Abstract
Many neurons display significant patterning in their spike trains (e.g. oscillations, bursting), and there is accumulating evidence that information is contained in these patterns. In many cases, this patterning is caused by intrinsic mechanisms rather than external signals. In this review, we focus on spiking activity that displays nonrenewal statistics (i.e. memory that persists from one firing to the next). Such statistics are seen in both peripheral and central neurons and appear to be ubiquitous in the CNS. We review the principal mechanisms that can give rise to nonrenewal spike train statistics. These are separated into intrinsic mechanisms such as relative refractoriness and network mechanisms such as coupling with delayed inhibitory feedback. Next, we focus on the functional roles for nonrenewal spike train statistics. These can either increase or decrease information transmission. We also focus on how such statistics can give rise to an optimal integration timescale at which spike train variability is minimal and how this might be exploited by sensory systems to maximize the detection of weak signals. We finish by pointing out some interesting future directions for research in this area. In particular, we explore the interesting possibility that synaptic dynamics might be matched with the nonrenewal spiking statistics of presynaptic spike trains in order to further improve information transmission.
Keywords: Neural code, Information theory, Interspike interval correlations, Nonrenewal, Weakly electric fish, Model, Noise shaping
Introduction
It is frequently assumed that neural activity can be described by the distribution of interspike intervals (ISIs: i.e. the times between consecutive action potentials) (Moore et al. 1966; Perkel et al. 1967a, b; Shadlen and Newsome 1998; Bastian and Nguyenkim 2001). This measure has been used to describe the firing patterns of both peripheral and central neurons (Maimon and Assad 2009). Such a description assumes that the spiking activity can be described by renewal process where all memory is erased after each action potential firing (Cox and Lewis 1966). The Poisson process is, perhaps, the best-known example of a renewal process: it is characterized by an exponential distribution of ISIs and has been frequently used to describe the firing of cortical neurons (Shadlen and Newsome 1998; Kara et al. 2000). There is, however, accumulating evidence that the spiking activities of many neurons are not well described by renewal processes as they display memory in their spiking activity that persists over multiple action potential firings (i.e. the probability of firing is influenced by the past spiking activity). Such memory can be quantified in terms of the ISI serial correlation coefficients ρj (Perkel et al. 1967a):
where j is the lag. The ISI serial correlation coefficient, ρj, measures whether the value of ISIi has any influence on the value of ISIi+j (i.e. whether they are correlated). In particular, negative values of ρj imply that if the value of ISIi is greater than average, then ISIi+j will tend to have a value that is less than average and vice versa. Thus, if j = 1, then ρ1 < 0 implies that long ISIs will tend to be followed by short ISIs and vice versa. For a renewal process, we have ρj = 0 for all j ≠ 0. As such, the value of a given ISI is independent of the values of the previous ISIs. A nonrenewal process is thus a process for which ρj ≠ 0 for at least one j ≠ 0.
Here, we focus on reviewing accounts of nonrenewal spike train statistics in experimental data, the mechanisms that generate such statistics, and their function in information processing.
Experimental observations of nonrenewal spike train statistics
We first begin with a comprehensive review of the known reports of nonrenewal spike train statistics in experimental data.
Overview
Neurons displaying nonrenewal spiking statistics have been found in many brain areas. The oldest report that is known to the authors is in the activity of motoneurons in humans (Hagiwara 1949). Subsequent studies have found nonrenewal spiking statistics in retinal cat retinal ganglion cells (Kuffler et al. 1957; Rodieck 1967; Levine 1980), cat superior olivary neurons (Goldberg et al. 1964; Tsuchitani and Johnson 1985), cat peripheral auditory fibers (Lowen and Teich 1992), cat cochlear nuclear neurons (Goldberg and Greenwood 1966), cat medullary sympathetic neurons (Lewis et al. 2001), cat sympathetic efferent fibers (Floyd et al. 1982), pigeon vestibular afferents (Correia and Landolt 1977), weakly electric fish electroreceptor neurons (Longtin and Racicot 1997; Chacron et al. 2000; Ratnam and Nelson 2000; Chacron et al. 2001b; Chacron et al. 2005b; Gussin et al. 2007), paddlefish electroreceptors (Bahar et al. 2001; Neiman and Russell 2001; Neiman and Russell 2004), catfish electroreceptors (Schäfer et al. 1995), weakly electric fish electrosensory pyramidal neurons (Doiron et al. 2003; Chacron et al. 2007), honeybee mushroom body neurons (Farkhooi et al. 2009), grasshopper auditory neurons (Schwalger et al. 2010), primate spinothalamic neurons (Surmeier et al. 1989), rat mesencephalic reticular neurons (Lansky and Radil 1987), primate somatosensory cortical neurons (Yamamoto and Nakahama 1983; Lebedev and Nelson 1996; Nawrot et al. 2007), as well as rat entorhinal cortical pyramidal and interneurons (Engel et al. 2008). We note that the last five studies challenge the notion that cortical neurons can be described by a renewal process such as the Poisson process and shall return to this point later.
Negative interspike interval correlations at lag 1
Several investigators have reported nonrenewal spike train statistics in the form of a negative ISI serial correlation coefficient at lag 1 that is significantly different from zero (Kuffler et al. 1957; Goldberg et al. 1964; Yamamoto and Nakahama 1983; Tsuchitani and Johnson 1985; Schäfer et al. 1995; Chacron et al. 2000; Chacron et al. 2007; Nawrot et al. 2007; Engel et al. 2008; Farkhooi et al. 2009). As mentioned above, this implies that ISIs that are shorter than average tend to be followed by ISIs that are longer than average and vice versa, thus giving rise to patterning in the spike train. Figure 1a shows an example recording from a pyramidal cell: it can be seen that this neuron tends to fire doublets as well as triplets of action potentials. Thus, short ISI tends to be followed by long ones and vice versa. The ISI distribution (Fig. 1b) does not capture this. However, a plot of the current ISI as a function of the preceding one, also known as the ISI return map (Doiron et al. 2003; Ellis et al. 2007; Toporikova and Chacron 2009) (Fig. 1c), can be used to illustrate this fact. The slope of a linear fit in this graph will then give us the ISI correlation coefficient ρ1 (Wang 1998). It can be seen that, for this particular neuron, successive ISIs were negatively correlated, leading to negative serial ISI correlation coefficient at lag 1 (Fig. 1d).
In order to better illustrate the effects of ISI correlations on the different measures that were just presented, we have randomly shuffled the ISI sequence. Such a procedure will preserve first-order ISI statistics such as the ISI distribution (compare Fig. 1b, e) but will eliminate any correlations between the ISIs by definition. The ISI return maps were similar in shape (compare Fig. 1c, f). However, computing the average slope of the return map (gray lines in Fig. 1c, f) reveals that the ISI shuffling procedure removes any correlations by calculating the average correlation coefficient. This is reflected in the ISI serial correlation coefficients (compare Fig. 1d, g).
Positive ISI correlations
In some spike trains, small positive serial correlations can decay over several successive lags without alternating in sign (Levine 1980; Lowen and Teich 1992; Lewis et al. 2001; Longtin et al. 2003; Gussin et al. 2007). Although these small positive serial correlations can be quite weak and in some cases might not be detectable using the ISI serial correlation coefficients themselves, they can be detected using other measures of variability such as the Fano factor (Lowen and Teich 1992; Chacron et al. 2001a; Lewis et al. 2001; Longtin et al. 2003). The Fano factor (Fano 1947) is defined as the variance to mean ratio of the spike count distribution (i.e. the number of spikes) during a time window T (Snippe and Koenderink 1992). Note that the spike-count distribution is also referred to as the pulse-number distribution. The Fano factor measures the spike variability at long timescales. It has been observed that the Fano factor increases as a power law (i.e. F(T) ∞ Ta with “a” being the power law exponent) for large T for a wide variety of spike trains (Lowen and Teich 1992, 1996; Lowen et al. 1997; Teich et al. 1997; Ratnam and Nelson 2000; Gussin et al. 2007). As power law dependencies are self-similar on multiple timescales, this phenomenon has been referred to a fractal firing pattern and has been reviewed extensively. It appears that this phenomenon is seen ubiquitously in experimental data (Teich 1992).
ISI correlations that alternate in sign over successive lags
Some neurons display ISI correlations that decay over several lags in an alternating fashion (i.e. a negative ISI correlation coefficient is followed by a positive one, which is then followed by a negative one that is weaker in magnitude, etc…). This is, perhaps, seen most prominently in the electroreceptors of the paddlefish in which these serial correlation coefficients decay over hundreds of lags (Bahar et al. 2001; Neiman and Russell 2001, 2004). Some bursting electroreceptors of weakly electric fish display alternating ISI correlations, but they tend to decay over at most a few lags (Chacron et al. 2001b, 2005a).
Summary
In summary, there are multiple reports of ISI correlations across different brain areas and organisms. These reports as well as the type of correlations reported are summarized in the table below (Table 1).
Table 1.
Reference | Organism and neuron type | ISI correlation structure |
---|---|---|
Bahar et al. (2001), Neiman and Russell (2001), Neiman and Russell (2004) | Paddlefish electroreceptors | Alternating ISI correlations that decay over several lags |
Chacron et al. (2000), Chacron et al. (2005b), Gussin et al. (2007), Longtin and Racicot (1997), Ratnam and Nelson (2000) | Weakly electric fish electroreceptors | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 and positive ISI correlations that decay over several lags |
Chacron et al. (2001b) | Weakly electric fish electroreceptors | Alternating ISI correlations that decay over a few lags |
Chacron et al. (2007), Doiron et al. (2003) | Weakly electric fish electrosensory pyramidal neurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Correia and Landholt (1977) | Pigeon vestibular afferents | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Engel et al. (2008) | Rat entorhinal cortical pyramidal and interneurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Farkhooi et al. (2009) | Honeybee mushroom body neurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Floyd et al. (1982) | Cat sympathetic efferent fibers | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Goldberg et al. (1964), Tsuchitami and Johnson (1985) | Cat superior olivary neurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Goldberg and Greenwood (1966) | Cat cochlear nuclear neurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Hagiwara (1949) | Human motoneurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Kuffler et al. (1957), Rodieck (1967), Levine (1980) | Cat retinal ganglion cells | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Lansky and Radil (1987) | Rat mesencephalic reticular neurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Lewis et al. (2001) | Cat medullary sympathetic neurons | Positive ISI correlations that decay over several lags |
Lowen and Teich (1992) | Cat peripheral auditory fibers | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 and positive ISI correlations that decay over several lags |
Nawrot et al. (1997), Yamamoto and Nakahama (1983), Lebedev and Nelson (1996) | Primate somatosensory cortical neurons | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Schäfer et al. (1995) | Catfish electroreceptors | Negative ISI correlation at lag 1. |
Schwalger et al. (2010) | Grasshopper auditory neurons | Positive ISI correlations that decay over several lags |
Surmeier et al. (1989) | Primate spinothalamic neurons | Positive and negative ISI correlation at lag 1 |
Mechanisms that underlie the generation of nonrenewal spike train statistics
In this section, we review some of the mechanisms and mathematical models that give rise to nonrenewal spike train statistics.
Mechanisms that give rise to a single negative ISI correlation coefficient at lag 1
We will first concentrate on the mechanisms that give rise to a negative ISI correlation coefficient at lag 1. Intuitively, these mechanisms must have the following in common: when two or more action potentials occur during a short-time period (i.e. a short ISI), the mechanism must prevent or delay the onset of the next action potential firing thereby giving rise to a long ISI. There are both intrinsic and network mechanisms that can accomplish this.
Cumulative growth in the spike after hyperpolarization
It was recognized early on that growth in the afterhyperpolarization (AHP) following each action potential during repetitive firing could give rise to negative interspike interval correlations (Eccles 1953; Goldberg et al. 1964). This has led to the formulation of a phenomenological model by Geisler and Goldberg (1966). This model is of the leaky integrate-and-fire type (Tuckwell 1988) in which the membrane potential is integrated in time until it reaches a given threshold value and is illustrated in Fig. 2a. Immediately after this, the threshold is set to infinity during the duration of the absolute refractory period and the voltage is decremented, thereby mimicking an increased AHP (Geisler and Goldberg 1966). It is important to note that repetitive firing will lead to an accumulation in the AHP (Fig. 2a, light gray), thereby increasing the distance between voltage and threshold and thereby delaying the next firing (Fig. 2a). As such, a sequence of short inters-pike intervals will give rise to large AHPs whereas longer interspike intervals will give rise to small AHPs. It is then easy to see that short/long interspike intervals will tend to be followed by long/short interspike intervals as larger AHPs will tend to delay the next action potential more than smaller AHPs (Geisler and Goldberg 1966). While the ISI probability distribution does not capture this (Fig. 2b), this is illustrated in the ISI return map (Fig. 2c) as well as the ISI serial correlation coefficients (Fig. 2d). Variants of this model have been later proposed and studied in detail (Smith and Goldberg 1986; Lansky et al. 1992; Keat et al. 2001; Liu and Wang 2001; Benda et al. 2010).
Biophysical models of the accumulation in the AHP during repetitive firing based on the Hodgkin–Huxley formalism (Hodgkin and Huxley 1952) have also been proposed and studied with respect to accumulation in the AHP during repetitive firing. In particular, calcium-activated potassium channels have been shown to mediate the spike AHP (Sah 1996; Faber and Sah 2003). These channels are not sensitive to the transmembrane potential but are instead activated by increases in the internal calcium concentration (Sah and Faber 2002). A mathematical model that incorporates these channels has been shown to give rise to negative ISI correlations (Wang 1998). In this model, calcium accumulates inside the cell during repetitive firing, which causes an accumulation in the AHP, thereby delaying the onset of the next action potential, thereby giving rise to negative ISI correlations.
Threshold fatigue
While it was once thought that the action potential threshold was fixed, there is accumulating experimental evidence that this is not the case. Indeed, it is known that the threshold varies dynamically and is strongly dependent on the previous spiking history (Azouz and Gray 1999, 2000, 2003; Chacron et al. 2007). Variations in the action potential threshold have been frequently considered in mathematical models (Geisler and Goldberg 1966; Weiss 1966; MacGregor and Oliver 1974; Horn and Usher 1989; Eckhorn et al. 1990; Abeles 1991; Gabbiani 1996; Jolivet et al. 2006). In particular, a phenomenological model of the leaky integrate-and-fire type has also been studied. In this model, the threshold is incremented by a constant amount immediately after action potential firing and decreases exponentially to a baseline value during quiescence (Fig. 2e). Repetitive firing will lead to a cumulative increase in threshold that will delay the next firing due to an increased distance between voltage and threshold. We refer to this cumulative increase in threshold as threshold fatigue.
As such, although the biophysical mechanisms of cumulative growth of the AHP and of cumulative increases in the threshold are quite different in nature, the basic principle by which they give rise to negative ISI correlations is the same: an increased distance between voltage and threshold after repetitive firing. It is thus not surprising that both the leaky integrate-and-fire model with dynamic threshold and the previously mentioned model based on the accumulation of the AHP give rise to similar ISI distributions (compare Fig. 2b, f), ISI return maps (compare Fig. 2c, g), and ISI serial correlation coefficients (compare Fig. 2d, h). Moreover, both models have been shown to be qualitatively equivalent for the most part (Liu and Wang 2001), although a recent study has pointed out some differences in terms of spike frequency adaptation (Benda et al. 2010). The leaky integrate-and-fire model with dynamic threshold can furthermore successfully reproduce experimental data from the electroreceptors of weakly electric fish (Chacron et al. 2000, 2003, 2004a, c; Brandman and Nelson 2002; Chacron 2006; Savard et al. 2011) as well as vestibular afferents (Sadeghi et al. 2007). Recent experimental evidence shows that threshold fatigue is also present in electrosensory pyramidal neurons (Chacron et al. 2007). Such fatigue can be modeled by shifting the activation curve of the sodium current to larger voltages, which effectively raises the action potential threshold (Chacron et al. 2007). It is thus likely that threshold fatigue is due to cumulative sodium channel desensitization (Mickus et al. 1999). Multiple biophysical mechanisms can thus give rise to negative ISI correlations at lag 1.
Synaptic depression
The parallels between the leaky integrate-and-fire model with threshold fatigue and models of synaptic depression (i.e. a decrease in the postsynaptic potential amplitude during repetitive presynaptic stimulation) have been pointed out before (Chacron et al. 2003). Indeed, synaptic depression can give rise to relative refractoriness (Häusser and Roth 1997), and models of synaptic depression show similarities with the previously described leaky integrate-and-fire model with threshold fatigue (Fuhrmann et al. 2002; Goldman et al. 2002). In fact, it has been shown that such models can transform a Poisson presynaptic spike train (note that Poisson spike trains are renewal in nature) into a postsynaptic spike train that displays a negative serial correlation coefficient at lag 1 (Goldman et al. 2002). This can be understood intuitively as follows: it is clear that two or more presynaptic action potentials that occur in rapid succession will give rise to an amount of synaptic depression that is inversely proportional to the time between consecutive action potentials (i.e. the ISI). As such, the probability of an EPSP causing a postsynaptic action potential decreases with each subsequent presynaptic action potential as the EPSP amplitude decreases. Let us assume that only the first two EPSPs cause postsynaptic action potentials (i.e. a short ISI). A long period of silence in the presynaptic spike train is then necessary for the synapse to recover from depression so that a presynaptic action potential can again have sufficiently large amplitude to give rise to a postsynaptic action potential. As such, the short ISI in the postsynaptic neuron will tend to be followed by a long one. We therefore expect that, under these conditions, the postsynaptic spike train would display negative ISI correlations.
Delayed feedback
It is well known that neurons in cortical areas of the nervous system are the target of massive descending projections (Cajal 1909; Hollander 1970; Ostapoff et al. 1990; Maler et al. 1991; Sherman and Guillery 2002, 2006) and that such pathways can give rise to interesting dynamics such as bursting (Krahe and Gabbiani 2004; Chacron and Bastian 2008; Toporikova and Chacron 2009; Avila Akerberg et al. 2010). In particular, Doiron et al. (2003) showed that neurons with intrinsic renewal spike train statistics could display nonrenewal spike train statistics simply by introducing coupling between them through global delayed inhibitory feedback. This can be understood intuitively as follows. Because of temporal summation, short interspike intervals can give rise to delayed inhibition that can prevent subsequent firing in a manner similar to that of the AHP described above. As such, it is expected that the spike trains of individual neurons in a network coupled with inhibitory delayed feedback can display a negative ISI correlation at lag 1. We note that it is, however, possible that other network mechanisms might give rise to a negative ISI correlation at lag 1.
Mechanisms that give rise to positive ISI correlations
As mentioned above, weak and positive ISI correlations are often present in experimental data. One example spike train from an electrosensory afferent neuron shows a power law dependency at large time windows for the Fano factor (Fig. 3a, black). As randomly shuffling the ISI sequence eliminates this dependency (Fig. 3a, gray), it must be due to correlations between the ISIs (Lowen and Teich 1992). It has been shown experimentally (Lewis et al. 2001) that such behavior is caused by positive ISI correlations that decay over many lags.
Modeling and theoretical studies have shown that addition of a weak but colored zero-mean noise with a positive and slowly decaying autocorrelation function (i.e. a long correlation time) can give rise to positive ISI correlations that decay over many lags (Chacron et al. 2001a; Middleton et al. 2003a, 2003b). Intuitively, this can be understood as follows. Imagine that the noise is positive and thus promotes increased action potential firing (i.e. ISIs that are shorter than average). Since the noise has a long correlation time, it will tend to stay positive for a long time. As such, the ISIs will tend to stay shorter than average for this time, leading to positive ISI correlations (note that a similar argument can be made when the noise is negatively correlated). The Fano factor curve from such a model also shows power law scaling but only for a finite range of time windows before saturating (Fig. 3b, black). This is because the noise process has a finite correlation time and is unlike experimental data whose Fano factor seems to scale as a power law for as long as experimentally measurable (Lowen and Teich 1992). However, the range of time windows over which the Fano factor displays power law scaling can be extended to arbitrarily large values in the model by simply increasing the noise’s correlation time (Longtin et al. 2003; Middleton et al. 2003a, 2003b). Analysis of the serial correlation coefficients of the model reveals that coefficients at lags ≥2 are positive and decay over hundreds of lags (Fig. 3c). A recent study has uncovered a possible source for this noise in the stochastic opening and closing of voltage-gated potassium channels (Schwalger et al. 2010). Possible functional consequences for such correlations are discussed further in section IIIB.
Mechanisms that give rise to alternating ISI correlations
Alternating ISI correlations have been observed experimentally in paddlefish electroreceptors (Bahar et al. 2001; Neiman and Russell 2001, 2002, 2004, 2005). The sensory epithelial hair cells display ~25-Hz oscillations that can be recorded as voltage or current fluctuations in the canals leading from skin pores to the sensory epithelia. These cells then make excitatory synaptic contact unto the afferent terminals. These fire continuously (“spontaneously”) at a nearly fixed frequency in the range of 30–70 Hz. As such, paddlefish electroreceptors can be modeled as two unidirectionally coupled oscillators: the noisy 25-Hz oscillation from the epithelial hair cells drives the afferent fibers that show an intrinsic tendency to oscillate at 30–70 Hz. It was shown that both types of oscillators affect the firing of primary afferents, which results in an output that shows both fundamental frequencies (i.e. 25 Hz and 30–70 Hz). In particular, it was found that these ISI correlations were strongest in magnitude when the epithelial oscillation frequency is half that of the afferent fiber oscillator frequency (e.g. 25 and 50 Hz) (Neiman and Russell 2004). In this situation, one will get two action potentials per epithelial hair cell oscillation cycle. Therefore, an ISI shorter than average will be followed by an ISI longer than average, then an ISI shorter than average, etc… This alternating pattern of short and long ISIs is what causes the spike trains of paddlefish electroreceptors to display decaying ISI correlations that alternate in sign over successive lags (Neiman and Russell 2001, 2004, 2005).
Function of nonrenewal spike train statistics
In this section, we review the known functional consequences of nonrenewal spike train statistics. Before doing so, however, we review some concepts in signal detection, information theory, and noise shaping.
Neural coding
Neurons display trial-to-trial variability in their responses to repeated presentations of a given stimulus. While the function of this variability is still being debated (Stein et al. 2005), it is generally agreed that at least part of this variability arises from noise, which is due to the random opening of ion channels as well as the large synaptic bombardment that neurons are subjected to in vivo (Destexhe et al. 2003; London et al. 2010). It is possible to simply average away this variability in order to obtain an estimate of the mean response to a given stimulus, and this is frequently done when computing measures such as the peristimulus time histogram. However, it is at best unclear how the organism might perform this operation (Zohary et al. 1994). We will thus take the point of view here that neural variability must be taken into account.
Signal detection theory
Signal detection theory (Green and Swets 1966) aims at quantifying the performance of an ideal observer in distinguishing between different alternatives. In most cases, signal detection theory is used to distinguish between two alternatives that give rise to different measured outputs. In most cases, the output is the number of action potentials (i.e. the pulse number) that occurs within a given time window (Snippe and Koenderink 1992). Since neurons display variability in their responses to repeated presentations of the same stimulus (Stein et al. 2005), the pulse number obeys a distribution with nonzero variance. The performance of the ideal observer is inversely related to the amount of overlap between the two distributions (Fig. 4a) and has been quantified using various measures such as the discriminability index d′ or the minimum probability of misclassification (Poor 1994). The discriminability index d′ is defined by (Green and Swets 1966):
Within this framework, the optimal detector is the one that minimizes the degree of overlap between the pulse-number distributions obtained under different conditions and therefore maximizes d′. This can be done by increasing the difference between their means and/or decreasing their variances (Fig. 4a). We shall see below that negative ISI correlations reduce the spike count variance while positive ISI correlations increase the spike count variance and how this can increase or decrease signal detectability, respectively.
Information theory
Information theory was first developed in the context of communication channels (Shannon 1948) and has gained popularity in neuroscience (Borst and Theunissen 1999). Our goal here is not to provide an exhaustive review of the uses of information theory in neuroscience but merely to introduce some intuitive concepts. The interested reader is directed to the following references that provide a good summary of the uses of information theory in neuroscience (Rieke et al. 1996; Borst and Theunissen 1999; Dayan and Abbott 2001; Gabbiani and Cox 2010).
Let us assume that there is a stimulus set S = [S1,…, SN] and that each stimulus Si gives rise to a set of neural responses Ri = [R1i,…, RNi]. For example, the response could be the mean firing rate. When stimulus S1 is presented, the response is R11 on trial 1, R12 on trial 2, etc… Because neurons display trial-to-trial variability in their responses to sensory input, a given sensory stimulus gives rise to a set of responses Ri (Fig. 4b). The number of possible responses increases with larger variability in general. In contrast, the distance between sets Ri and Rj is primarily determined by how different the responses to the different stimuli Si and Sj are. Intuitively, if the sets Ri are disjoint (i.e. they do not overlap), then observing an individual response Rik on trial k would allow us to tell which stimulus Si was given on that trial (Fig. 4b). On the other hand, if there is large overlap between the sets Ri, then observing a given response Rik will not allow us to tell which stimulus Si was given on trial k. It is possible to make these intuitive concepts about information mathematically formal using the following definition for information (Shannon 1948; Cover and Thomas 1991):
where I(R, S) is the information contained in the response R about the stimulus S. H(R) is the entropy of the response and H(R/S) is the entropy of the response given that we have observed the stimulus S. Intuitively, the entropy is related to the amount of uncertainty or randomness in a process. Thus, a process that has only one possible outcome (i.e. is completely predictable) would have zero entropy (the minimum), while a process that has uncertainty (e.g. a coin toss) would have higher entropy. We note that the entropy is always positive. As such, the mutual information I(R, S) is maximized when H(R) is maximized and H(R/S) is minimized. This corresponds to a case illustrated above where the set of responses obtained to different stimuli are as different as possible, but the response to a given stimulus is always the same. The mutual information is usually measured in bits, and X bits implies that the system can discriminate between 2X stimuli in the noise-free situation (Thomson and Kristan 2005). As such, ISI correlations can alter information transmission by affecting the response entropy H(R) or the noise entropy H(R/S), or both. We will see below that ISI correlations can affect both.
Moreover, as sensory stimuli are frequently characterized by their temporal frequency content, it is more informative to look at the mutual information rate density (i.e. the mutual information rate per frequency) rather than the mutual information rate itself. This allows one to then quantify which frequency component contained in the stimulus is the neuron transmitting more information (Borst and Theunissen 1999). Te mutual information rate density at a given frequency f is related to the ratio of signal power to noise power in the response at that frequency (i.e. the signal-to-noise ratio or SNR). Greater SNR values give rise to greater MI density values. We will see below that ISI correlations can have differential effects on the MI density that are frequency dependent.
Noise shaping
We have mentioned earlier that neurons display variability in their responses to repeated presentations of the same stimulus. If one assumes that noise is detrimental to information transmission by neurons, then it makes sense to minimize this noise. However, as this might not be possible, one could then imagine the following scenario. If the signal that we want to encode contains temporal frequencies within a given range, then it may be possible to minimize the noise’s frequency content within that range by transferring power from that range to another. One would then have a greater SNR and thus increased information transmission in the former frequency range at the expense of a weaker SNR and thus reduced information in the latter frequency range. This concept is illustrated in Fig. 4c and is known as noise shaping. Note that, in the case of noise shaping, the total noise power (i.e. the power integrated over frequency) remains the same. Noise shaping is found in engineering devices such as Josephson junctions and sigma–delta modulators (Wiesenfeld and Satija 1987; Norsworthy et al. 1997; Yacomotti et al. 1999) and has been proposed to be used by the brain as a mechanism to deal with noise (Shin 1993; Shin 2001). Further, a modeling study has shown that a network of neurons coupled with inhibition could display noise shaping (Maar et al. 1999).
Effects of ISI correlations on neural coding
In this section, we review some of the important results on the function of ISI correlations in neural coding.
Effects of ISI correlations on signal detection
We first focus on the role of negative ISI correlations. Using a model of a spinal motoneuron and assuming a rate code, Birk (1972) showed that the decoded representation of a spike train with negative ISI correlations will display less variability than a spike train with the same firing rate but with independent ISIs (Birk 1972). Numerical simulations have since then shown that the pulse-number distribution of a spike train with negative ISI correlations displays less variance than that of a spike train with independent ISIs (Ratnam and Nelson 2000; Chacron et al. 2001a). This has important consequences for the detection of weak signals using signal detection theory as negative ISI correlations can then reduce the variance of the pulse-number distribution in a fixed time window (Fig. 5a), which can increase discriminability (Ratnam and Nelson 2000; Chacron et al. 2001a; Brandman and Nelson 2002; Goense and Ratnam 2003). However, the magnitude of the effect depends on the length of the time window (i.e. the integration time). The magnitude of the effect essentially depends on the difference between the Fano factors of the raw and shuffled ISI sequences (Fig. 5a, black and dark gray curves): for small time windows, these correlations will have little effect and maximum effect for larger time windows (Fig. 5b, black and dark gray curves).
In contrast to negative ISI correlations, the functional role of positive ISI correlations is less well understood. As mentioned previously, positive ISI correlations will tend to increase spike train variability, which is detrimental for signal detection. Their effect is to increase the spike count variance for large time windows (Fig. 5b, compare light and dark gray). It was shown that an interaction between short-term negative ISI and long-term positive ISI correlations could create a minimum in spike train variability as quantified by the Fano factor for a given time window (Ratnam and Nelson 2000; Chacron et al. 2001a; Longtin et al. 2003). This effect is illustrated in Fig. 5. One can then show that signal detection as quantified by d′ is minimum at that time window with respect to a renewal process (Fig. 5c). It is possible that such a coding strategy is used by weakly electric fish to detect prey as the pulse-number distributions in their peripheral electroreceptive neurons display minimal variability for integration timescales that correspond to the integration time window inferred from prey capture behavior (Nelson and MacIver 1999; Ratnam and Nelson 2000; Chacron et al. 2001a; Longtin et al. 2003). A recent study has shown similar effects in the Paddlefish electro-receptors (Engel et al. 2009). It is important to note that the value of the integration time for which the spike train variability as quantified by the Fano factor is minimum depends on many parameters such as the bias current and noise intensity. As such, it is possible to have neurons display minimum variability over a wide range of integration time window values (Middleton et al. 2003a, b).
Effects of ISI correlations on information transmission: noise shaping
It was also found that negative ISI correlations can improve information transmission of time-varying stimuli (Chacron et al. 2001a) (Fig. 6a). This was done by comparing two mathematical models that had similar ISI distributions and responses to sensory input as quantified by linear systems identification (Chacron et al. 2001a). Chacron et al. (2001a) found that although negative ISI correlations reduced the signal entropy H(R), they reduced the noise entropy H(R/S) even more, thereby increasing the mutual information I(R, S) = H(R) − H(R/S).
Any comprehensive understanding of coding in stimulus-evoked spike trains of neurons requires a characterization of neural discharge components that are directly associated with the stimulus (signal) and those that are not (noise) (Roddey et al. 2000; Stein et al. 2005; Faisal et al. 2008). In neurons that display high resting (i.e. in the absence of stimulation) discharge activity, the component that is associated with noise is given by the power spectrum of this resting discharge activity (Chacron et al. 2005a). This assumes that the stimulus intensity is small so that the perturbations brought about by the stimulus are small in magnitude and will thus scale linearly with the stimulus intensity. Experimental results have shown that this is the case in both electroreceptor (Chacron et al. 2005b) and vestibular afferents (Sadeghi et al. 2007), provided that the stimulus intensity is not too high (but see (Savard et al. 2011)). Investigators have therefore focused on understanding how ISI correlations can alter the power spectrum of the baseline activity. Comparing the baseline power spectra of renewal and nonrenewal models that have the same first-order ISI statistics reveals important differences (Fig. 6b). In particular, it is seen that the nonrenewal model displays lower power at low (<100 Hz) frequencies but higher power for higher (between 100 Hz and 200 Hz) frequencies (Fig. 6b).
This has prompted researchers to build two simple models for which it is possible to compute the resting discharge power spectrum analytically (Chacron et al. 2004b; Lindner et al. 2005). In the first model, once the voltage reaches a threshold, it is decremented by a constant amount following each action potential (Fig. 6c). This way it will keep the memory of the previous ISI onto the next one. In the second model, once the voltage reaches a threshold, it is reset to a random value (Fig. 6d). It is thus not surprising that the first model displays negative ISI correlations (as it will preserve memory) while the second does not. We shall henceforth refer to the first model as nonrenewal and the second model as renewal. Comparing the noise power spectra of both models revealed qualitatively similar differences with those seen with more realistic models (compare Fig. 6a, e). Further investigation revealed that both models have identical signal discharge components as quantified by their transfer functions (Chacron et al. 2004b) and that frequencies associated with a lower/higher resting discharge spectrum are associated with higher/lower information rates (Fig. 6f). This demonstrates how noise shaping by ISI correlations can lead to information shaping (i.e. increasing and reducing information in different frequency ranges).
Effects of nonrenewal spiking statistics on neural coding at the network level
It is well known that the brain uses a distributed representation of information through neural populations. How well do networks of nonrenewal neurons transmit information in comparison with networks of renewal neurons? Two recent studies have explored this interesting question (Avila Akerberg and Chacron 2009; Avila Akerberg and Chacron 2010). These studies showed that renewal and nonrenewal neurons transmit information differentially when all-to-all coupling is introduced (Fig. 7a). Indeed, nonrenewal networks with excitatory/inhibitory coupling transmit more/less information than nonrenewal networks of the same size with no coupling. In contrast, renewal networks with excitatory/inhibitory coupling transmit less/ more information than renewal networks of the same size with no coupling (Fig. 7b). As such, excitatory and inhibitory couplings each have opposite effects on networks of renewal and nonrenewal model neurons. As anatomical studies have found that the majority of synaptic connections in the brain are excitatory, it is possible that such connections increase information transmission by neurons that display nonrenewal spike train statistics.
Conclusions and future directions
In this section, we provide a brief summary of the reviewed results and outline some future directions for research pertaining to the effects of nonrenewal spike train statistics on neural coding.
Summary
We have reviewed the known mechanisms that give rise to ISI correlations and the known consequences of ISI correlations on information transmission and neural coding. Any of cumulative growth of the AHP, cumulative increase in threshold during repetitive firing, synaptic depression, or delayed inhibitory feedback can give rise to negative ISI correlations. In contrast, a weak noise with a long correlation time can give rise to positive ISI correlations that decay over many lags. In general, negative ISI correlations can significantly increase information transmission and signal detection by decreasing the noise entropy and spike count variance, respectively. However, their effects are dependent on the stimulus’ temporal frequency content as well as coupling between neurons. As such, it is clear that ISI correlations are found across senses and organisms and can have important effects on information transmission. However, the exact nature of these is likely to depend on multiple factors including the nature of the neural code used by a given brain area where neurons display nonrenewal spike train statistics and the decoding of the information transmitted by these neurons in higher-order areas. We thus conclude by highlighting some future directions addressing the following key issues: (1) developing an integrative approach to obtain a better understanding of the functional roles of nonrenewal spike train statistics; (2) understanding whether information carried in nonrenewal spike trains is actually decoded by higher-order neurons and, if so, how; and (3) are the effects of ISI correlations universal across systems?
Toward an integrative approach to understand how the mechanisms that give rise to nonrenewal spike train statistics affect information transmission
As mentioned previously, the mechanisms that give rise to nonrenewal spike train statistics can have other effects such as spike frequency adaptation. Moreover, recent studies have shown that these mechanisms not only alter the spike count variance but also can alter its mean and possibly higher moments (Chacron et al. 2007). Moreover, these mechanisms can also strongly alter temporal frequency tuning (Ellis et al. 2007; Mehaffey et al. 2008; Lindner et al. 2009; Peron and Gabbiani 2009). Yet, to date, all studies that have explored the role of nonrenewal spike train statistics on information transmission or signal detection have done so by comparing models that display identical first-order ISI statistics and frequency tuning. How these effects interact is largely unknown to this day. In order to better illustrate our point, we give a couple of examples below.
For example, in terms of signal detection theory, this implies that the mechanisms that give rise to ISI correlations can not only affect the spike count variance but also the mean spike count. As such, one could conceive a scenario where negative ISI correlations would lead to a lower spike count variance of each distribution as well as lead to a greater difference between their means. Both effects would then act synergistically to lead to better signal detection. Alternatively, one could conceive another scenario where the advantage of a reduced spike count variance is offset by a smaller separation between the means.
Alternatively, theoretical studies have shown that spike frequency adaptation reduces the response gain to low-frequency stimuli (Benda and Herz 2003), which might offset the decrease in noise power caused by negative ISI correlations and therefore the signal-to-noise ratio might remain the same. A recent study suggests that synaptic depression, which can cause negative ISI correlations as reviewed above, might actually not alter the signal-to-noise ratio (Lindner et al. 2009), although this is strongly dependent on the measure used.
We thus argue that future studies should take a more integrative approach (i.e. consider all the effects of the mechanisms that give rise to nonrenewal spike train statistics) in order to gain a better understanding of the effects of ISI correlations on information transmission.
Nonrenewal spike train statistics and population coding
Another shortcoming of previous studies interested in understanding the role of nonrenewal spike train statistics on information transmission is that they have mostly concentrated on single neurons. Although some recent theoretical studies have highlighted potentially interesting effects in networks of nonrenewal neurons (Avila Akerberg and Chacron 2009, 2010), the effects of nonrenewal spike trains statistics on population coding remain largely unexplored to this day. While peripheral sensory neurons such as auditory fibers or electroreceptors displaying non-renewal spike trains might not be coupled, this is certainly not the case for central neurons. Indeed, cortical neurons (Engel et al. 2008), cochlear nuclear neurons (Goldberg and Greenwood 1966) as well as electrosensory pyramidal cells (Chacron and Bastian 2008) all receive thousands of synaptic input and display nonrenewal spike train statistics. Such input most likely contributes to experimentally observed correlations between the activities of neighboring neurons across sensory systems (Meister et al. 1995; Averbeck et al. 2006; Averbeck and Lee 2006). While there is still an ongoing debate on whether these correlations are beneficial or detrimental for information transmission, it is widely agreed that their presence will significantly alter the way that information is transmitted by neural populations and its subsequent decoding by higher-order neurons (Nirenberg et al. 2001; Schneidman et al. 2003, 2006; Latham and Nirenberg 2005; Averbeck et al. 2006). How nonrenewal spike train statistics affect correlated activity among neurons is an interesting topic that has been largely overlooked so far. For example, it is known that electroreceptor afferents in weakly electric fish that display negative ISI correlations can synchronize in response to transient high-frequency stimuli (Benda et al. 2005, 2006; Chacron et al. 2005b), but do nonrenewal spike train statistics influence this synchronization and, if so, how? In general, further experimental and theoretical studies are needed to understand the effects of nonrenewal spike train statistics on population coding across systems.
Decoding information carried by nonrenewal spike trains
While it has been shown that ISI correlations can significantly alter information transmission, this information is only useful to the organism if it is being decoded by higher-order neurons. If such decoding does not occur, then one could argue ISI correlations are nothing more than an artifact caused by mechanisms with another function. We argue that this hypothesis has to be tested on a case-by-case basis and review some recent experimental results suggesting that such decoding actually does occur.
The decoding of information carried by nonrenewal spike trains is an important problem. We shall consider two cases here: decoding information carried by the spike count (rate coding) and decoding information carried in the timing of spikes (temporal coding).
For the spike count, it is clear that a temporal integrator neuron can simply integrate over the time window for which the spike train variability of its afferent nonrenewal neurons is minimum and therefore take advantage of the reduced variability provided by negative ISI correlations, and this has been argued previously (Ratnam and Nelson 2000; Chacron et al. 2001a; Engel et al. 2009). Such neurons have been observed in the anuran midbrain, and the mechanisms by which they achieve this function are well understood and have been described elsewhere (Edwards et al. 2008; Leary et al. 2008; Rose et al. 2010).
However, decoding information that might be contained in the timing of action potentials in nonrenewal spike trains is less well understood. A recent theoretical study has, however, proposed how synaptic plasticity could actually decorrelate a presynaptic spike train that displays ISI correlations (Nesse et al. 2010). Such decorrelation has obvious advantages for information transmission such as redundancy reduction and can be more easily decoded by downstream neurons (Barlow and Levick 1969). Nesse et al. (2010) provide a simple algorithm for decorrelation that can be tested experimentally.
Future experimental studies should focus on whether such algorithms are used by the central nervous system to decode information contained either in the spike count or in the spike timing of nonrenewal neurons.
Matching synaptic dynamics to nonrenewal spike train statistics in order to optimize weak signal detectability
While it has been shown that negative ISI correlations can reduce the spike count variance and thereby increase signal detectability, it is possible that other mechanisms might act in concert to further increase signal detectability. Recent experimental and theoretical evidences suggest that the dynamics of fast synaptic depression is matched to nonrenewal spike train statistics in order to maximize signal detection (Khanbabaie et al. 2010). It is important to note that the effects of synaptic depression are independent of those due to nonrenewal presynaptic spike train statistics. However, both act in concert in order to reduce variability and maximize signal detectability. This combination of these effects might be able to explain the remarkable behavioral acuity displayed by weakly electric fish when detecting prey (Khanbabaie et al. 2010). This study provides compelling evidence that the central nervous system uses multiple mechanisms in order to reduce spike train variability and support the hypothesis that information contained in nonrenewal spike trains is being decoded by higher-order neurons. However, further studies are needed to test for such cooperative mechanisms in other systems where sensory neurons display nonrenewal spike train statistics. In particular, the short-term synaptic depression described by Khanbabaie et al. (2010) is found in both the auditory and somatosensory systems.
Universality of nonrenewal spike train statistics
While it is frequently assumed that cortical neurons can be modeled by Poisson processes with a refractory period (Shadlen and Newsome 1998; Kara et al. 2000) due to their high variability (Softky and Koch 1993), this view is being challenged more and more (Engel et al. 2008; Farkhooi et al. 2009; Maimon and Assad 2009). In particular, recent studies have found that cortical neurons could display nonrenewal spike train statistics in vivo (Engel et al. 2008). A more recent paper has argued that it is possible that nonrenewal spike train statistics are far more prevalent in cortical neurons than was originally thought and might have been missed in extracellular recordings due to missed spikes (Farkhooi et al. 2009). Intracellular recordings from cortical neurons have shown that they can display significant patterning in their spiking activities (Engel et al. 2008), and further studies are needed to test this hypothesis.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we have reviewed the mechanisms that give rise to ISI correlations in neural spike trains, the known consequences of these correlations on information transmission, and have highlighted some key issues that future studies on this subject should focus on.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by Conacyt (O.A.A.), as well as the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Canada Foundation for Innovation and the Canada Research Chairs program (M.J.C.).
Contributor Information
Oscar Avila-Akerberg, Department of Physics, McGill University, 3655 Sir William Osler, Room 1137, Montreal, QC H3G 1Y6, Canada.
Maurice J. Chacron, Department of Physics, McGill University, 3655 Sir William Osler, Room 1137, Montreal, QC H3G 1Y6, Canada, Department of Physiology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
References
- Abeles M. Corticonics. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Averbeck BB, Lee D. Effects of noise correlations on information encoding and decoding. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:3633–3644. doi: 10.1152/jn.00919.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avila Akerberg O, Chacron MJ. Noise shaping in neural populations. Phys Rev E. 2009;79:011914. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.79.011914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avila Akerberg O, Chacron MJ. Noise shaping in neural populations with global delayed feedback. Math Model Nat Phenom. 2010;5:100–124. doi: 10.1051/mmnp/20105204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avila Akerberg O, Krahe R, Chacron MJ. Neural heterogeneities and stimulus properties affect burst coding in vivo. Neuroscience. 2010;168:300–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azouz R, Gray CM. Cellular mechanisms contributing to response variability of cortical neurons in vivo. J Neurosci. 1999;19:2209–2223. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-06-02209.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azouz R, Gray CM. Dynamic spike threshold reveals a mechanism for synaptic coincidence detection in cortical neurons in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:8110–8115. doi: 10.1073/pnas.130200797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azouz R, Gray CM. Adaptive coincidence detection and dynamic gain control in visual cortical neurons in vivo. Neuron. 2003;37:513–523. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01186-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bahar S, Kantelhardt JW, Neiman A, Rego HHA, Russell DF, Wilkens L, Bunde A, Moss F. Long-range temporal anti-correlations in paddlefish electroreceptors. Eur Phys Lett. 2001;56:454–460. [Google Scholar]
- Barlow HB, Levick WR. Three factors limiting the reliable detection of light by the retinal ganglion cells of the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 1969;200:1–24. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastian J, Nguyenkim J. Dendritic modulation of Burst-like firing in sensory neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85:10–22. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benda J, Herz AV. A universal model for spike-frequency adaptation. Neural Comput. 2003;15:2523–2564. doi: 10.1162/089976603322385063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benda J, Longtin A, Maler L. Spike-frequency adaptation separates transient communication signals from background oscillations. J Neurosci. 2005;25:2312–2321. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4795-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benda J, Longtin A, Maler L. A synchronization-desynchronization code for natural communication signals. Neuron. 2006;52:347–358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benda J, Maler L, Longtin A. Linear versus nonlinear signal transmission in neuron models with adaptation currents or dynamic thresholds. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:2806–2820. doi: 10.1152/jn.00240.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birk JR. Enhanced specificity of information in axons with negatively correlated adjacent interspike intervals. Kybernetik. 1972;10:201–203. doi: 10.1007/BF00288738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borst A, Theunissen F. Information theory and neural coding. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:947–957. doi: 10.1038/14731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandman R, Nelson ME. A simple model of long-term spike train regularization. Neural Comput. 2002;14:1575–1597. doi: 10.1162/08997660260028629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cajal RS. Histologie du système nerveux de l’Homme et des vertébrés. Maloine; Paris: 1909. [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ. Nonlinear information processing in a model sensory system. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:2933–2946. doi: 10.1152/jn.01296.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Bastian J. Population coding by electrosensory neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:1825–1835. doi: 10.1152/jn.01266.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Longtin A, St-Hilaire M, Maler L. Suprathreshold stochastic firing dynamics with memory in P-type electroreceptors. Phys Rev Lett. 2000;85:1576–1579. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.85.1576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Longtin A, Maler L. Negative interspike interval correlations increase the neuronal capacity for encoding time-varying stimuli. J Neurosci. 2001a;21:5328–5343. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-14-05328.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Longtin A, Maler L. Simple models of bursting and non-bursting electroreceptors. Neurocomputing. 2001b;38:129–139. [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Pakdaman K, Longtin A. Interspike interval correlations, memory, adaptation, and refractoriness in a leaky integrate-and-fire model with threshold fatigue. Neural Comput. 2003;15:253–278. doi: 10.1162/089976603762552915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Lindner B, Longtin A. ISI correlations and information transfer. Fluctuations Noise Lett. 2004a;4:L195–L205. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.92.080601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Lindner B, Longtin A. Noise shaping by interval correlations increases information transfer. Phys Rev Lett. 2004b;92:080601. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.92.080601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Longtin A, Maler L. To burst or not to burst? J Comput Neurosci. 2004c;17:127–136. doi: 10.1023/B:JCNS.0000037677.58916.6b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Lindner B, Longtin A, Maler L, Bastian J. Experimental and theoretical demonstration of noise shaping by interspike interval correlations. Proc SPIE. 2005a;5841:150–163. [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Maler L, Bastian J. Electroreceptor neuron dynamics shape information transmission. Nat Neurosci. 2005b;8:673–678. doi: 10.1038/nn1433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chacron MJ, Lindner B, Longtin A. Threshold fatigue and information transfer. J Comput Neurosci. 2007;23:301–311. doi: 10.1007/s10827-007-0033-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Correia MJ, Landolt JP. A point process analysis of the spontaneous activity of anterior semicircular canal units in the anesthetized pigeon. Biol Cybern. 1977;27:199–213. doi: 10.1007/BF00344141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cover T, Thomas J. Elements of information theory. Wiley; New York: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Cox DR, Lewis PAW. The statistical analysis of series of events. Methuen; London: 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Abbott LF. Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. MIT Press; Cambridge: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Destexhe A, Rudolph M, Pare D. The high-conductance state of neocortical neurons in vivo. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:739–751. doi: 10.1038/nrn1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doiron B, Chacron MJ, Maler L, Longtin A, Bastian J. Inhibitory feedback required for network oscillatory responses to communication but not prey stimuli. Nature. 2003;421:539–543. doi: 10.1038/nature01360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eccles JC. The neurophysiological basis of mind. Oxford University Press; London: 1953. [Google Scholar]
- Eckhorn R, Reitboeck HJ, Arndt M, Dicke P. Feature linking via synchronization among distributed assemblies: simulations of results from cat visual cortex. Neural Comput. 1990;2:293–307. [Google Scholar]
- Edwards CJ, Leary CJ, Rose GJ. Mechanisms of long-interval selectivity in midbrain auditory neurons: roles of excitation, inhibition, and plasticity. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:3407–3416. doi: 10.1152/jn.90921.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis LD, Mehaffey WH, Harvey-Girard E, Turner RW, Maler L, Dunn RJ. SK channels provide a novel mechanism for the control of frequency tuning in electrosensory neurons. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9491–9502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1106-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel TA, Schimansky-Geier L, Herz AV, Schreiber S, Erchova I. Subthreshold membrane-potential resonances shape spike-train patterns in the entorhinal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:1576–1589. doi: 10.1152/jn.01282.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel TA, Helbig B, Russell DF, Schimansky-Geier L, Neiman AB. Coherent stochastic oscillations enhance signal detection in spiking neurons. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2009;80:021919. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.80.021919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faber ES, Sah P. Calcium-activated potassium channels: multiple contributions to neuronal function. Neuroscientist. 2003;9:181–194. doi: 10.1177/1073858403009003011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM. Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fano U. Ionization yield of radiations. II. The fluctuations of the number of ions. Phys Rev. 1947;72:26–29. [Google Scholar]
- Farkhooi F, Strube-Bloss MF, Nawrot MP. Serial correlation in neural spike trains: experimental evidence, stochastic modeling, and single neuron variability. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2009;79:021905. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.79.021905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd K, Hick VE, Holden AV, Koley J, Morrison JFB. Non-Markov negative correlation between interspike intervals in mammalian efferent discharge. Biol Cybern. 1982;45:89–93. doi: 10.1007/BF00335234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuhrmann G, Segev I, Markram H, Tsodyks M. Coding of temporal information by activity-dependent synapses. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:140–148. doi: 10.1152/jn.00258.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabbiani FC. Coding of time-varying signals in spike trains of integrate-and-fire neurons with random threshold. Neural Comput. 1996;8 [Google Scholar]
- Gabbiani F, Cox SJ. Mathematics for neuroscientists. Academic Press; London: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Geisler CD, Goldberg JM. A stochastic model of the repetitive activity of neurons. Biophys J. 1966;6:53–69. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(66)86639-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goense JB, Ratnam R. Continuous detection of weak sensory signals in afferent spike trains: the role of anti-correlated interspike intervals in detection performance. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2003;189:741–759. doi: 10.1007/s00359-003-0449-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg JM, Greenwood DD. Response of neurons of the dorsal and posteroventral cochlear nuclei of the cat to acoustic stimuli of long duration. J Neurophysiol. 1966;29:72–93. doi: 10.1152/jn.1966.29.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg JM, Adrian HO, Smith FD. Response of neurons of the superior olivary complex of the cat to acoustic stimuli of long duration. J Neurophysiol. 1964;27:706–749. doi: 10.1152/jn.1964.27.4.706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman MS, Maldonado P, Abbott LF. Redundancy reduction and sustained firing with stochastic depressing synapses. J Neurosci. 2002;22:584–591. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-02-00584.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green D, Swets J. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Wiley; New York: 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Gussin D, Benda J, Maler L. Limits of linear rate coding of dynamic stimuli by electroreceptor afferents. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:2917–2929. doi: 10.1152/jn.01243.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagiwara S. On the fluctuations of the interval of the rhythmic excitation. I. The efferent impulse of the human motor unit during the voluntary contraction. Bull Physiogr Sci Res Inst Tokyo Univ. 1949;3:19–31. [Google Scholar]
- Häusser M, Roth A. Dendritic and somatic glutamate receptor channels in rat cerebellar Purkinje cells. J Physiol (Lond) 1997;501:77–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1997.077bo.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgkin AL, Huxley AF. A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J Physiol (Lond) 1952;117:500–544. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollander H. The projection from the visual cortex to the lateral geniculate body (LGB). An experimental study with silver impregnation methods in the cat. Exp Brain Res. 1970;10:219–235. doi: 10.1007/BF00235047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horn D, Usher M. Neural networks with dynamical thresholds. Phys Rev A. 1989;40:1036–1040. doi: 10.1103/physreva.40.1036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jolivet R, Rauch A, Luscher HR, Gerstner W. Predicting spike timing of neocortical pyramidal neurons by simple threshold models. J Comput Neurosci. 2006;21:35–49. doi: 10.1007/s10827-006-7074-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kara P, Reinagel P, Reid RC. Low response variability in simultaneously recorded retinal, thalamic, and cortical neurons. Neuron. 2000;27:635–646. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00072-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keat J, Reinagel P, Reid RC, Meister M. Predicting every spike: a model for the responses of visual neurons. Neuron. 2001;30:803–817. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00322-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khanbabaie R, Nesse WH, Longtin A, Maler L. Kinetics of fast short-term depression are matched to spike train statistics to reduce noise. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:3337–3348. doi: 10.1152/jn.00117.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krahe R, Gabbiani F. Burst firing in sensory systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:13–23. doi: 10.1038/nrn1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuffler SW, Fitzhugh R, Barlow HB. Maintained activity in the cat’s retina in light and darkness. J Gen Physiol. 1957;40:683–702. doi: 10.1085/jgp.40.5.683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lansky P, Radil T. Statistical inference on spontaneous neuronal discharge patterns. I. Single neuron. Biol Cybern. 1987;55:299–311. doi: 10.1007/BF02281976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lansky P, Musila M, Smith CE. Effects of afterhyperpolarization on neuronal firing. Biosystems. 1992;27:25–38. doi: 10.1016/0303-2647(92)90044-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latham PE, Nirenberg S. Synergy, redundancy, and independence in population codes, revisited. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5195–5206. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5319-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leary CJ, Edwards CJ, Rose GJ. Midbrain auditory neurons integrate excitation and inhibition to generate duration selectivity: an in vivo whole-cell patch study in anurans. J Neurosci. 2008;28:5481–5493. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5041-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebedev MA, Nelson RJ. High-frequency vibratory sensitive neurons in monkey primate somatosensory cortex: entrained and nonentrained responses to vibration during the performance of vibratory-cued hand movements. Exp Brain Res. 1996;111:313–325. doi: 10.1007/BF00228721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine MW. Firing rate of a retinal neuron are not predictable from interspike interval statistics. Biophys J. 1980;30:9–25. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(80)85073-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis CD, Gebber GL, Larsen PD, Barman SM. Long-term correlations in the spike trains of medullary sympathetic neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85:1614–1622. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.4.1614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindner B, Chacron MJ, Longtin A. Integrate-and-fire neurons with threshold noise: a tractable model of how interspike interval correlations affect neuronal signal transmission. Phys Rev E. 2005;72:021911. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.72.021911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindner B, Gangloff D, Longtin A, Lewis JE. Broadband coding with dynamic synapses. J Neurosci. 2009;29:2076–2087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3702-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu YH, Wang XJ. Spike Frequency adaptation of a generalized leaky integrate-and-fire neuron. J Comput Neurosci. 2001;10:25–45. doi: 10.1023/a:1008916026143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- London M, Roth A, Beeren L, Hausser M, Latham PE. Sensitivity to perturbations in vivo implies high noise and suggests rate coding in cortex. Nature. 2010;466:123–127. doi: 10.1038/nature09086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Longtin A, Racicot DM. Spike train patterning and forecastability. Biosystems. 1997;40:111–118. doi: 10.1016/0303-2647(96)01636-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Longtin A, Laing C, Chacron MJ. Correlations and memory in neurodynamical systems. In: Rangarajan G, Ding M, editors. Long-range dependent stochastic processes. Springer; Berlin: 2003. pp. 286–308. [Google Scholar]
- Lowen SB, Teich MC. Auditory-nerve action potentials form a nonrenewal point process over short as well as long time scales. J Acoust Soc Am. 1992;92:803–806. doi: 10.1121/1.403950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowen SB, Teich MC. The periodogram and Allan variance reveal fractal exponents greater than unity in auditory-nerve spike trains. J Acoust Soc Am. 1996;99:3585–3591. doi: 10.1121/1.414979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowen SB, Cash SS, Poo M, Teich MC. Quantal neurotransmitter secretion rate exhibits fractal behavior. J Neurosci. 1997;17:5666–5677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-15-05666.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maar DJ, Chow CC, Gerstner W, Adams RW, Collins JJ. Noise shaping in populations of coupled model neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1999;96:10450–10455. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.18.10450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacGregor RJ, Oliver RM. A model for repetitive firing in neurons. Kybernetik. 1974;16:53–64. doi: 10.1007/BF00270295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maimon G, Assad JA. Beyond Poisson: increased spike-time regularity across primate parietal cortex. Neuron. 2009;62:426–440. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maler L, Sas E, Johnston S, Ellis W. An atlas of the brain of the weakly electric fish Apteronotus Leptorhynchus. J Chem Neuroanat. 1991;4:1–38. doi: 10.1016/0891-0618(91)90030-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehaffey WH, Ellis LD, Krahe R, Dunn RJ, Chacron MJ. Ionic and neuromodulatory regulation of burst discharge controls frequency tuning. J Physiol (Paris) 2008;102:195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meister M, Lagnado L, Baylor DA. Concerted signaling by retinal ganglion cells. Science. 1995;270:1207–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.270.5239.1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mickus T, Jung HY, Spruston N. Properties of slow cumulative sodium channel inactivation in rat hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Biophys J. 1999;76:846–860. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77248-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton JW, Chacron MJ, Lindner B, Longtin A. Correlated noise and memory effects in neural firing dynamics. In: Beruzkov SM, editor. Unsolved problems of noise and fluctuations. Vol. 665. 2003a. pp. 183–190. AIP Conference Proceedings. [Google Scholar]
- Middleton JW, Chacron MJ, Lindner B, Longtin A. Firing statistics of a neuron driven by long-range correlated noise. Phys Rev E. 2003b;68:021920. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.68.021920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore GP, Perkel DH, Segundo JP. Statistical analysis and functional interpretation of neuronal spike data. Annu Rev Physiol. 1966;28:493–522. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.28.030166.002425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nawrot MP, Boucsein C, Rodriguez-Molina V, Riehle A, Aertsen A, Grun S, Rotter S. Serial interval statistics of spontaneous activity in cortical neurons in vivo and in vitro. Neurocomputing. 2007;70:1717–1722. [Google Scholar]
- Neiman A, Russell DF. Stochastic byperiodic oscillations in the electroreceptors of paddlefish. Phys Rev Lett. 2001;86:3443–3446. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.86.3443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neiman A, Russell DF. Synchronization of noise-induced bursts in noncoupled sensory neurons. Phys Rev Lett. 2002;88:138103. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.88.138103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neiman AB, Russell DF. Two distinct types of noisy oscillators in electroreceptors of paddlefish. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92:492–509. doi: 10.1152/jn.00742.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neiman AB, Russell DF. Models of stochastic biperiodic oscillations and extended serial correlations in electroreceptors of paddlefish. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2005;71:061915. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.061915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson ME, MacIver MA. Prey capture in the weakly electric fish Apteronotus albifrons: sensory acquisition strategies and electrosensory consequences. J Exp Biol. 1999;202:1195–1203. doi: 10.1242/jeb.202.10.1195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nesse W, Maler L, Longtin A. Biophysical information representation in temporally correlated spike trains. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:21973–21978. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008587107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nirenberg S, Carcieri SM, Jacobs AL, Latham PE. Retinal ganglion cells act largely as independent encoders. Nature. 2001;411:698–701. doi: 10.1038/35079612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norsworthy SR, Schreier R, Temes GC, editors. Delta-sigma data converters. IEEE Press; Piscataway: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Ostapoff EM, Morest DK, Potashner SJ. Uptake and retrograde transport of [3H]GABA from the cochlear nucleus to the superior olive in the guinea pig. J Chem Neuroanat. 1990;3:285–295. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkel DH, Gerstein GL, Moore GP. Neuronal spike trains and stochastic point processes. I. The single spike train. Biophys J. 1967a;7:391–418. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(67)86596-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkel DH, Gerstein GL, Moore GP. Neuronal spike trains and stochastic point processes. II. Simultaneous spike trains. Biophys J. 1967b;7:419–440. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(67)86597-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peron S, Gabbiani F. Spike frequency adaptation mediates looming stimulus selectivity in a collision-detecting neuron. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:318–326. doi: 10.1038/nn.XXX. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poor HV. An introduction to signal detection and estimation. Springer; New York: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Ratnam R, Nelson ME. Non-renewal statistics of electrosensory afferent spike trains: implications for the detection of weak sensory signals. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6672–6683. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-06672.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rieke F, Warland D, van Steveninck RR, Bialek W. Spikes: exploring the neural code. MIT; Cambridge: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Roddey JC, Girish B, Miller JP. Assessing the performance of neural encoding models in the presence of noise. J Comput Neurosci. 2000;8:95–112. doi: 10.1023/a:1008921114108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodieck RW. Maintained activity of cat retinal ganglion cells. J Neurophysiol. 1967;30:1043–1071. doi: 10.1152/jn.1967.30.5.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose GJ, Leary CJ, Edwards CJ. Interval-counting neurons in the anuran auditory midbrain: factors underlying diversity of interval tuning. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2010;197:97–108. doi: 10.1007/s00359-010-0591-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeghi SG, Chacron MJ, Taylor MC, Cullen KE. Neural variability, detection thresholds, and information transmission in the vestibular system. J Neurosci. 2007;27:771–781. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4690-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sah P. Ca 2+-activated K+ currents in neurones: types, physiological roles and modulation. Trends Neurosci. 1996;19:150–154. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)80026-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sah P, Faber ES. Channels underlying neuronal calcium-activated potassium currents. Progr Neurobiol. 2002;66:345–353. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savard M, Krahe R, Chacron MJ. Neural heterogeneities influence envelope and temporal coding at the sensory periphery. Neuroscience. 2011;172:270–284. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.10.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schäfer K, Braun HA, Peters C, Bretschneider F. Periodic firing pattern in afferent discharges from electroreceptor organs of catfish. Eur J Physiol. 1995;429:378–385. doi: 10.1007/BF00374153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneidman E, Bialek W, Berry MJ., II Synergy, redundancy, and independence in population codes. J Neurosci. 2003;23:11539–11553. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-37-11539.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneidman E, Berry MJ, II, Segev R, Bialek W. Weak pairwise correlations imply strongly correlated network states in a neural population. Nature. 2006;440:1007–1012. doi: 10.1038/nature04701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwalger T, Fisch K, Benda J, Lindner B. How noisy adaptation of neurons shapes interspike interval histograms and correlations. PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6:e1001026. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J Neurosci. 1998;18:3870–3896. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03870.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shannon CE. The mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst Tech J. 1948;27(379–423):623–656. [Google Scholar]
- Sherman SM, Guillery RW. The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2002;357:1695–1708. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherman SM, Guillery RW. Exploring the thalamus and its role in cortical function. MIT Press; Cambridge: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Shin J. Novel neural circuits based on stochastic pulse coding noise feedback pulse coding. Int J Electron. 1993;74:359–368. [Google Scholar]
- Shin J. Adaptation in spiking neurons based on the noise shaping neural coding hypothesis. Neural Netw. 2001;14:907–919. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(01)00077-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith CE, Goldberg JM. A stochastic afterhyperpolarization model of repetitive activity in vestibular afferents. Biol Cybern. 1986:41–51. doi: 10.1007/BF00337114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snippe HP, Koenderink JJ. Discrimination thresholds for channel-coded systems. Biol Cybern. 1992;66:543–551. doi: 10.1007/BF00201025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Softky WR, Koch C. The highly irregular firing of cortical cells is inconsistent with temporal integration of random EPSPs. J Neurosci. 1993;13:334–350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-01-00334.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein RB, Gossen ER, Jones KE. Neuronal variability: noise or part of the signal? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:389–397. doi: 10.1038/nrn1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surmeier DJ, Honda CN, Willis WD. Patterns of spontaneous discharge in primate spinothalamic neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1989;61:106–115. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.1.106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teich MC. Fractal neuronal firing patterns. In: McKenna T, Davis J, Zornetzer SF, editors. Single neuron computation. Academic Press; San Diego: 1992. pp. 589–622. [Google Scholar]
- Teich MC, Heneghan C, Lowen SB, Ozaki T, Kaplan E. Fractal character of the neural spike train in the visual system of the cat. J Opt Soc Am A. 1997;14:529–546. doi: 10.1364/josaa.14.000529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomson EE, Kristan WB. Quantifying stimulus discriminability: a comparison of information theory and ideal observer analysis. Neural Comput. 2005;17:741–778. doi: 10.1162/0899766053429435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toporikova N, Chacron MJ. Dendritic SK channels gate information processing in vivo by regulating an intrinsic bursting mechanism seen in vitro. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102:2273–2287. doi: 10.1152/jn.00282.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsuchitani C, Johnson DH. The effects of ipsilateral tone burst stimulus level on the discharge patterns of cat lateral superior olivary units. J Acoust Soc Am. 1985;77:1484–1496. doi: 10.1121/1.392043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuckwell HC. Nonlinear and stochastic theories. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ. Calcium coding and adaptive temporal computation in cortical pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:1549–1566. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss TF. A model of the peripheral auditory system. Kybernetik. 1966;3:153–175. doi: 10.1007/BF00290252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesenfeld K, Satija I. Noise tolerance of frequency locked dynamics. Phys Rev B. 1987;36:2483–2492. doi: 10.1103/physrevb.36.2483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yacomotti AM, Eguia MC, Aliaga J, Martinez OE, Mindlin GB. Interspike time distribution in noise driven excitable systems. Phys Rev Lett. 1999;83:292–295. [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto M, Nakahama H. Stochastic properties of spontaneous unit discharges in somatosensory cortex and mesencephalic reticular formation during sleep-waking states. J Neurophysiol. 1983;49:1182–1198. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.49.5.1182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zohary E, Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature. 1994;370:140–143. doi: 10.1038/370140a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]