Abstract
Self-narratives of patients have received increasing interest in schizophrenia since they offer unique material to study patients’ subjective experience related to their illness, in particular the alteration of self that accompanies schizophrenia. In this study, we investigated the life narratives and the ability to integrate and bind memories of personal events into a coherent narrative in 27 patients with schizophrenia and 26 controls. Four aspects of life narratives were analyzed: coherence with cultural concept of biography, temporal coherence, causal-motivational coherence and thematic coherence. Results showed that in patients cultural biographical knowledge is preserved, whereas temporal coherence is partially impaired. Furthermore, causal-motivational and thematic coherence are significantly impaired: patients have difficulties explaining how events have modeled their identity, and integrating different events along thematic lines. Impairment of global causal-motivational and thematic coherence was significantly correlated with patients’ executive dysfunction, suggesting that cognitive impairment observed in patients could affect their ability to construct a coherent narrative of their life by binding important events to their self. This study provides new understanding of the cognitive deficits underlying self-disorders in patients with schizophrenia. Our findings suggest the potential usefulness of developing new therapeutic interventions to improve autobiographical reasoning skills.
Disorders of self in patients with schizophrenia are probably one of the most intriguing symptoms of the illness and certainly remain one of most difficult to describe and understand. These disorders have been long considered as defining schizophrenia1,2, or as representing “the core feature” of schizophrenia3,4. The concept of self is however complex and ranges from very basic processes anchored in neurobiological processes to more social or transcendental aspects. Therefore, models are needed to circumscribe the research to particular aspects of the concept of self. One of the most influential models of the self has been developed by Gallagher5 and proposes the distinction between two complementary dimensions of the self: a minimal and a narrative self. The minimal self consists in a pre-reflexive dimension of the self that supports an immediate “sense of self”. The narrative self corresponds to the dimension of self that is extended in time and provides a sense of continuity and coherence to the self across time6,7. It is intimately linked to the stories we tell about ourselves and it grounds on the memory of our personal past, called autobiographical memory. This form of memory is essential for the self and is conceived as the “memory of the self” since it groups together various type of information that all relate to the self8.
The clinical and experimental literature devoted to self-disorders in schizophrenia has mainly considered aspects of the minimal self. With regard to the narrative dimension of the self, self-narratives of patients have recently received increasing interest in schizophrenia research. Indeed, these narratives offer unique material for figuring out the kind of subjective experience patients have during the course of their illness, in particular the alteration of self which accompanies schizophrenia9,10. These abnormalities have been extensively described in clinical reports and qualitative research work on schizophrenia, albeit not clearly objectified in experimental research11,12,13. The analysis of first-person accounts has revealed that the lack of coherence in patients’ life is often mirrored in the disorganization of patients’ narratives of their life. Recent research on patients’ self-narratives has benefited from the empirical approaches developed in the domain of autobiographical memory. Several studies have thus shown that autobiographical memory is impaired in schizophrenia and that patients’ self is supported by less vivid, less specific and less organized memories14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22. Moreover, patients have difficulties to interpret and give meaning to significant personal events23,24. Lack of autobiographical reasoning skills was correlated with patients’ executive dysfunction and negative symptoms23 and was suggested to account for the reduced coherence of their narratives24. These results lead to the hypothesis that executive dysfunction and autobiographical reasoning impairment may represent cognitive mechanisms explaining self-disorders in schizophrenia23.
To date, studies have mostly focused on memories of single personal events. However, impairment of single memories does not allow for assumptions to be made about alterations in coherence and self-continuity in patients with schizophrenia. Thus, investigating the life story as represented by entire life narratives appears to be the most relevant approach to this question. Indeed, mature life narratives coherently organize multiple single event narratives within the context of personal development6,25. Furthermore, the life narrative has been considered the best suited format for ego identity, because it serves to create a sense of personal coherence, unity, continuity and purpose in one’ life across change. This is of prime importance for mental health and well-being6,25.
The ability to create a coherent narrative of one’s life emerges only during adolescence, a critical time for identity formation25,26. According to Habermas and Bluck26, the global coherence of the life story has four major aspects (Fig. 1). A first aspect is coherence with a cultural concept of biography, at its core the cultural life script27 which defines normative transitional life events and their normative timing (for instance, get a job at 22 years old, get married at 25 years old or have the first child at 27 years old). A second aspect is temporal coherence that reflects narrator’s ability to identify when and in what order events took place and that allows the listener to understand the chronology of the life narrative. In this paper, these two aspects were termed narrative framework, as they serve to form the skeleton of the life story, allowing a sequence order of major events. The two other aspects of global coherence are causal-motivational and thematic coherence. They involve complementary processes that unify diversified experiences, thereby maintaining the sense of self-continuity28. On the one hand, causal-motivational coherence enables listeners to understand the narrator’s personal development. Within the narrative, causal-motivational coherence is supported by arguments covering personal change and specifying how events cause other events and influence one’s personality evolution (for instances, “That journey changed many things for me; in that moment I understood what is meant by the meaning of life, and since then I am a little more self-confident”; “When I came back to Vietnam, I realized that in the meantime I had grown away from my own culture, the Vietnamese way of life, let’s say from these Vietnamese traditional mentalities”). On the other hand, thematic coherence enables listeners to apprehend what is stable in narrator’s life across dominant life themes or stable personality traits. This stability is expressed using arguments which explain an action by an enduring personality trait, declare an action as reflecting a trait or clarify in what an event contradicts personality (for instance, “In puberty I was always extremely shy and well-behaved. I mean, I never rebelled against anything. So I was very restricted and limited in my ideas and possibilities. That’s why I had never a boyfriend.”; “Normally, I and the guys in my class, we are really uncool, I mean very well-behaving the whole time. But on that school trip, we freaked out. Oh man, I was so drunk.”). Both types of arguments correspond to the concept of autobiographical reasoning and make it possible to compensate personal discontinuity by embedding autobiographical disruptions and changes in a coherent life story28. Hence, global coherence is a property of the entire life narrative and can be measured by ratings (i.e. global ratings of coherence). Specific local text elements contribute to temporal, thematic and causal-motivational global coherence26,29,30 and can be counted in life narratives (i.e. local indicators of coherence).
In this study we investigated possible impairments in the narrative framework and in causal-motivational and thematic coherence of life narratives of patients with schizophrenia. Indeed, the life story framework is highly relevant for understanding better the cognitive mechanisms underlying the lack of personal coherence clinically described in patients with schizophrenia. Based on the above-mentioned studies, we expected to find specific impairment of autobiographical reasoning and consequently of causal-motivational and thematic coherence of patients’ life narratives, which in turn may be related to executive dysfunction23,24 and a deficit of general self-representation12,17. We did not make predictions regarding temporal coherence, considering the contradictory results of previous studies showing either a preserved temporal organization of autobiographical memory clusters14,19 or a reduced chronological coherence within memories of patients with schizophrenia24.
Methods
Participants
Twenty-seven stabilized outpatients (11 women) were recruited from the Psychiatric Department of Strasbourg’s University Hospital, all fulfilling the DSM-IV-TR diagnosis of schizophrenia31. Patients with a major depressive episode (Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia, CDSS32 score higher than 6) and patients with an IQ score below 70 (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised, WAIS-R33) were excluded. All patients except one were receiving long-term neuroleptic treatment (first generation, n = 22; second generation, n = 2; both, n = 2). Nine patients were treated with benzodiazepines and 4 with antiparkinsonian medication. The control group included 26 healthy participants (11 women). Patients and controls had neither current substance abuse nor a history of traumatic brain-injury, epilepsy, or other neurological disorders. Our groups did not differ significantly in terms of age, level of schooling, or IQ.
The IRB of Lille III approved this study and all experiments were conducted in accordance with approved guidelines and regulations. All participants gave their informed written consent.
Procedure
Clinical and neuropsychological assessments
Symptom severity of schizophrenia in patients was assessed using the Positive And Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS)34. All patients were clinically assessed for depression, with the CDSS32, and controls completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)35. Participants’ anxiety level and self-esteem were assessed by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)36 and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem (RSE) Scale37 respectively.
Executive functioning, particularly mental flexibility and strategic retrieval of information in memory, was assessed with (1) the Trail-Making Test (TMT, Part A and B)38 and (2) the semantic and phonologic verbal fluency tasks39. We selected executive functions considered to be particularly involved in life narration and autobiographical reasoning abilities23,40.
Biographical practices
Given that the frequency of biographical practices can influence the construction of the life narrative, they were assessed by asking for frequency ratings of biographical activities29 (such as keeping a diary, looking at old pictures, talking about problems with friends).
Subjective Sense of Coherence
Participants rated their subjective sense of coherence on the scale developed by Antonovsky41 which comprises three subscales: comprehensibility of external events, their manageability, and the meaningfulness ensuing from them. This sense of coherence is a salutogenic index reflecting individual resilience.
Life narratives
The protocol developed by Habermas and de Silveira29 was used. Participants were asked to recall the seven most important events they had ever experienced and to write them down on cards (see Supplementary Material S1). Then, participants narrated their life story in 20 minutes. Instructions aimed at encouraging participants to orally narrate specific memories, integrating them in the narrative, and explaining how they had become the person they are today (see Supplementary Material S2). The experimenter did not interrupt participants but encouraged them to pursue the narrative if they had time left. All life narratives were audio-recorded.
Afterwards, participants dated the seven memories, rated their vividness and their emotional valence and intensity on 7-point scales. Finally, participants completed the short version of the Centrality of Event Scale (CES)42, assessing the extent to which each of the seven events had become a reference point for personal identity or a turning point in their life.
Cultural Life Script
To assess knowledge of the Cultural Life Script, participants imagined the seven most important events they thought would happen in a newborn’s life across the life span27 and estimated the typical age for each event.
Scoring
Segmentation
After a verbatim transcription, narratives were divided into propositions, which correspond to minimal meaning sentences (see Supplementary Material S3, for examples). Two independent coders segmented 16 life stories into propositions and had an agreement of 93%. Each disagreement was resolved by discussion, remaining narratives were segmented by one of the two coders.
Coding
The causal-motivational and thematic coherence as well as the narrative framework were measured at the global and local level, using manuals developed by Habermas and de Silveira29 (see Supplementary Material S4 and S5). At the local level, the proportion of indicators of causal-motivational, thematic and temporal coherence was calculated by dividing the number of each category of indicators by the total number of propositions. At the global level, causal-motivational, thematic and temporal coherence of the entire narratives were rated using three different 7-point scales.
Causal-motivational and thematic coherence
At the local level, indicators of causal-motivational coherence encompass self-event connections generating identity changes, and other autobiographical arguments relating to behavioral and personality changes. They indicate that the narrator tries to maintain self-continuity despite the occurrence of identity changes. Local indicators of thematic coherence include self-event connections reflecting the stability of identity (e.g. “I’ve always been a shy person and had problems to make friends”). Both causal-motivational and thematic local indicators correspond to autobiographical reasoning.
At the global level, the scale for global causal-motivational coherence of life narratives measures how well they portray the development of the narrator’s personality, whereas the scale for global thematic coherence measures the presence of both a guiding thread and thematic connections across different life episodes29,30.
All narratives were coded or rated by two independent raters with good inter-rater reliability for both local indicators (causal: ĸ = 0.80; thematic: ĸ = 0.71) and global coherence scales (causal-motivational: ric = .85; thematic: ric = .73).
Narrative framework
Temporal coherence.
We used temporal indicators of date, age, life period, and distance from the present as local indicators of temporal coherence. They allow the reader to locate events across the course of the story.
Moreover, the linear temporal order was also assessed through the proportion of anachronies present in narratives. Every deviation from the linear temporal order encompassing at least four propositions was counted as an anachrony43.
Finally, the global temporal coherence was assessed using a 7-point scale measuring how the narrator was able to identify when and in what order events took place.
Inter-rater reliability between the two independent raters was good for local indicators (ĸ = 0.85), anachronies (ĸ = 0.74) and the global coherence scale (ric = .89).
Scoring of the Cultural Life Script. A typicality score was calculated from the 7 events chosen by the participants, by weighing each nomination by its relative frequency in the normative sample27.
Statistical Analysis
Between-group comparisons of clinical and neuropsychological data were performed using separate one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Separate MANOVAs were performed on the proportions of local indicators and on global scales of causal-motivational and thematic coherences. Concerning temporal coherence, given that years of schooling was significantly correlated with both the proportion of local indicators and global ratings of temporal coherence, separate ANCOVAs were performed on both measures with the number of years of schooling as a covariate. The Cultural Life Script score was analyzed with an ANOVA. Finally, Pearson correlations were calculated between the proportions of local indicators of coherence and the ratings of global coherence on one side, and both clinical and neuropsychological data on the other side.
Results
Clinical and neuropsychological measures
Both state and trait anxiety level were higher in the patient group compared to the control group (ps < .03), whereas self-esteem was lower in patients than in controls (p < .001). Patients’ executive functioning performances were significantly lower than those of controls in both the verbal fluency task (p = .002) and the TMT shifting score (TMT B-A) (p = .02), taking into consideration that neuropsychological data were missing for two patients (Table 1).
Table 1. Demographic, clinical and neuropsychological data of patients with schizophrenia and controls.
Patients with schizophrenia(n = 27) | Controls(n = 26) | ANOVA | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | (SD) | Mean | (SD) | F | p-value | Effect size (ŋ2) | |
Age (years) | 35.15 | (8.91) | 33.88 | (9.80) | 0.24 | .62 | 0.05 |
Years of schooling | 12.70 | (2.37) | 12.92 | (2.40) | 0.11 | .74 | 0.02 |
BDIa | 2.96 | (3.38) | |||||
CDSSb | 1.67 | (1.63) | |||||
STAI-YAc | 47.63 | (7.25) | 42.74 | (7.06) | 4.88 | .03 | 0.11 |
STAI-YBd | 49.58 | (9.08) | 39.04 | (7.62) | 16.73 | <.001 | 0.29 |
Rosenberg Self-Esteem (RSE) | 29.67 | (4.52) | 34.50 | (3.83) | 17.56 | <.001 | 0.26 |
Onset of the illness (years) | 22.00 | (5.59) | |||||
Duration of illness (years) | 13.08 | (8.28) | |||||
PANSSe (total score) | 64.67 | (19.37) | |||||
- Postive symptoms | 15.81 | (5.37) | |||||
- Negative symptoms | 18.52 | (6.81) | |||||
- General symptoms | 30.33 | (10.05) | |||||
f-NARTf (premorbid IQg) | 106.96 | (8.34) | 108.65 | (4.74) | 0.81 | .37 | 0.02 |
WAIS-Rh (current IQ) | 95.88 | (13.59) | 99.85 | (8.56) | 1.57 | .22 | 0.03 |
Verbal fluency (z-score) | −0.09 | (0.93) | 0.63 | (0.68) | 10.11 | .002 | 0.17 |
Shifting score (TMTi B-A) | −0.42 | (0.91) | 0.13 | (0.68) | 5.91 | .02 | 0.11 |
Biographical practices | 31.74 | (11.27) | 31.27 | (13.11) | 0.02 | .89 | 0.00 |
Sense of Coherence (total score) | 127.04 | (21.14) | 160.08 | (21.02) | 32.54 | <.001 | 0.39 |
- Manageability | 43.52 | (7.57) | 55.11 | (8.23) | 28.54 | <.001 | 0.36 |
- Meaningfullness | 38.37 | (6.43) | 47.69 | (6.47) | 27.64 | <.001 | 0.35 |
- Comprehensibility | 45.15 | (10.32) | 57.27 | (9.61) | 19.53 | <.001 | 0.28 |
aBeck Depression Inventory.
bCalgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia.
cState-Trait Anxiety Inventory manual; part A assessing state anxiety.
dState-Trait Anxiety Inventory manual; part B assessing trait anxiety.
ePositive And Negative Syndrome Scale.
fFrench National Adult Reading Test.
gIntelligence Quotient.
hWechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised.
iTrail-Making Test.
Biographical practices
Biographical practices did not differ between groups.
Subjective Sense of Coherence
Patients displayed a significantly lower sense of coherence than controls in all the three subscales (all ps < .001). This indicates that patients have difficulties to deal with stressful situations and to preserve a sense of coherence in spite of them.
Characteristics of autobiographical memories and life narratives
The characteristics of autobiographical memories selected to structure the life narratives did not differ between patients and controls in their vividness, or centrality score (ps > .052). Further analyses of CES items revealed that events chosen by patients were neither more disruptive (items 5 and 7) nor less integrated (items 1 to 4) than those of controls (ps > .14). Only the mean emotional valence of memories was less positive in patients (p = .004). The average time taken to narrate the life story did not differ between groups (p = .59), but the patients’ narratives were significantly shorter (p = .04) (see Supplementary Material S6).
Causal-motivational and thematic coherence
A MANOVA revealed that autobiographical reasoning (as reflected by both causal-motivational and thematic local indicators) was significantly lower in the patients compared to controls (F(1,51) = 9.57; p = .003; ŋ2 = .16). Univariate analyses showed that indicators of causal-motivational coherence were significantly reduced in patients’ narratives (F(1,51) = 6.72; p = .01; ŋ2 = .12), whereas indicators of thematic coherence did not differ between groups (F(1,51) = 2.60; p = .11; ŋ2 = .05) (Fig. 2).
A MANOVA revealed that global causal-motivational and thematic coherence were significantly lower in the patients than in controls (F(1,51) = 12.45; p < .001; ŋ2 = .20). Univariate analyses showed that both scores were significantly lower in patients (F(1,51) = 8.85; p < .01; ŋ2 = .15 and F(1,51) = 6.63; p = .01; ŋ2 = .12 respectively) (Fig. 2).
Narrative framework
Temporal Coherence.
The proportion of local temporal indicators did not differ significantly between groups (F(1,50) = 0.01; p = .92; ŋ2 = .00). The proportion of total anachronies was significantly higher in the patient group (M = 1.87; S.D. = 1.71) compared to control group (M = 1.11; S.D. = 0.66) (F(1,51) = 4.43; p = .04; ŋ2 = .08). Finally, global temporal coherence was significantly lower in patients’ life narratives (F(1,50) = 4.62; p = .04; ŋ2 = .08) (Fig. 3).
Cultural Life Script. The typicality of the cultural life script did not differ significantly between groups (F(1,47) = 1.27; p=.26; ŋ2 = .03) (Fig. 3).
Correlational analyses
Correlations between local indicators and global coherence were found positive and significant in both groups for the temporal coherence, significant only in controls for causal-motivational coherence and not significant in either group for thematic coherence (see Table 2).
Table 2. Correlations between global ratings and local indicators of coherence in patients with schizophrenia and controls.
Patients with schizophrenia (n = 27) | Controls (n = 26) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Global temporal coherence | Global causal-motivational coherence | Global thematic coherence | Global temporal coherence | Global causal-motivational coherence | Global thematic coherence | |
Local temporal coherence | 0.45* | 0.45* | ||||
Local causal-motivational coherence | 0.27 | 0.52** | ||||
Local thematic coherence | 0.32 | −0.21 |
*p < .05; **p < .01.
Concerning the centrality of chosen events, positive and significant correlations were observed between CES items in both groups, in particular items related to disruption were correlated with those related to integration (rs > .43; n = 26; p < .03) (see Supplementary Material S7).
In the patient group and at the local level, the proportion of both causal-motivational and thematic indicators of coherence (reflecting autobiographical reasoning) correlated with the global subjective sense of coherence (r = .41; n = 27; p = .03) and the correlation was even stronger with the subscale “meaningfulness” (r = .51; n = 27; p < .01). The averaged ratings of global causal-motivational and thematic coherence correlated positively with the shifting score (TMT B-A) (r = .40; n = 25; p = .047). Finally, measures of coherence did not correlate significantly with clinical symptoms in patients.
Discussion
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study which demonstrates that patients with schizophrenia are impaired in their ability to recount coherent narratives of their entire life. This study also provides an experimental assessment or objectification of what psychiatrists have been observing clinically for many decades, that is, patients displayed a dramatic reduction of autobiographical reasoning skills, which are essential to create and maintain coherence in life narratives despite autobiographical disruptions and changes. This impairment was correlated both with patients’ executive dysfunction and with the overall reduction of the subjective sense of coherence experienced by patients. Moreover, the narrative framework, which represents the basic scaffolding of the life story upon which major life events are ordered, was partially weakened in patients. These results cannot be explained by group differences in terms of IQ level, years of schooling, or of daily biographical practices of participants. Moreover, memories which were selected to structure the life story were rated as highly central for the self in both groups, which means that patients understood the procedure correctly. Finally, the reduced coherence of patients’ life narratives is apparently not due to a lack of the basic cultural knowledge which typically grounds the life story.
Regarding the narrative framework, our results showed that patients used as many temporal indicators as controls and that correlations between scores of local and global temporal coherence were significant in both groups. However, the global temporal coherence of patients’ narratives was high and yet significantly lower than that of controls’ narratives. Indeed, despite the presence of temporal indicators, anachronies were significantly increased in patients’ narratives, making it harder to understand the temporal location and order of events. This is consistent with previous studies showing that patients with schizophrenia narrated less chronologically ordered personal narratives than controls24. Taken together, our results show a weakness of the temporal component of the narrative framework of the life story despite a preservation of its cultural component.
As expected, causal-motivational and thematic coherence was dramatically reduced in almost all patients’ narratives. Patients with schizophrenia made fewer comments on changes that occurred throughout their life and to explain how these events have influenced the person they are today, which was reflected in a reduced global causal-motivational coherence. Our findings are in line with previous results showing that patients have difficulties to draw meanings from past personally significant events16,17,44. Contrary to controls, local indicators and global ratings of causal-motivational coherence were not significantly correlated in patients, suggesting that the impaired causal-motivational coherence of the entire life narrative of patients may involve other mechanisms than those involved in the ability to draw causal-motivational relationships with particular events.
In contrast, patients did not use fewer local indicators of thematic coherence than controls. In line with this result, one study14 showed that patients were able to give as many self-statements as controls to define themselves. However, thematic references were rare in our control group and we cannot rule out a possible floor effect that may have masked actual impairments in patients and a correlation between local indicators and global thematic coherence as found previously in healthy subjects30. In fact, when reading patients’ life narratives, the thematic guiding thread appeared to be more difficult to identify than in control narratives. Patients displayed difficulties interpreting events and integrating them along thematic lines, as confirmed by their lower score of global thematic coherence. These data confirm the reduction of thematic coherence that has been highlighted in narratives of Self-Defining Memories in patients with schizophrenia44. Moreover, it is worth mentioning that even if the proportion of self-statements was not significantly reduced in patients’ narratives, these self-statements are generally more uncertain and more changing over time than in controls45. Patients have difficulties articulating coherently different self-positions12. Taken together, this weakness of self-definition and self-representation across time may also account for the overall impairment of thematic coherence of patients’ narratives.
From the perspective of psychopathology, we argue that the reduced coherence of patients’ life narratives is related to disorders of the self in schizophrenia40. We suggest that the relationship between life story and self should be examined under the light of two complementary perspectives. First, the lack of life narrative coherence may point out the metacognitive dysfunction which is characteristic of schizophrenia17,46,47. Indeed, self-reflectivity deficiency may reduce patients’ ability to integrate different self-positions12 and to organize a coherent narrative of their life that establishes self-continuity and provides self-understanding of their own life48,49. Second, the reduced coherence of patients’ life narratives may also derive from an executive dysfunction, which is a core symptom of schizophrenia as well50. Executive functions play a critical role in the organization of autobiographical knowledge8, and our study showed a significant positive correlation between mental flexibility reduction and the decrease of coherence in patients’ narratives. Moreover, mental flexibility and the ability to draw lessons from past events have been shown impaired in schizophrenia23,49. While correlation analyses do not allow interpreting a causal direction of this association, one may consider that executive dysfunction may affect patients’ ability to construct a coherent life narrative and consequently alter, in the long run, the biographical and narrative representations of the self. In fact, these metacognitive and cognitive perspectives should be seen as complementary, because several studies have shown reciprocal relationships between metacognitive dysfunction and measures of executive functioning in schizophrenia17,46,47.
Based on our results, several questions remain unanswered. For instance, the lack of coherence of patients’ narratives may not actually derive from the impairment of autobiographical reasoning but could simply reflect the breakdown in life-continuity which follows the emergence of the illness in their life or mirror the disorganization of their daily life due to the illness. Against this hypothesis, our analyses of the CES items showed that life events chosen by the patients were neither more disruptive nor less integrated than those of controls, and that in both groups, the more disrupted an event is, the more integrated it is.
Our work has clinical implications because in addition to the objective impairments in life story coherence, patients reported a reduced subjective sense of coherence in their life. Indeed, patients experienced their environment as less coherent, predictable and explicable, and they reported lower meaning of their life. Interestingly, we found that patients with lower autobiographical reasoning skills reported weaker sense of coherence in their life. Previous studies have shown that autobiographical reasoning helps compensate effects of disruptions on the sense of self-continuity28, and that individuals with more coherent and well-integrated life story have a higher sense of self-continuity and self-understanding of their life51,52. Thus, patients’ difficulty to integrate changes in life into the self is reflected in their life narratives. This might represents one factor accounting for both the reduced sense of coherence reported by the patients and the reduced sense of self reported by clinicians.
Finally, our results advocate for the importance of therapeutic approaches such as narrative enhancement-based therapies to improve autobiographical reasoning skills in patients with schizophrenia53. These interventions have demonstrated positive changes on measures of well-being, lifestyle, cognitive skills, and self-experience in patients with severe mental disorders11,54,55,56 and may be recommended for reinforcing both self-experience and well-being51,52 in patients with schizophrenia.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Allé, M. C. et al. Impaired coherence of life narratives of patients with schizophrenia. Sci. Rep. 5, 12934; doi: 10.1038/srep12934 (2015).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Dr. Hiroshi Kobayashi, Laure Bodet and Sébastien Kirchherr for helping to assess life narratives coherence. This study was funded by INSERM (Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale), France.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author Contributions F.B. and J.M.D. designed the study. M.C.A. collected the data. M.C.A. and F.B. performed the statistical analyses and wrote the first complete draft of the manuscript. J.P., C.K., T.H. and J.M.D. provided substantial modifications and all authors contributed to the final version and approved it.
References
- Lysaker P. H. & Lysaker J. T. Schizophrenia and alterations in self-experience: a comparison of 6 perspectives. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 331–340 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bleuler E. Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias (International Universities Press, 1950). [Google Scholar]
- Sass L. A. & Parnas J. Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr. Bull. 29, 427–444 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parnas J. & Handest P. Phenomenology of anomalous self-experience in early schizophrenia. Compr. Psychiatry 44, 121–134 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher S. Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 14–21 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baerger D. R. & McAdams D. P. Life Story Coherence and its Relation to Psychological Well-Being. Narrat. Inq. 9, 69–96 (1999). [Google Scholar]
- Ricoeur P. Narrative Identity. Philos. Today 35, 73–81 (1991). [Google Scholar]
- Conway M. A. Memory and the Self. J. Mem. Lang. 53, 594–628 (2005). [Google Scholar]
- Chadwick P. K. Peer-Professional First-Person Account: Schizophrenia From the Inside-Phenomenology and the Integration of Causes and Meanings. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 166–173 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman B. J. The subjective experience of perceptual and cognitive disturbances in schizophrenia: A review of autobiographical accounts. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 30, 333–340 (1974). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P. H., Wickett A. M., Campbell K. & Buck K. D. Movement towards coherence in the psychotherapy of schizophrenia: a method for assessing narrative transformation. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 191, 538–541 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P. H. & Lysaker J. T. Psychosis and the disintegration of dialogical self-structure: Problems posed by schizophrenia for the maintenance of dialogue. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 74 Part 1, 23–33 (2001). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saavedra J., Cubero M. & Crawford P. Incomprehensibility in the narratives of individuals with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Qual. Health Res. 19, 1548–1558 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennouna-Greene M. et al. Self-images and related autobiographical memories in schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 247–257 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuervo-Lombard C. et al. Autobiographical memory of adolescence and early adulthood events: an investigation in schizophrenia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 335–343 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Argembeau A., Raffard S. & Van der Linden M. Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 247–251 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimaggio G., Salvatore G., Popolo R. & Lysaker P. H. Autobiographical memory and mentalizing impairment in personality disorders and schizophrenia: clinical and research implications. Front. Psychol. 3 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinstein A., Goldberg T. E., Nowlin B. & Weinberger D. R. Types and characteristics of remote memory impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 30, 155–163 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morise C., Berna F. & Danion J.-M. The organization of autobiographical memory in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 128, 156–160 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potheegadoo J., Berna F., Cuervo-Lombard C. & Danion J.-M. Field visual perspective during autobiographical memory recall is less frequent among patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 150, 88–92 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potheegadoo J., Cuervo-Lombard C., Berna F. & Danion J.-M. Distorted perception of the subjective temporal distance of autobiographical events in patients with schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 90–99 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ricarte J. J., Hernández J. V., Latorre J. M., Danion J. M. & Berna F. Rumination and autobiographical memory impairment in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 160, 163–168 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berna F. et al. Impaired ability to give a meaning to personally significant events in patients with schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 703–711 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raffard S. et al. Narrative identity in schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 328–340 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erikson E. H. Identity: Youth and crisis (W.W. Norton & Company, 1968). [Google Scholar]
- Habermas T. & Bluck S. Getting a life: the emergence of the life story in adolescence. Psychol. Bull. 126, 748–769 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntsen D. & Rubin D. C. Cultural life scripts structure recall from autobiographical memory. Mem. Cognit. 32, 427–442 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habermas T. & Köber C. Autobiographical reasoning in life narratives buffers the effect of biographical disruptions on the sense of self-continuity. Memory. 9, 1–11 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habermas T. & de Silveira C. The development of global coherence in life narratives across adolescence: temporal, causal, and thematic aspects. Dev. Psychol. 44, 707–721 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köber C., Schmiedek F. & Habermas T. Characterizing lifespan development of three aspects of coherence in life narratives: A cohort-sequential study. Dev. Psychol. 51, 260–275 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV-TR, 4th ed (American Psychiatric Association Press, 2004). [Google Scholar]
- Addington D., Addington J. & Maticka-Tyndale E. Assessing depression in schizophrenia: the Calgary Depression Scale. Br. J. Psychiatry. Suppl. 22, 39–44 (1993). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman A. S. & Lichtenberger E. O. Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence (John Wiley & Sons, 2005). [Google Scholar]
- Kay S. R., Fiszbein A. & Opler L. A. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13, 261–276 (1987). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A. T. A systematic investigation of depression. Compr. Psychiatry 2, 163–170 (1961). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger C. D. State-trait anxiety inventory: a comprehensive bibliography (Consulting Psychologists Press, 1993). [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image (Princeton University Press Princeton, NJ, 1965). [Google Scholar]
- Reitan R. M. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J. Consult. Psychol. 19, 393–394 (1955). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardebat D., Doyon B., Puel M., Goulet P. & Joanette Y. Formal and semantic lexical evocation in normal subjects. Performance and dynamics of production as a function of sex, age and educational level. Acta Neurol. Belg. 90, 207–217 (1990). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P. H., Wickett A. & Davis L. W. Narrative qualities in schizophrenia: associations with impairments in neurocognition and negative symptoms. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 193, 244–249 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antonovsky A. The structure and properties of the sense of coherence scale. Soc. Sci. Med. 36, 725–733 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntsen D. & Rubin D. C. The centrality of event scale: a measure of integrating a trauma into one’s identity and its relation to post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 219–231 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habermas T., Ehlert-Lerche S. & de Silveira C. The development of the temporal macrostructure of life narratives across adolescence: beginnings, linear narrative form, and endings. J. Pers. 77, 527–559 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raffard S. et al. Exploring self-defining memories in schizophrenia. Memory 17, 26–38 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boulanger M., Dethier M., Gendre F. & Blairy S. Identity in Schizophrenia: A study of trait-knowledge. Psychiatry. Res. 30, 367–374 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P. H. et al. Metacognition in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: methods of assessment and associations with neurocognition, symptoms, cognitive style and function. Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci. 51, 54–62 (2014). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P. H. et al. Metacognition amidst narratives of self and illness in schizophrenia: associations with neurocognition, symptoms, insight and quality of life. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 112, 64–71 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park C. L. Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol. Bull. 136, 257–301 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer J. A., Blagov P., Berry M. & Oost K. M. Self-Defining Memories, Scripts, and the Life Story: Narrative Identity in Personality and Psychotherapy. J. Pers. 81, 569–582 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch D. M. & Ceaser A. Cognition in schizophrenia: core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 27–34 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lilgendahl J. P. & McAdams D. P. Constructing stories of self-growth: how individual differences in patterns of autobiographical reasoning relate to well-being in midlife. J. Pers. 79, 391–428 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean K. C., Breen A. V. & Fournier M. A. Constructing the Self in Early, Middle, and Late Adolescent Boys: Narrative Identity, Individuation, and Well-Being. J. Res. Adolesc. 20, 166–187 (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Dimaggio G. Disorganized Narratives in Clinical Practice. J. Constr. Psychol. 19, 103–108 (2006). [Google Scholar]
- Roe D., Hasson-Ohayon I., Derhi O., Yanos P. T. & Lysaker P. H. Talking about life and finding solutions to different hardships: a qualitative study on the impact of narrative enhancement and cognitive therapy on persons with serious mental illness. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 198, 807–812 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smorti A., Pananti B. & Rizzo A. Autobiography as tool to improve lifestyle, well being, and self-narrative in patients with mental disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 198, 564–571 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smorti A., Risaliti F., Pananti B. & Cipriani V. Autobiography as a tool for self-construction: a study of patients with mental disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 196, 531–537 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.