Abstract
Sensorimotor rhythms (SMR, 8–15 Hz) are brain oscillations associated with successful motor performance, imagery, and imitation. Voluntary modulation of SMR can be used to control brain–machine interfaces (BMI) in the absence of any physical movements. The mechanisms underlying acquisition of such skill are unknown. Here, we provide evidence for a causal link between function of the primary motor cortex (M1), active during motor skill learning and retention, and successful acquisition of abstract skills such as control over SMR. Thirty healthy participants were trained on 5 consecutive days to control SMR oscillations. Each participant was randomly assigned to one of 3 groups that received either 20 min of anodal, cathodal, or sham transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over M1. Learning SMR control across training days was superior in the anodal tDCS group relative to the other 2. Cathodal tDCS blocked the beneficial effects of training, as evidenced with sham tDCS. One month later, the newly acquired skill remained superior in the anodal tDCS group. Thus, application of weak electric currents of opposite polarities over M1 differentially modulates learning SMR control, pointing to this primary cortical region as a common substrate for acquisition of physical motor skills and learning to control brain oscillatory activity.
Keywords: brain stimulation, Hebbian learning, motor cortex, sensorimotor rhythms
Introduction
Repetitive practice is required to learn physical skills like playing sports or a musical instrument (Lashley et al. 1951; Brady 2008). Practice is also required to learn more abstract skills irrespective of physical movements, like control of brain oscillatory activity (Koralek et al. 2012) present with successful performance of motor and cognitive functions like attention (Schafer and Moore 2011), gating of sensory information (Klimesch et al. 2007), movement planning (Joundi et al. 2012), speech (Saarinen et al. 2006), and reasoning (Sheth et al. 2009).
Brain oscillations may be topographically specific, like occipital alpha rhythms related to visual processing (Chatrian et al. 1959) or sensorimotor rhythms (SMR, 8–15 Hz) predominantly localized over primary sensorimotor regions and related to motor actions (Pfurtscheller and Aranibar 1979). In the motor domain, performance, imagination, and/or imitation of movements is associated with event-related desynchronization of SMR (SMR-ERD) (Pfurtscheller and Aranibar 1979; Pfurtscheller and Neuper 1997) reflecting activity in a distributed sensorimotor cortico-subcortical network that includes the primary motor cortex (M1) (Stocco et al. 2010; Buch et al. 2012), a region crucial for motor control and motor learning (Reis et al. 2009). Previous work demonstrated that healthy volunteers and patients with brain lesions can learn a novel operant brain–machine interface task in which they are required to modulate SMR-ERD, rather than executing a physical movement, to obtain reward through associative (Hebbian) learning (Buch et al. 2008; Soekadar et al. 2011). This type of abstract learning is important because it allows, for example, neural control of robotic and prosthetic devices (Ganguly et al. 2011; Buch et al. 2012) in the absence of physical movements (Hebbian control). Sensory feedback is critical to learn this task because when successful generation of SMR-ERD did not result in any feedback during training, participants did not learn (Soekadar et al. 2011; Censor et al. 2013).
There is, however, a gap in knowledge on the mechanisms and neural substrates underlying such abstract learning and it is unclear whether targeting primary cortical regions can modulate acquisition of such skill. Here, we studied the involvement of M1 as a substrate of learning to control SMR oscillations in the absence of physical movements. We explored this idea by evaluating polarity-specific effects of M1 stimulation on learning SMR control across multiple days. Previous work indicated that repeated stimulation can stabilize the long-lasting effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) (Reis et al. 2009) and result in larger cumulative effects when applied daily (Alonzo et al. 2012). We reasoned that repeated application of anodal tDCS over M1, shown to increase motor cortical excitability (Nitsche and Paulus 2000; Galea et al. 2009) and acquisition of physical motor skills (Nitsche and Schauenburg et al. 2003; Reis et al. 2009; Stagg, Jayaram et al. 2011), would improve Hebbian learning to control brain oscillatory activity relative to sham tDCS, and that application of cathodal tDCS, known to decrease motor cortical excitability (Nitsche and Paulus 2000), would reduce the acquisition of this skill relative to sham tDCS.
Methods
Participants and Study Design
Thirty-six young adults (28.6 ± 8.2 years, 15 males) participated in a prescreening session to evaluate handedness and baseline SMR-ERD during motor imagery. All participants were recruited at the laboratory of the Human Cortical Physiology and Neurorehabilitation Section (HCPS), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) and were able to elicit detectable SMR-ERD of at least 20% during imagery (Soekadar et al. 2011). All participants were right handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield 1971), had no physical or neurological symptoms, had no past history of neurological or psychiatric diseases, and did not take any medication on a regular basis. Four participants were excluded because they were not able to elicit detectable SMR-ERD of at least 20% during imagery. Two participants retracted from the study after the prescreening due to the required time commitment. All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study before the experiment, which was approved by the NINDS IRB, and were assigned to 1 of 3 different groups that received anodal, cathodal, or sham tDCS for a period of 20 min in association with each training session (Fig. 1a,b). All 3 groups were trained for a total of 1 h daily on 5 consecutive days. A follow-up test was done 30 days later. During training, the participants were instructed to imagine opening their left hand in response to a GO signal, which elicits SMR-ERD (Buch et al. 2008; Soekadar et al. 2011). Successful production of SMR-ERD through the imagery task resulted in online contingent left hand passive opening motions driven by an orthotic device attached to the participants' hand (Fig. 2a). Electromyographic activity was recorded from multiple muscle groups (finger and arm extensors and flexors) to assess possible muscle contractions during the task. The endpoint measure of control of neural oscillatory activity was the amount of SMR-ERD, which was computed for every single trial (Fig. 2b).
Task Description and Procedure
Participants were trained to control SMR oscillations recorded with a whole-head magnetoencephalography (MEG) sensor array composed of 275 gradiometers (CTF Systems, Canada) (Fig. 2a) over a 5-day period. Brain oscillatory activity was recorded at a sampling rate of 600 Hz with a band-width of 0–300 Hz. Synthetic 3rd gradient balancing was used for online removal of background noise. In contrast to magnetometers, use of a high number of gradiometers allows detection of neuromagnetic activity right below the sensors with high topographical specificity to cortical sources (Hillebrand and Barnes 2002) being relatively insensitive to deeper sources. The 3 sensors showing highest SMR-ERD values were selected for the following training. Successful production of SMR-ERD through the imagery task resulted in online contingent left hand passive opening motions driven by an orthotic device attached to the subject's hand (see Fig. 2a, Supplementary Fig. 1 and Soekadar et al. 2011 for detailed description). Thus, they observed and felt their hand opening upon eliciting successful SMR-ERD. Online electromyography (EMG) was monitored from finger and arm flexors and extensors (m. brachioradialis and m. flexor carpi ulnaris, m. biceps brachii, and m. triceps brachii) of both arms using radio-translucent surface electrodes (Biopac®, interelectrode distance: 2 cm). Trials in which EMG activity during imagery exceeded that recorded during rest were excluded from further analysis. Head movements were continuously recorded using individually registered head coils to account for different head shapes (Wilson 2004; Stolk et al. 2013). Trials were interrupted and discarded from further analysis when head motion exceeded 5 mm. For detection and online translation of SMR-ERD, the platform BCI2000 was used (Schalk et al. 2004; Soekadar et al. 2011). Sessions consisted of 5 blocks with 23 trials each (Fig. 1a). Start and end of each trial was indicated by auditory stimuli. Before the first training Day 2 baseline blocks were recorded in the absence of any stimulation (BAS).
Computation of ERD included the power spectrum estimation (an autoregressive model of order 16 using the Yule–Walker algorithm) of each incoming sample at the frequency found to be optimal in each participant (optimal defined as frequency showing highest SMR-ERD values during motor imagery in the range of 8–15 Hz; 9 Hz in 2 participants, 11 Hz in 27 participants and 13 Hz in one participant). Resulting values were compared with mean power values of the preceding intertrial intervals continuously updating the ERD threshold according to the method of Pfurtscheller and Aranibar (1979):
(1) |
(2) |
where t represents the recorded sample block, Tref the event-related task condition period and Pt the power estimate in a given frequency band of t. RV (reference value) represents power estimates during the rest (task-free) condition. Positive feedback consisted of hand opening and was delivered online during each trial when SMR-ERD was detected (Fig. 2b). Correlation of changes in SMR-ERD and the amount of time positive feedback (hand opening) was provided (amount of feedback) were calculated offline to confirm proper translation of successful SMR-ERD control into amount of feedback (see Supplementary Fig. 1).
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS)
tDCS was applied via 2 conducting 5 × 5 cm electrodes covered with a saline-soaked sponge using a Phoresor II Auto® (model PM850, IOMED®, Salt Lake City, UT). A bipolar electrode montage (right M1 and left supraorbital area) was used (Fig. 1b) to deliver a current of 1 mA for 20 min (current density 0.04 mA/cm2; total charge 0.048 C/cm2), previously shown to have polarity dependent (Nitsche and Paulus 2000; Stagg, Jayaram et al. 2011) and focal (Faria et al. 2011) long-lasting effects (Nitsche and Paulus 2001) on motor cortical excitability, learning, and retention of skill (Abe et al. 2010; Censor et al. 2010; Schambra et al. 2011). In the sham group, anodal tDCS was applied for 30 s and at the offset, the current was decreased in a ramp-like fashion, a method shown to achieve a good level of blinding between sessions (Gandiga et al. 2006). In the 3 groups, the active electrode was placed over the right M1. Prior to training Day 1, the M1 hand-area was localized in all participants based on the motor evoked potential (MEP) hotspot of the first digit's interosseus muscle (FDI) using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Participants and investigators performing testing and data analysis were blind to group assignment.
Offline Data Analysis
For all outcome measures, assumption of a normal distribution (Shapiro–Wilk test of normality) was tested. Parametric tests were corrected by Greenhouse–Geisser estimates if Mauchly's sphericity test indicated significance. Mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVAs (rmANOVAs) with factors “group” (anodal/cathodal/sham) as between-subject factor and “training day” (Day 1–Day 5) as within-subject variable was performed for SMR-ERD. Multiple pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni adjustments were calculated to compare changes of baseline-corrected SMR-ERD values across groups at each time point. To evaluate differences in successful SMR-ERD control, baseline-corrected SMR-ERD values of training Day 1 were compared with training Day 5 in each group using a paired-samples t-test. Effect size of anodal and cathodal tDCS was calculated using Cohen's d transformed into a regression coefficient r, where r < 0.3 is considered as small, r < 0.5 is considered as medium, and r > 0.5 is considered as large effect (Cohen 1988). To detect differences in successful SMR-ERD control between stimulation groups, independent sample t-tests were applied. To calculate topographic specificity of learned SMR control, centro-parietal sensors were sub-categorized into those in immediate proximity (PS1, 10 sensors) and extended proximity (PS2, 26 sensors) to the sensors selected for training (training sensors, TS) (Supplementary Fig. 2). Topographical specificity was calculated as the ratio between SMR-ERD learning recorded by the neighboring centro-parietal sensors and the 3 training sensors (PS1/TS and PS2/TS, respectively). For comparison between groups, a 2-way ANOVA with “group” (anodal/cathodal/sham) and “sensors” (TS/PS1/PS2) as independent variables was conducted. To evaluate differences in topographical specificity of learned SMR-control between stimulation conditions, post hoc t-tests for independent samples were used. All analyses were performed in SPSS 17.0. Significance level was set to P < 0.05. Variance is expressed as standard errors of the means (SEM).
Results
Learning to Control SMR Across Groups
At baseline, the ability to control SMR-ERD was comparable between the 3 groups (one-way ANOVA, F2,177 = 1.404, P = 0.248). A mixed-model rmANOVA revealed main effects of “stimulation” (F2,177 = 4.127, P < 0.05), “training day” (F3.589,635.17 = 2.546, P < 0.05) and their interaction (F7.718,635.17 = 4.796, P < 0.001) on SMR-ERD, indicating a different time course of learning control of neural oscillatory activity between the groups (see Fig. 3 for individual subject examples and Fig. 4 for group data). In the sham and anodal tDCS groups, training led to a progressive increase in SMR-ERD from training Day 1 to training Day 5, pointing to successful learning control of neural oscillatory activity (P < 0.05 and P < 0.001, respectively, independent-samples t-test, Fig. 4b). During the training task, the number of excluded trials in which EMG activity present in arm muscles was comparable across groups (one-way ANOVA, F2,149 = 0.632, P = 0.533), and thus could not account for group differences in learning.
Increase in SMR-ERD from training Day 1 to training Day 5 (ΔDay 1/Day 5) was significantly larger in the anodal than in the sham (P < 0.05; independent samples t-test) or cathodal (P < 0.001) tDCS groups. Cathodal tDCS significantly reduced the benefits of training on SMR-ERD control relative to the sham group (P < 0.01). Thus, anodal tDCS increased, while cathodal tDCS reduced the beneficial effects of training as evidenced in the sham group (Fig. 4b). Calculation of the effect size indicated large effects for both, anodal (r = 0.742) and cathodal (r = −0.785) stimulation.
Retention of Skill
Learned SMR-ERD control at the follow-up test 30 days later (where no stimulation was applied) was better in the anodal tDCS group compared with the sham (P < 0.05) and cathodal (P < 0.01) groups, and better in the sham compared with the cathodal tDCS (P < 0.05) group, indicating long-lasting polarity-specific effects of M1 stimulation on the learned skill. At this time point, only participants in the anodal tDCS group showed significantly larger SMR-ERD relative to the first training day (P < 0.01). Of note, SMR-ERD control of participants trained under cathodal stimulation at follow-up was not different from SMR-ERD control of participants trained under anodal (P = 0.78) or sham (P = 0.74) stimulation on training Day 1.
Task-related Changes of Brain Oscillatory Activity in Other Frequency Bands and Topographical Specificity of Learned SMR Control
In relation to other frequency bands, a rmANOVA revealed no significant effects of “stimulation”, “training day” or their interaction on theta, 4–8 Hz: F2,122 = 0.22, P = 0.80, beta, 17–30 Hz: F2,122 = 1.91, P = 0.153, or gamma, 30–150 Hz: F2,122 = 3.06, P = 0.45) rhythms (Supplementary Fig. 3). For evaluation of gamma activity a band was chosen that transcends 30–120 Hz following previous studies that indicated strongest cross-frequency coupling in the 80–150 Hz band (Canolty et al. 2006). Analysis of topographical specificity of learned SMR-control using a 2-way ANOVA indicated a significant main effect for “condition” (F2,54 = 38.27, P < 0.01) and, sensors' (F1,54 = 132.23, P < 0.01) with no interaction between factors (F2,42 = 0.74, P = 0.241). Specificity was higher in the anodal compared with the cathodal (P < 0.01) tDCS group, and similar compared with the sham group (P = 0.43) which also yielded higher specificity when compared with the cathodal tDCS group (P < 0.01) (Supplementary Fig. 2 and Table 1).
Discussion
We studied the contribution of M1 to learning control of oscillatory brain activity in healthy humans. We found that the combination of anodal tDCS over M1 with training improved learning of this skill to a larger extent than cathodal or sham tDCS. This effect was polarity-specific as cathodal tDCS significantly reduced skill gains relative to sham. Retention of the newly acquired skill 1 month later remained superior with anodal compared with cathodal or sham tDCS.
Control of brain oscillatory activity constitutes a qualitatively fundamentally different skill than the ability to perform visuomotor sequences (Reis et al. 2009), finger movements (Onal-Hartmann et al. 2012), or adapting to perturbations (Shadmehr et al. 2010), all of which require physical movements (Koralek et al. 2012). Successful SMR oscillatory control through imagery in this task results in positive passive rewarding feedback, consistent of contingent online passive hand motions driven by an orthotic device (Buch et al. 2008; Soekadar et al. 2011). With its distinct features (Wolpaw et al. 2002), successful control of brain oscillatory activity represents a skill that is relevant for successful performance of motor movements (Joundi et al. 2012), speech (Saarinen et al. 2006), and imitation (Pfurtscheller and Neuper 1997) and may be useful in the management of seizure activity (Sterman and Friar 1972), attention deficits (Thompson and Thompson 1998; Monastra et al. 2005), and stroke (Birbaumer et al. 2009). In spite of being ubiquitous in successful normal and abnormal behavior and of having been recently investigated in animal models (Koralek et al. 2012), the neural substrates underlying learned control of brain oscillatory activity in humans are incompletely understood.
The contribution of the primary motor cortex in learning this abstract skill was evaluated by modulating neural activity in M1 using tDCS. The combination of anodal tDCS over M1 with training led to better learned control of SMR oscillations than cathodal or sham tDCS over the 5 training days. There was no indication in our data that resting baseline power differed across the differently stimulated groups (Supplementary Fig. 4). Inspection of Figure 4a shows that significant group differences did not become obvious until training Day 3. This indicates that the effects of tDCS on this type of skill acquisition require cumulative days to become detectable, and do not operate simply through SMR-ERD modulation elicited by passive hand movements (Matsumoto et al. 2010). An additional study in which training was continued over the time of 10 days indicated that learning in the absence of stimulation does not reach a ceiling effect by Day 5 (Supplementary Fig. 5) and is comparable to the improvements identified in the sham tDCS group (Fig. 4a). While anodal tDCS increased the effects of training on SMR control, cathodal tDCS blocked them (Fig. 4a), altogether providing a causal link between M1 function and successful learning of this skill. Such findings indicate that it is possible to modulate abstract learning to control brain oscillatory activity using noninvasive brain stimulation targeting a primary cortical region.
Within our experimental design, the effects of tDCS were specific to the trained SMR, since tDCS did not affect other frequency bands such as theta (4–8 Hz), upper beta (17–30 Hz), and gamma (30–150 Hz) (Supplementary Fig. 3). More generally, our results show for the first time that application of weak electric currents (1 mA) of opposite polarities over a relevant primary cortical region differentially modulate Hebbian learning to control oscillatory brain activity across multiple days partially originated in that region. Learned SMR control showed to be spatially specific to the sensors selected for training (Supplementary Fig. 2 and Table 1).
Four weeks after the end of training, skill was superior within the anodal tDCS group relative to baseline and also relative to the other 2 groups. This finding is consistent with the idea that tDCS in combination with training influence neural processes beyond immediate effects on cortical mechanisms (Matsumoto et al. 2010;Kasashima et al. 2012), e.g. long-term potentiation (LTP) (Fritsch et al. 2010) that is N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor dependent (Liebetanz et al. 2002; Nitsche, Fricke et al. 2003; Koralek et al. 2012) or modulation of the GABAergic system (Stagg, Bachtiar et al. 2011). Interestingly, learning to control firing rates in single neurons in rodents' M1 is also, at least partially, NMDA receptor dependent because blocking NMDA receptor function in striato-cortical connections disrupts acquisition of the skill (Koralek et al. 2012).
Our results provide causal evidence for the involvement of the primary motor cortex in learning a skill that does not involve physical movements, possibly as part of a more extensive basal-ganglia-thalamo-cortical circuit influenced by feedback and reward (Hinterberger et al. 2005; Stocco et al. 2010; Buch et al. 2012). Moreover, they point to common substrates between acquisition and retention of motor skills (Abe et al. 2010; Censor et al. 2010; Schambra et al. 2011) and learned brain control. Learned SMR control was topographically specific to sensors overlying the primary sensorimotor cortex, which is interconnected with premotor regions involved in motor planning and shaping of the hand (Murata et al. 1997). While likely to be of small effect, use of a different sphericity test than Mauchly's test could have been advantageous to avoid type II errors in small samples. Future work should address the relative contribution of tangential and perpendicular current sources to anodal and cathodal tDCS effects (Manola et al. 2007) using a multimodal approach with complementary sensitivity towards tangential and perpendicular current sources, e.g. recording electroencephalography and MEG simultaneously during transcranial electric current stimulation (Soekadar et al. 2013).
The findings presented here might have important implications for the development and design of novel rehabilitation strategies aiming at restoration of brain function (“neurorestoration”) using brain–machine interfaces (BMI), e.g. in the context of stroke recovery (Ramos-Murguialday et al. 2013).
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material can be found at: http://www.cercor.oxfordjournals.org/
Funding
This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program (IRP) of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS), Bethesda, MD, USA; the Center for Neuroscience and Regenerative Medicine (CNRM), Uniformed Services University of Health Sciences, Bethesda, MD, USA; the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF, grant number 01GQ0831, 16SV5838K to S.R.S. and N.B.); the European Commission under the project WAY (grant number 288551 to S.R.S. and N.B.); the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, grant number SO932-1 to S.R.S. and Reinhart Koselleck Project support to N.B.); Volkswagenstiftung (VW) and the Baden-Württemberg Stiftung.
Supplementary Material
Notes
Conflict of Interest: None declared.
References
- Abe M, Schambra H, Wassermann EM, Luckenbaugh D, Schweighofer N, Cohen LG. 2010. Reward improves long-term retention of a motor memory through induction of offline memory gains. Curr Biol. 21:557–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alonzo A, Brassil J, Taylor JL, Martin D, Loo CK. 2012. Daily transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) leads to greater increases in cortical excitability than second daily transcranial direct current stimulation. Brain Stimul. 5:208–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birbaumer N, Ramos Murguialday A, Weber C, Montoya P. 2009. Neurofeedback and brain–computer interface clinical applications. Int Rev Neurobiol. 86:107–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady F. 2008. The contextual interference effect and sport skills. Percept Mot Skills. 106:461–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buch E, Weber C, Cohen LG, Braun C, Dimyan MA, Ard T, Mellinger J, Caria A, Soekadar S, Fourkas A, et al. 2008. Think to move: a neuromagnetic brain–computer interface (BCI) system for chronic stroke. Stroke. 39:910–917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buch ER, Modir Shanechi A, Fourkas AD, Weber C, Birbaumer N, Cohen LG. 2012. Parietofrontal integrity determines neural modulation associated with grasping imagery after stroke. Brain. 135:596–614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT. 2006. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science. 313:1626–1628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Censor N, Dimyan MA, Cohen LG. 2010. Modification of existing human motor memories is enabled by primary cortical processing during memory reactivation. Curr Biol. 20:1545–1549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Censor N, Sagi D, Cohen LG. 2013. Common mechanisms of human perceptual and motor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci. 13:658–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chatrian GE, Petersen MC, Lazarte JA. 1959. The blocking of the rolandic wicket rhythm and some central changes related to movement. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 11:497–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Faria P, Hallett M, Miranda PC. 2011. A finite element analysis of the effect of electrode area and inter-electrode distance on the spatial distribution of the current density in tDCS. J Neural Eng. 8:066017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritsch B, Reis J, Martinowich K, Schambra HM, Ji Y, Cohen LG, Lu B. 2010. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron. 66:198–204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea JM, Jayaram G, Ajagbe L, Celnik P. 2009. Modulation of cerebellar excitability by polarity-specific noninvasive direct current stimulation. J Neurosci. 29:9115–9122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gandiga PC, Hummel FC, Cohen LG. 2006. Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 117:845–850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganguly K, Dimitrov DF, Wallis JD, Carmena JM. 2011. Reversible large-scale modification of cortical networks during neuroprosthetic control. Nat Neurosci. 14:662–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillebrand A, Barnes GR. 2002. A quantitative assessment of the sensitivity of whole-head MEG to activity in the adult human cortex. NeuroImage. 16:638–650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinterberger T, Veit R, Wilhelm B, Weiskopf N, Vatine JJ, Birbaumer N. 2005. Neuronal mechanisms underlying control of a brain–computer-interface. Eur J Neurosci. 21:3169–3181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joundi RA, Jenkinson N, Brittain JS, Aziz TZ, Brown P. 2012. Driving oscillatory activity in the human cortex enhances motor performance. Curr Biol. 22:403–407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasashima Y, Fujiwara T, Matsushika Y, Tsuji T, Hase K, Ushiyama J, Ushiba J, Liu M. 2012. Modulation of event-related desynchronization during motor imagery with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in patients with chronic hemiparetic stroke. Exp Brain Res. 221:263–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. 2007. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev. 53:63–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koralek AC, Jin X, Long JD, 2nd, Costa RM, Carmena JM. 2012. Corticostriatal plasticity is necessary for learning intentional neuroprosthetic skills. Nature. 483:331–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lashley KS, Chow KL, Semmes J. 1951. An examination of the electrical field theory of cerebral integration. Psychol Rev. 58:123–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liebetanz D, Nitsche MA, Tergau F, Paulus W. 2002. Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain. 125:2238–2247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manola L, Holsheimer J, Veltink P, Buitenweg JR. 2007. Anodal vs cathodal stimulation of motor cortex: a modeling study. Clin Neurophysiol. 118:464–474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto J, Fujiwara T, Takahashi O, Liu M, Kimura A, Ushiba J. 2010. Modulation of mu rhythm desynchronization during motor imagery by transcranial direct current stimulation. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 7:27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monastra VJ, Lynn S, Linden M, Lubar JF, Gruzelier J, LaVaque TJ. 2005. Electroencephalographic biofeedback in the treatment of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 30:95–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murata A, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Raos V, Rizzolatti G. 1997. Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. J Neurophysiol. 78:2226–2230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitsche MA, Fricke K, Henschke U, Schlitterlau A, Liebetanz D, Lang N, Henning S, Tergau F, Paulus W. 2003. Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J Physiol. 553:293–301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitsche MA, Paulus W. 2000. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. 527:633–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitsche MA, Paulus W. 2001. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. 57:1899–1901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitsche MA, Schauenburg A, Lang N, Liebetanz D, Exner C, Paulus W, Tergau F. 2003. Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. J Cogn Neurosci. 15:619–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield C. 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 9:97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onal-Hartmann C, Fiorio M, Gentner R, Zeller D, Pauli P, Classen J. 2012. After-training emotional interference may modulate sequence awareness in a serial reaction time task. Exp Brain Res. 219:75–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Aranibar A. 1979. Evaluation of event-related desynchronization (ERD) preceding and following self-paced movement. Electroencephgr Clin Neurophysiol. 46:138–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Neuper C. 1997. Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans. Neurosci Lett. 239:65–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramos-Murguialday A, Broetz D, Rea M, Läer L, Yilmaz Ö, Brasil F, Liberati G, Curado M, Garcia-Cossio E, Vyziotis A, et al. 2013. Brain–machine interface in chronic stroke rehabilitation: a controlled study. Ann Neurol. 74:100–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, Celnik PA, Krakauer JW. 2009. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 106:1590–1595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saarinen T, Laaksonen H, Parviainen T, Salmelin R. 2006. Motor cortex dynamics in visuomotor production of speech and non-speech mouth movements. Cereb Cortex. 16:212–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer RJ, Moore T. 2011. Selective attention from voluntary control of neurons in prefrontal cortex. Science. 332:1568–1571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schalk G, McFarland DJ, Hinterberger T, Birbaumer N, Wolpaw JR. 2004. BCI2000: a general-purpose brain–computer interface (BCI) system. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 51:1034–1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schambra HM, Abe M, Luckenbaugh DA, Reis J, Krakauer JW, Cohen LG. 2011. Probing for hemispheric specialization for motor skill learning: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. J Neurophysiol. 106:652–661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW. 2010. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci. 33:89–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheth BR, Sandkühler S, Bhattacharya J. 2009. Posterior beta and anterior gamma oscillations predict cognitive insight. J Cogn Neurosci. 21:1269–1279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soekadar SR, Witkowski M, Cossio EG, Birbaumer N, Robinson SE, Cohen LG. 2013. In vivo assessment of human brain oscillations during application of transcranial electric currents. Nat Commun. 4:2032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soekadar SR, Witkowski M, Mellinger J, Ramos A, Birbaumer N, Cohen LG. 2011. ERD-based online brain–machine interfaces (BMI) in the context of neurorehabilitation: optimizing BMI learning and performance. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 19:542–549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, Johansen-Berg H. 2011. The role of GABA in human motor learning. Curr Biol. 21:480–484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stagg CJ, Jayaram G, Pastor D, Kincses ZT, Matthews PM, Johansen-Berg H. 2011. Polarity and timing-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in explicit motor learning. Neuropsychologia. 49:800–804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterman MB, Friar L. 1972. Suppression of seizures in an epileptic following sensorimotor EEG feedback training. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 33:89–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stocco A, Lebiere C, Anderson JR. 2010. Conditional routing of information to the cortex: a model of the basal ganglia's role in cognitive coordination. Psychol Rev. 117:541–574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stolk A, Todorovic A, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R. 2013. Online and offline tools for head movement compensation in MEG. NeuroImage. 68:39–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson L, Thompson M. 1998. Neurofeedback combined with training in metacognitive strategies: effectiveness in students with ADD. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 23:243–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HS. 2004. Continuous head-localization and data correction in a whole-cortex MEG sensor. Neurol Clin Neurophysiol. 2004:56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpaw JR, Birbaumer N, McFarland DJ, Pfurtscheller G, Vaughan TM. 2002. Brain–computer interfaces for communication and control. Clin Neurophysiol. 113:767–791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.