Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2015 Aug 11;10(8):e0134997. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134997

Copy Number Variation Screen Identifies a Rare De Novo Deletion at Chromosome 15q13.1-13.3 in a Child with Language Impairment

Kerry A Pettigrew 1, Emily Reeves 2, Ruth Leavett 2, Marianna E Hayiou-Thomas 2, Anahita Sharma 1, Nuala H Simpson 3, Angela Martinelli 1, Paul Thompson 4, Charles Hulme 5, Margaret J Snowling 4,6, Dianne F Newbury 3, Silvia Paracchini 1,*
Editor: Bert De Smedt7
PMCID: PMC4532445  PMID: 26262844

Abstract

A significant proportion of children (up to 7% in the UK) present with pronounced language difficulties that cannot be explained by obvious causes like other neurological and medical conditions. A substantial genetic component is predicted to underlie such language problems. Copy number variants (CNVs) have been implicated in neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions, such as autism and schizophrenia, but it is not fully established to what extent they might contribute to language disorders. We conducted a CNV screen in a longitudinal cohort of young children with language-related difficulties (n = 85), focusing on single events at candidate loci. We detected a de novo deletion on chromosome 15q13.1–13.3. The adjacent 15q11-13.1 locus is disrupted in Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes, while disruptions across the breakpoints (BP1-BP6) have previously been implicated in different neurodevelopmental phenotypes including autism, intellectual disability (ID), seizures and developmental delay (DD). This is the first report of a deletion at BP3-BP5 being linked to a deficit confined to language impairment, in the absence of ID, expanding the range of phenotypes that implicate the chromosome 15q13 locus.

Introduction

Approximately 3–7% of pre-school English-speaking children have pronounced language impairment (LI) [1], which cannot be accounted for by intellectual, sensory or physical impairment, or by poor educational opportunities [2,3]. LI is often the manifestation of other profound deficits or conditions. Furthermore, it often co-occurs with learning difficulties, such as reading problems (or dyslexia). [4,5], attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [6,7] and autism spectrum disorder (ASD)[8,9]. These observations exemplify the challenges of defining clear and distinct diagnostic criteria for disorders that could in fact be different manifestations along a spectrum of a core deficit and might share some common genetic determinants [3].

Family and twin studies indicate a substantial genetic influence in LI [10], estimating heritability to be around 70%, with a complex multifactorial mode of inheritance. Genetic associations reported so far explain only a small fraction of the estimated heritability [11]. Common variants have been identified mainly through association studies at specific loci, including the CNTNAP2, CMIP and ATP2C2 genes[11]. A few genome-wide association studies (GWAS) for language abilities have been conducted in epidemiological samples [11]. The most significant finding has been the association between ROBO2 and expressive vocabulary in young children[12] The only locus reaching genome-wide significance in clinical samples collected for LI was reported at the NOP9 locus, when modelling for parent-of-origin effects [13]. A few chromosomal rearrangements have also been identified. SEMA6D, a gene located on chromosome 15q21, has been implicated in language difficulties by breakpoint mapping of a balanced duplication [14].An emerging role for copy number variants (CNV) in disease aetiology has been reported for several neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders [15]. Rare deletions and duplications have been implicated in intellectual disability (ID), developmental delay (DD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [1618], all of which can manifest language problems. Several studies have shown a greater CNV burden of large events in ID, DD and ASD cases compared with unaffected individuals [18,19].

In addition to overall CNV burden across the genome as a whole, recurrent rearrangements at specific loci are often associated with syndromes characterised by speech and language delay in conjunction with other specific features. Examples include the 22q13 deletion syndrome [20], Smith-Magenis syndrome caused by deletions at 17p11 [21] and Potocki-Lupski syndrome (PTLS), caused by a duplication at chromosome 17p11.2 [22], presenting with language delay and speech difficulties.

Disruptions at 15q11-q13.3 have been implicated in different neurodevelopmental phenotypes depending on the locations of breakpoints. Recurrent chromosomal rearrangements at this locus occur as a result of non-allelic homologous recombination between six breakpoints (BP1-BP6) (S1 Fig), corresponding to regions containing multiple copies of the human chromosome 15 low-copy repeat (LCR15) duplicon [23]. While deletions at BP2-BP3 occur in Prader-Willi (PWS) [24] and Angelman syndromes (AS) [25], BP4-BP5 deletions and duplications are associated with a range of neuropsychiatric outcomes such as ID/DD, epilepsy, speech problems, ASD, schizophrenia, mood disorder and ADHD[26].

To date, very few studies have assessed the contribution of CNVs to LI. One CNV screening, in a cohort of children with LI, identified a microdeletion in the ZNF277 gene in an individual with severe receptive and expressive language impairment. This microdeletion was inherited from both parents, who were heterozygous and had a history of language difficulties [27]. Further analysis of this locus in an additional 321 families selected for LI identified another 5 carriers of the microdeletions, showing an increased frequency (1.1%) compared to control cohorts (0.4%). Genome-wide burden analysis in 152 families selected for LI implicated inherited common CNVs, not as single events, but in combination with other genetic risk factors [28]. However, de novo CNV appeared to be relevant in simplex families.

Here, we report a screen for CNV events in 85 children selected for having language difficulties and/or a family risk of reading problems from a longitudinal study of language and literacy development. We report a de novo BP3-BP5 deletion at chromosome 15q13.1–13.3 in a child with early-diagnosed language impairment. This is the first report of a 15q13 deletion at the BP3-BP5 breakpoints to be implicated in LI with no other apparent medical conditions or generalised developmental problems.

Materials and Methods

Samples

Eighty-six individuals (n = 64 probands, n = 22 siblings) were selected from a sample of children participating in a longitudinal study of reading and language development. The 64 probands were recruited because either showed preschool language impairment and/or had a family risk of dyslexia (FRD) [29]. As expected, many children with preschool language impairment resolved their spoken language difficulties by age 5. At the time of entering the genetic screening, when children were 8–9 years old, 44 children were classified as having language impairment and/or dyslexia and the remaining 42 children were classified as typically developing.

The cohort has been described previously [29,30] and further details are provided in S1 File. Exclusionary criteria included chronic illness, deafness, English as a second language or known neurological disorder (e.g. epilepsy, cerebral palsy, ASD).

Saliva was collected from probands, siblings and parents using the Oragene Collection kit and DNA extracted according to the manufacturer’s instructions (DNA Genotek Inc, Ottawa, Canada). DNA from parents and unscreened siblings was available for validation experiments to determine whether CNVs were de novo or inherited. Classification of probands’ status according to language, reading and mathematical abilities was determined using assessments undertaken during the course of the study. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed using the Mplus software [31] in order to condense these individual assessments to seven constructs at pre-school and school age (S1 Table). Additional details were available, including age of first words, age of first walking, birth weight and medical conditions. The study was approved by NHS Research Ethics (Yorkshire and the Humber—Humber Bridge) and the University of York, Department of Psychology Ethics Committee. Written consent was obtained from all study participants or from the parents or guardians on behalf of the children enrolled in this study.

Copy Number Variation screening

All 86 children were genotyped using the Illumina Human OmniExpress-24 platform which includes ~700,000 SNPs. Analysis was conducted with Illumina GenomeStudio 2011 (Genotyping module v1.9.4, Genome Viewer v1.9.0). Any SNP with a GenTrain (internal quality) score of < 0.5 or genotyping success rate of < 95% was excluded from further analyses. One individual was excluded because genotype data indicated a possible contamination. The chromosome plots of all individuals were examined manually to detect any large copy number events. CNVs were predicted from SNP data within PennCNV (2011 Jun16 version) [32] and QuantiSNP v2.2 [33] packages. All individuals had a standard deviation (SD) for the LogR ratio < 0.35, a B-allele frequency drift < 0.002 and waviness factor within ± 0.04 in PennCNV analyses, and an average LogR ratio SD < 0.3 and B-allele frequency SD < 0.15 in QuantiSNP. Consistent high confidence CNVs were selected, based on the following criteria: spanning at least three consecutive genetic variants, confidence score > 10, and predicted by both algorithms. The consistent high confidence CNV calls were then compared with those reported in control individuals (Database of Genomic Variants (DGV), version GRCh37/hg19, release date 16/10/2014) and were allocated to the following categories: (i) common (overlapping ≥ 50% with ≥ 5 events in the control database), (ii) rare (overlapping ≥ 50% with < 5 events), (iii) rare with low overlap (overlapping with known events but < 50%) and (iv) novel (not overlapping with any known event).

CNV validation

We selected CNVs for validation that were either rare or novel (according to the (ii), (iii) and (iv) categories described above) and disrupted loci previously implicated in neurodevelopmental conditions or disorders presenting symptoms of language difficulties. We prioritised rare or novel events recurring in our cohort at higher frequency than expected and events that spanned all or part of at least one exon. Selected CNVs were validated by quantitative PCR (qPCR) in a 10μl reaction volume containing genomic DNA, 1X SsoFast EvaGreen supermix (Bio-Rad Laboratories Ltd, Hertfordshire, UK) and an assay-dependent amount of each primer. Primer sequences are detailed in S2 Table. Reactions were cycled using a Viia7 instrument (Life Technologies, Paisley, UK), for 40 cycles with an annealing temperature of 64°C. Melt curve analysis was performed, showing a single clear peak for each assay. Relative quantification of the amplicons was calculated based on comparison with a known two-copy control fragment, using the 2-ΔΔCt method [34]. All assays were conducted in at least three replicates. Analysis of confirmed CNVs was extended to all available family members both by qPCR and genome-wide genotyping using the Illumina Human OmniExpress platform. The latter data were used to test for relatedness across family members and checking for Medelian errors, and to infer the parental chromosome on which the deletion originated.

Results and Discussion

CNV screening

Of the 86 individuals genotyped with the Omni-Express Illumina platform, 85 passed standard quality control criteria. QuantiSNP predicted individual CNVs of size ranging from 306 bp to 828 kb, while PennCNV predicted a size range of 306 bp to 1.86 Mb (S3 and S4 Tables). In order to ensure detection of all possible events, low detection thresholds were applied (confidence levels > 10 and spanning ≥ 3 SNPs). In total, 3169 consistent high-confidence CNVs were identified, of which 89.1% were duplications (S5 Table). A full list of all consistent high-confidence CNVs identified in probands and siblings has been submitted to the DGV archive (http://www.ebi.ac.uk/dgva; accession number estd220).

On average, 37.28 CNVs were detected per individual. Depending on the predictive algorithm used, 26–41% of CNVs were larger than 100 kb in size, and 4–12% larger than 200 kb (S4 Table). Individual CNV sizes mentioned in this study are consensus predictions of both algorithms. The majority of detected CNVs (88%, n = 2795) were located within at least one known gene, and 36.1% of those (n = 1010) were rare or novel CNVs.

Because of the relatively small size of our cohort we focused our analysis on individual large events rather than comprehensive CNV assessment, such as burden analysis. Three CNVs were selected for follow-up and validation by qPCR because of their occurrence at loci previously reported to be associated with neurodevelopmental phenotypes in the literature. These included a deletion at chromosome 15q13.1–13.3, which spanned 3.08 Mb, consisting of two adjacent deletions of 1.86 Mb and 868.7 kb separated by a repetitive region. Other events were selected for investigation but did not validate. These included a duplication (789.2 kb) at chromosome 16q13.11, and two overlapping duplications spanning exon 1 of the Semaphorin 3A (SEMA3A) gene predicted in two unrelated probands (64.6 kb and 45.7 kb).

The chromosome 15q deletion was the largest event to be identified in this cohort. The next largest event (> 1.5 Mb; S4 Table) was a duplication at chromosome 14, a common CNV, previously reported in DGV.

Chromosome 15q13.1–13.3 deletion

We detected a BP3-BP5 deletion at chromosome 15q13.1–13.3 predicted to be 3.08 Mb in size (Fig 1A), in a child with language impairment (proband 62). The deletion was validated by qPCR (Fig 1B). Analysis in the unaffected family members (mother, father and sibling) by both qPCR and genome-wide genotyping clearly indicated that this event has a de novo origin. Genome-wide genotyped data confirmed the relatedness of the family members and showed that the deletion originated on the paternal chromosome. There were 94 mendelian errors out of 860 SNP analysed at the chromosome 15 locus all indicating lack of paternal alleles. In contrast, only 81 other errors were found across the rest of the genome and IBD (identical by descendent) sharing between parents and both siblings was 0.5 as expected.

Fig 1. Deletion at the chr15q13.1–13.3 locus in a language impaired proband.

Fig 1

(A) A snapshot from the UCSC Genome Browser (http://genome.ucsc.edu/; hg19) showing the genomic region encompassed by the chr15q13.1–13.3 deletion identified in a child with language impairment. The tracks at the bottom indicate the breakpoints previously mapped at this region (BP3; BP4 and BP5); the deletion predictions by QuantiSNP, PennCNV and by merging the two algorithms; and the location of the six amplicons used for validation. (B) qPCR results for the six amplicons in the proband’s family (colour coded bars indicate: father in green; mother in red; proband in blue and sibling in orange; the reference in grey). Approximately 50% reduction in copy number at amplicons 2–5 in the proband relative to reference and family members indicates a de novo deletion occurring at BP3-BP5.

The 15q11-13 locus is a highly unstable genomic region, which originates CNVs across six specific breakpoints (BP1-BP6) (S1 Fig). Deletions at the 15q11-13 locus have been implicated in several neurodevelopmental conditions. Prader-Willi (PWS) and Angelman (AS) syndromes most commonly occur as the result of deletions between BP2-BP3, although a more severe phenotype is observed for larger events across BP1-BP3 [24,25]. CNVs restricted to the BP1-BP2 region have been associated with reading and mathematical difficulties and general cognitive functioning [35]. Disruptions between BP4 and BP5 (15q13.2–13.3) have been reported in individuals with seizures and ID [36,37]. Larger BP3-BP5 deletions (15q13.1–13.3), similar to the CNV reported here, have only been observed in isolated cases presenting a variable phenotype (Table 1).

Table 1. Clinical characteristics of previously reported cases carrying a BP3-BP5 deletion.

Case Clinical characteristics Reference
1 Epilepsy, mild learning difficulties, lax thumb joint, squint, developmental delay [36]
2 Severe learning difficulties, autism, moderate language delay [38]
3 Epilepsy [39]
4 Hypotonia, dyspraxia, autism [40]
5 Epilepsy, learning difficulties, microcephaly, growth <3rd centile, reduced hemisphere volume, psychomotor retardation, optic nerve atrophy? [41]
6 Low birth weight, natal teeth, heart murmur, microcephaly, delayed speech and walking, poor attention [42]
7 Epilepsy, Mild learning difficulties [43]
8 Moderate intellectual disability, obesity, ADHD, hypotonia, delayed speech, strabismus [26]

Proband 62 was recruited to the study because of clinical concerns regarding speech and language development. Both the parents and sibling score above average on cognitive tests, and there is no family history of dyslexia or language impairment. The proband spoke the first words at 21 months (> 1 SD of the cohort mean age at first word) and presented at 3 years and 7 months with difficulty producing speech-sounds coupled with problems of expressive and receptive language development. The first steps of proband 62 occurred at 16 months (1 SD above the cohort mean age of first walking), and performance was poor on the Movement-ABC test for fine motor skills. A family history of motor problems was reported. Proband 62 was born prematurely after an induced birth, weighing just below 2.5 kg, but otherwise has no record of other medical problems that could explain these language difficulties.

Proband 62 was assessed five times at approximately annual intervals from age 3 years and 7 months to 7 years and 8 months (see S6 Table for scores on measures of non-verbal ability, language and literacy measures). There was some variation in levels of performance over time, as might be expected given that language and cognitive skills are less stable across this period of development than later in childhood. At the first time point (T1), the proband fulfilled criteria for language impairment and speech sound disorder. Generally the pattern was persistent and at 6 years some immature speech processes (e.g. fronting) were still present. Non-verbal IQ was above 70 throughout, indicating no evidence of intellectual disability but was notably lower at age 7.8 years than earlier in development. While the poorer performance may be due to poor attention, such a decline is often observed in children with LI [44]. Proband 62 made a slow start in learning to read, and phonological awareness remains an area of weakness, but reading and spelling skills are within the normal range. In summary, in early primary school, language impairment was present in the absence of literacy difficulties.

Discussion

We conducted a CNV screen in 85 children recruited in a longitudinal cohort designed to study the development of children with impaired language and/or a family history of dyslexia [29,30]. Because our small sample is not adequate to conduct a comprehensive and generalised assessment of the role of CNVs in reading and language abilities at the burden level, we focused our analysis on single large events affecting loci previously implicated in neurodevelopmental phenotypes.

We identified a large de novo deletion at chromosome 15q13.1–13.3 in a child who at 3 ½ years presented with language impairment and comorbid speech-sound disorder. Symptoms of language impairment persisted through to the age of 7 years when speech difficulties were mostly resolved, but scores on non-verbal tasks had weakened. Literacy development was normal in all respects at the latest assessment. The deletion was confirmed to be between BP3 and BP5, spanning more than 3 Mb and at least 15 genes (Fig 1A and 1B).

To date, only a limited number of cases carrying a BP3-BP5 deletion have been reported, presenting a variety of symptoms including seizures and general neurodevelopmental impairment (Table 1). In addition to language problems, only weak executive motor skills were observed in the child described in the current study. The observation of a family history of motor control problems suggest this trait might not be the direct consequence of the deletion which has a de novo origin. Seizures and overt physical disability were exclusionary criteria for this study, and no medical conditions were reported since recruitment. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report of a BP3-BP5 deletion in an individual with language impairment, normal reading skills and no evidence of sensory and neurological problems. Language difficulties are often considered as secondary effects of a broader condition, but our results suggest that haploinsufficiency of genes in the BP3-BP5 region can influence language ability directly, reinforcing the view that recurrent CNVs relevant for neurodevelopmental phenotypes tend to be incompletely penetrant and associated with a variable phenotype [45]. The BP3-BP5 deletion has not yet been reported in healthy controls, despite screenings in large cohorts. For example, Lowther et al. reported that none of the 23,838 adult controls screened for 15q13.3 deletion before 2014 presented a BP3-BP5 deletion[26]. The rarity of this event is further confirmed by the detection of only four carriers of BP3-BP5 deletions across 34, 046 individuals referred for clinical testing because of symptoms of mental retardation, developmental delay, or multiple congenital anomalies [42] (Table 1). Our results, however, can also be interpreted in the context of very limited CNV screenings in cohorts selected specifically for language impairments. Individuals with seizures or ID are more systematically assessed for CNV and, consequently, rare deletions such as at BP3-BP5 have a higher probability of being detected in these groups. A recent CNV screening in children with LI did not identify any events comparably large at the chromosome 15 locus, but did show a slightly higher frequency of rearrangement at this region compared to controls (15.7% vs. 13.8%); however, this difference was entirely driven by duplications (7.1% vs. 5.2%) [28]. By screening larger groups with LI it will be possible to assess the relevance of deletions at this locus, as well as other large chromosomal rearrangement, in contributing to language difficulties. This is consistent with our previous report showing a relatively higher frequency of sex chromosome aneuploidies in children with language impairment [46].

It is clear that many genetic factors still need to be identified to understand the genetic architecture underlying the aetiology of language difficulties, but we expect to see the same scenario that is emerging for most common and complex disorders, demonstrating the contributions of both rare and common variants [11]. In particular, resequencing technology is likely to identify many rare variants private to very few individuals that might explain language difficulties under a monogenic model.

The absence of the deletion in controls suggests that this event is sufficient to lead to a range of neurodevelopmental phenotypes, the severity of which might depend on additional factors. The rarity of the BP3-BP5 deletion, the broad spectrum of its clinical manifestations, and the large number of genes involved make it extremely difficult to hypothesise what molecular mechanisms might lead to language problems specifically. Genetic background and environmental factors are likely to account for much of the phenotypic variation observed between individuals carrying the same deletion. Parental origin could be another factor contributing to heterogeneity. Our analysis indicates that the deletion originated on the paternal chromosome. It would be interesting to systematically assess parental effects of previously reported BP3-BP5 deletions, but that is prevented by lack of data. It has been suggested that deletion of the CHRNA7 gene alone, which was predicted to be deleted in the proband described in this study, might mediate most of the phenotypes associated to the BP4-BP5 deletions [4749].

The medical history of proband 62 did not suggest any causal risk factors for language problems, except perhaps induced pre-term birth with low birth weight. It has been observed that near-term children, born at 33–37 weeks, have poorer educational outcomes and a higher incidence of neurological, behavioural and developmental disturbances than full-term children [50,51]. However, it is difficult to establish a causal link and it remains possible that an underlying genetic condition may increase the risk of developing obstetric complications and requiring induced birth.

From a technical point of view, we have shown that two of the three CNVs predicted by two algorithms were not validated by qPCR. Both CNVs were predicted to be duplications, so it is possible that complex chromosomal rearrangements have affected either the original prediction or the replication experiments. We accept that our results can still be considered inconclusive but, by reporting them, we highlight the necessity of validating predicted CNVs with alternative methods.

To summarise, we have identified a de novo deletion at chromosome 15q13.1–13.3 in a child with language impairment. This is the first report showing that a deletion between BP3-BP5 breakpoints affects language abilities directly rather than leading to a more general and profound phenotype. While there is some evidence that non-verbal abilities in proband 62 had fallen to just below the average range at 7 years, such a decline is not uncommon among children with SLI. In addition to extend the characterisation of the phenotypic manifestations of the chromosome 15q BP3-BP5 deletion, our results implicate for the first time the chromosome 15q13.1–13.3 locus in the aetiology of language difficulties, further expanding our understanding of the genetic risk factors underlying this condition.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. UCSC Genome Browser snapshot (http://genome.ucsc.edu/; hg19) showing the chromosome 15q11-13 CNV region defined by breakpoints (BP) 1–6.

(TIF)

S1 File. Cohort Description and Assessment.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Confirmatory factor analysis.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Quantitative PCR primers.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Predicted CNV sizes.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Size distribution of predicted CNVs.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. CNVs frequency.

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Raw scores for performance of Proband 62.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors have no competing interests to declare. The authors wish to thank Mary Wilson for technical assistance in DNA extraction. We also thank the Wellcome Language for Reading team who conducted the behavioural assessments.

Data Availability

All CNV data resulted from this study have been made available through the Database of Genomics Variants archive (DGVa; http://www.ebi.ac.uk/dgva, accession number estd220; direct link ftp://ftp.ebi.ac.uk/pub/databases/dgva/estd220_Pettigrew_et_al_2015/gvf/).

Funding Statement

SP is a Royal Society University Research Fellow and this work was supported by a Royal Society Research Grant [RG110387]. This work was supported by a Wellcome Trust Programme Grant to MJS [WT082032MA]. Genotyping at the Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics was supported by the Wellcome Trust [090532/Z/09/Z] and a Medical Research Council Hub Grant [G0900747 91070]. Support to the analysis was provided by the St Andrews Bioinformatics Unit funded by the Wellcome Trust [097831/Z/11/Z]. DFN is an MRC Career Development Fellow and supported by the MRC [G1000569/1 and MR/J003719/1]. AS was funded by a Wellcome Trust Vacation Scholarship.

References

  • 1. Law J, Boyle J, Harris F, Harkness A, Nye C (2000) Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: findings from a systematic review of the literature. Int J Lang Commun Disord 35: 165–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Habib M (2000) The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: an overview and working hypothesis. Brain 123: 2373–2399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Pennington BF, Bishop DV (2009) Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annu Rev Psychol 60: 283–306. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Snowling M, Bishop DVM, Stothard SE (2000) Is Preschool Language Impairment a Risk Factor for Dyslexia in Adolescence? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41: 587–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. McArthur GM, Hogben JH, Edwards VT, Heath SM, Mengler ED (2000) On the ‘‘Specifics” of Specific Reading Disability and Specific Language Impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41: 869–874. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Pennington BF (2006) From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 101: 385–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Beitchman JH, Nair R, Clegg M, Ferguson B, Patel P (1986) Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in children with speech and language disorders. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 25: 528–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Conti-Ramsden G, Simkin Z, Botting N (2006) The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47: 621–628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Kjelgaard MM, Tager-Flusberg H (2001) An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Lang Cogn Process 16: 287–308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Stromswold K (1998) Genetics of spoken language disorders. Hum Biol 70: 297–324. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Newbury DF, Monaco AP, Paracchini S (2014) Reading and language disorders: the importance of both quantity and quality. Genes (Basel) 5: 285–309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. St Pourcain B, Cents RA, Whitehouse AJ, Haworth CM, Davis OS, O'Reilly PF, et al. (2014) Common variation near ROBO2 is associated with expressive vocabulary in infancy. Nat Commun 5: 4831 10.1038/ncomms5831 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Nudel R, Simpson NH, Baird G, O'Hare A, Conti-Ramsden G, Bolton PF, et al. (2014) Genome-wide association analyses of child genotype effects and parent-of-origin effects in specific language impairment. Genes Brain Behav 13: 418–429. 10.1111/gbb.12127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Ercan-Sencicek AG, Davis Wright NR, Sanders SJ, Oakman N, Valdes L, Bakkaloglu B, et al. (2012) A balanced t(10;15) translocation in a male patient with developmental language disorder. Eur J Med Genet 55: 128–131. 10.1016/j.ejmg.2011.12.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Malhotra D, Sebat J (2012) CNVs: harbingers of a rare variant revolution in psychiatric genetics. Cell 148: 1223–1241. 10.1016/j.cell.2012.02.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Coe BP, Witherspoon K, Rosenfeld JA, van Bon BW, Vulto-van Silfhout AT, Bosco P, et al. (2014) Refining analyses of copy number variation identifies specific genes associated with developmental delay. Nat Genet 46: 1063–1071. 10.1038/ng.3092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Cooper GM, Coe BP, Girirajan S, Rosenfeld JA, Vu TH, Baker C, et al. (2011) A copy number variation morbidity map of developmental delay. Nat Genet 43: 838–846. 10.1038/ng.909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Girirajan S, Brkanac Z, Coe BP, Baker C, Vives L, Vu TH, et al. (2011) Relative burden of large CNVs on a range of neurodevelopmental phenotypes. PLoS Genet 7: e1002334 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Pinto D, Pagnamenta AT, Klei L, Anney R, Merico D, Regan R, et al. (2010) Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature 466: 368–372. 10.1038/nature09146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Koolen DA, Reardon W, Rosser EM, Lacombe D, Hurst JA, Law CJ, et al. (2005) Molecular characterisation of patients with subtelomeric 22q abnormalities using chromosome specific array-based comparative genomic hybridisation. Eur J Hum Genet 13: 1019–1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Elsea SH, Girirajan S (2008) Smith-Magenis syndrome. Eur J Hum Genet 16: 412–421. 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5202009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Potocki L, Chen KS, Park SS, Osterholm DE, Withers MA, Kimonis V, et al. (2000) Molecular mechanism for duplication 17p11.2- the homologous recombination reciprocal of the Smith-Magenis microdeletion. Nat Genet 24: 84–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Mignon-Ravix C, Depetris D, Luciani JJ, Cuoco C, Krajewska-Walasek M, Missirian C, et al. (2007) Recurrent rearrangements in the proximal 15q11-q14 region: a new breakpoint cluster specific to unbalanced translocations. Eur J Hum Genet 15: 432–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Varela MC, Kok F, Setian N, Kim CA, Koiffmann CP (2005) Impact of molecular mechanisms, including deletion size, on Prader-Willi syndrome phenotype: study of 75 patients. Clin Genet 67: 47–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Varela MC, Kok F, Otto PA, Koiffmann CP (2004) Phenotypic variability in Angelman syndrome: comparison among different deletion classes and between deletion and UPD subjects. Eur J Hum Genet 12: 987–992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Lowther C, Costain G, Stavropoulos DJ, Melvin R, Silversides CK, Andrade DM, et al. (2015) Delineating the 15q13.3 microdeletion phenotype: a case series and comprehensive review of the literature. Genet Med 17: 149–157. 10.1038/gim.2014.83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Ceroni F, Simpson NH, Francks C, Baird G, Conti-Ramsden G, Clark A, et al. (2014) Homozygous microdeletion of exon 5 in ZNF277 in a girl with specific language impairment. Eur J Hum Genet 22: 1165–1171. 10.1038/ejhg.2014.4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Simpson NH, Ceroni F, Reader RH, Covill LE, Knight JC, SLI-Consortium, et al. (2015) Genome-wide analysis identifies a role for common copy number variants in specific language impairment. Eur J Hum Genet [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Moll K, Loff A, Snowling MJ (2013) Cognitive Endophenotypes of Dyslexia. Sci Stud Read 17: 385–397. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Nash HM, Hulme C, Gooch D, Snowling MJ (2013) Preschool language profiles of children at family risk of dyslexia: continuities with specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54: 958–968. 10.1111/jcpp.12091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Muthén LK, Muthén BO (2010) Mplus User’s Guide (1998–2010). Los Angeles: Muthen & Muthen. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Wang K, Li MY, Hadley D, Liu R, Glessner J, Grant SFA, et al. (2007) PennCNV: An integrated hidden Markov model designed for high-resolution copy number variation detection in whole-genome SNP genotyping data. Genome Res 17: 1665–1674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Colella S, Yau C, Taylor JM, Mirza G, Butler H, Clouston P, et al. (2007) QuantiSNP: an Objective Bayes Hidden-Markov Model to detect and accurately map copy number variation using SNP genotyping data. Nucleic Acids Res 35: 2013–2025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Schmittgen TD, Livak KJ (2008) Analyzing real-time PCR data by the comparative CT method. Nat Protocols 3: 1101–1108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Stefansson H, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Steinberg S, Magnusdottir B, Morgen K, Arnarsdottir S, et al. (2014) CNVs conferring risk of autism or schizophrenia affect cognition in controls. Nature 505: 361–366. 10.1038/nature12818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Sharp AJ, Mefford HC, Li K, Baker C, Skinner C, Stevenson RE, et al. (2008) A recurrent 15q13.3 microdeletion syndrome associated with mental retardation and seizures. Nat Genet 40: 322–328. 10.1038/ng.93 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Shinawi M, Schaaf CP, Bhatt SS, Xia ZL, Patel A, Cheung SW, et al. (2009) A small recurrent deletion within 15q13.3 is associated with a range of neurodevelopmental phenotypes. Nat Genet 41: 1269–1271. 10.1038/ng.481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Ben-Shachar S, Lanpher B, German JR, Qasaymeh M, Potocki L, Nagamani SCS, et al. (2009) Microdeletion 15q13.3: a locus with incomplete penetrance for autism, mental retardation, and psychiatric disorders. J Med Genet 46: 382–388. 10.1136/jmg.2008.064378 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Helbig I, Mefford HC, Sharp AJ, Guipponi M, Fichera M, Franke A, et al. (2009) 15q13.3 microdeletions increase risk of idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Nat Genet 41: 160–162. 10.1038/ng.292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. van Bon BWM, Mefford HC, Menten B, Koolen DA, Sharp AJ, Nillesen WM, et al. (2009) Further delineation of the 15q13 microdeletion and duplication syndromes: a clinical spectrum varying from non-pathogenic to a severe outcome. J Med Genet 46: 511–523. 10.1136/jmg.2008.063412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Endris V, Hackmann K, Neuhann TM, Grasshoff U, Bonin M, Haug U, et al. (2010) Homozygous Loss of CHRNA7 on Chromosome 15q13.3 Causes Severe Encephalopathy With Seizures and Hypotonia. Am J Med Genet A 152A: 2908–2911. 10.1002/ajmg.a.33692 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Rosenfeld JA, Stephens LE, Coppinger J, Ballif BC, Hoo JJ, French BN, et al. (2011) Deletions flanked by breakpoints 3 and 4 on 15q13 may contribute to abnormal phenotypes. Eur J Hum Genet 19: 547–554. 10.1038/ejhg.2010.237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Jahn JA, von Spiczak S, Muhle H, Obermeier T, Franke A, Mefford HC, et al. (2014) Iterative phenotyping of 15q11.2, 15q13.3 and 16p13.11 microdeletion carriers in pediatric epilepsies. Epilepsy Res 108: 109–116. 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2013.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Botting N (2005) Non-verbal cognitive development and language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46: 317–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. O'Donovan MC, Kirov G, Owen MJ (2008) Phenotypic variations on the theme of CNVs. Nat Genet 40: 1392–1393. 10.1038/ng1208-1392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Simpson NH, Addis L, Brandler WM, Slonims V, Clark A, Watson J, et al. (2013) Increased prevalence of sex chromosome aneuploidies in specific language impairment and dyslexia. Dev Med Child Neurol. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Masurel-Paulet A, Andrieux J, Callier P, Cuisset JM, Le Caignec C, Holder M, et al. (2010) Delineation of 15q13.3 microdeletions. Clin Genet 78: 149–161. 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2010.01374.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Mikhail FM, Lose EJ, Robin NH, Descartes MD, Rutledge KD, Rutledge SL, et al. (2011) Clinically relevant single gene or intragenic deletions encompassing critical neurodevelopmental genes in patients with developmental delay, mental retardation, and/or autism spectrum disorders. Am J Med Genet A 155A: 2386–2396. 10.1002/ajmg.a.34177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Hoppman-Chaney N, Wain K, Seger PR, Superneau DW, Hodge JC (2013) Identification of single gene deletions at 15q13.3: further evidence that CHRNA7 causes the 15q13.3 microdeletion syndrome phenotype. Clin Genet 83: 345–351. 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2012.01925.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Nomura Y, Halperin JM, Newcorn JH, Davey C, Fifer WP, Savitz DA, et al. (2009) The Risk for Impaired Learning-related Abilities in Childhood and Educational Attainment Among Adults Born Near-term. J Pediatr Psychol 34: 406–418. 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Teune MJ, Bakhuizen S, Bannerman CG, Opmeer BC, van Kaam AH, van Wassenaer AG, et al. (2011) A systematic review of severe morbidity in infants born late preterm. Am J Obstet Gynecol 205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Fig. UCSC Genome Browser snapshot (http://genome.ucsc.edu/; hg19) showing the chromosome 15q11-13 CNV region defined by breakpoints (BP) 1–6.

(TIF)

S1 File. Cohort Description and Assessment.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Confirmatory factor analysis.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Quantitative PCR primers.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Predicted CNV sizes.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Size distribution of predicted CNVs.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. CNVs frequency.

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Raw scores for performance of Proband 62.

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

All CNV data resulted from this study have been made available through the Database of Genomics Variants archive (DGVa; http://www.ebi.ac.uk/dgva, accession number estd220; direct link ftp://ftp.ebi.ac.uk/pub/databases/dgva/estd220_Pettigrew_et_al_2015/gvf/).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES