Abstract
Formation of episodic memories (i.e. remembered experiences) requires a process called consolidation which involves communication between the neocortex and hippocampus. However, the neuromodulatory mechanisms underlying this neocortico-hippocampal communication are poorly understood. Here, we examined the involvement of dopamine D1 receptors (D1R) and D2 receptors (D2R) mediated signaling on memory consolidation using the Novel Object Recognition (NOR) test. We conducted the tests in male Hartley guinea pigs and cognitive behaviors were assessed in customized Phenotyper home cages utilizing Ethovision XT software from Noldus enabled for the 3-point detection system (nose, center of the body, and rear). We found that acute intraperitoneal injections of either 0.25 mg/kg SCH23390 to block D1Rs or 1.0 mg/kg sulpiride to block D2Rs soon after acquisition (which involved familiarization to two similar objects) attenuated subsequent discrimination for novel objects when tested after 5-hours in the NOR test. By contrast guinea pigs treated with saline showed robust discrimination for novel objects indicating normal operational processes undergirding memory consolidation. The data suggests that involvement of dopaminergic signaling is a key post-acquisition factor in modulating memory consolidation in guinea pigs.
Introduction
In humans memory is defined as the ability to acquire, store, and retrieve information. Several different types of long-term memory (LTM) have been distinguished, including declarative and non-declarative memory. Declarative memory is further subdivided into episodic memory (i.e. experiences and events) and semantic memory (i.e. facts, meanings, and concepts) which both require conscious recall. Thus episodic memory is the term given to the capacity to recall or ‘remember’ experienced events and situations [1]. Once thought to be unique to humans, it is now clear that the core behavioral properties of episodic memory are present across mammals as well as other animal species [2]; the major brain regions responsible for episodic memory in humans have anatomical and functional homologs in other species [2, 3]. Significant progress has been made in our understanding of the operational features of the neural circuits underlying memory but there is still much that is unknown about the establishment of LTM in the brain. For example, encoding of newly acquired information into engrams (i.e. memory traces) is fast and may occur on a single trial. However, memories are initially labile and later become resistant to loss but the processes that make short-term memories (STM) take on a permanent form are not well understood.
The formation of episodic memory critically depends on the integrity of the hippocampus but also involves a large network of cortical areas that includes the adjacent parahippocampal region and the prefrontal cortex [2, 4–5]. It is theorized and supported by experimental data that ‘consolidation’ is a process by which initially labile memories become permanent and impervious to disruption [6, 7]. Memory consolidation is commonly addressed at two complementary levels of description and analysis namely the cellular/synaptic level (synaptic consolidation) and the brain systems level (systems consolidation) [6, 8]. Consolidation is separated into a molecular-cellular process of ‘fixation’ of a memory trace that occurs for several minutes after learning [9–11], and a time-dependent ‘reorganization’ of neural networks resulting in episodic memory storage [7, 12–18]. Thus, current research is being directed towards the discovery of specific patterns of neuromodulatory activity underpinning system memory consolidation [19]. Here, we used guinea pigs as an animal model to verify or refute the neuromodulatory role of dopaminergic signaling on memory consolidation.
We sought to check the involvement of dopaminergic signaling in memory consolidation for several reasons. Accumulating evidence implicates endogenous dopamine from dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) as a key regulator of synaptic changes observed at certain stages of learning and memory and of synaptic plasticity in CA1 area of the hippocampus [20, 21]. Specifically, the VTA and hippocampus are theorized to form a functional loop designed to detect novelty. This novelty signal would then serve as a gate to convert behaviorally relevant STM into LTM [21]. Consistent with this postulate are findings showing that novel stimuli trigger burst firing of VTA cells [22–24] which send projections to the hippocampus [24, 25]. This dopaminergic novelty signal from the VTA is presumably detected by D1/D5 receptors that are expressed in hippocampal pyramidal cells [26–28]. In the CA1 region, D1/D5 receptors have previously been reported to modify electrically induced CA3/CA1 long-term potentiation (LTP: a cellular correlate of mnesic process [29]) and LTM formation when drug antagonists were administered prior to learning [30–34]. However, studies have yet to distinguish whether the impact of dopaminergic signaling is limited to the learning (acquisition) phase or if it also affects processes after learning, i.e. consolidation. We present in this report data showing that blocking dopaminergic receptors after learning impairs novel object recognition (NOR) memory in guinea pigs.
Materials and Methods
Animals and Drug Administration
Forty-two male Hartley guinea pigs (outbred; weight 200–250 g) obtained from Charles River Laboratories were housed in pairs without environmental enrichment (i.e. no toys or objects); they had free access to food and water. Guinea pigs were kept on a 12-hour light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 AM) in the American Association for Laboratory Accreditation Council certified Animal Care Facility (ACF) at Meharry Medical College. The ACF maintains animal rooms at 65–70°F and 40–70% relative humidity. After adjustment to the ACF for 7 days, guinea pigs housed in pairs were later assigned to 3 experimental groups (n = 14 per group) for treatment with the following: (a) phosphate buffered saline (PBS: hereafter denoted as saline), the control group, (b) SCH23390, the dopamine D1 receptor (D1R) antagonist [35, 36], or (c) sulpiride, the dopamine D2 receptor (D2R) antagonist [36]. To make drug solutions, 10 mg SCH23390 or 20 mg sulpiride were first dissolved in 0.5 mL dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO) and then 0.5 mL saline was added to make final solution volumes of 1.0 mL respectively. These stock solutions were further diluted with saline to provide drug concentrations of either 0.25 mg/kg/mL SCH23390 or 1.0 mg/kg/mL sulpiride that were the doses administered to guinea pigs, respectively. Thus, the dilution factor for DMSO in the final 1.0 mL injections given to animals ranged between 1:20 and 1:40 which corresponds to 0.01225 μM–0.025 μM DMSO (molecular weight of DMSO = 78.13). The DMSO concentrations we used are below those that have been utilized by other investigators but did not affect novel object recognition (NOR) performance in control animals. For example, Rossato and co-workers tested local DMSO concentrations that were ~12.8 mM (i.e. 0.1% DMSO in saline; molecular weight of DMSO = 78.3) and this apparently did not modify NOR performances in their controls animals [37]. NOR tests the animals’ natural instinct to explore new objects more than familiar objects, and can, therefore, test its recognition memory [38]. In our hands, it is unlikely the systemic DMSO concentrations exposed to guinea pigs ever attain DMSO concentrations as cited above in view of the further dilution caused by the large blood volume of guinea pigs, i.e. 20 mL for a guinea pig weighing approximately 300 g [39]. Sulpiride, DMSO and phosphate buffered tablets were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO), whereas SCH23390 was acquired from Tocris (Ellisville, MO).
Guinea Pig Activity Assessments
When performing behavior experiments, it is important for the testing times to match the animal’s arousal state and this was done in the present study. Behavior experiments were conducted in the Phenotyper multi-purpose observation cage from Noldus that is constructed of four transparent polymethylmethacrilate (PMMA) walls and a PMMA dual satin opaque cage bottom (cage dimensions in cm: 45 long x 45 wide, 65 high). The cage bottom was covered with Cell-Sorb Plus bedding (Fig 1) made from recycled paper and is odor free (Mednet Direct Farmington, CT). The Phenotyper cage had a top unit that contained an infrared (IR) sensitive camera and operated with EthoVision XT 7.0 video-tape and data acquisition and analysis software. The EthoVision XT 7.0 programs were used to electronically demarcate user defined boundaries of the open arena and the objects within the arena (Fig 1B and 1C). This video-tracking software automatically tracked guinea pigs activities and this data was used to quantify time spent exploring objects and distance moved while undertaking the NOR tests as described below.
Novel Object Recognition Test
After adjustment in the ACF for 7 days, guinea pigs underwent habituation for 5 days in the empty Phenotyper cage (Fig 1A). Hence, guinea pigs explored an open arena with floor bedding in the Phenotyper cages for 3 min, as described previously by Clark and Squire [40] with some modifications. Subsequently, on Day-6, guinea pigs were brought back to the experiment room between 8:00 AM–9:00 AM and each guinea pig was placed in the empty Phenotyper cage with floor bedding for 1 min to re-habituate to the cage. Guinea pigs were removed and two of the same objects (denoted as FAM1 and FAM2) were placed in the center regions of the cages, about 4–7 cm away from the cage side walls and 2–3 cm apart (Fig 1B). Following a ~2 min interval, guinea pigs were brought back to the Phenotyper cages and allowed to spontaneously explore two identical objects for 3 min. This was the familiarization phase (denoted as FAM) and represented the learning phase for the two identical objects. Immediately after the FAM phase, guinea pigs were randomly assigned to different treatment groups and given acute intraperitoneal injections of one of the following: a) 1 ml phosphate buffered saline, denoted as ‘saline’ from hereon, n = 14; b) 0.25 mg/kg SCH23390, n = 14; or c) 1 mg/kg sulpiride, n = 14. It should be noted that the basic method of administering dopaminergic antagonists immediately after training to study consolidation of object recognition memory has been used by other investigators [37]. In our study, guinea pigs were processed in batches of 6–10 animals. In each case, a pair of animals was randomly assigned to the saline group whereas the remainder were assigned to SCH23390 and sulpiride groups. To avoid possible time-dependent effects when drugs were administered, we ensured that all treatments were conducted at the same time-points during the day. After the injections, guinea pigs were returned to their home cages (with free access to food and water) and taken back to the ACF for an interval of 5 hours before novel object recognition tests were done. During the 5-hour interval, one familiar object (FAM1 or FAM2; Fig 1B) was randomly exchanged with a novel object (denoted as NOV; Fig 1C). After the 5-hour interval, guinea pigs returned to the same Phenotyper cages for the test phase, to explore the two objects (i.e. familiar and novel) for 3 min. Guinea pig activities during the five days of habituation, familiarization, and novel object recognition testing were video-tracked using the EthoVision XT 7.0 software. The experimenter stayed in the behavior lab during these recordings but was out of sight to the guinea pigs. Between experiments the Phenotyper cages (including objects) were cleaned with soap and hot water and disinfected with 70% isopropyl alcohol and the floor bedding was replaced in between animals to prevent cross contamination.
We quantified total distances moved by each guinea pig during the first three minutes of both the familiarization and testing phases to detect any differences in locomotor activity among the treatment groups. Then, we determined time spent exploring each object (i.e. FAM1, FAM2 and NOV) for the first 3 minutes during the different phases as applicable. Last, we calculated the discrimination index (DI) particularly between NOV and FAM as follows:
Euthanasia
At the end of experiments, guinea pigs were euthanized with an overdose of pentobarbital (150 mg/kg; source IP: Henry Schein).
Ethics Statement
All guinea pigs were utilized at Meharry Medical College in accordance with American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA) Guidelines for the Euthanasia of Animals and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol Number: 060619JGT10201).
Statistical Analysis
The data was analyzed using GraphPad Prism 5 for Windows, GraphPad Software, San Diego, California, USA, www.graphpad.com. One-way ANOVA with Tukeys post-hoc test or two-way ANOVA (Model I) with Bonferroni’s post-hoc test were used to compare variables of interest. Comparisons between two groups were done with paired t-test. Data fitness for Gaussian distribution was verified with D’Agostino and Pearson Omnibus normality test [41]. We evaluated 42 guinea pigs for the NOR tests, and at α = 0.05, this had a statistical power equal to 80% sufficient to detect a 7.52 sec difference in object preference with a standard deviation of ± 2.63 sec.
Results
Guinea Pigs Exhibit Thigmotaxic Behavior
The current study investigated the involvement of dopaminergic signaling on memory consolidation after learning using the NOR test in the guinea pig animal model. A recent study from the laboratory showed that guinea pigs remained active throughout daytime and nighttime but showed peak activities at dusk–i.e. lights off set at 7:00 PM [42]. Consequently, the behavioral experiments in the current study were started in the morning (8:00 AM–9:00 AM) each day such that the test phase for NOR occurred in the afternoon between 1:00 PM–3:00 PM respectively. The results showed that the average distance travelled by guinea pigs was 762.8 ± 38.8 cm during the habituation phase (values are mean ± SEM in this and subsequent entries). Data fitness for Gaussian distribution was confirmed using the D’Agostino and Pearson omnibus normality test (K2 = 1.362, p = 0.5061, N = 42 guinea pigs). Guinea pigs typically stayed close to the walls (i.e. displayed thigmotaxis behaviors) rather than the center regions in the Phenotyper cages. Therefore, objects were placed in the central sectors of the cages, which were less frequently visited, to best capture 'purposeful' exploration of objects in the NOR experiments (Fig 2).
Blocking Dopamine D1Rs or D2Rs Signaling with Antagonists Impairs Recognition Memory Formation
After concluding the 5-day habituation phase, experiments started with guinea pigs being re-habituated in the Phenotyper cages for 1 minute. Animals were then removed to allow placements of two identical objects in the cages (i.e. FAM1 and FAM2, Fig 1B) and returned back into the cage and video-tracked for 3 minutes. Guinea pigs explored the two identical objects equally well during the familiarization phase and this was characterized by animals touching and sniffing the objects (Fig 2). Plots of time spent exploring the left (FAM1) and the right (FAM2) identical objects, assessed in 30-sec time intervals, are shown in Fig 3A. The two-factor analysis of variance showed no significant differences in time spent with either object, F(1, 41) = 0.7819, p = 0.3817; no significant differences for the interaction between time interval and time spent with either object, F(5, 205) = 0.2359, p = 0.9463; but the time interval factor was significant, F(5, 205) = 2.770, p = 0.0191. Furthermore, guinea pigs spent the highest time exploring the two objects within the initial 30 seconds time interval but this tapered down with progression of time in the cage (Fig 3A). Altogether, the results indicated guinea pigs spent the same amount of total time exploring FAM1 (24.37 ± 2.29 sec) as they did exploring FAM2 (21.89 ± 2.58 sec), (paired t(41) = 0.8845, p = 0.3816; Fig 3B).
Immediately after the familiarization phase was over, guinea pigs were randomly assigned to separate treatment groups and administered one of the following intraperitoneal injections: a) 1 ml saline, n = 14; b) 0.25 mg/kg SCH23390, n = 14, or c) 1 mg/kg sulpiride, n = 14. After a 5-hour interval each guinea pig was brought back to the Phenotyper cage and underwent testing for novel object recognition memory. The results obtained from the NOR tests for guinea pigs in the three treatment groups are shown in Fig 4. It was important that we evaluated only those guinea pigs that had exhibited similar preferences for FAM1 and FAM2 during the familiarization phase. Consequently, z-scores were computed for time spent with FAM1 and FAM2 and used to detect ‘outliers’ which were guinea pigs with z-scores that exceeded three standard deviations from the mean. Thus, 35 out of the 42 guinea pigs met the criterion for inclusion in final data analyses (10 saline, 13 SCH23390, and 12 sulpiride) and these were evaluated for recognition memory at the 5-hour test interval.
Starting with the saline group, the two-factor analysis of variance showed significant differences in the following factors: a) time spent with the novel object versus the familiar object, F(1, 9) = 8.695, p = 0.0022; b) time interval, F(5, 45) = 9.711, p < 0.0001; and c) the interaction between time interval and time spent with objects, F(5, 45) = 2.707, p = 0.032. As was the case during the familiarization phase, guinea pigs spent a greater proportion of time exploring objects within the initial 30 seconds time interval but this tapered down over the 3-minute test window in the saline group (Fig 4A). By contrast, guinea pigs in the SCH23390 group had no significant preference for the novel object over the familiar object, F(1, 12) = 1.045, p = 0.3268; no significant effect for the time interval factor, F(5, 60) = 1.143, p = 0.3481; and no significance effect for the interaction between object and time interval, F (5, 60) = 0.3604, p = 0.8735. In fact, guinea pigs in this group did not exhibit the characteristic feature of mostly exploring objects in the initial 30 second time interval as seen with the saline group or during the familiarization phase (Fig 4B). Similar results to those seen with the SCH23390 group were obtained in the sulpiride group where the two-way ANOVA yielded an F ratio of F(1, 10) = 0.0007, p = 0.9790, indicating that the preference for the novel object was not significantly different than for the familiar object (Fig 4C). The main effect of time interval in this group was not significant, F(5, 50) = 1.029; the interaction effect of object and time interval was also not significant, F(5, 50) = 1.045, p = 0.4021. This meant guinea pigs in the sulpiride group explored the two objects (novel and familiar) with similar frequencies throughout the 3-minute testing interval (Fig 4C). Overall, only guinea pigs in the saline group spent significantly more time with the novel object than the familiar object (paired t(9) = 4.247, p = 0.0022; data is in Fig 4D), whereas there were no significant differences in time spent between the novel and familiar objects in the SCH23390 group (paired t(12) = 1.022, p = 0.3269; data in Fig 4E) or the sulpiride group (paired t(10) = 0.0286, p = 0.9778; data is in Fig 4F). Finally, the calculated discrimination index (DI) confirmed that only guinea pigs in the saline group (DI score: 0.4130 ± 0.0947), but not the SCH23390 group (DI score: -0.0657 ± 0.0615) or sulpiride group (DI score: 0.0475 ± 0.0896), had a significant DI confirming the development of object recognition memory in the saline group (F(2, 32) = 8.791, p = 0.0008; one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post hoc test; Fig 4G). Thus, we concluded blocking dopaminergic receptors soon after learning interfered with the development of object recognition memory.
Acute Administrations of Dopaminergic Receptor Antagonists Do Not Alter Locomotor Activity
One non-cognitive plausible explanation of the above mentioned results is that both SCH23390 and sulpiride could have affected locomotor activity which then curtailed movement during the NOR testing phase. To check this, an assessment of the distances moved during the familiarization (pre-drug) phase and the testing (post-drug) phase of the NOR assay was performed. The data from this comparison is shown in Fig 5. The dependent one way analysis of variance showed significant differences in total locomotor activities between the NOR phases, F(5. 64) = 4.715, p = 0.001. Post hoc analyses using the Tukey’s post hoc criterion for significance indicated that the average locomotor activity during the testing phase was significantly less in the saline group, but was not significantly different in both the SCH23390 group and sulpiride group. In fact the saline group quickly explored the novel and familiar objects in the initial 30 second interval and then decreased perhaps indicating a rapidly developing ‘habituation’ to the two objects. On the other hand, guinea pigs in both the SCH23390 group and the sulpiride group kept exploring the two objects (novel and familiar) equally well for nearly the entire 3 minutes.
Discussion
We have successfully utilized the guinea pig animal model to assess the involvement of D1R and D2R mediated signaling on object recognition memory consolidation. A key feature of these experiments was that the competitive antagonists used to block D1Rs or D2Rs signaling (i.e. SCH23390 and sulpiride respectively) were administered soon after (but not before) the learning phase. The primary finding is that blocking dopaminergic signaling soon after learning impairs recognition memory consolidation in the guinea pig animal model. We injected drugs via the intraperitoneal route partly to mimic the majority of therapeutic drugs that are administered systemically in humans and, therefore, has translational relevance. For instance, sulpiride administered orally in humans is known to be significantly more effective than haloperidol, risperidone and olanzapine in schizophrenic treatment [43]. However, only limited information is available on the potential risks associated with sulpiride treatment on higher cognitive functions [44]. Certainly, our method for drug administration did not permit us to localize the predominant site(s) in the brain where the antagonists exerted their effects to cause changes in memory consolidation. Nonetheless, our findings set the stage for designing studies that will examine dopaminergic antagonists applied in specific brain targets to better localize the integrative sites of drug actions. The above findings are discussed in detail below.
First, guinea pigs treated with the two dopaminergic antagonists clearly did not exhibit preferences for novel objects relative to familiar objects after a 5-hour test interval. By contrast, saline-treated animals showed significant preferences for novel objects within 30 seconds of entering the cage. This is in agreement with published work that has reported that animals tend to explore novel objects within 30 seconds of being exposed to the object [40]. Thus, we concluded that blocking dopaminergic signaling with drug antagonists soon after learning induced disruptions of processes undergirding memory consolidation. Second, we applied drug doses that we and others have found to block late-LTP [45], alter cAMP levels [46], and/or interfere with memory formation [28] when administered prior to learning. We predict that the drugs undergo rapid distribution after an intraperitoneal injection and reach pharmacologically relevant concentrations in the brain within minutes. We do not know the half-lives of SCH23390 or sulpiride in the guinea pig model but it is probable that their optimal effects span over the 30–60 min interval in view of the following. Our published data, taken together with reports in the literature, indicate that late-LTP (or even memory tests) are attenuated in animals injected with SCH23390 ~30 min prior to tetanic stimulation [45] or learning phase in NOR studies [28]. This suggests a relatively long duration of action after acute injections in vivo. However, the effects of the drug antagonists would be absent or minimal after the 5-hour interval from acute administration when NOR tests were conducted. In a separate study, we found that SCH23390 and sulpiride lacked effects on motor activities as early as 2 hours after acute drug administration [42]. Third, it was intriguing that two dopaminergic antagonists that primarily work on receptors with opposing effects on the regulation of cAMP, PKA, Ca++ levels [47], and ultimately the synthesis of proteins, could elicit the same behavioral phenotypes of memory impairment. However, the data is consistent with published reports that have indicated that infusion of sulpiride in the nucleus accumbens impaired memory consolidation [48]. Studies have reported that blocking D2Rs in the nucleus accumbens caused memory impairment during various task requiring recognition, attention, and retrieval [49–52]. By contrast, rats infused with SCH23390 into the lateral ventricles subsequently were found to be slow learners of novel information in hippocampal-dependent memory tasks [28]. Consequently, it is possible sulpiride and SCH23390 exerted their effects in different brain regions that still led to disruption of memory consolidation. Within this context, it was interesting to discover that object exploration times did not evolve with time in the SCH23390 animal group which probably reflected absence of habituation during the NOR test phase. By comparison, guinea pigs in the sulpiride group exhibited signs of habituation during exploration time but this was not different between the familiar versus the novel object. Taken together, we speculate that SCH23390 might have interfered with ‘working memory’ whereas sulpiride altered ‘short-term memory’ but these ideas require experimental validation. Fourth, we found guinea pigs to be robust small animal models with unique features that render them well suited for assessments of mnemonic functions and behaviors. They are neither nocturnal nor diurnal [42, 53] and, therefore, do not require reversal of light–dark cycles often necessary when using mice or rats. Their relatively consistent exploratory activity allows for unambiguous characterization of behaviors. For example, an increasing number of studies in the literature report that animal attentiveness is a prerequisite for the development of robust memory consolidation [54, 55]. This trait is prominent in guinea pigs making them useful animal models for measuring unambiguous cognitive behaviors. Taken together, our study provides support for the post-learning involvement of D1R and D2R signaling in memory consolidation in the guinea pig animal model and this is consistent with recent findings reported in other rodent species [37, 56–57].
In the broader context, our study has provided additional evidence for the involvement of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors signaling in the modulation of memory consolidation. Other neuromodulators that have been implicated in published reports include acetylcholine [58], GABA [59] and norepinephrine [60]. Taken together, this information contributes towards a better understanding of the neurochemical processes underlying memory consolidation leading to stable LTM. New memories are initially represented within the hippocampus and then become integrated into existing memories in the neocortex during consolidation [61] to form updated memory networks termed ‘schema’ [62–64]. Strong experimental evidence suggests that the prefrontal cortex ‘dictates’ schema development and updating [65]. Overall, the neural pathways for mnemonic information flow between the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex are reasonably well understood [64, 66, 67]. What is less known are the interactive nature of the neuromodulators and related transduction cascades underlying the various steps during memory consolidation. Indeed, much is already known about the involvement of locus ceruleus noradrenergic (LC-NA) system in orchestrating memory consolidation after initial learning [68, 69]. The LC-NA system has been shown to be critically involved in the late phase of memory consolidation and engagement of the prelimbic regions of the prefrontal cortex [70–72]. Thus, β-adrenergic receptors have been found to be necessary for reconsolidation after reactivation of well-established memories of varying emotional valences [73–75]. The question is what then might be the nature of interaction between the DA we have reported in our study and NA systems to support consolidation? Sara and co-workers proposed that the LC-NA outputs contribute towards the transition from the ‘down-state’ (i.e. hyperpolarization) to ‘up-state’ (i.e. depolarization) in cortical structures [76]. The facilitating effects of the LC-NA input on cortical excitability [77, 78], spike timing and synaptic plasticity in cortex [76, 79], and hippocampus [80] are well known. It is tempting to infer that the LC-NA spearheads the ‘dialogue’ between brain structures that produce LTM formation. We further speculate that a mechanism is required to link on-going cortical activity and protein synthesis that, in turn, underpins enduring system consolidation in LTM. We have previously presented data indicating that conditioning stimulation in guinea pig hippocampus led to D1R activation that correlated with both LTP maintenance and up-regulation of growth associated protein 43 (GAP-43) transcript and protein expression [45]. It is conceivable that the LC-NA system helps to prime the down-to-up cortical state and that dopaminergic system helps to sustain the cortical up-state as well as couple this activity to protein synthesis essential for LTM formation. This hypothesis should be further explored in future investigations.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health research grant R24 DA021471, and training grants R25 GM059994, T32 NS061201. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Tulving E, Szpunar KK (2009) Episodic memory. Scholarpedia, 4(8):3332. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Allen TA, Fortin NJ (2013) The evolution of episodic memory. PNAS 110; 10379–10386. 10.1073/pnas.1301199110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Dere E, Kart-Teke E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA (2006) The case for episodic memory in animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30: 1206–1224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Broadbent NJ, Gaskin S, Squire LR, Clark RE. (2010) Object recognition memory and the rodent hippocampus. Learn. Mem. 17: 5–11. 10.1101/lm.1650110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Buzsaki G, Moser EI (2013) Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat. Neurosci. 16: 130–138. 10.1038/nn.3304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Dudai Y (2012) The restless engram: consolidations never end. Annu Rev. Neurosci. 35: 227–247. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Eichenbaum H (2012) The cognitive neuroscience of memory: An introduction 2nd Ed. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Born J, Wilhelm I (2012) System consolidation of memory during sleep. Psychol. Res. 76: 192–203. 10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. McGaugh JL (1966) Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science 153: 1351–1358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. O’Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map Clarendon Press, Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Kentros C, Hargreaves E, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Shapiro M, Muller RV (1998) Abolition of long term stability of new hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade. Science 280: 2121–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Scoville WB, Milner B (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol. Neursurg. Psychiatry 20: 11–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Zola-Morgan SM, Squire LR (1990) The primate hippocampal formation: evidence for a time-limited role in memory storage. Science 250: 288–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Bontempi B, Laurent-Demir C, Destrade C, Jaffard R (1999) Time dependent reorganization of brain circuitry underlying long term memory storage. Nature 400: 671–675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Haist F, Bowden JG, Mao H (2001) Consolidation of human memory over decades revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Neurosci 4: 1139–1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Takehara K, Kawahara S, Kirino Y (2003) Time-dependent reorganization of the brain components underlying memory retention in trace eyeblink conditioning. J. Neurosci. 23: 9897–9905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Remondes M, Schuman EM (2004) Role for a cortical input to hippocampal area CA1 in the consolidation of a long-term memory. Nature 431: 699–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Takashima A, Petersson KM, Rutters F, Tendolkar I, Jensen O, Zwarts MJ, McNaughton BL, Fernandez G (2006) Declarative memory consolidation in humans: A prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study PNAS 103: 756–761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Sara SJ, Hars B (2014) In memory of consolidation. Learning Mem. 13: 515–521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Jenkins TA, Amin E, Pearce JM, Brown MW, Aggleton JP (2004) Novel spatial arrangements of familiar visual stimuli promote activity in the rat hippocampal formation but not the parahippocampal cortices: a c-fos expression study. Neurosci. 124: 43–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Lisman J, Grace A (2005) The hippocampal-VTA loop: controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron 46: 703–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Steinfels GF, Heym J, Strecker RE, Jacobs BL (1983) Response of dopaminergic neurons in cat to auditory stimuli presented across the sleep-waking cycle. Brain Res. 277: 150–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W (1992) Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. J. Neurophysiol. 67: 145–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Horvitz JC, Stewart T, Jacobs BL (1997) Burst activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons is elicited by sensory stimuli in the awake cat. Brain Res. 759: 251–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Gasbari A, Sulli A, Packard MG (1997) The dopaminergic mesencephalic projections to the hippocampal formation in the rat. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psych. 21: 1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Ciliax BJ, Nash N, Heilman C, Sunahara R, Hartney A, Tiberi M, Rye DB, Caron MG, Niznik HB, levey AI (2000) Dopamine D(5) receptor immunolocalization in rat and monkey brain. Synapse 37: 125–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Khan ZU, Gutierrez A, Martin R, Penafiel A, Rivera A, de la Calle A (2000) Dopamine D5 receptors of rat and human brain. Neurosci. 100: 689–699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Lemon N, Manahan-Vaughan D (2006) Dopamine D1/D5 receptors gate the acquisition of Novel Information through Hippocampal long term potentiation and long term depression. J. Neurosci. 26: 7723–7729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Bliss TVP, Collingridge GL (1993) A Synaptic model of memory: long term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361: 31–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Frey U, Matthies H, Reymann KG, Matthies H (1991) The effect of dopaminergic D1 receptor blockade during tetanization on the expression of long-term potentiation in the rat CA1 region in vitro. Neurosci. Lett. 129: 111–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Huang Y, Kandel ER (1995) D1/D5 receptor agonists induce a protein synthesis dependent late potentiation in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92: 2446–2450 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Chen Z, Ito K, Fujii S, Miura M, Furuse H, Sasaki H, Kaneko K, Kato H, Miyakawa H (1996) Roles of dopamine receptors in long-term depression: enhancement via D1 receptors and inhibition via D2 receptors. Receptors Channels 4: 1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Otmakhova NA, Lisman JE (1996) D1/D5 dopamine receptor activation increases the magnitude of early long-term potentiation at CA1 hippocampal synapses. J. Neurosci. 16: 7478–7486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Swanson-Park JL, Coussens CM, Mason-Parker SE, Raymond CR, Hargreaves EL, Dragunow M, Cohen AS, Abraham WC (1996) A double dissociation within the hippocampus of dopamine D1/D2 receptor and beta-adrenergic receptor contributions to the persistence of long-term potentiation. Neurosci. 92: 485–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Bourne JA (2001) SCH23390: the first selective dopamine D1-like receptor antagonist. CNS Drug Rev. 7: 399–414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Seeman P, Van Tol HH (1994) Dopamine receptor pharmacology. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 15: 264–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Rossato JI, Radiske A, Kohler CA, Gonzalez C, Bevilaqua LR, Medina JH, Martín Cammarota M (2013) Consolidation of object recognition memory requires simultaneous activation of dopamine D1/D5 receptors in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex but not in the hippocampus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 106: 66–70. 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Ennanceur A, Delacour J (1988) A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats I: behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 31: 47–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Ancill RJ (1956) The blood volume of the normal guinea pig. J. Physiol. 132: 469–475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Clark RE, Zola SM, Squire LR (2000) Impaired Recognition Memory in rats after damage to the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2: 8853–8860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. DʼAgostino R, Pearson ES (1973) Tests for departure from Normality: Empirical results for the distributions of b2 and √b1 . Biometrika. 60: 613–622. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Lee KN, Pellom ST, Oliver E, Chirwa S (2014) Characterization of the guinea pig animal model and subsequent comparison of the behavioral effects of selective dopaminergic drugs and methamphetamine. Synapse. 68: 221–233. 10.1002/syn.21731 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Lai EC, Chang CH, Kao Yang YH, Lin SJ, Lin CY (2012) Effectiveness of sulpiride in adult patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 39: 673–683. 10.1093/schbul/sbs002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Lai EC-C, Hsieh C-Y, Kao Yang Y-H, Lin S-J (2014) Detecting potential adverse reactions of sulpiride in schizophrenic patients by prescription sequence symmetry analysis. PLoS ONE 9(2): e89795 10.1371/journal.pone.0089795 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Chirwa S, Aduonum A, Pizarro J, Reasor J, Kawai Y, Gonzalez M, McAdory B, Onaivi E, and Barea-Rodriguez EJ (2005) Dopaminergic DA1 signaling couples growth-associated protein-43 and long term potentiation in guinea pig hippocampus. Brain Res. Bull., 64: 433–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Williams S, Mmbaga N, Chirwa S (2006) Dopaminergic D1 receptor agonist SKF 38393 induces GAP-43 expression and long term potentiation in hippocampus in vivo. Neurosci. Lett. 402: 46–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Robinson SW, Caron MG (1997) Interactions of dopamine receptors with G proteins In: Neve KA and Neve RL (editors). The dopamine receptors. Humana Press Inc. pp. 137–165. [Google Scholar]
- 48. Manago F, Castellano C, Oliverio A, Mele A, De Leonibus E (2008) Role of dopamine receptors subtypes, D1-like and D2-like, within the nucleus accumbens subregions, core and shell, on memory consolidation in the one trail inhibitory avoidance task. Learn Mem. 16:46–52. 10.1101/lm.1177509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Frank MJ, O’Reilly RC (2006) A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behav Neurosci 120:497–517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Mehta MA, McGowan SW, Lawrence AD, Aitken MR, Montgomery AJ, Grasby PM (2003) Systemic sulpiride modulates striatal blood flow: relationships to spatial working memory and planning. Neuroimage 20(4):1982–1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Mehta MA, Manes FF, Magnolfi G, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (2004) Impaired set-shifting and dissociable effects on tests of spatial working memory following the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 176:331–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Mehta MA, Montgomery AJ, Kitamura Y, Grasby PM (2008) Dopamine D2 receptor occupancy levels of acute sulpiride challenges that produce working memory and learning impairments in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 196: 157–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Tobler I, Franken P, Jaggi (1993) Vigilance states, EEG spectra, and cortical temperature in the guinea pig. Am J Physiol Regulatory Integrative Comp Physiol. 264: 1125–1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Muzzio IA, Levita L, Kulkarni J, Monaco J, Kentros C, Stead M, Abbott LF, Kandel ER (2009) Attention enhances the retrieval and stability of visuospatial and olfactory representations in the dorsal hippocampus. Plos Biol. 7:e1000140 (1–20). 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Rowland DC, Yanovich Y, Kentros CG (2011) A stable hippocampal representation of a space requires its direct experience. PNAS 108: 14654–14658. 10.1073/pnas.1105445108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Balderas I, Moreno-Castilla P, Bermudez-Rattoni F (2013) Dopamine D1 receptor activity modulates object recognition memory consolidation in the perirhinal cortex but not in the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 23: 873–878. 10.1002/hipo.22143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Furini CRG, Myskiw JC, Schmidt BE, Marcondes LA, Izquierdo I (2014) D1 and D5 dopamine receptors participate on the consolidation of two different memories. Behav. Brain Res. 271: 212–217. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Hasselmo ME, McGaughy J (2004) High acetylcholine levels set circuit dynamics for attention and encoding and low acetylcholine levels set dynamics for consolidation. Prog. Brain Res. 145: 207–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Hollnagel JO, Maslarova A, Haq RU, Heinemann U (2014) GABAB receptor dependent modulation of sharp wave-ripple complexes in the rat hippocampus in vitro. Neurosci. Lett. 574: 15–20. 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.04.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Ul Haq R, Liotta A, Kovacs R, Rosler A, Jarosch MJ, Heinemann U, Behrens CJ (2012) Adrenergic modulation of sharp wave-ripple activity in rat hippocampal slices. Hippocampus 22: 516–533. 10.1002/hipo.20918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O'Reilly RC (1995) Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psych Rev. 102: 419–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Wang SH, Morris RG (2010) Hippocampal-neocortical interactions in memory formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation. Annu Rev Psychol. 61: 49–79. 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100523 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. McKenzie S, Eichenbaum H (2011) Consolidation and reconsolidation: Two lives of memories? Neuron. 71: 224–233. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Preston AR, Eichenbaum H. Interplay of hippocampal and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr. Biol. 2013; 23: R764–R773. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Depue BE (2012) A neuroanatomical model of prefrontal inhibitory modulation of memory retrieval. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 36: 1382–1399. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Komorowski RW, Garcia CG, Wilson A, Hattori S, Howard MW, Eichenbaum H (2013) Ventral hippocampal neurons are shaped by experience to represent behaviorally relevant contexts. J. Neurosci. 33: 8079–8087. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5458-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Li M, Long C, Yang L. Hippocampal-prefrontal circuit and disrupted functional activity in psychiatric and neurodegenerative disorders. Biomed Res. Intern. Volume 2015 article ID 810548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Sara SJ (2010) Reactivation, retrieval, replay and reconsolidation in and out of sleep: connecting the dots. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4: 185 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B (2008) Drug enhancement of memory consolidation: historical perspective and neurobiological implications. Psychopharmac. (Berl.) 202: 3–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Roullet P, Sara S (1998) Consolidation of memory after its reactivation: involvement of beta noradrenergic receptors in the late phase. Neural Plast. 6: 63–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Tronel S, Feenstra MGP, Sara SJ (2004) Noradrenergic action in prefrontal cortex in the late stage of memory consolidation. Learning Mem. 11: 453–458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Eschenko O, Sara SJ (2008) Learning-dependent, transient increase of activity in noradrenergic neurons of locus coeruleus during slow wave sleep in the rat: brain stem-cortex interplay for memory consolidation? Cereb. Cortex 18: 2596–2603. 10.1093/cercor/bhn020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Przybyslawski J, Sara SJ (1997) Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behav. Brain Res. 84: 241–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Przybyslawski J, Roullet P, Sara SJ (1999). Attenuation of emotional and non-emotional memories after their reactivation: role of beta adrenergic receptors. J. Neurosci. 19: 6623–6628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Debiec J, LeDoux J (2006) Noradrenergic signaling in the amygdala contributes to the reconsolidation of fear memory: treatment implications for PTSD. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071: 521–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Bouret S, Sara SJ (2002) Locus coeruleus activation modulates firing rate and temporal organization of odour-induced single-cell responses in rat piriform cortex. Eur J Neurosci 16: 2371–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. McCormick DA, Pape HC, Williamson A (1991) Actions of norepinephrine in the cerebral cortex and thalamus: implications for function of the central noradrenergic system. Prog. Brain Res. 88: 293–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Eschenko O, Magri C, Panzeri S, Sara SJ (2012) Noradrenergic neurons of the locus coeruleus are phase locked to cortical up-down states during sleep. Cereb. Cortex 22: 426–435. 10.1093/cercor/bhr121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Marzo A, Bai J, Otani S (2009) Neuroplasticity regulation by noradrenaline in mammalian brain. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 7: 286–295. 10.2174/157015909790031193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Harley CW (2007) Norepinephrine and the dentate gyrus. Prog. Brain Res. 163: 299–318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]