A4 genotyping at murder trials. Journal of Forensic Science. 2007;52:1362–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1556-4029.2007.00562.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1556-4029.2007.00562.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blume JH, Johnson SL, Sundby SE. Competent capital representation: the necessity of knowing and heeding what jurors tell us about mitigation. Hofstra Law Review. 2008;36:1035–65. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bowers DA. Giving people what they want: An exploratory analysis of felony sentencing in 49 states. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice. 1998;22:119–130. doi: 10.1080/01924036.1998.9678612. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brunner HG, Nelen M, Breakefield XO, Ropers HH, van Oost BA. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science. 1993;262(5133):578–80. doi: 10.1126/science.8211186. doi: 10.1126/science.8211186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buckholtz JW, Meyer-Lindenberg A. MAOA and the bioprediction of antisocial behavior: science fact and science fiction. In: Singh I, Sinnott-Armstrong WP, Savulescu J, editors. Bioprediction, biomarkers, and bad behavior: Scientific, legal, and ethical challenges. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 2014. pp. 131–152. [Google Scholar]
  12. Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality data? Perspectives in Psychological Science. 2011;6:3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Byrd AL, Manuck SB. MAOA, childhood maltreatment, and antisocial behavior: meta-analysis of a gene-environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry. 75:9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.004. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Choe DE, Shaw DS, Hyde LW, Forbes EE. Interactions between monoamine oxidase A and punitive discipline in African American and Caucasian men's antisocial behavior. Clinical Psychological Science, published online. 2014 Mar 14; doi: 10.1177/2167702613518046. doi: 10.1177/2167702613518046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen J. Statistical power analyses for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  16. Condit CM. How the public understands genetics: non-deterministic and non-discriminatory interpretations of the “blueprint” metaphor. Public Understanding of Science. 1999;8:169–80. doi: 10.1088/0963-6625/8/3/302. [Google Scholar]
  17. Denno DW. Courts’ increasing consideration of behavioral genetics evidence in criminal cases: results of a longitudinal study. Michigan State Law Review. 2011;2011:967–1047. [Google Scholar]
  18. Denno DW. What real-world criminal cases tell us about genetics evidence. Hastings Law Journal. 2013;64:1591–618. [Google Scholar]
  19. Derringer J, Krueger RF, Irons DE, Iacono WG. Harsh discipline, childhood sexual assault, and MAOA genotype: an investigation of main and interactive effects on diverse clinical externalizing outcomes. Behavior Genetics. 2010;40(5):639–48. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9358-9. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9358-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DeVellis RF. Scale development: Theory and applications. Vol. 26. Sage Publications; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ducci F, Enoch M-A, Hodgkinson C, Xu K, Catena M, Robin RW, Goldman D. Interaction between a functional MAOA locus and childhood sexual abuse predicts alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder in adult women. Molecular Psychiatry. 2008;13(3):334–47. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002034. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4002034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Farahany N, editor. The impact of behavioral genetics on criminal law. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  23. Farahany N. Research on the genetics of antisocial behavior and violence: implications for social control and criminal justice.. Presented at ELSI Congress; Chapel Hill, NC. April 12.2011. [Google Scholar]
  24. Farah MJ, Hook CJ. The seductive allure of “seductive allure.”. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2013;8(1):88–90. doi: 10.1177/1745691612469035. doi:10.1177/1745691612469035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Feresin E. Lighter sentence for murder with ‘bad genes.’. Nature. 2009 published online Oct. 30, doi:10.1038/news.2009.1050. [Google Scholar]
  26. Ferguson CJ, Beaver KM. Natural born killers: The genetic origins of extreme violence. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2009;14(5):286–294. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2009.03.005. [Google Scholar]
  27. Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ, Miller AL, Kennedy MA. MAOA, abuse exposure and antisocial behaviour: 30-year longitudinal study. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;198(6):457–63. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.086991. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.086991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ficks CA, Waldman ID. Candidate genes for aggression and antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of association studies of the 5HTTLPR and MAOA-uVNTR. Behavior Genetics. 2014;44:427–444. doi: 10.1007/s10519-014-9661-y. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9661-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Friedland SI. The criminal law implications of the human genome project: reimagining a genetically oriented criminal justice system. Kentucky Law Journal. 1997;86:303–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Foley DL, Eaves LJ, Wormley B, Silberg JL, Maes HH, Kuhn J, Riley B. Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase a genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61(7):738–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.7.738. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.7.738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Garvey SP. Aggravation and mitigation in capital cases: what do jurors think? Columbia Law Review. 1998;98:1538–76. doi: 10.2307/1123305. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gosling SD, Vazire S, Srivastava S, John OP. Should we trust web-based studies?—a comparative analysis of six preconceptions about Internet questionnaires. American Psychologist. 2004;59:93–104. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.93. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.59.2.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Greely H. Another “brain mitigation” criminal sentence from Italy. 2011 http://blogs.law.stanford.edu/lawandbiosciences/2011/09/03/another-brain-mitigation-criminal-sentence-from-italy/
  34. Greene E, Cahill BS. Effects of neuroimaging evidence on mock juror decision making. Behavioral Sciences & the Law. 2012;30(3):280–96. doi: 10.1002/bsl.1993. doi:10.1002/bsl.1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gruber D, Dickerson JA. Persuasive images in popular science: Testing judgments of scientific reasoning and credibility. Public Understanding of Science. 2012;21(8):938–48. doi: 10.1177/0963662512454072. doi:10.1177/0963662512454072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Haberstick BC, Lessem JM, Hopfer CJ, Smolen A, Ehringer MA, Timberlake D, Hewitt JK. Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) and antisocial behaviors in the presence of childhood and adolescent maltreatment. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2005;135B(1):59–64. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30176. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Haberstick BC, Lessem JM, Hewitt JK, Smolen A, Hopfer CJ, Halpern CT, Killeya-Jones LA, Boardman JD, Tabor J, Siegler IC, Williams RB, Mullan Harris K. MAOA genotype, childhood maltreatment, and their interaction in the etiology of adult antisocial behaviors. Biological Psychiatry. 2014;75(1):25–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hans VP, Kaye DH, Dann BM, Farley EJ, Albertson S. Science in the jury box: jurors’ comprehension of mitochondrial DNA evidence. Law and Human Behavior. 2011;35:60–71. doi: 10.1007/s10979-010-9222-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hoffman MB. The case for jury sentencing. Duke Law Journal. 2003;52:951–1010. [Google Scholar]
  40. Huang Y-Y, Cate SP, Battistuzzi C, Oquendo MA, Brent D, Mann JJ. An association between a functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter, impulsive traits and early abuse experiences. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(8):1498–505. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300455. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Huizinga D, Haberstick BC, Smolen A, Menard S, Young SE, Corley RP, Hewitt JK. Childhood maltreatment, subsequent antisocial behavior, and the role of monoamine oxidase A genotype. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60(7):677–83. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.022. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Iofrida C, Palumbo S, Pellegrini S. Molecular genetics and antisocial behavior: Where do we stand? Experimental Biology and Medicine, published online. 2014 Apr 24; doi: 10.1177/1535370214529508. doi:10.1177/1535370214529508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Johnson M. Genetic technology and its impact on culpability for criminal actions. Cleveland State Law Review. 1998;46:443–470. [Google Scholar]
  44. Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Taylor A, Williams B, Newcombe R, Craig IW, Moffitt TE. MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children's mental health: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry. 2006;11(10):903–13. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001851. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McCabe DP, Castel AD. Seeing is believing: the effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition. 2008;107(1):343–52. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McCann SJH. Societal threat, authoritarianism, conservatism, and U.S. state death penalty sentencing (1977-2004). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008;94(5):913–923. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.913. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Michael RB, Newman EJ, Vuorre M, Cumming G, Garry M. On the (non) persuasive power of a brain image. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2013;20(4):720–5. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0391-6. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0391-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mobley v. State. 455 S.E.2d 61 (Ga. 1995) [Google Scholar]
  49. Morse SJ. Genetics and criminal responsibility. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(9):378–380. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.009. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nilsson KW, Sjöberg RL, Damberg M, Leppert J, Ohrvik J, Alm PO, Oreland L. Role of monoamine oxidase A genotype and psychosocial factors in male adolescent criminal activity. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59(2):121–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.024. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Paulhus DL, Carey JM. The FAD–Plus: Measuring lay beliefs regarding free will and related constructs. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2011;93(1):96–104. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.528483. doi:10.1080/00223891.2010.528483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pennington N, Hastie R. Evidence evaluation in complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51(2):242–258. [Google Scholar]
  53. Pew Research Religious Landscape Survey. 2007 Available at: http://religions.pewforum.org/reports.
  54. Prichard Z, Mackinnon A, Jorm AF, Easteal S. No evidence for interaction between MAOA and childhood adversity for antisocial behavior. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2008;147B(2):228–32. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30581. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Reif A, Rösler M, Freitag CM, Schneider M, Eujen A, Kissling C, Retz W. Nature and nurture predispose to violent behavior: serotonergic genes and adverse childhood environment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(11):2375–83. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301359. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Reti IM, Xu JZ, Yanofski J, McKibben J, Uhart M, Cheng Y-J, Nestadt G. Monoamine oxidase A regulates antisocial personality in whites with no history of physical abuse. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2011;52(2):188–94. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.05.005. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128(3):490–529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.3.490. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Saks MJ, Schweitzer NJ, Aharoni E, Kiehl KA. The impact of neuroimages in the sentencing phase of capital trials. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies. 2014;11(1):105–131. doi: 10.1111/jels.12036. [Google Scholar]
  59. Schweitzer NJ, Saks MJ, Murphy ER, Roskies AL, Sinnott-Armstrong W, Gaudet LM. Neuroimages as evidence in a mens rea defense: no impact. Psychology, Public Policy and Law. 2011;17(3):357–393. doi: 10.1037/a0023581. [Google Scholar]
  60. Shariff AF, Greene JD, Karremans JC, Luguri JB, Clark J, Schooler JW, Baumeister RF, Vohs KD. Free will and punishment: a mechanistic view of human nature reduces retribution. Psychological Science, published online. 2014 Jun 10; doi: 10.1177/0956797614534693. 2014. doi: 10.1177/0956797614534693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Silver E, Cirincione C, Steadman HJ. Demythologizing inaccurate perceptions of the insanity defense. Law & Human Behavior. 1994;18(1):63–70. doi: 10.1007/bf01499144. [Google Scholar]
  62. Slobogin C. Bioprediction in criminal cases. In: Singh I, Sinnott-Armstrong WP, Savulescu J, editors. Bioprediction, biomarkers, and bad behavior: scientific, legal, and ethical challenges. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 2014. pp. 77–90. [Google Scholar]
  63. Stix G. My brain made me pull the trigger: Neuroscience-based defenses are flooding the courtroom. Scientific American Mind. 2014 May-Jun;25:14. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind0514-14b. [Google Scholar]
  64. Taylor A, Kim-Cohen J. Meta-analysis of gene-environment interactions in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19(4):1029–37. doi: 10.1017/S095457940700051X. doi:10.1017/S095457940700051X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tiihonen J, Rautiainen M-R, Ollila HM, Repo-Tiihonen E, Virkkunen M, Palotie A, Pietilainen O, Kristiansson K, Joukamaa M, Lauerma H, Saarela J, Tyni S, Vartiainen H, Paananen J, Goldman D, Paunio T. Genetic background of extreme violent behavior. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.130. advance online publication 28 October; doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Tuvblad C, Narusyte J, Grann M, Sarnecki J, Lichtenstein P. The genetic and environmental etiology of antisocial behavior from childhood to emerging adulthood. Behavior Genetics. 2011;41(5):629–40. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9463-4. doi:10.1007/s10519-011-9463-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. U.S. Census Bureau Current population survey, voting and registration supplement file. 2008 Nov; Available at: http://www.census.gov/prod/techdoc/cps/cpsnov08.pdf.
  68. U.S. Census Bureau American community survey, 2010 data release. 2010 Available at: http://www.census.gov/acs/www/data_documentation/2010_release/
  69. Sentencing Commission US. Guidelines Manual, Chap. 5, Section K.2.13. 2014 Available at: http://www.ussc.gov/guidelines-manual/2014/2014-chapter-5#5k213.
  70. Vanyukov MM, Maher BS, Devlin B, Kirillova GP, Kirisci L, Yu L-M, Ferrell RE. The MAOA promoter polymorphism, disruptive behavior disorders, and early onset substance use disorder: gene-environment interaction. Psychiatric Genetics. 2007;17(6):323–32. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32811f6691. doi:10.1097/YPG.0b013e32811f6691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Vassos E, Collier DA, Fazel S. Systematic meta-analysis and field synopsis of genetic association studies of violence and aggression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014;19:471–477. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.31. doi:10.1038/mp.2013.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Weisberg DS, Keil FC, Goodstein J, Rawson E, Gray JR. The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20(3):470–7. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20040. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Witherspoon v. Illinois, 391 U.S. 510. 1968.
  74. Widom CS, Brzustowicz LM. MAOA and the “cycle of violence:” childhood abuse and neglect, MAOA genotype, and risk for violent and antisocial behavior. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60(7):684–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.039. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Yeh MT, Coccaro EF, Jacobson KC. Multivariate behavior genetic analyses of aggressive behavior subtypes. Behavior Genetics. 2010;40(5):603–17. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9363-z. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9363-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Young SE, Smolen A, Hewitt JK, Haberstick BC, Stallings MC, Corley RP, Crowley TJ. Interaction between MAO-A genotype and maltreatment in the risk for conduct disorder: failure to confirm in adolescent patients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163(6):1019–25. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.6.1019. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.6.1019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1

RESOURCES