Abstract
Objectives
To examine the impact of health warnings on smokers by comparing the short-term impact of new graphic (2006) Australian warnings with: (i) earlier (2003) United Kingdom (UK) larger text-based warnings; (ii) and Canadian graphic warnings (late 2000); and secondarily, to extend our understanding of warning wear-out.
Methods
The International Tobacco Control Policy Evaluation Survey (ITC Project) follows prospective cohorts (with replenishment) of adult smokers annually (5 waves: 2002–2006), in Canada, United States, UK, and Australia (around 2000 per country per wave; total n=17,773). Measures were of pack warning salience (reading and noticing); cognitive responses (thoughts of harm and quitting); and two behavioural responses: forgoing cigarettes and avoiding the warnings.
Results
All four indicators of impact increased markedly among Australian smokers following the introduction of graphic warnings. Controlling for date of introduction, they stimulated more cognitive responses than the UK (text-only) changes, and were avoided more, did not significantly increase forgoing cigarettes, but were read and noticed less. The findings also extend previous work showing partial wear-out of both graphic and text-only warnings, but the Canadian warnings have more sustained effects than UK ones.
Conclusions
Australia’s new health warnings increased reactions that are prospectively predictive of cessation activity. Warning size increases warning effectiveness and graphic warnings may be superior to text-based warnings. While there is partial wear-out in the initial impact associated with all warnings, stronger warnings tend to sustain their effects for longer. These findings support arguments for governments to exceed minimum FCTC requirements on warnings.
Keywords: Health warnings, graphic warnings, tobacco product labelling, consumer information, tobacco health information
Introduction
Tobacco is a very unusual consumer product, given that it is highly addictive and kills around half of its long-term users.1 For these health reasons and for consumer rights reasons,2 there are strong arguments for governments to mandate effective health warnings on tobacco products. Such health warnings cost tax-payers nothing and potentially reach smokers every time they take a cigarette from a pack, buy a pack, or otherwise notice one.
The Framework Convention for Tobacco Control (FCTC)3 states that warning labels “should be 50% or more of the principal display areas but shall be no less than 30% of the principal display areas” (Article 11). The guidance on graphic images is less definitive: “may be in the form of or use pictures or pictograms”. Indeed, despite a growing body of research on tobacco warning labels,4 there is still some uncertainty over the relative impacts of warnings using graphics versus just text, for the optimal size of warnings, and for how long the impact persists. The rationale for the potentially greater effectiveness of graphic warnings over text-only, is that they provide more information (a picture tells a thousand words) including the evoking of emotional responses to the images, and that together this is more likely to stimulate concerns.
Countries vary considerably in the health warnings they mandate (see Figure 1). For example, the USA has had a small text-based warning on one side of the pack since 1984. The UK introduced new larger warnings in early 2003, increasing the size from 6% on the front and back to 30% on the front and 40% on the back surrounded by a border of 3–4mm, which adds approximately 13% to the total size (in response to an EU Directive 2001/37/EC). Also the number of warnings was increased from 6 to 16 (two for the front, and 14 on the back). Positioning on the pack was not specified, but is usually at the bottom. Australia introduced new graphic warnings from March 2006.5,6 Australian warnings went from six black text on white background warnings covering 25% of the front and 33% of the back of the package, to seven graphic warnings covering 30% of the front and 90% of the back. Canada has had graphic health warnings covering 50% of both main faces since December 2000.7 Both Australian and Canadian warnings are mandated to be at the top of the faces on which they appear.
Research indicates greater impact of the larger graphic Canadian health warnings over the smaller US text warnings8–12 and over small text-only warnings in Mexico.13 Similarly, graphic warnings in Thailand have greater impact than text-based Malaysian warnings,14 which are similar to those in the USA. There is also evidence that if a country changes from smaller to larger more contrasting warnings, the impact of the warnings is increased like in Australia (which in 1995 changed from four UK pre-2003-like warnings to six of the kind depicted in Figure 1 for Australia)15 and the UK (which increased size as well as contrast and content).16
Previous evidence from the ITC Project surveys indicates greater impact on smokers from Canadian graphic warnings relative to text warnings in the USA, UK and Australia.16,17 The differences between the impact of the large graphic Canadian warnings and newer larger text warnings in the UK (but still smaller than Canadian warnings) were mixed. Salience (reading and noticing) was higher for the text warnings when controlling for length of time since introduction. However, for cognitive and behavioural reactions the graphic Canadian warnings had more sustained effects. Further, the levels of foregoing cigarettes in reaction to the new UK warnings never reached the levels in Canada on any wave even though the Canadian warnings were more than two years older.16 The most recent ITC Project work18 has strengthened the existing evidence19,20 that reactions to warnings predict subsequent quitting. We found that forgoing cigarettes as a result of noticing warnings and quit-related cognitive reactions to warnings are consistent prospective predictors of making quit attempts, while warning salience and avoidance were also positively predictive in bivariate analyses, but these effects disappeared in multivariate analyses, suggesting that these effects are mediated through some combination of cognitive reactions and/or forgoing.18
The purpose of the current study was primarily to provide new information on the impact of health warnings on smokers by comparing the short-term impact of new (2006) graphic Australian warnings with; (i) the earlier (2003) UK move to larger text-based warnings; (ii) the continuation of (2000) graphic warnings in Canada; and secondarily, to further assess the sustainability of effects over time.
Methods
The analyses reported here are based on responses of smokers who responded to at least one of the 5 waves of data collection. A full description of the ITC Project conceptual framework and methods have been published elsewhere.21,22 Further details surrounding the ITC Project methods relating to questions on health warnings has also been published.16 To summarise, the ITC Project uses a prospective multi-country cohort design and involves annual telephone surveys of representative cohorts of adult smokers with replenishment each year (to maintain country sample size at 2000 or more, sampled via random-digit dialling). The analyses reported in this paper are restricted to current smokers as ex-smokers have less opportunity to see cigarette packs. Over the five waves of the survey (2002–2006) reported here the country specific samples of smokers are (n=17,773): Canada (n=4,305), United States (n=5,083), United Kingdom (n=4,274), and Australia (n=4,111). The sample size and the year-to-year replenishment and attrition are depicted in Figure 2. In each wave, attrition is largely due to loss, but also due to having quit at that wave (21–30% in waves 2–5).
Measures
At each wave, salience of the health warnings were assessed by asking how often over the preceding month respondents had noticed the warnings and read or looked closely at them (both on 5-point scales: Never to Very often). Using the same time frame they were also asked about two behavioural reactions: frequency (if ever) of forgoing cigarettes as a result of the warnings (coded Ever versus Never), and about four kinds of avoiding the warnings (cover-up, keep out of sight, use cigarette case, or avoid particular labels) from which a binary variable, “no avoidance-any avoidance”, was computed. From wave 2, we also asked about cognitive responses in terms of the extent to which the warnings both made the respondent think about the health risks of smoking, and made them more likely to quit smoking (4-point scales: “Not at all” to “A lot”). Respondents were also asked about various factors that may have motivated them to think about quitting in the last six months, including “warning labels on cigarette packages” with three response options: “Not at all”, “Somewhat”, and “Very much”.
To simplify the analyses, the above measures were factor analysed for each wave and two multi-item factors emerged. As a result, we combined the two salience measures (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.67 – 0.81, waves 1–5) and the three cognitive measures (alpha = 0.78 – 0.80, waves 2–5). For comparisons that controlled for time since implementation, UK levels in 2003 were compared with Australian rates in 2006 (reflecting the three year difference in implementation dates), and UK rates were compared with Canadian rates 2 and 3 years earlier (reflecting the 2.5 year difference in implementation dates).
All analyses were conducted using Stata 10 SE. Generalized estimating equation (GEE) models were employed to test between country differences, as well as any longitudinal changes within countries over the survey waves. The GEE models were conducted on all available data from all waves at which the respondent was a smoker. All point estimates were weighted to reflect appropriate age and gender prevalence estimates within geographic strata, as well as to account for non-response and the survey design.
Results
New graphic Australian warnings
At Wave 5 (2006), after the new (and changed to graphic) Australian warnings were implemented, all four measures of self-reported impact increased significantly among Australian smokers (Figure 3). These significant changes in Wave 5 occurred relative to the Wave 4 survey (all p<0.001), relative to any other wave-to-wave changes, and relative to the other three countries over those two waves (all p<0.001). The increase in avoidance of the warnings was particularly marked (Figure 3d).
New Australian warnings vs other countries
We next compared the absolute levels and wave 4–5 (2005–2006) changes found in Australia with those found in the UK in 2003 (and where possible the 2002–2003 changes) (see Table 1 and Figure 3). Planned comparisons revealed that the salience of the UK warnings both increased more and reached a higher level than the new Australian pack warnings in the survey following their appearance. By contrast, peak levels of cognitive responses were higher in Australia in 2006 than the UK in 2003 (Figure 3b), but because two of these questions were not asked in 2002, we were unable to assess change. There were no significant differences in forgoing cigarettes (Figure 3c) either for peak values or for change. By contrast, avoidance was much greater for the Australian graphic warnings on both peak and change measures (Figure 3d).
Table 1.
Topic area / question | Comparison of peak values (AU vs UK) |
Comparison of mean increase |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AU in 2006 |
UK in 2003 |
p-value | AU 2005 to 2006 |
UK 2002 to 2003 |
p-value# | |
Salience of warnings | 3.51 | 3.90 | <0.001 | 0.75 | 1.16 | <0.001 |
Cognitive reactions to warnings | 2.05 | 1.96 | 0.002 | - | - | - |
Any foregoing of cigarettes | 0.15 | 0.17 | 0.073 | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.984 |
Any avoidance of warnings | 0.37 | 0.21 | <0.001 | 0.25 | 0.11 | <0.001 |
NB. # p-value for Wave-by-Country interaction
We also compared smokers’ levels of response to three measures in Australia in 2006 with those obtained from smokers in Canada in 2002 (at which time the warnings had been in place for nearly 2 years). Smokers in Australia reported higher peak salience levels of the warnings and avoiding looking at the warnings compared to their counterparts in Canada (all p-values <0.001). However, there was no difference between the two countries in forgoing cigarettes as a result of looking at the warnings. Cognitive responses to the warnings were higher in Australia in 2006 (around 6 months post-implementation) than in Canada in 2003, 2.5 years post implementation (p<0.001).
Warning wear-out
We were also able to extend the comparison of Hammond et al16 between the reactions of UK smokers to their text based warnings in 2006 (3.5 years after their introduction) with reactions to the Canadian graphic warnings in 2003 and 2004, allowing for the fact that the UK warnings were introduced around 2.5 years after the Canadian warnings. Reported salience of the UK warnings remained higher than the Canadian warnings (both p-values<0.001). However, for the other measures a different picture emerges. For both cognitive responses and forgoing cigarettes, when controlling for implementation, levels in 2006 in the UK were lower than for Canada (cognitive reaction: p<0.001, for both 2 and 3 year corrections: forgoing cigarettes: p=0.004 and <0.001, respectively).
Finally, we compared reactions to the warnings in 2006 in the two countries that had augmented warnings in recent years to those in the US, which has the least prominent warnings and has not changed them for decades. All comparisons for Canada showed higher responses than the US levels (all p-values<0.001) as did two of the four for the UK (salience and cognitive responses both p values<0.001).
Discussion
Principal findings
Introduction of new graphic warnings in Australia increased their salience, cognitive reactions and behavioural responses to them. Focussing on the two strongest predictors of subsequent quitting, cognitive responses and forgoing cigarettes,18 the Australian warnings produced stronger cognitive responses and a trend towards greater forgoing than the UK text-only warnings, suggesting they are more effective, at least in the short term. Whether this is due to the graphics or the greater total size is less immediately clear.
Hammond et al16 showed that, controlling for time since implementation, Canadian warnings elicited greater avoidance, cognitive responses and forgoing than the UK ones, and we extended this for one more wave, although the effect for forgoing was no-longer significant. We have shown that in the year after implementation the Australian warnings elicited more avoidance and cognitive responses than the UK ones, and non-significantly greater forgoing. Taken together, this suggests that the Canadian warnings are probably the most effective, followed by the Australian ones, then the UK ones.
The crucial policy question is what determines this relative ranking. Identification of even marginal benefits for one form of warning over another is of potential public health importance as the costs of implementing different systems only differ marginally (at least to governments) and the evidence is that warnings can influence subsequent quitting18 and thus any additional benefit is bound to be cost-effective. Warning size is almost certainly critical, but it is less clear as to how to assess it. If it were the total amount of pack warning on the faces, then the Australian warnings, with on average 60% (30% front, 90% back), would be expected to perform better than the Canadian ones (50% of the two main faces) (assuming no impact from the pack inserts in Canada). Consistent with the front of pack being more important, one of us (RB) conducted a survey of how packs (with the pre-2006 text warnings) were displayed by smokers in public settings (tables at restaurants and cafés) in inner Melbourne, Australia. We found that 94% of 160 observed packs were sitting front face up, and only 2% with the back face up (the remaining 4% were standing on one of the smaller faces). Also, it is much easier to remove a cigarette from a flip-top pack when the front face is facing the smoker. All this suggests that it is the size of the warning on the front of the pack that is critical to its immediate impact on smokers (at least for packs with a natural front).
One other factor that might affect warning impact is the degree of difference from the warnings that they replace. The UK warnings are much larger and far more prominent than the warnings they replaced, whereas, for the front of pack, the new Australian warnings are only marginally larger, the main difference being the addition of the graphic imagery. This suggests that any contrast effect probably should favour the UK warnings, so cannot explain why they have slightly lower potency.
If the front of pack warning size is critical to warning impact, then the superiority of the Australian warnings over the UK ones is the strongest evidence to date of graphic warnings being superior to text-only ones as they are the same size as the UK ones, or noticeably smaller if the border is taken into account. Additional evidence from this study which favours graphic over text-only warnings comes from the comparison between the Canadian (graphic) and UK (text-only) warnings. Both the cognitive reactions and forgoing cigarettes were larger and more sustained in response to the Canadian warnings, especially when controlling for the difference in implementation time (see also Hammond et al16). It seems unlikely that these benefits could be explained by the marginally smaller size of the front of pack UK warnings, especially given the comparison with the even smaller Australian ones. We also think it unlikely that positioning on the pack (UK bottom of face, Australia and Canada top of face) could account for the difference, but acknowledge that we cannot test this. The evidence is strengthening for the superiority of graphic warnings in areas where it appears to make the most difference to quitting activity.
We have found marked increases in avoidance of warnings, especially to graphic warnings. There has been concern that this might be a net negative.23 However, the evidence shows that such reactions are generally positively associated with quitting,11,18,24 which should assuage these concerns.
The evidence is building that text-only and graphically-enhanced warnings have at least partly different routes to effect, with the graphic warnings being associated with greater warning avoidance and the text-only ones associated with greater noticing and/or close scrutiny (e.g., from previous ITC Project studies14,16,17,25 and other research26–28). The effect may be in part because text needs to be read to be taken in, while pictures can be taken in at a glance, and partly because smokers are more prone to avoid the more confronting graphic warnings. These findings are also consistent with the evidence29 for increased potency of public health campaigns that arouse an emotional response.
This all suggests that the mechanism of effect for the superiority of graphic warnings is that they elicit greater emotional engagement with the information, and that it is this emotional engagement that drives much of the subsequent quitting related activity.
The overall decline in reactions to new warnings in Canada and the UK over time appears to be only partial given the comparison with levels of reactions to the small text-only warnings in the US. There would appear to be both a multi-year long-term effect of warnings as well as an additional shorter-term effect. This would suggest that there may be health promoting advantages in changing the health warnings from time to time, but failure to do so will not negate all the beneficial effects.
Strengths and weaknesses of the study
The prospective multi-country cohort design used in this study has many strengths for evaluating tobacco control policies.30 Because we can demonstrate both effects on absolute levels and on changes, we are effectively controlling for any differences in characteristics in respondents between the various countries.
The main weakness of the study, as noted in relevant places above, is that the warning systems studied varied on a range of features, so it is not possible to control for some of those effects. Also, the within country and between country control effect is only partial, as the magnitude of the difference between old and new warnings may be a factor affecting responses over and above the features of the new warnings. Responses to questions on warnings in Australia may have also been partly influenced by exposure to tobacco control mass media campaigns using identical imagery to that of some of the new warnings. However, the relevant campaigns run in Australia were conducted several months before the ITC survey months. The fact that graphic warnings on packs lend themselves to be used with complementary campaigns in other media, should not be seen as a limitation for determination of effects, but only a potential additional mechanism for the magnitude of effects.
There are other limitations with such surveys including bias from non-response and attrition, but previous ITC Project studies16 using the first four waves of data found no evidence of “time-in-sample” effects or sample bias due to attrition when analyses were repeated with the “cohort” sample only (i.e., only those who completed all waves) and the “repeat cross-sectional” sample (i.e., the first wave of data from each respondent only). This study relies on self-reports, but we have no reason to believe that they are not valid indicators. Indeed, we do have independent data that new warnings do have behavioral impacts. There were increased calls to the National Australian Quitline after the new warnings were introduced (Unpublished National Quitline data). Others have found similar effects on quitline calls elsewhere.31–33 It is also consistent with reduced tobacco sales in Canada after new warnings were introduced (the biggest decline in a decade34,35). Further, as noted earlier, there is evidence that some of the outcomes we assessed are prospectively predictive of making quit attempts.18–20
In our analyses we have not assessed the impact of the actual thematic content of the warnings. It is very difficult to separate out the effects of multiple warnings as we have no control over exposures, so cannot say anything about the relative merits of the specific warnings in the three countries that have sets of warnings. There is other evidence that the more graphically compelling warnings are rated by smokers as most effective,28 but we don’t know what effects other aspects of the material might add; for example, what impact provision of quit smoking advice (as in Canada on the inside of the pack) or of the Quitline number (as in Australia) might have on overall impact.
Finally this study focuses on reactions of existing smokers, we can say nothing about the potential of strong warnings to prevent uptake, but a benefit is plausible here also based on other work.8,36
Research implications
Further work is necessary to determine sustainability of warning effects, and particularly the use of two rotating sets of warnings, as introduced in Australia. In this study smokers had only been exposed to a set of seven warnings for around 6 months. We need more research on longer-term effects, and of the impact of having a second set of rotating warnings on warning wear-out (6 more were introduced in the months after our survey in Australia and these will rotate year and year about with the 7 original ones). Further research is also necessary to help fine-tune the use of warnings in terms of all the components that remain understudied: the mix of graphics and text, the optimal warning size (e.g., perhaps up to 90% of the front of the pack), the thematic content, the optimal refreshment rate, and the generalisability of the findings to different cultures and stages of the tobacco epidemic.
Implications for health policy
The balance of evidence, to which this study adds, is that larger warnings are more effective; and that graphic warnings evoke stronger cognitive reactions and may produce more sustained cognitive and behavioural reactions than text-only warnings. As there is no additional cost to government of mandating stronger warnings, any benefit, however marginal, should be exploited. Health workers can therefore make evidence-based arguments for their governments exceeding FCTC recommendations (i.e., 50% of the front of the pack) rather than just meeting the FCTC’s minimal requirements. They also have evidence to advocate for graphic warnings in preference to text-only warnings, and should encourage a regulatory system that requires frequent updating of the warnings.
What this paper adds.
This paper builds on the accumulating data from the ITC study to demonstrate the potency of new graphic health warnings introduced in Australia, and of persisting greater impact of Canadian graphic warnings over UK text-only ones. It also provides more evidence that graphic warnings produce greater levels of emotionally-charged reactions. This evidence is used to build the case that graphic warnings are generally more effective than text-only warnings in that they stimulate more quitting activity and that their effects persist for longer than text-only warnings. This strengthens the case for countries going beyond base FCTC obligations and implementing larger graphic warnings.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by grants from the National Cancer Institute of the United States (R01 CA 100362), the Roswell Park Transdisciplinary Tobacco Use Research Center (P50 CA111236), Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (045734), Canadian Institutes of Health Research (57897 and 79551), National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (265903 and 450110), Cancer Research UK (C312/A3726), and Canadian Tobacco Control Research Initiative (014578), with additional support from the Centre for Behavioural Research and Program Evaluation, National Cancer Institute of Canada/ Canadian Cancer Society. None of the sponsors played any direct role in the design and conduct of the study; the collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or the preparation, review, and approval of the manuscript. RB had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. NW, HHY, WH assisted in statistical analyses and the other authors all helped to design the study and write the manuscript.
Footnotes
Ethics clearance: The study protocol was approved by the institutional review boards or research ethics boards of the University of Waterloo (Canada), Roswell Park Cancer Institute (United States), University of Strathclyde (UK), University of Stirling (UK), The Open University (UK), and The Cancer Council Victoria (Australia).
Competing interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
The Corresponding Author has the right to grant on behalf of all authors and does grant on behalf of all authors, an exclusive licence (or non exclusive for government employees) on a worldwide basis to the BMJ Publishing Group Ltd and its Licensees to permit this article (if accepted) to be published in Tobacco Control and any other BMJPGL products to exploit all subsidiary rights, as set out in our licence (http://tc.bmj.com/ifora/licence.pdf).
References
- 1.Doll R, Peto R, Boreham J, et al. Mortality in relation to smoking: 50 years' observations on male British doctors. BMJ. 2004;328:1519. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38142.554479.AE. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chapman S, Liberman J. Ensuring smokers are adequately informed: reflections on consumer rights, manufacturer responsibilities, and policy implications. Tob Control. 2005;14(Suppl 2):ii8–ii13. doi: 10.1136/tc.2005.012591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.World Health Organization. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2003. Final text of WHO Framework Convention on Tobacco Control. http://www.who.int/tobacco/en/. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Strahan EJ, White K, Fong GT, et al. Enhancing the effectiveness of tobacco package warning labels: a social psychological perspective. Tob Control. 2002;11:183–190. doi: 10.1136/tc.11.3.183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Department of Health and Ageing. Canberra: Department of Health and Ageing, Australian Government; 2006. Tobacco Warnings Cigarette Packs Set A. http://www.health.gov.au/internet/wcms/publishing.nsf/Content/health-pubhlth-strateg-drugs-tobacco-warning-packs-A.htm, [Google Scholar]
- 6.Department of Health and Ageing. Canberra: Department of Health and Ageing, Australian Government; 2006. Tobacco Warnings Cigarette Packs Set B. http://www.health.gov.au/internet/wcms/publishing.nsf/Content/health-pubhlth-strateg-drugs-tobacco-warning-packs-B.htm, [Google Scholar]
- 7.Non-Smokers Rights’ Association. Images of Canadian Health Warnings. Toronto: Non-Smokers Rights’ Association; 2003. http://www.nsra-adnf.ca/cms/index.cfm?group_id=1211, [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nimbarte A, Aghazadeh F, Harvey C. Comparison of current U.S. and Canadian cigarette pack warnings. Int Q Community Health Educ. 2005;24:3–27. doi: 10.2190/9PX0-NBG1-0ALA-G5YH. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.O'Hegarty M, Pederson LL, Yenokyan G, et al. Young adults' perceptions of cigarette warning labels in the United States and Canada. Prev Chronic Dis. 2007;4:A27. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.O'Hegarty M, Pederson LL, Nelson DE, et al. Reactions of young adult smokers to warning labels on cigarette packages. Am J Prev Med. 2006;30:467–473. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Peters E, Romer D, Slovic P, et al. The impact and acceptability of Canadian-style cigarette warning labels among U.S. smokers and nonsmokers. Nicotine Tob Res. 2007;9:473–481. doi: 10.1080/14622200701239639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fong G, Cameron R, Brown K, et al. Effects of the Canadian graphic warning labels among high school students: a quasi-experimental longitudinal survey. Presentation at: The 2002 National Conference on Tobacco or Health; 20 November 2002; San Francisco, California. 2002. http://ncth.confex.com/ncth/2002/techprogram/paper_6056.htm. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Thrasher J, Rousu M, Anaya-Ocampo R, et al. Estimating the impact of different cigarette package warning label policies: The auction method. Addict Behav. 2007;32:2916–2925. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Omar M, Fong G, Borland R, et al. Evidence to support graphic warning labels on cigarette packs (Article 11). Presentation at: Evaluating the Evidence Base for FCTC Policies, APACT Workshop; 17 October; Taipei, Taiwan. 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Borland R, Hill D. Initial impact of the new Australian tobacco health warnings on knowledge and beliefs. Tob Control. 1997;6:317–325. doi: 10.1136/tc.6.4.317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hammond D, Fong GT, Borland R, et al. Text and graphic warnings on cigarette packages: findings from the international tobacco control four country study. Am J Prev Med. 2007;32:202–209. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hammond D, Fong GT, McNeill A, et al. Effectiveness of cigarette warning labels in informing smokers about the risks of smoking: findings from the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey. Tob Control. 2006;15(Suppl 3):iii19–iii25. doi: 10.1136/tc.2005.012294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Borland R, Yong HH, Wilson N, et al. How reactions to cigarette packet health warnings influence quitting: findings from the ITC Four-Country survey. Addiction. 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02508.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Borland R. Tobacco health warnings and smoking-related cognitions and behaviours. Addiction. 1997;92:1427–1435. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hammond D, Fong GT, McDonald PW, et al. Impact of the graphic Canadian warning labels on adult smoking behaviour. Tob Control. 2003;12:391–395. doi: 10.1136/tc.12.4.391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Fong G, Cummings K, Borland R, et al. The conceptual framework of the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Policy Evaluation Project. Tob Control. 2006;15(Suppl 3):iii3–iii11. doi: 10.1136/tc.2005.015438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Thompson M, Fong G, Hammond D, et al. The methodology of the International Tobacco Control Policy Evaluation (Four-Country) Survey (ITCPES) Tob Control. 2006;15(suppl III):iii12–iii18. doi: 10.1136/tc.2005.013870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ruiter R, Kok G. Saying is not (always) doing: cigarette warning labels are useless [letter] Eur J Public Health. 2005;15:329. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cki095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hammond D, Fong GT, McDonald PW, Brown KS, Cameron R. Graphic Canadian cigarette warning labels and adverse outcomes: evidence from Canadian smokers. Am J Public Health. 2004;94:1442–1445. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.8.1442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Thrasher JF, Hammond D, Fong GT, et al. Smokers' reactions to cigarette package warnings with graphic imagery and with only text: a comparison between Mexico and Canada. Salud Publica de Mexico. 2007;49(Suppl 2):S233–S240. doi: 10.1590/s0036-36342007000800013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Health Canada. Prepared by Environics Research Group. Ottawa: Health Canada; 1999. Health Warning Testing: Final Report. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Commonwealth of Australia. Canberra: Population Health Division, Department of Health and Ageing; 2003. Developmental Research for New Australian Health Warnings on Tobacco Products. http://www.health.gov.au/Internet/wcms/Publishing.nsf/Content/health-pubhlthstrateg-drugs-tobacco-warnings.htm, [Google Scholar]
- 28.Environics Research Group Ltd. Evaluation of New Warnings on Cigarette Packages (Prepared for Canadian Cancer Society) Toronto: Canadian Cancer Society; 2001. http://www.cancer.ca/ccs/internet/standard/0,3182,3172_334419_436437_langId-en,00.html. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav. 2000;27:591–615. doi: 10.1177/109019810002700506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.IARC. IARC Handbooks of Cancer Prevention. Lyon, France: International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC), World Health Organization; 2008. Methods for Evaluating Tobacco Control Policies (Vol 12) [Google Scholar]
- 31.Willemsen M, Simons C, Zeeman GI. Impact of the new EU health warnings on the Dutch quit line. Tob Control. 2002;11:382. doi: 10.1136/tc.11.4.381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Department of Health. Consultation on the introduction of picture warnings on tobacco packs: Department of Health, (United Kingdom) 2006 URL: http://www.dh.gov.uk/assetRoot/04/13/54/96/04135496.pdf,
- 33.Li J, Grigg M. New Zealand: new graphic warnings encourage registrations with the quitline. Tob Control. 2009;18:72. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.027649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Martens D. Graphic tobacco warnings having desired effect. CMAJ. 2002;166:1453. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Statistics Canada. Production and disposition of tobacco products. [29 April];The Daily. 2002 http://www.statcan.ca/Daily/English/020429/d020429e.htm. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fong G, Cameron R, Brown K, et al. Effects of the Canadian graphic warning labels among high school students: a quasi-experimental longitudinal survey. Presentation at: The 2002 National Conference on Tobacco or Health; 20 November 2002; San Francisco, California. 2002. Abstract at: http://ncth.confex.com/ncth/2002/techprogram/paper_6056.htm. [Google Scholar]