Significance
Autism is a pervasive disorder that broadly impacts perceptual, cognitive, social, and motor functioning. Across individuals, the disorder manifests with a large degree of phenotypic diversity. Here, we propose that autism symptomatology reflects alterations in neural computation. Using neural network simulations, we show that a reduction in the amount of inhibition occurring through a computation called divisive normalization can account for perceptual consequences reported in autism, as well as proposed changes in the extent to which past experience influences the interpretation of current sensory information in individuals with the disorder. A computational perspective can help bridge our understandings of the genetic/molecular basis of autism and its behavioral characteristics, providing insights into the disorder and possible courses of treatment.
Keywords: autism, neural computation, divisive normalization, E/I imbalance, Bayesian inference
Abstract
Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder that manifests as a heterogeneous set of social, cognitive, motor, and perceptual symptoms. This system-wide pervasiveness suggests that, rather than narrowly impacting individual systems such as affection or vision, autism may broadly alter neural computation. Here, we propose that alterations in nonlinear, canonical computations occurring throughout the brain may underlie the behavioral characteristics of autism. One such computation, called divisive normalization, balances a neuron’s net excitation with inhibition reflecting the overall activity of the neuronal population. Through neural network simulations, we investigate how alterations in divisive normalization may give rise to autism symptomatology. Our findings show that a reduction in the amount of inhibition that occurs through divisive normalization can account for perceptual consequences of autism, consistent with the hypothesis of an increased ratio of neural excitation to inhibition (E/I) in the disorder. These results thus establish a bridge between an E/I imbalance and behavioral data on autism that is currently absent. Interestingly, our findings implicate the context-dependent, neuronal milieu as a key factor in autism symptomatology, with autism reflecting a less “social” neuronal population. Through a broader discussion of perceptual data, we further examine how altered divisive normalization may contribute to a wide array of the disorder’s behavioral consequences. These analyses show how a computational framework can provide insights into the neural basis of autism and facilitate the generation of falsifiable hypotheses. A computational perspective on autism may help resolve debates within the field and aid in identifying physiological pathways to target in the treatment of the disorder.
Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder that is dramatically increasing in prevalence (Fig. 1). Recent data place the number of children being diagnosed with autism in the United States at 1 in 68, more than doubling in the last decade (1–4). The disorder is highly pervasive, affecting individuals at cognitive, motor, and perceptual levels. It is furthermore a “spectrum disorder,” with symptoms that manifest in varying degrees across individuals. This heterogeneity presents significant challenges to establishing a comprehensive characterization of the disorder.
Research investigating the genetic and molecular basis of autism implicates over 100 genes (5), many of which are involved in synaptic development and function (6–8). As such, one prominent hypothesis is that autism arises from a neurophysiological excitation-to-inhibition (E/I) imbalance (9, 10). However, the connection between an E/I imbalance and the behavioral characteristics of the disorder remains unclear. Considering the pervasive nature of autism, and the covariance of loosely related symptoms (11–14), one possibility is that an E/I imbalance widely affects neural computation, in turn giving rise to the broad behavioral symptoms recognized as autism.
Here, we propose that autism symptomatology arises from alterations in nonlinear, canonical computations occurring throughout the brain; in particular, divisive normalization, a computation that divides the activity of individual neurons by the combined activity of the neuronal population in which they are embedded. Divisive normalization inherently reflects the E/I balance, and is implicated in a wide range of processes ranging from sensory encoding to decision making (15–17). Using neural network simulations, we show that a reduction in the amount of inhibition that occurs through divisive normalization can account for perceptual consequences reported in the disorder, providing a bridge between an E/I imbalance and the behavioral characteristics of autism. The simulations further establish a link between divisive normalization and high-level theories about how autism may alter the influence of past experience on the interpretation of current sensory information (18–20). A key result of the simulations is the implication of the neuronal milieu (the contextual environment of neuronal population activity in which neurons are embedded) in autism. Specifically, autism-like symptomatology arises in the model when the influence of the population on the activity of individual neurons is reduced, in essence making the neurons less “social.” A broader discussion of behavioral data further suggests that alterations in divisive normalization may contribute to the phenotypic diversity of autism.
Evidence for an E/I Imbalance in Autism
In this section, we briefly discuss evidence linking autism to a neurophysiological E/I imbalance. Excitatory (e.g., glutamatergic) and inhibitory (e.g., GABAergic) neurons together establish an E/I balance that is essential for normal brain development and function (21). This E/I balance plays an important role in determining the timing of critical periods in neurodevelopment. For example, GABA-mediated inhibition is important for determining the critical period in primary visual cortex (22), and experience-dependent plasticity is altered in mice lacking GAD, an enzyme involved in converting glutamate to GABA (23). Such findings suggest that an E/I imbalance can alter neurodevelopment, but is this related to autism?
One hypothesis proposes that autism symptomatology arises from an increased E/I ratio (9, 10), which may explain the frequent comorbidity of the disorder and seizures (24). An E/I imbalance in autism may originate in several ways including increased glutamate activity (25), decreased GABA release (7, 8), or decreased numbers of GABA receptors (26). Consistent with the possibility of an E/I imbalance in autism, many of the susceptibility genes may be related to the E/I balance. A few examples include single-nucleotide polymorphisms at chromosome 6q21, which encodes a glutamate receptor (27), the gene for the β3 GABAA receptor subunit (28), and MeCP2, which is critical for GABAergic function (7).
An increased E/I ratio is also supported by biochemical analyses. For example, decreased GABA receptor protein subunits are reported in autism (29, 30). Histological analysis further shows decreased mRNA levels of the enzyme GAD in autism (31, 32), and mice lacking GAD have decreased GABA levels (23). In a mouse model of autism with increased translation of neuroligins, both an increased E/I ratio and autistic-like behaviors are observed (33). In the next section, we describe divisive normalization, a canonical neural computation that is inherently related to the balance of excitation and inhibition. We then use divisive normalization to establish a computational bridge between an increased E/I ratio and autism symptomatology.
Connecting the E/I Balance to Neural Computation
We refer to stereotyped functions occurring throughout the brain as “canonical computations.” One such computation is divisive normalization, which divides the net excitatory drive to a neuron by a measure of the population activity (15). The effect of divisive normalization on a single neuron’s response is described by the following equation:
[1] |
Here, R is the neuron’s response, is its net excitatory drive, (the “semisaturation constant”) determines the rate at which the neuron’s response saturates as increases, (the “suppressive field”) is the pooled activity of multiple neurons including the neuron being normalized, and is a gain term scaling the suppressive field. The suppressive field is a weighted sum of the excitatory drives across the population: , where is the excitatory drive to the ith neuron and is the corresponding weight (SI Appendix). The extent of neuronal pooling (determined by the set of weights, ) in the suppressive field can be thought of as an anatomical property reflecting the lateral connectivity across the population. The suppressive field gain term () can be thought of as a physiological property determining the context sensitivity of individual neurons to the neuronal milieu in the sense that it scales how much each neuron’s response is influenced by the stimulus-dependent population activity. The dependency of neural activity on and is illustrated in Fig. 2. Note that reducing either of these parameters decreases the strength of the divisive normalization signal (thus increasing the E/I ratio), resulting in higher amplitude responses. Importantly, the denominator, , determines how much the population suppresses a neuron’s activity, and thus the balance of excitation to inhibition is reflected in the ratio .
Divisive normalization is a widespread neural computation. In primary visual cortex, it was first used to explain why responses saturate as stimulus contrast increases (34). It is also thought to adjust the gain of neural responses to efficiently make use of their dynamic range (15). Divisive normalization further accounts for a number of phenomena in auditory cortex (35), multisensory integration (36), and higher-order processes such as attention (37) and rationality (16). In addition, divisive normalization may be critical for neural circuits to implement marginalization, a type of probabilistic inference that eliminates irrelevant information, so-called “nuisance variables” (38).
To illustrate how divisive normalization affects neural activity, we simulated a population of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) and compared their response properties before and after divisive normalization. Across the population, the receptive fields varied in retinotopic position () and orientation () (Fig. 3A). For each model neuron, the excitatory drive elicited by a stimulus depends on the position and orientation of the receptive field relative to the stimulus (SI Appendix). The excitatory drive is inhibited divisively by a suppressive field reflecting the pooled activity of neurons with nearby receptive fields. The strength of the suppressive field, and therefore its inhibitory effect, increases with the excitatory drive as well as the extent of neuronal pooling. The response of each model neuron, indexed by its receptive field position and orientation, is thus described by the following equation:
[2] |
The effect of divisive normalization on the responses of a model V1 neuron to sinusoidal gratings of different contrasts, orientations, and sizes is shown in Fig. 3 B–D. Without divisive normalization, the response amplitude grows without bound as the stimulus contrast increases. However, the responses of real V1 neurons saturate with increasing contrast (34), which also occurs in the model with divisive normalization (Fig. 3B). Neurons in V1 also display a context sensitivity in which responses to a sinusoidal grating at the preferred orientation are suppressed by the simultaneous presentation of a second grating with an orthogonal orientation. This property of V1 responses is similarly accounted for by divisive normalization (Fig. 3C). Surround suppression, which contributes to the size selectivity of V1 neurons (39), can also be explained by divisive normalization. Without divisive normalization, the model responses increase monotonically with stimulus size and saturate. However, with divisive normalization, the behavior is qualitatively different. As the stimulus size increases, the excitatory drive initially outweighs the network inhibition and the response amplitude increases. This occurs until a critical stimulus size determined by is reached, at which point the inhibition becomes sufficiently strong to reduce the response amplitude, resulting in a well-defined peak in the size tuning curve (Fig. 3D).
Connecting Divisive Normalization and Autism
There are several ways an increased E/I ratio can be modeled using Eq. 2. One possibility is an increase in excitation, which can be achieved by multiplying the excitatory drive () by a number greater than 1. An increase in could reflect increased glutamate-related excitation, consistent with physiological data on autism (25). An increased E/I ratio could also be due to decreased inhibition, which can be modeled as a decrease in either the semisaturation constant () or the suppressive field gain term () (Fig. 2). Decreases in or may reflect decreases in GABA-related inhibition that are also consistent with physiological data on autism (7, 23, 31, 32). Changes in the extent of neuronal pooling in the suppressive field (e.g., altered lateral connectivity) could also alter the amount of inhibition acting on a neuron. In particular, reducing the extent of neuronal pooling in the model increases the E/I ratio (SI Appendix). Considering the large number of autism susceptibility genes, any or all of these factors may contribute on an individual basis.
Although autism may conceptually affect multiple parameters in Eq. 2, here we focus on the suppressive field gain term (), which determines the context sensitivity of the neurons, controlling how much each neuron’s responses are influenced by the stimulus-dependent population activity. An increased E/I ratio, as is implicated in autism (9), can be simulated by reducing , thereby attenuating the inhibitory influence of the neuronal milieu on the activity of individual neurons. Autism can thus be putatively simulated by decreasing , with the degree of severity increasing as decreases. In this way, we defined an “autism model” of primary visual cortex in which there is a 25% reduction in relative to the “typically developing control” model used above (SI Appendix). With the exception of this change in the suppressive field gain term, the autism and typically developing control models are identical.
In the following sections, we examine how divisive normalization can account for perceptual consequences of autism by comparing the activity of the autism and typically developing control models. We first present simulations of two psychophysical studies exploring how autism affects performance in simple visual tasks relying heavily on primary visual cortex, thus allowing the studies to be simulated using the V1 models. Interestingly, the same change in the suppressive field gain term () qualitatively accounts for the findings of both studies. A third simulation is then performed to show how Bayesian priors can be implemented through divisive normalization, linking alterations in neural computation to high-level hypotheses about how autism affects the ability to make inferences about the world. Last, broader connections between autism and divisive normalization are discussed to link major theories and findings on the disorder to divisive normalization, as well as make predictions about the behavioral consequences of autism.
Simulation 1: Visual Spatial Suppression
Recent studies report that visual spatial suppression, which is linked to divisive normalization (39) (Fig. 3D), may be altered in autism (40, 41). In one study, Foss-Feig et al. (40) presented drifting gratings that varied in size and contrast, and had subjects indicate the direction of motion (left vs. right). When the stimuli were presented at high contrast, performance worsened (the time required to judge the direction of motion lengthened) as the size of the stimulus increased for both typically developing controls and subjects with autism. However, subjects with autism performed systematically better than controls across all stimulus sizes. This result is illustrated in Fig. 4A, where inverse duration thresholds are plotted such that larger values indicate better performance. For small stimuli presented at low contrast, the performance difference between the groups disappeared (Fig. 4B). These results raise two questions: (i) what changes in neural computation can account for the findings, and (ii) are these changes consistent with the physiology of autism?
Because our model simulates neuronal population activity, answering these questions requires a consideration of how this activity can translate into behavior. One possibility is that population activity is decoded to determine the most probable stimulus, and the appropriate behavioral response is then selected accordingly (17, 38, 42). This strategy captures the intuition that greater activity in a subpopulation of neurons with similar response properties leads to greater certainty about the stimulus (SI Appendix). More specifically, the certainty about a presented stimulus increases with the amplitude of the population response, which is called the population gain (SI Appendix, Fig. S1). A key implication connecting behavior to neural activity is therefore that task performance will be better when the population gain is larger, and worse when that gain is lower. As demonstrated below, larger population gains and thus improved task performance can occur as a consequence of an attenuated divisive normalization signal (increased E/I ratio).
Based on these considerations, the psychophysical findings of Foss-Feig et al. (40) imply several testable predictions about the underlying neural activity. First, spatial suppression should appear as a reduction in the population gain as the stimulus size increases. Second, for subjects with autism to perform better than controls at high contrasts, population gains should be higher in autism. Third, similar task performance at low contrasts predicts that, under these conditions, population gains will be similar for controls and subjects with autism. To test these predictions in silico, we examined the model responses to sinusoidal gratings of different sizes and contrasts. As predicted, the population gains decreased for high-contrast stimuli of increasing size, but were always higher for the autism than the control model (Fig. 4C). Moreover, the population gains of the two models were nearly identical for small stimuli at low contrasts, but diverged progressively as the contrast increased (Fig. 4D). This result suggests that performance differences between controls and subjects with autism in this task will increase smoothly with stimulus contrast. In addition, the similarity of the population gains at low contrasts may account for why autism does not affect contrast sensitivity (the lowest contrast at which sinusoidal gratings can be reliably detected), since contrast sensitivities are themselves very low (43). Additional simulations examining how changes in the semisaturation constant and the extent of neuronal pooling affect the model responses are shown in SI Appendix, Fig. S2. These simulations show that an increased E/I ratio resulting from altered divisive normalization can affect neural computation in a manner that qualitatively predicts perceptual consequences of autism.
An intuition for the results shown in Fig. 4 C and D can be found by examining Eq. 2. First consider the finding of similar psychophysical performance at low contrasts, and note that the magnitude of the suppressive field is small at low contrasts due to weak stimulus drives. As such, the inhibition from is roughly independent of at low contrasts. In other words, the population activity is not large enough to strongly engage divisive normalization, and so little to no difference is observed between the control and autism models. However, at high contrasts, the magnitude of the suppressive field is large enough to strongly engage divisive normalization, allowing different values of the suppressive field gain term to exert differential effects. In particular, the lower value of the suppressive field gain term () in the autism compared with the control model results in less inhibition and thus larger population gains at high contrasts, leading to better task performance.
Simulation 2: Tunnel Vision
The visual detection of a target is facilitated by presenting it within close proximity to an attentional cue (44). Recently, Robertson et al. (45) found that, as the distance between the attentional cue and target increases (SI Appendix, Fig. S3A), the rate at which performance falls off is greater for subjects with autism than typically developing controls, suggesting there is a sharper gradient of attention in autism, or “tunnel vision.” This result is illustrated in Fig. 5A, where relative performance scores taking into account both reaction time and accuracy are plotted. Larger values indicate faster, more accurate detection. Because the stimuli were tailored to each subject to match baseline task difficulty, and the subject-averaged performance scores were mean subtracted separately for the control and autism groups, only the steepness of the gradient can be compared across groups. Interestingly, the study found that the steepness of the attention gradient was correlated with the degree of autism symptomatology (Fig. 5B). Here, we show that the same alteration in divisive normalization that accounts for the findings of Foss-Feig et al. (40) can qualitatively account for these results.
For this simulation, an attentional field was incorporated to define an attention-modulated excitatory drive: (37) (SI Appendix). The parameters of the attentional field were the same for the control and autism models, and with the exception of introducing the attentional field, all aspects were the same as in the previous simulation. The attentional field also affects the divisive normalization signal as a consequence of neuronal pooling in the suppressive field, . Incorporating the effects of attention, the response of each model neuron is described by the following equation:
[3] |
To examine the effect of the attentional field on the model responses, sinusoidal gratings were presented at different distances from the center of the attentional field, and the population gain at those locations measured. Recall that better task performance is achieved when the population gain is larger (SI Appendix, Fig. S1). Consistent with the findings of Robertson et al. (45), the population gain decreased for both models as the distance between the target and attentional cue increased, with faster falloff in the autism model (Fig. 5C). Moreover, as we increased the simulated degree of autism symptomatology by further decreasing the suppressive field gain term, the rate at which the population gain fell off increased (Fig. 5D).
These results reflect an interaction between the attentional field and divisive normalization. The existence of the gradient in the population gain is due to the attentional field, but the steepness depends on the strength of the divisive normalization signal. The gradient arises because the attentional field produces a location-dependent increase in the strength of the excitatory drive, . This effect is balanced by inhibition from the suppressive field, , with the magnitude of the divisive normalization signal depending on the suppressive field gain term (). Because the smaller value of in the autism model results in less inhibition, the attentional field has a larger effect that gives rise to a sharper gradient in the population gain. We additionally found that the lower value of in the autism model resulted in larger population gains (implying better task performance) at the largest target distances where the attentional field has little effect (Fig. 5C). We therefore ran a second simulation at a reduced contrast, such that the population gains for the control and autism models were approximately matched at large target distances (SI Appendix, Fig. S3B). This manipulation is similar to the measures Robertson et al. (45) took to match baseline task difficulty across subjects. Importantly, under this condition, the modeling results remained consistent with the psychophysical findings; namely, a steeper gradient in the population gain for the autism than the control model, which increased as the suppressive field gain term decreased (SI Appendix, Fig. S3C). Additional simulations examining how changes in the extent of neuronal pooling and the semisaturation constant affect the model responses are shown in SI Appendix, Fig. S3 D and E. It is further interesting to note that our results may explain the apparent conflict between the study by Robertson et al. (45) and another recent study suggesting that exogenous spatial attention may not be affected in autism (46). Specifically, because the attentional field was the same for the control and autism models, the differences in their responses were entirely due to the difference in their suppressive field gain terms. This result indicates that a sharper gradient of attention in autism may reflect altered divisive normalization rather than a change in attentional mechanisms.
Simulation 3: Neural Implementation of Bayesian Priors
Several papers have suggested that autism impairs the ability to perform statistical inference about the sensory environment (18–20), reducing the influence of past experience on the interpretation of current sensory information. Although this hypothesis can be formulated in several ways, the Bayesian framework provides an intuitive approach, describing how incoming sensory information is combined with past experience to infer the most likely stimulus. In particular, attenuation of the Bayesian prior (the collected representation of past experience) in autism would reduce the influence of past experience on the interpretation of current sensory information (19). To the extent that a Bayesian prior accurately reflects the statistics of the world, it will improve task performance by reducing the number of probable stimuli. As such, an attenuated prior in autism could potentially increase behavioral sensitivity to sensory noise, as well as increase variability in stimulus-driven neural activity, both of which are consistent with recent findings on the disorder (47, 48). We next simulate how Bayesian priors can be implemented through divisive normalization, establishing a link between alterations in neural computation and high-level hypotheses about how autism affects the ability to perform statistical inference.
In the model, experience modifies the strength of lateral connectivity across the population, transforming the suppressive field gain term () from a constant into a function of the neuronal tuning properties (49) (SI Appendix, Fig. S4A). In this way, a prior turns stimulus-driven responses into a reflection of both current sensory information and past experience. As an example, we modeled the “oblique effect,” which describes that humans are most visually sensitive to vertically and horizontally orientated contours (50), presumably reflecting that cardinal orientations are more frequent in natural scenes than oblique orientations (51). For the oblique effect, the prior facilitates responses to cardinal orientations and attenuates responses to oblique orientations (SI Appendix, Fig. S4B). Decoding the population activity to discriminate changes in stimulus orientation reveals the effect of the prior; namely, greater sensitivity to cardinal than oblique orientations (SI Appendix, Fig. S5). The weaker the prior, the less performance depends on the orientation of the stimulus (SI Appendix, Fig. S4C). An attenuation of Bayesian priors thus correctly predicts reduced differences in sensitivity to cardinal and oblique orientations in autism (52).
How can divisive normalization account for the proposed attenuation of Bayesian priors in autism (19)? One possibility is that a reduction in the suppressive field gain term (; as in the first two simulations) attenuates the influence of the population on individual neurons, thereby reducing the effect of priors. The extent to which experience modifies could also be attenuated, such that a given level of experience produces smaller changes in for individuals with autism than typically developing peers. In other words, plasticity in the strength of experience-dependent lateral connectivity may be diminished in the disorder. This possibility is consistent with the observation that many autism susceptibility genes code for synaptic proteins or control synaptic development and function, suggesting that the effect of experience on synapses is altered in autism (53).
Local vs. Global Processing
Compared to typically developing peers, individuals with autism are thought to have a bias toward the processing of “local” stimulus features (54–57). The hierarchical figures test, in which local features are used to define a global feature, provides an illustrative example. Consider a local feature, the letter “A” reproduced many times in a small font size and arranged to create a large letter “F,” the global feature. When the task is to report if a particular letter is present, subjects with autism make fewer errors if the letter is the local feature than if it is the global feature, whereas controls make fewer errors if the letter is the global feature (55). When the letter of interest is the global feature, the local feature is a nuisance variable in the sense that it makes no difference if it is the letter “A” or any other letter. The elimination of nuisance variables is called marginalization, and theoretical work suggests that the neural implementation of this process requires divisive normalization (38). A bias toward local feature processing in autism is therefore consistent with the possibility of impaired marginalization through altered divisive normalization. More generally, behaviors requiring marginalization such as social cognition (58), visual search (59), reference frame transformations (60), and causal inference (38), may be altered in autism. Consistent with this prediction, impaired social cognition is a diagnostic criterion for autism, visual search is altered in the disorder (61), and an increased egocentric perspective in autism (62) may reflect an impairment in reference frame transformations.
Simple vs. Complex Stimuli
Individuals with autism are often reported to perform better at tasks involving “simple” stimuli, whereas typically developing controls may be better at tasks involving “complex” stimuli (63, 64). For example, in the auditory system, enhanced performance is reported in autism when a discrimination task is performed using pure tones, but no difference or deficits are observed when the stimuli are more complex (65–67). Consider that the amount of processing, and thus the number of processing stages (e.g., hierarchically organized brain regions), required to analyze sensory information is linked to the complexity of the stimulus. For example, contour orientation is readily decoded from V1 (68), but decoding object identity is accomplished in higher stages of cortex (69). Theoretical work shows that, for biologically realistic, hierarchical neural network models to perform complex tasks such as object identification accurately, it is critical to include divisive normalization-like computations at every processing stage (70, 71). If divisive normalization is altered beyond the early stages of sensory processing in autism, then it could result in impaired processing specifically for complex, but not simple, stimuli. Such a deficit would be particularly detrimental when local features must be combined to form a complex whole and may therefore account for impaired face processing in autism, which appears to reflect a more feature-based analysis and reduced use of contextual information in creating face composites (72). Indeed, the importance of divisive normalization in performing hierarchical processing may create a trade-off in the ability to perform tasks requiring fewer vs. more levels of processing, such that an overall decrease in the strength of divisive normalization signals would not be globally beneficial.
Multisensory Processing
Altered multisensory processing, for example, in the integration of visual and auditory signals, is reported by some autism studies (73–75). However, it is unclear whether these results reflect differences in earlier unisensory representations or in the process of multisensory integration itself. The possibility of intact multisensory processing is supported by a recent study reporting near-optimal integration of visual and vestibular cues in self-motion perception in both controls and subjects with autism (48). Importantly, this finding is not inconsistent with the hypothesis of altered divisive normalization in autism since cue integration does not theoretically require divisive normalization (42). Altered divisive normalization in autism does, however, predict changes in other forms of multisensory processing that require divisive normalization, such as cue conflict conditions and causal inference (36, 38).
Rationality
Rational behavior in value-based decision tasks implies that choices should reflect the context-independent values of available options. In other words, choices about an option should depend neither on the quantity/quality of other options, nor on how the option is framed (e.g., in terms of gain vs. loss). However, individuals are often irrational, making value-based decisions that show context dependence. As discussed above, divisive normalization makes neurons context sensitive; for example, resulting in cross-orientation suppression (Fig. 3C). In an analogous manner, divisive normalization acting on a population of neurons encoding the value of available options in a decision task can account for the context dependence, and therefore irrationality, of value-based decisions (16). In such a population, the suppressive field represents the overall value of available options, and divisive normalization transforms an absolute value representation into a relative representation that depends on the context of available choices. Divisive normalization can thus contribute to context-dependent violations of rationality in which the suppressive field gain term is particularly important because it scales how much each option’s value is influenced by other options. It is therefore intriguing to speculate that a decrease in the suppressive field gain term in autism can result in reduced contextual sensitivity in value-based decisions. Consistent with this prediction, enhanced logical consistency in value-based decisions is observed in autism as a consequence of reduced susceptibility to contextual framing (76).
Conclusions
A problem currently faced in autism research is that the disorder is both genetically heterogeneous and phenotypically diverse. Here, we examined how computational models of nonlinear neural circuits can bridge levels of analysis, connecting genetic and molecular findings supporting an increased E/I ratio in autism to perceptual data on the disorder. In particular, we showed that a reduction in the amount of inhibition occurring through divisive normalization can account for perceptual consequences of autism. Interestingly, our simulations implicate the context-dependent neuronal milieu as having a critical role in autism, such that the less influence the population has on the activity of individual neurons, the more severe the degree of autism symptomatology. Importantly, computational frameworks such as presented here can provide a systematic mechanism for generating falsifiable hypotheses about the neural basis of autism. For example, altered divisive normalization in autism predicts that the disorder will broadly affect processes requiring marginalization (38), such as social cognition (58) and visual search (59).
The phenotypic diversity of autism may in part reflect that the neural mechanisms implementing divisive normalization differ across brain regions (15). Our results thus predict that the behavioral consequences of autism will vary across individuals, depending qualitatively and quantitatively on where and how the disorder impacts divisive normalization. Qualitative differences between individuals should occur when alterations in divisive normalization impact different brain areas. Quantitative differences should instead reflect the extent to which divisive normalization is affected; for example, the percent reduction in the suppressive field gain term (Fig. 5D). Supporting this idea, the GABAergic pathways likely contribute to autism (7, 8, 31, 32), and GABAA and GABAB can contribute differently to divisive normalization. In V1, GABAA modulates response gain but not contrast sensitivity (77), suggesting it contributes to the suppressive field gain term () but not the semisaturation constant (). In the auditory system, GABAB is implicated in both response gain and sensitivity, suggesting it may contribute to as well as (78).
Here, we focused on how reducing can account for perceptual consequences of autism, including phenotypic diversity within the same task. A reduction in is consistent with reduced GABAA inhibition in V1 (77), and our results correspondingly predict higher average V1 firing rates in autism than controls. Given the statistical properties of spiking neural activity (79), this result also predicts greater trial-by-trial response variability in autism. Evidence supporting these predictions comes from imaging studies reporting larger stimulus-driven response amplitudes (80) or increased variance (47) in autism, as well as recent psychophysical findings (81). In one imaging study, increased stimulus-driven activity in auditory cortex associated with low-level feature processing was observed in subjects with autism compared to typically developing controls (82). Interestingly, this finding was specific to subjects with autism showing speech onset delay, a subgroup for which enhanced performance on low-level auditory tasks such as pitch discrimination may be specific (67). A reduction in is also consistent with the finding that contrast sensitivity is not affected in autism (43), which would have otherwise suggested a change in (Fig. 2A). Although normal contrast sensitivity is consistent with intact gain control, it is possible that sensory hypersensitivity in autism (83) reflects gain control deficits. It is thus interesting to speculate that steady-state values of are not altered in autism, but the temporal process by which changes based on sensory stimulation is impaired (20). In addition, a reduction in could affect neural decoding by reducing noise correlations between neurons (84), or decreasing the efficiency of simple decoders (85), both of which are potential consequences of a reduced influence of the neuronal milieu on the activity of single cells.
Divisive normalization parameters other than may also contribute to the phenotypic diversity of autism. This possibility was explored by examining the effects of changing the extent of neuronal pooling and (SI Appendix, Figs. S2 and S3). For the spatial suppression simulation, results similar to changing (Fig. 4) were achieved by decreasing the extent of neuronal pooling (increasing the E/I ratio), but the effects were weaker. Moreover, as the size of the stimulus increased, there was a reduction in the rate of spatial suppression (SI Appendix, Fig. S2) that appears inconsistent with the psychophysical data (40). A decrease in (increasing the E/I ratio) also produced similar spatial suppression results, but the effects were highly attenuated and would alter contrast sensitivity, inconsistent with previous findings (43). For the tunnel vision simulation (Fig. 5), sharper gradients of attention resulted from increases in the extent of neuronal pooling or (SI Appendix, Fig. S3), decreasing the E/I ratio. Changes in these parameters thus provide a less parsimonious explanation for the autism symptomatology explored here than a change in , because they needed to be in opposite directions for the two sets of psychophysical findings. Importantly, this observation does not imply that these parameters are unaffected in autism; in fact, their diverse effects may contribute to the disorder’s phenotypic diversity. By considering where in the brain and how much these parameters are affected, it may be possible to account for a wide array of behavioral data on autism. Although we focused on how divisive normalization can account for psychophysical findings in simple tasks relying heavily on a single brain area, it will be important for future computational work to investigate how autism affects complex functions (e.g., face processing) involving multiple brain areas. Such work will call for hierarchical, multilayered networks in which divisive normalization is essential (70, 71), and may provide insights into the finding of decreased functional connectivity across brain areas in autism (86–89). Indeed, a recent study implicates divisive normalization in cross-area information coupling (90), suggesting that altered functional connectivity in autism may be partly attributable to divisive normalization.
The computational framework described in this study provides a formalism for investigating how alterations in neural computation may give rise to autism symptomatology. Adaptations of this approach may provide insights into other mental health disorders such as schizophrenia (10, 91, 92), and perhaps some aspects of aging (93). The results of our simulations further suggest that behavioral assays combined with computational modeling may be useful in identifying altered physiological pathways in individuals, and thus in facilitating the development of individualized treatment plans. We suggest that computational perspectives can play an important role in the future of mental health research, providing insights that will aid in understanding and treating complex disorders such as autism.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Ryan Ash, Reuben Fan, Eliana Klier, Xaq Pitkow, Adhira Sunkara, and Mingshan Xue for comments. This work was supported by Integrative Graduate Education and Research Traineeship Training Grant 43413-I (to J.S.P.) and Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative 247992 (to D.E.A.).
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1510583112/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Wing L, Gould J. Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. J Autism Dev Disord. 1979;9(1):11–29. doi: 10.1007/BF01531288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Newschaffer CJ, Falb MD, Gurney JG. National autism prevalence trends from United States special education data. Pediatrics. 2005;115(3):e277–e282. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2000 Principal Investigators; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2007) Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, six sites, United States, 2000. MMWR Surveill Summ 56(1):1–11. [PubMed]
- 4. Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2010 Principal Investigators; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2014) Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders among children aged 8 years: Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill Summ 63(2):1–21. [PubMed]
- 5.Ronemus M, Iossifov I, Levy D, Wigler M. The role of de novo mutations in the genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Nat Rev Genet. 2014;15(2):133–141. doi: 10.1038/nrg3585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Walsh CA, Morrow EM, Rubenstein JL. Autism and brain development. Cell. 2008;135(3):396–400. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chao HT, et al. Dysfunction in GABA signalling mediates autism-like stereotypies and Rett syndrome phenotypes. Nature. 2010;468(7321):263–269. doi: 10.1038/nature09582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Han S, Tai C, Jones CJ, Scheuer T, Catterall WA. Enhancement of inhibitory neurotransmission by GABAA receptors having α2,3-subunits ameliorates behavioral deficits in a mouse model of autism. Neuron. 2014;81(6):1282–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rubenstein JL, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: Increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2(5):255–267. doi: 10.1034/j.1601-183x.2003.00037.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Yizhar O, et al. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature. 2011;477(7363):171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature10360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Freitag CM, Kleser C, Schneider M, von Gontard A. Quantitative assessment of neuromotor function in adolescents with high functioning autism and Asperger Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(5):948–959. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0235-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gabriels RL, et al. Is there a relationship between restricted, repetitive, stereotyped behaviors and interests and abnormal sensory response in children with autism spectrum disorders? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2008;2(4):660–670. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Haswell CC, Izawa J, Dowell LR, Mostofsky SH, Shadmehr R. Representation of internal models of action in the autistic brain. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(8):970–972. doi: 10.1038/nn.2356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Marko MK, et al. Behavioural and neural basis of anomalous motor learning in children with autism. Brain. 2015;138(Pt 3):784–797. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Carandini M, Heeger DJ. Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(1):51–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn3136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Louie K, Khaw MW, Glimcher PW. Normalization is a general neural mechanism for context-dependent decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(15):6139–6144. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1217854110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Seilheimer RL, Rosenberg A, Angelaki DE. Models and processes of multisensory cue combination. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;25:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Qian N, Lipkin RM. A learning-style theory for understanding autistic behaviors. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:77. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pellicano E, Burr D. When the world becomes “too real”: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(10):504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sinha P, et al. Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111(42):15220–15225. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416797111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hensch TK. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(11):877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Inhibitory threshold for critical-period activation in primary visual cortex. Nature. 2000;404(6774):183–186. doi: 10.1038/35004582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hensch TK, et al. Local GABA circuit control of experience-dependent plasticity in developing visual cortex. Science. 1998;282(5393):1504–1508. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5393.1504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Volkmar FR, Nelson DS. Seizure disorders in autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1990;29(1):127–129. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199001000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shinohe A, et al. Increased serum levels of glutamate in adult patients with autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006;30(8):1472–1477. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Fatemi SH, et al. Downregulation of GABAA receptor protein subunits α6, β2, δ, ε, γ2, θ, and ρ2 in superior frontal cortex of subjects with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(8):1833–1845. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2078-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jamain S, et al. Paris Autism Research International Sibpair (PARIS) Study Linkage and association of the glutamate receptor 6 gene with autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7(3):302–310. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cook EH, Jr, et al. Linkage-disequilibrium mapping of autistic disorder, with 15q11-13 markers. Am J Hum Genet. 1998;62(5):1077–1083. doi: 10.1086/301832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fatemi SH, Reutiman TJ, Folsom TD, Thuras PD. GABAA receptor downregulation in brains of subjects with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(2):223–230. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0646-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Fatemi SH, Folsom TD, Reutiman TJ, Thuras PD. Expression of GABAB receptors is altered in brains of subjects with autism. Cerebellum. 2009;8(1):64–69. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0075-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Yip J, Soghomonian JJ, Blatt GJ. Decreased GAD67 mRNA levels in cerebellar Purkinje cells in autism: Pathophysiological implications. Acta Neuropathol. 2007;113(5):559–568. doi: 10.1007/s00401-006-0176-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Yip J, Soghomonian JJ, Blatt GJ. Decreased GAD65 mRNA levels in select subpopulations of neurons in the cerebellar dentate nuclei in autism: An in situ hybridization study. Autism Res. 2009;2(1):50–59. doi: 10.1002/aur.62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gkogkas CG, et al. Autism-related deficits via dysregulated eIF4E-dependent translational control. Nature. 2013;493(7432):371–377. doi: 10.1038/nature11628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Heeger DJ. Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1992;9(2):181–197. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800009640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rabinowitz NC, Willmore BD, Schnupp JW, King AJ. Contrast gain control in auditory cortex. Neuron. 2011;70(6):1178–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ohshiro T, Angelaki DE, DeAngelis GC. A normalization model of multisensory integration. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(6):775–782. doi: 10.1038/nn.2815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Reynolds JH, Heeger DJ. The normalization model of attention. Neuron. 2009;61(2):168–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Beck JM, Latham PE, Pouget A. Marginalization in neural circuits with divisive normalization. J Neurosci. 2011;31(43):15310–15319. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1706-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sceniak MP, Hawken MJ, Shapley R. Visual spatial characterization of macaque V1 neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85(5):1873–1887. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Foss-Feig JH, Tadin D, Schauder KB, Cascio CJ. A substantial and unexpected enhancement of motion perception in autism. J Neurosci. 2013;33(19):8243–8249. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1608-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Flevaris AV, Murray SO. Orientation-specific surround suppression in the primary visual cortex varies as a function of autistic tendency. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:1017. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ma WJ, Beck JM, Latham PE, Pouget A. Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(11):1432–1438. doi: 10.1038/nn1790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Koh HC, Milne E, Dobkins K. Spatial contrast sensitivity in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40(8):978–987. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0953-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Müller NG, Mollenhauer M, Rösler A, Kleinschmidt A. The attentional field has a Mexican hat distribution. Vision Res. 2005;45(9):1129–1137. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Robertson CE, Kravitz DJ, Freyberg J, Baron-Cohen S, Baker CI. Tunnel vision: Sharper gradient of spatial attention in autism. J Neurosci. 2013;33(16):6776–6781. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5120-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Grubb MA, et al. Exogenous spatial attention: Evidence for intact functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Vis. 2013;13(14):9. doi: 10.1167/13.14.9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Dinstein I, et al. Unreliable evoked responses in autism. Neuron. 2012;75(6):981–991. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zaidel A, Goin-Kochel RP, Angelaki DE. Self-motion perception in autism is compromised by visual noise but integrated optimally across multiple senses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(20):6461–6466. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1506582112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Schwartz O, Coen-Cagli R. Visual attention and flexible normalization pools. J Vis. 2013;13(1):25. doi: 10.1167/13.1.25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Westheimer G, Beard BL. Orientation dependency for foveal line stimuli: Detection and intensity discrimination, resolution, orientation discrimination and Vernier acuity. Vision Res. 1998;38(8):1097–1103. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00248-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Coppola DM, Purves HR, McCoy AN, Purves D. The distribution of oriented contours in the real world. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95(7):4002–4006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.7.4002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Dickinson A, Jones M, Milne E. Oblique orientation discrimination thresholds are superior in those with a high level of autistic traits. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(11):2844–2850. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2147-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ebert DH, Greenberg ME. Activity-dependent neuronal signalling and autism spectrum disorder. Nature. 2013;493(7432):327–337. doi: 10.1038/nature11860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Mottron L, Belleville S, Ménard E. Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: Perceptual hierarchization or working memory deficit? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40(5):743–755. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Plaisted K, Swettenham J, Rees L. Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40(5):733–742. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Mottron L, Burack JA, Iarocci G, Belleville S, Enns JT. Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44(6):904–913. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Dakin S, Frith U. Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron. 2005;48(3):497–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Baker CL, Saxe R, Tenenbaum JB. Action understanding as inverse planning. Cognition. 2009;113(3):329–349. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Mazyar H, van den Berg R, Seilheimer RL, Ma WJ. Independence is elusive: Set size effects on encoding precision in visual search. J Vis. 2013;13(5):8. doi: 10.1167/13.5.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Rosenberg A, Angelaki DE. Gravity influences the visual representation of object tilt in parietal cortex. J Neurosci. 2014;34(43):14170–14180. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2030-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.O’Riordan MA, Plaisted KC, Driver J, Baron-Cohen S. Superior visual search in autism. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001;27(3):719–730. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.3.719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Begeer S, Bernstein DM, van Wijhe J, Scheeren AM, Koot HM. A continuous false belief task reveals egocentric biases in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2012;16(4):357–366. doi: 10.1177/1362361311434545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Bertone A, Mottron L, Jelenic P, Faubert J. Motion perception in autism: A “complex” issue. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15(2):218–225. doi: 10.1162/089892903321208150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bertone A, Mottron L, Jelenic P, Faubert J. Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain. 2005;128(10):2430–2441. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Bonnel A, et al. Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: A signal detection analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15(2):226–235. doi: 10.1162/089892903321208169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Russo NM, et al. Deficient brainstem encoding of pitch in children with autism spectrum disorders. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(8):1720–1731. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.01.108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bonnel A, et al. Enhanced pure-tone pitch discrimination among persons with autism but not Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(9):2465–2475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Graf AB, Kohn A, Jazayeri M, Movshon JA. Decoding the activity of neuronal populations in macaque primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(2):239–245. doi: 10.1038/nn.2733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hung CP, Kreiman G, Poggio T, DiCarlo JJ. Fast readout of object identity from macaque inferior temporal cortex. Science. 2005;310(5749):863–866. doi: 10.1126/science.1117593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kouh M, Poggio T. A canonical neural circuit for cortical nonlinear operations. Neural Comput. 2008;20(6):1427–1451. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.02-07-466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Jarrett K, Kavukcuoglu K, Ranzato M, LeCun Y. 2009. What is the best multi-stage architecture for object recognition? 2009 IEEE 12th International Conference on Computer Vision (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ), pp 2146–2153.
- 72.Teunisse JP, de Gelder B. Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects. Brain Cogn. 2003;52(3):285–294. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00042-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Brandwein AB, et al. The development of multisensory integration in high-functioning autism: High-density electrical mapping and psychophysical measures reveal impairments in the processing of audiovisual inputs. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(6):1329–1341. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Collignon O, et al. Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism. Cortex. 2013;49(6):1704–1710. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Foxe JJ, et al. Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(2):298–312. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.De Martino B, Harrison NA, Knafo S, Bird G, Dolan RJ. Explaining enhanced logical consistency during decision making in autism. J Neurosci. 2008;28(42):10746–10750. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2895-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Katzner S, Busse L, Carandini M. GABAA inhibition controls response gain in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31(16):5931–5941. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5753-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Magnusson AK, Park TJ, Pecka M, Grothe B, Koch U. Retrograde GABA signaling adjusts sound localization by balancing excitation and inhibition in the brainstem. Neuron. 2008;59(1):125–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Gur M, Beylin A, Snodderly DM. Response variability of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) of alert monkeys. J Neurosci. 1997;17(8):2914–2920. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-08-02914.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Schwarzkopf DS, Anderson EJ, de Haas B, White SJ, Rees G. Larger extrastriate population receptive fields in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurosci. 2014;34(7):2713–2724. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4416-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Haigh SM, Minshew N, Heeger DJ, Dinstein I, Behrmann M. Over-responsiveness and greater variability in roughness perception in autism. Autism Res. 2015 doi: 10.1002/aur.1505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Samson F, Zeffiro TA, Doyon J, Benali H, Mottron L. Speech acquisition predicts regions of enhanced cortical response to auditory stimulation in autism spectrum individuals. J Psychiatr Res. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.psychires.2015.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Tomchek SD, Dunn W. Sensory processing in children with and without autism: A comparative study using the short sensory profile. Am J Occup Ther. 2007;61(2):190–200. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(5):358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Olsen SR, Bhandawat V, Wilson RI. Divisive normalization in olfactory population codes. Neuron. 2010;66(2):287–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Belmonte MK, et al. Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J Neurosci. 2004;24(42):9228–9231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ. Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(4):951–961. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Schipul SE, Keller TA, Just MA. Inter-regional brain communication and its disturbance in autism. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5:10. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Wass S. Distortions and disconnections: Disrupted brain connectivity in autism. Brain Cogn. 2011;75(1):18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Saproo S, Serences JT. Attention improves transfer of motion information between V1 and MT. J Neurosci. 2014;34(10):3586–3596. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3484-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Yoon JH, et al. GABA concentration is reduced in visual cortex in schizophrenia and correlates with orientation-specific surround suppression. J Neurosci. 2010;30(10):3777–3781. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6158-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Tibber MS, et al. Visual surround suppression in schizophrenia. Front Psychol. 2013;4:88. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Betts LR, Taylor CP, Sekuler AB, Bennett PJ. Aging reduces center-surround antagonism in visual motion processing. Neuron. 2005;45(3):361–366. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.