Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Sci. 2015 Jul 17;26(8):1304–1315. doi: 10.1177/0956797615588751

A Survival Analysis of Adolescent Friendships: The Downside of Dissimilarity

Amy C Hartl a, Brett Laursen a,*, Antonius H N Cillessen b
PMCID: PMC4529362  NIHMSID: NIHMS688523  PMID: 26187246

Abstract

The present study examined whether adolescent friendships dissolve because of characteristics of friends, differences between friends, or both. Participants included 410 adolescents (201 boys, 209 girls) who reported a total of 573 reciprocated friendships that originated in the 7th grade (M age = 13.20 years). Discrete-time survival analyses were conducted in which peer nominations and teacher ratings collected in Grade 7 predicted the occurrence and timing of friendship dissolution across grades 8 to 12. Grade 7 individual characteristics were unrelated to friendship stability, but Grade 7 differences in sex, peer acceptance, physical aggression, and school competence predicted subsequent friendship dissolution. The findings suggest that compatibility is a function of similarity between friends rather than the presence or absence of a particular trait.

Keywords: Friends, relationship dissolution, survival analysis, aggression, peer acceptance


No one likes to lose a friend. For adolescents navigating autonomy from the family, the dissolution of a friendship can be especially challenging (Hartup, 1993). But why do friendships end? High levels of an undesirable characteristic, such as aggression, threaten friendships (Piehler & Dishion, 2007). Dissimilarity also anticipates dissolution (Poulin & Chan, 2010). Previous studies have focused on either levels of individual characteristics or differences between friends (known henceforth as dyadic differences), making it difficult to draw firm conclusions about adolescent friendship dissolution because the two are confounded: The largest dyadic differences occur when individual characteristics are most pronounced (e.g., those with the highest levels of aggression are also the least similar to the bulk of their peers, whose scores fall in the normal range of the distribution). In the present study, we compared dyadic differences in school-related behaviors and individual levels of each to predict the occurrence and timing of the dissolution of friendships originating in middle school.

Most early adolescents have at least one friend and most of these friendships eventually end (Bukowski, Motzoi, & Meyer, 2009). Roughly half of middle school friendships do not last an academic year (Bowker, 2004). The loss can be painful, even debilitating (Bowker, 2011). Middle school adolescents are especially vulnerable to problems arising from the loss of a friend because cognitive and emotional changes elevate the significance of friendships at the same time that growing independence from parents heightens interconnections between friends (Laursen & Collins, 2009).

The voluntary nature of friendships allows participants to leave unsatisfactory relationships with few countervailing external pressures. As is the case in voluntary romantic relationships, dissatisfaction with friends is assumed to arise from an imbalance of relationship rewards and costs (Levinger, 1979). Dissatisfaction may have origins in dissimilarity or in the presence of one or more undesirable attributes in a partner. We review each in turn.

Dissimilarity as a Source of Friendship Dissolution

Evidence for homophily, the tendency for friends to resemble one another, (Lazarsfeld & Merton, 1954) suggests that similarity is important for establishing and maintaining friendships. Dissimilarity propagates conflict and negative affect (Laursen, Hartup, & Koplas, 1996). In social exchange terms (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959), dissimilarity threatens perceptions that relationship participants share equally in the benefits of affiliation. One partner is apt to bear more costs (e.g., the less aggressive friend) or accrue more rewards (e.g., the less scholastically able friend) than the other. Thus, differences foster negativity and create inequities, both of which take a toll on relationship satisfaction. Dissatisfied friends are inclined to find new, more appealing friends.

Demographic dissimilarity is an important predictor of friendship instability. Mixed-sex and mixed-ethnicity friendships dissolve faster than same-sex and same-ethnicity friendships (e.g., Aboud, Mendelson, & Purdy, 2003). The degree to which friends differ in aggression and victimization (e.g., Ellis & Zarbatany, 2007) also predicts friendship dissolution, as do differences in school achievement (e.g., Flashman, 2012). No studies have compared the relative importance of dyadic difference variables, however, so their unique contributions to adolescent friendship dissolution are unclear.

Individual Characteristics as a Source of Friendship Dissolution

A similarly strong case can be made for the argument that undesirable characteristics place individuals at risk for relationship instability. Poor psychosocial adjustment interferes with friendship maintenance. Adolescents with emotional and behavioral problems are prone to affectively charged interactions, which increases the costs of affiliation (Piehler & Dishion, 2007). Adolescents with poor social skills may neither understand social cues provided by friends nor appreciate the rules of social exchange that govern their interactions (Laursen & Hartup, 2002). As a consequence, support may not be offered when it is needed and expected. Characteristics that make it difficult to initiate and maintain friendships also make it difficult to be tolerated as a friend. Poorly regulated individuals may impulsively break off relationships, but it is probably more likely that their partners decide to withdraw from the relationship (Hartup & Stevens, 1997).

There is evidence that individual characteristics forecast adolescent friendship stability. Girls have less stable friendships than boys (Benenson & Christakos, 2003). Youth who are aggressive, victimized, and antisocial (Dishion, Andrews, & Crosby, 1995; Ellis & Zarbatany, 2007) and youth who have poor grades (Flashman, 2012) all tend to have short-lived friendships, while high status prosocial peers tend to have the most stable friendships (Bowker, Rubin, Burgess, Booth-LaForce, & Rose-Krasnor, 2006). As with dissimilarity, no studies have simultaneously examined the unique contributions that different individual characteristics make to friendship stability.

Using Survival Analyses to Identify Sources of Relationship Dissolution

We looked to the marital literature for guidance as to how multiple, correlated predictors can be used to predict relationship dissolution. Survival analyses (also known as event history analyses) hold several advantages over alternative strategies. Discrete-time survival analyses measure the occurrence and timing of events through an array of logistic regressions, assessing dissolution at multiple time points. In survival analyses, all relationships start at the same time, so new relationships are not confounded with existing relationships. Survival analyses work best when there is heterogeneity in the timing of relationship dissolution because the procedure is designed to identify events spread across more than one time lag (Graham, Willett, & Singer, 2013).

In the present study, discrete-time survival analysis was used to examine the occurrence and timing of adolescent friendship dissolution. All friendships originated in the 7th grade, to maximize the number of middle school friendships lasting more than one year. Friendships that begin in the 6th grade are highly unstable because primary school friendship groups are transformed across the first year of middle school (Cantin & Boivin, 2004). Friendships that begin in the 8th grade are highly unstable because most new friendships do not survive the transition into high school (Eccles, Lord, & Buchanan, 1996).

The study is unique in that it was designed to determine the relative contribution of friend dissimilarity and individual attributes in the prediction of adolescent friendship dissolution. Predictor variables, identified from previous studies of friendship instability, represent attributes associated with interpersonal success and socio-emotional difficulties. Sex, age, ethnicity, peer acceptance (being liked), peer rejection (being disliked), leadership, physical aggression (bodily harm), relational aggression (emotional or interpersonal harm), peer victimization, and school competence all have been found to predict friendship dissolution, either as individual traits or as differences between friends (Poulin & Chan, 2010). We examined the potential role of sex as a moderator of the predictors of friendship dissolution, given pervasive findings of sex differences in mean levels of social and academic adjustment.

Method

Participants

Participants were drawn from a longitudinal study of students who attended public schools in lower-middle and middle-class neighborhoods in a small Northeastern U.S. city. Only adolescents who identified at least one new, reciprocated friendship in the 7th grade were included. A total of 410 adolescents (201 boys, 209 girls), ranging from 12 to 15 years old (M=13.20, SD=0.46) met this criterion. Of these participants, 72.4% (n=297) identified themselves as European-American, 16.1% (n=66) as African-American, 10.0% (n=41) as Latin-American, and 1.5% (n=6) as Asian-American.

Procedure

Passive consent procedures, consistent with school system policy, were employed to ensure adequate network coverage for nomination data. Letters were sent to the parents of all students in each grade. Parents not wishing their child to participate returned a form stating such. Less than 1% did so. Child verbal assent was required for participation. All students (including those who did not complete questionnaires) were included in nomination rosters.

Participants attended two middle schools (grades 6–8), each with about 300 students per grade. Middle schools fed into a single high school (grades 9–12), which had about 600 students per grade. Research assistants collected data annually in the spring semester, during one 90-minute session in a required English class.

Of the 410 7th grade participants included in this study, 93% (n=383) participated in data collection in the 8th grade, 76% (n=311) in the 9th grade, 67% (n=275) in the 10th grade, 64% (n=262) in the 11th grade, and 59% (n=240) in the 12th grade. With one exception, there were no statistically significant differences between students who participated in data collection from 7th to 12th grade and those who did not on any demographic or study variable: Participants who remained in the study in grades 10, 11, and 12 had higher school competence scores than those who did not (OR range: 1.38–1.46, p<.05). Most students (71%, n = 121) who discontinued participation did so because they changed schools.

Measures

Friendship nominations

Each year, students completed friendship nominations using rosters that included the names of all students in the grade (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004). Students responded to the question “Who are your best friends?” by circling the number preceding the name. Unlimited same-sex and other-sex nominations were allowed, but self-nominations were not.

Reciprocated friendships originating in Grade 7 were defined as dyads in which both partners nominated one another as friends for the first time in the 7th grade. Of the 597 students in Grade 7, 465 were present on the day of data collection and agreed to participate. Each participant nominated at least one best friend. Of this total, 410 participated in at least one reciprocated friendship that originated in Grade 7. Considering those excluded from analyses, 26 were not involved in any reciprocated friendships in the 7th grade and 29 were only involved in reciprocated friendships that began prior to the 7th grade. Friendships that began prior to the 7th grade were eliminated because survival analyses require the same starting point for all cases. Independent samples t-tests and χ2 tests on demographic and study variables did not reveal any greater than chance differences between students included in the analyses and students who were excluded because they did not have a new friend in the 7th grade.

Most students (n=308) were involved in multiple reciprocated friendships, yielding a total of 573 friendships originating in the 7th grade. The number of reciprocated friendships represents the total number of friendships that a participant reported that originated in the 7th grade (range: 1–11, M=2.80, SD=1.71). Independent samples t-tests revealed no statistically significant sex differences in the number of reciprocated friendships that began in the 7th grade.

Friendship dissolution occurred when at least one member of a reciprocated friendship originating in the 7th grade failed to nominate the other as a friend at a later point in time (Cairns, Leung, Buchanan, & Cairns, 1995). Of the 573 friendships that began in the 7th grade, 32 dissolved but were reconstituted at a later time point.

Peer nominations

In the 7th grade, participants completed a peer nomination inventory, using single item variables drawn from multiple item indicators with demonstrated validity and reliability (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004). The participation rate (78%) was well above the minimum required for valid nomination data (Bukowski, Cillessen, & Velásquez, 2012). For each question, participants received a roster with the names of all students in the grade. Participants circled the number preceding the name of each student who fit the description provided. Unlimited same-sex and other-sex nominations were allowed, but self-nominations were not. Single-item peer nominations are a widely used, valid measure of behavior, because each informant is considered a unique indicator (Bukowski et al., 2012). For each descriptor, the number of peer nominations a student received was summed and z-standardized within school.

Peer acceptance describes the number of liked most nominations received (Who are the people in your grade you like the most?). Peer rejection describes the number of liked least nominations received (Who are the people in your grade you like the least?). Additional nominations addressed physical aggression (Who are the people in your grade that start fights, pick on, or tease?), relational aggression (Who are the people in your grade that ignore others when mad at them?), peer victimization (Who are the people in your grade that get picked on or teased?), and leadership (Who are the people in your grade that are leaders?). Test-retest stability (from Grade 7 to Grade 8) was adequate (r=.70 to .88). Compared to boys, independent samples t-tests revealed that girls exhibited significantly higher levels of peer acceptance, leadership, and relational aggression (d=0.24 to 0.26), and significantly lower levels of physical aggression (d=−0.25). There were no statistically significant sex differences on peer rejection or victimization. Compared with boys, the friendships of girls were significantly more similar (i.e., less different) in peer acceptance, physical aggression, and victimization (d=−0.25 to −0.30), and significantly less similar in leadership (d=0.26). There were no statistically significant differences between the friendships of boys and girls in peer rejection or relational aggression,

Teacher-reported school competence

In the 7th grade, teachers rated each participant on a 4-item measure of school competence (e.g., Does well on tests) taken from the Multidimensional Self-concept Scale (MSCS: Bracken, 1992). Items were rated on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true). Item scores were averaged and z-standardized. Internal reliability was good (α=.87). Independent samples t-tests indicated that girls scored significantly higher than boys on school competence (d=0.28).

Plan of Analysis

Discrete-time survival analyses were conducted in a latent variable framework using Mplus v7.12 (Muthén & Muthén, 2014). The analyses were designed to predict the occurrence and timing of the dissolution of friendships originating in the 7th grade from initial characteristics of each member of the dyad and from initial differences between friends on each characteristic.

What is the likelihood of a friendship dissolving during a specific grade? The hazard curve depicts the probability that a reciprocated friendship originating in the 7th grade will dissolve at each grade, given that it had not already dissolved. The discrete-time hazard rate describes the conditional probability that a reciprocated friendship originating in the 7th grade will dissolve in a specific grade, given that it did not dissolve in an earlier grade.

What is the likelihood of a friendship continuing to a later grade? The survival curve depicts the probability that a reciprocated friendship originating in the 7th grade will continue at each grade. The discrete-time survival rate describes the proportion of reciprocated friendships originating in the 7th grade that continued at each grade.

Model construction followed a four-step procedure. Figure 1 describes a single-class latent class analysis with binary time-specific event indicators that approximate the conventional discrete-time survival analysis (Muthén & Masyn, 2005). Five binary time-specific event indicators were included, one for each annual interval from grade 7 to 12. For each friendship, the event indicator at each annual interval was coded as reciprocated, dissolved, or previously dissolved. Friendships that did not dissolve by the 12th grade were censored (Graham et al., 2013) and all event indicators were coded as reciprocated.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Measurement model of discrete-time survival analysis with binary time-specific event indicators (grade 8–12), Grade 7 predictors with time-invariant effects (solid lines) or time-varying effects (dashed lines), and two-way sex by predictor interactions.

Note. The hazard rate at each grade is 11+eτX. The log odds ratio of a predictor is β. The hazard odds ratio of a predictor is eβx. The estimates predicting dissolution from the hazard function are tau (τ).

The first step of model construction evaluates the constant hazard assumption to determine if the probability of friendship dissolution varied across grades. To this end, an unconditional survival model was estimated, in which hazard rates were allowed to vary freely across grades, and a conditional model was estimated, in which the hazard rates were constrained to be equal across grades. A likelihood-ratio test compared the fit of the unconditional and conditional models (Graham et al., 2013). A statistically significant result indicates that hazard rates were different across grades and should be allowed to vary.

The second step of model construction evaluates the proportionality assumption to determine whether a predictor’s effect varied across grades. For each predictor, an unconditional survival model was estimated in which the predictor’s effects were allowed to vary freely across grades, and a conditional model was estimated in which the predictor’s effects were constrained to be equal across grades. A likelihood-ratio test compared the fit of the conditional and unconditional models. A statistically significant result indicates that the effects of a predictor differed across grades and should be allowed to vary.

There were two types of predictors: Grade 7 individual characteristics and Grade 7 dyadic difference scores. Individual characteristics included participant scores for sex (0=male, 1=female), age (in months), ethnicity (0=European American, 1=all others), number of reciprocated friends, peer acceptance, peer rejection, leadership, physical aggression, relational aggression, peer victimization, and school competence. Dyadic difference scores for peer acceptance, peer rejection, leadership, physical aggression, relational aggression, peer victimization, and school competence, represent the absolute value of the difference between the z-standardized scores of the two reciprocated friends. Dyadic difference scores for age and number of reciprocated friends represent the absolute value of the difference between the raw scores of the two reciprocated friends. Dyadic difference scores for sex were coded as same-sex (0; n=499) or different-sex (1; n=74). Dyadic difference scores for ethnicity were coded as same ethnicity (0; n=409) or different ethnicity (1; n=164). Table 1 provides descriptive statistics. Table 2 provides correlations among predictors.

Table 1.

Means and Standard Deviations for Individual Characteristics and Dyadic Difference Scores

Individual Characteristics Dyadic Difference Scores

Grade 7 Variable M (SD) M (SD)
1. Sex 0.51 (0.50) 0.13 (0.34)
2. Age (in months) 170.30 (5.52) 5.67 (4.93)
3. Ethnicity 0.72 (0.45) 0.29 (0.45)
4. Number of Reciprocated Friends 2.80 (1.71) 2.10 (1.75)
5. Peer Acceptance 0.15 (0.98) 0.85 (0.77)
6. Peer Rejection −0.04 (0.91) 0.86 (0.83)
7. Leadership 0.10 (1.11) 0.95 (1.29)
8. Physical Aggression −0.04 (0.92) 0.67 (1.05)
9. Relational Aggression 0.03 (1.00) 0.91 (0.96)
10. Victimization −0.07 (0.81) 0.42 (0.78)
11. School Competence −0.01 (1.00) 0.82 (0.70)

Note. N = 410 participants in 573 friendship dyads. Sex is coded as 0 (male) or 1 (female). Difference in sex is coded as 0 (same-sex) or 1 (different-sex). Ethnicity is coded as 0 (European American) or 1 (all others). Difference in ethnicity is coded as 0 (same ethnicity) or 1 (different ethnicity). Difference scores for all other variables represent the absolute value of the difference between the scores of both friends in the dyad. Higher values reflect greater dissimilarity. Peer acceptance, peer rejection, leadership, physical aggression, relational aggression, and victimization were z-standardized within school. School competence was z-standardized.

Table 2.

Correlations between Individual Characteristics and between Dyadic Difference Scores

Grade 7 Variable Correlation (r)
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Age (in months) -- −.03 [−.13,.07] .06 [−.06,.16] .01 [−.08,.11] −.03 [−.12,.05] .16* [.04,.28] .09 [−.01,.19] .00 [−.07,.07] −.22** [−.32,−.12]
2. Number of Reciprocated Friends −.01 [−.10,.07] -- .34** [.25,.44] .04 [−.06,.14] .26** [.17,.35] .15* [.04,.26] .21** [.10,.30] −.08 [−.17,.01] .04 [−.06,.14]
3. Peer Acceptance −.01 [−.10,.08] .27** [.18,.36] -- .06 [−.07,.16] .61** [.56,.67] .18** [.10,.27] .35** [.25,.44] −.27** [−.37,−.23] .11* [.02,.20]
4. Peer Rejection .01 [−.07,.10] −.03 [−.11,.07] .02 [−.06,.10] -- .10 [−.07,.23] .53** [.44,.62] .65** [.57,.72] .49** [.35,.62] −.11* [−.21,−.01]
5. Leadership −.09* [−.15,−.03] .12* [.03,.22] .41** [.33,.48] .17** [.06,.27] -- −.02 [−.10,.06] .22** [.06,.35] −.18** [−.25,−.15] .33** [.26,.39]
6. Physical Aggression .12* [.03,.21] .09* [.01,.18] .13* [.04,.22] .44** [.34,.55] .01 [−.05,.08] -- .70** [.62,.77] .08 [−.02,.18] −.42** [−.51,−.33]
7. Relational Aggression .08 [−.01,.16] .00 [−.08,.08] .16** [.06,.24] .57** [.48,.65] .21** [.08,.31] .62** [.54,.69] -- .11* [.04,.19] −.23** [−.34,−.12]
8. Victimization −.04 [−.09,.03] −.06 [−.14,−.02] −.11* [−.16,−.07] .35** [.18,.48] −.12* [−.16,−.09] .01 [−.03,.05] .05 [−.03,.12] -- −.08 [−.17,.02]
9. School Competence .13* [.03,.23] .00 [−.09,.09] .06 [−.03,.14] −.02 [−.10,.06] −.03 [−.12,.06] .12* [.04,.21] .08 [−.01,.17] −.04 [−.11,.03] --

Note. N = 410 participants in 573 friendship dyads. Correlations between individual characteristics (n = 410 participants) are given above the diagonal. Correlations between dyadic difference scores (n = 573 friendships) are given below the diagonal. Dyadic difference scores represent the absolute value of the difference between friends. Higher values reflect greater dissimilarity. Peer nominations for peer acceptance, peer rejection, leadership, physical aggression, relational aggression, and victimization were summed and z-standardized within school. Teacher-reported school competence was z-standardized.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .001, two-tailed.

The third step of model construction estimates separate models that include either (a) all Grade 7 individual characteristics as predictors or (b) all Grade 7 dyadic difference scores as predictors. The individual characteristics model included scores for each friend, with their variances, covariances, means, and path estimates constrained to be equal. Likelihood Ratio Tests indicated that friends were indistinguishable on each predictor. Figure 1 presents the measurement model. The five annual event indicators (i.e., friendship dissolution at grades 8–12) were simultaneously regressed on the Grade 7 predictors. Predictors were centered to reduce collinearity. Sex by predictor interactions were included in each model to test for sex moderation.

The fourth step of model construction estimates the final model, which includes all statistically significant (p<.05) predictors from the individual characteristics model and all statistically significant predictors from the dyadic difference model estimated in Step 3. Predictors were allowed to covary. For ease of interpretation, results for uncentered predictors are presented. To compare predictors, we estimated the hazard curve for a hypothetical reference group of same-sex friends in which each member of the dyad has the same score on each predictor (i.e., difference scores equal 0). Fitted hazard curves illustrate each predictor’s effect on the occurrence and timing of friendship dissolution compared to the reference group.

An average of 1.0% (range 0.0% to 5.6%) of reports were missing. Little’s MCAR test revealed that data were missing completely at random, χ2(74)=87.45, p=.14. Missing data on predictor variables were handled with Full Information Maximum-likelihood (FIML) estimation with robust standard errors, allowing friendships with incomplete data to be included in the models.

The same pattern of statistically significant results emerged in supplemental analyses that (a) excluded reciprocated friendships originating in the 7th grade (n=198) that became unilateral (i.e., only one friend nominated the other) before dissolving (i.e., neither friend nominated the other); (b) included the 29 participants (n = 33 friendships) who were only involved in friendships that began prior to the 7th grade; and (c) included the 26 participants (n = 48 friendships beginning in the 8th grade) who were not involved in friendships that began in the 7th grade. Results did not change when participants who were outliers on the number of friend nominations were (a) limited to 7 friendships (3 standard deviations above the mean) and (b) omitted from analyses.

Results

Hazard and Survival Curves Describing Friendship Dissolution

Figure 2 depicts the hazard and survival curves. The hazard curve indicates that friendships that started in the 7th grade were at greatest risk for dissolution during the subsequent year (8th grade Hazard Rate: 76%). The risk of dissolution was somewhat smaller during the second and third year (9th–10th grade Hazard Rate: 62%–64%), with rates declining during the fourth year (11th grade Hazard Rate: 47%) and fifth year (12th grade Hazard Rate: 30%). A likelihood-ratio test (Graham et al., 2013) revealed that the hazard rate for friendship dissolution declined significantly over time, χ2(4)=25.33, p<.001. As a consequence, hazard rates were allowed to vary across grades in subsequent analyses.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Hazard curve and survival curve for the dissolution of friendships originating in the 7th grade.

Note. N = 410 participants in 573 friendship dyads. The survival curve reflects the probability of friendship continuation. The hazard curve reflects the conditional probability of friendship dissolution.

The survival curve indicated that fewer than 1 in 4 friendships that started in Grade 7 were maintained across the next school year (8th grade Survival Rate: 24%). Fewer than 1 in 10 friendships that started in Grade 7 survived the transition from middle school to high school (9th grade Survival Rate: 9%). Only 1% of friendships that began in the 7th grade continued to the 12th grade.

Multivariate Survival Models Predicting Adolescent Friendship Dissolution

Likelihood ratio tests evaluated the proportionality assumption to determine whether a predictor’s effect varied across grades. There were no differences between the conditional and unconditional models for any of the individual characteristics, χ2(4)=1.65–7.98, p=.09–80, or dyadic difference scores, χ2(4)=0.71–9.16, p=.06–.95. As a consequence, predictor effects were fixed to be equal across grades in all subsequent analyses.

Separate survival models were initially conducted for individual characteristics and for dyadic difference scores to identify predictors of the occurrence and timing of friendship dissolution. The final model included all statistically significant predictors from these two preliminary models.

Individual characteristics model

Results for the model that included all Grade 7 individual characteristics as predictors indicated that only the number of reciprocated friendships originating in 7th grade significantly predicted the occurrence and timing of friendship dissolution. Across grades, the odds of friendship dissolution increased with each additional reciprocated friendship that an adolescent reported. Statistically significant sex by predictor interactions did not arise at levels greater than chance.

Dyadic difference model

Results for the model that included all Grade 7 dyadic difference scores as predictors indicated that differences between friends in sex, peer acceptance, physical aggression, and school competence significantly predicted the occurrence and timing of friendship dissolution. Across grades, the odds of friendship dissolution were higher for different-sex friends than for same-sex friends, and increased with each standard deviation difference between friends in peer acceptance, physical aggression, and teacher-reported school competence. Statistically significant sex by predictor interactions did not arise at levels greater than chance.

Final model

Figure 3 depicts the odds ratios for the predictors in the final model. Results indicated that differences between friends in sex, peer acceptance, physical aggression, and school competence statistically significantly predicted friendship dissolution. The number of reciprocated friends was not statistically significant [β=0.04(0.03), p = .25]. Across grades, the odds of friendship dissolution were 3.90 times higher for different-sex friends than same-sex friends [β=1.59**(0.42)], 23% higher for every one standard deviation friends differed in peer acceptance [β=0.21*(0.11)], 43% higher for every one standard deviation friends differed in physical aggression [β=0.36*(0.14)], and 35% higher for every one standard deviation friends differed in teacher-reported school competence [β=0.30*(0.13)]. Fitted hazard curves for each of the statistically significant predictors are presented in Figure 4.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Final discrete-time survival model of the dissolution of friendships originating in the 7th grade.

Note. N = 410 participants in 573 friendship dyads. Unstandardized estimates indicate odds ratios for predictors and hazard rates of friendship dissolution at each grade. 95% confidence intervals are given in brackets. Double-headed arrows and estimates represent covariance between predictors. All predictors were dyadic difference scores except the number of reciprocated friendships originating in Grade 7, which was an individual characteristic. Separate scores for the number of reciprocated friends originating in Grade 7 were entered into the model for each member of the dyad, but because these scores were constrained to be equal, the variable is depicted only once. *p < .05, **p < .001, two-tailed.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Fitted hazard curves for significant predictors of friendship dissolution.

Note. N = 410 participants in 573 friendship dyads. Fitted hazard curves illustrate the effect of a one unit increase for each predictor in the final model on the risk of friendship dissolution. The reference group is a hypothetical group of same-sex friends in which each member of the dyad has the same score for each predictor (i.e., dyadic difference scores equal 0).

Discussion

The present study confirms what short-term longitudinal studies suggest, namely that adolescent friendships are fleeting. New to this study is the comparison of friend dissimilarity and individual attributes as predictors of friendship dissolution. The findings suggest that differences in social and academic behavior anticipate the dissolution of friendships across middle school and high school. Different-sex friendships were at greatest risk. Friends who differed on peer-nominated acceptance, peer-nominated physical aggression, and teacher-rated school competence had relationships that dissolved sooner than friends who were similar on these attributes. Of particular note, when individual characteristics were considered alongside differences between friends on these same characteristics, the former did not predict the occurrence or timing of the end of the friendship.

Differences are detrimental to friendships. Gender boundaries make mixed-sex friendships particularly tenuous. Grade school friendships are strictly sex segregated (Rose & Rudolph, 2006). The borders between same-sex groups begin to dissolve in middle school, but other-sex friendships are still highly atypical and pressure to conform to same-sex structures remains considerable. Sexual feelings and jealousy may arise creating conflict (Furman & Shaffer, 2011). It is also likely that some portion of mixed-sex friendship briefly passed through a romantic phase prior to relationship dissolution.

Differences in peer acceptance may give rise to differences in perceived social benefits. Given a choice, most individuals prefer to bask in the glory of high value affiliates (Sprecher & Regan, 2002). Status differentials may anticipate relationship dissolution because the higher accepted partner grows dissatisfied with the affiliation benefits provided by the lower accepted partner. Better-accepted partners may incur reputational costs from association with a less accepted partner, as well as the loss of benefits that are foregone in interactions with a friend who may be lacking in social skills (Allen, Porter, McFarland, Marsh, & McElhaney, 2005). The absence of congruent findings for peer rejection is puzzling. We can only speculate that acceptance is the more salient construct because it reflects differences in the availability of alternative relationship partners, an important factor in decisions about relationship investment and continuity (Rusbult & van Lange, 2003).

It is not difficult to understand why differences in physical aggression and differences in school competence predict friendship dissolution. Differences in physical aggression produce unequal relationship costs. The less aggressive friend is apt to be on the receiving end of partner aggression (Crick & Nelson, 2002). Even when delivered in a good natured fashion, most people do not enjoy being the target of aggression. In contrast, differences in school competence create unequal relationship rewards. The less academically capable friend may rely on his or her more successful friend for tutelage (DeLay et al., 2014). The higher ability friend may resent the investment in instruction with no academic return from the lower ability friend.

After accounting for friend dissimilarity, individual attributes did not predict friendship dissolution. Although caution is warranted in interpreting null effects, the results suggest that previous findings that emphasized undesirable individual traits as factors in friendship dissolution overlooked the confound of elevated individual traits with heightened dyadic differences. We are not the first to suggest that the characteristics of an individual are important primarily in relation to the characteristics of the interaction partner (Hinde, 1995). We cannot rule out the possibility, however, that individual characteristics predict relationship dissolution at extreme or clinical levels of maladjustment.

Our study is not without limitations. Between 5% and 10% of friendships were reconstituted after dissolving. All friendships in the present study were considered equivalent, but we know that some friendships are more important than others (Hartup, 1996), which may help to explain friendships that come and go. The relative magnitude of different predictors may vary as a function of the relative importance of the relationship. It did not appear to be the case, however, that adolescents who reported the most friends adopted the least stringent criteria for friendship or were the most socially skilled, although null findings must be interpreted with care given that the number of friends was a significant predictor in the individual characteristics model. The statistical power to test a large number of predictors was limited, so only the strongest effects were identified. There was insufficient power for multi-level survival analyses to examine the effect of the nesting of friendships within peer networks. Participants with complete data scored higher on teacher-rated school competence in grade 7 than those with incomplete data, suggesting that school competence findings should be interpreted with caution. Singe item measures may not fully capture the constructs they purportedly represent. Acceptance and popularity were so highly correlated that both could not be included in the same model. It will be up to future scholars to identify the unique contributions of each to friendship dissolution. Participant attrition and low teacher-response rates during high school precluded the testing of time-varying predictors. Finally, friendship nominations were collected at annual intervals, which is an imprecise measure of relationship beginnings and endings. Shorter assessment periods may be required to capture the effect of accumulating individual adjustment difficulties.

We conclude that friend compatibility and friendship stability are a function of similarity, and not the presence or absence of a particular individual attribute. Adolescents may desire to affiliate with different others (Thomas & Bowker, 2013), but they are more likely to enjoy successful long-term friendships with similar others.

Acknowledgments

The second author received support from the U.S. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD068421) and the U.S. National Science Foundation (1248598 and 0909733). This research was supported by a grant from the University of Connecticut Research Foundation to the third author. We are grateful for the students, parents, teachers, and school administrators who made this research possible.

Footnotes

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the study concept and design. The data were collected by A. H. N. Cillessen. A. C. Hartl performed the data analyses under the supervision of B. Laursen. A. C. Hartl and B. Laursen drafted the manuscript, and A. H. N. Cillessen provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

References

  1. Aboud F, Mendelson M, Purdy K. Cross-race peer relations and friendship quality. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2003;27(2):165–173. [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen JP, Porter MR, McFarland FC, Marsh P, McElhaney KB. The two faces of adolescents’ success with peers: Adolescent popularity, social adaptation, and deviant behavior. Child Development. 2005;76(3):747–760. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00875.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benenson JF, Christakos A. The greater fragility of females’ versus males’ closest same-sex friendships. Child Development. 2003;74(4):1123–1129. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bowker A. Predicting friendship stability during early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2004;24(2):85–112. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bowker JC. Examining two types of best friendship dissolution during early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2011;31(5):656–670. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bowker JC, Rubin KH, Burgess KB, Booth-LaForce C, Rose-Krasnor L. Behavioral characteristics associated with stable and fluid best friendship patterns in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2006;52(4):671–693. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bracken BA. The multidimensional self-concept scale. Austin, TX: PRO-ED; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bukowski WM, Cillessen AHN, Velásquez AM. Peer ratings. In: Laursen B, Little TD, Card NA, editors. Handbook of developmental research methods. New York: Guilford; 2012. pp. 211–228. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bukowski WM, Motzoi C, Meyer F. Friendship as process, function, and outcome. In: Rubin KH, Bukowski WM, Laursen B, editors. Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups. New York: Guilford; 2009. pp. 217–231. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cairns RB, Leung MC, Buchanan L, Cairns BD. Friendships and social networks in childhood and adolescence: Fluidity, reliability, and interrelations. Child Development. 1995;66(5):1330–1345. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cantin S, Boivin M. Change and stability in children’s social network and self-perceptions during transition from elementary to junior high school. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2004;28(6):561–570. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cillessen AHN, Mayeux L. From censure to reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association between aggression and social status. Child Development. 2004;75(1):147–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00660.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Crick NR, Nelson DA. Relational and physical victimization within friendships: Nobody told me there’d be friends like these. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30(6):599–607. doi: 10.1023/a:1020811714064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. DeLay D, Hartl AC, Laursen B, Denner J, Werner L, Campe S, Ortiz E. Learning from friends: Measuring influence in a dyadic computer instructional setting. International Journal of Research & Method in Education. 2014;37(2):190–205. [Google Scholar]
  15. Dishion TJ, Andrews DW, Crosby L. Antisocial boys and their friends in early adolescence: Relationship characteristics, quality, and interactional process. Child Development. 1995;66(1):139–151. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00861.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eccles JS, Lord S, Buchanan CM. School transitions in early adolescence: What are we doing to our young people? In: Graber JA, Brooks-Gunn J, Petersen AC, editors. Transitions through adolescence: Interpersonal domains and context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1996. pp. 251–284. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ellis W, Zarbatany L. Explaining friendship formation and friendship stability: The role of children’s and friends’ aggression and victimization. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2007;53(1):79–104. [Google Scholar]
  18. Flashman J. Academic achievement and its impact on friend dynamics. Sociology of Education. 2012;85(1):61–80. doi: 10.1177/0038040711417014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Furman W, Shaffer L. Romantic partners, friends, friends with benefits, and casual acquaintances as sexual partners. Journal of Sex Research. 2011;48(6):554–564. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2010.535623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Graham SE, Willet JB, Singer JD. Using discrete-time survival analysis to study event occurrence. In: Newsom J, Jones RN, Hofer SM, editors. Longitudinal data analysis: A practical guide for researchers in aging, health, and social sciences. New York: Routledge; 2013. pp. 329–371. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hartup WW. Adolescents and their friends. In: Laursen B, editor. Close friendships in adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development. Vol. 60. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1993. pp. 3–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hartup WW. The company they keep: Friendships and their developmental significance. Child Development. 1996;67(1):1–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hartup WW, Stevens N. Friendships and adaptation in the life course. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121(3):355–370. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hinde RA. A suggested structure for a science of relationships. Personal Relationships. 1995;2(1):1–15. [Google Scholar]
  25. Laursen B, Collins WA. Parent-child relationships during adolescence. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of adolescent psychology. 2. Vol. 2. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2009. pp. 3–42. [Google Scholar]
  26. Laursen B, Hartup WW. The origins of reciprocity and social exchange in friendships. In: Laursen B, Graziano WG, editors. Social exchange in development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development. Vol. 95. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2002. pp. 27–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Laursen B, Hartup WW, Koplas AL. Towards understanding peer conflict. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1996;42(1):76–102. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lazarsfeld PF, Merton RK. Friendship as a social process: A substantive and methodological analysis. In: Berger M, editor. Freedom and control in modern society. New York: Van Nostrand; 1954. pp. 18–66. [Google Scholar]
  29. Levinger G. A social exchange view on the dissolution of pair relationships. In: Burgess RL, Huston TL, editors. Social exchange in developing relationships. New York: Academic Press; 1979. pp. 169–193. [Google Scholar]
  30. Muthén B, Masyn K. Discrete-time survival mixture analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 2005;30(1):27–58. [Google Scholar]
  31. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 7. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2014. [Google Scholar]
  32. Piehler TF, Dishion TJ. Interpersonal dynamics within adolescent friendships: Dyadic mutuality, deviant talk, and patterns of antisocial behavior. Child Development. 2007;78(5):1611–1624. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01086.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Poulin F, Chan A. Friendship stability and change in childhood and adolescence. Developmental Review. 2010;30(3):257–272. [Google Scholar]
  34. Rose AJ, Rudolph KD. A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132(1):98–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rusbult CE, Van Lange PA. Interdependence, interaction, and relationships. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003;54(1):351–375. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Sprecher S, Regan PC. Liking some things (in some people) more than others: Partner preferences in romantic relationships and friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2002;19(4):463–481. [Google Scholar]
  37. Thibaut JW, Kelley HH. The social psychology of groups. Oxford, England: Wiley; 1959. [Google Scholar]
  38. Thomas KK, Bowker JC. An investigation of desired friendships during early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2013;33(6):867–890. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES