Abstract
Objectives. We examined the role of social cohesion as a component of vulnerability and resilience to the psychological distress of flooding.
Methods. A survey collected data from 2238 individuals living in flood-affected areas of England (South Yorkshire and Worcestershire) in 2007. We used Bayesian structural equation modeling to assess factors relating to the latent variables of resilience (years in area, family nearby, and social cohesion) and vulnerability (disruption of essential services, flood risk, and previous flood experience).
Results. Flooding was strongly associated with poor mental health; however, resilience factors (associated with the ability to cope with natural disasters), but not vulnerability, were strongly associated with a reduction in psychological distress.
Conclusions. Resilience and social cohesion were important influences on the risk of developing poor mental health following flooding. Increasing resilience of communities by strengthening social cohesion through measures that increase civic participation and changing land use should be considered as potentially inexpensive and effective defenses against avoidable mental harm that will result from increased climate instability.
The past decade has seen an unprecedented increase in extreme weather events in the northern latitudes,1 prominent among which are flooding disasters.2 The impact of flooding on public safety is obvious, but the burden also extends to psychological harm and other mental disorders.3–7 For example, following the summer floods in England in 2007, we found that the prevalence of mental health symptoms (psychological distress, anxiety, depression, and posttraumatic stress disorder) was up to fivefold higher among individuals affected by floodwater in the home.8 Evacuation from the home, disruption of essential services such as water, gas, and electricity, and concern about the effect on personal health and finances as a result of the flood were independently associated with increased risk of mental health symptoms. Although enhancing the resilience of individuals and communities to cope with the increasing threat of natural and manmade disasters is now a recognized public health priority, public health strategies have to look beyond the capabilities of institutions, organizations, and business to maintain or restore essential infrastructure and services and consider also the sociopsychological resources of individuals, families, and neighborhoods to sustain mental health and well-being.9,10
Aldrich has cogently argued that research in the social sciences on social capital and social networks is directly relevant to the concept of community resilience in the face of disasters and may help explain the observed variation between neighborhoods in postdisaster recovery.9 Social capital is still a debated concept, but it may be considered to have 2 main dimensions: structural (engagement in civic activities) and cognitive (perceptions of social relations, trust, and reciprocity). In the latter category, measures of individuals’ perceptions of trust in neighbors, reciprocity, and social bonds have been aggregated at the neighborhood level to measure the social cohesion of communities. Using this measure, studies have shown that high levels of social cohesion protect against the impact of neighborhood poverty on mental health.11,12
We tested the hypothesis that strong neighborhood social cohesion protects residents from the psychological harm caused by flooding. In doing so, we have addressed a major methodological issue in social cohesion research, which is the possibility of reverse causality in cross-sectional studies.13,14 Social cohesion survey measures are based on individuals’ perceptions, and it is possible that false-positive associations are found because those experiencing poorer mental health may be more likely to view themselves as more isolated and may be more pessimistic about the level of social cohesion in their communities.
METHODS
In June and July 2007, South Yorkshire and Hull, Worcestershire, Gloucestershire, and the Thames Valley areas in England were particularly badly affected by flooding. The national public health protection agency at the time, the Health Protection Agency (now Public Health England), conducted a survey in South Yorkshire (September–October 2007) and Worcestershire (January–February 2008). Local public health staff identified areas with homes that experienced flooding to varying degrees and areas with homes that were not affected; unaffected households were included as controls. Details of sampling and data collection methods have been published.8 Local directors of Public Health sent out letters of invitation to participate in the survey to “the occupier” of households, outlining a choice of 3 methods to complete the survey: (1) by telephone, (2) online, or (3) by return of a paper questionnaire using a postage-free envelope. In 2 villages in Worcestershire, data collection was carried out by face-to-face interviews, as these areas were small enough for this method of data collection to be feasible. Nonresponders received reminder postcards after 3 weeks.
Demographics and Exposure Variables
The survey, which included data on a range of sociodemographic variables (Table 1), has been described in detail elsewhere.8 Variables related to severity of flood exposure were also collected, including whether the homes in a flood-affected area were actually flooded. Psychological distress was measured with the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12).15 A “case” of psychological distress was defined as a score on the GHQ-12 questionnaire of 3 or more. A maximum of 2 missing scores for the GHQ-12 were allowed, with the missing value(s) being imputed as the rounded average for the 11 or 10 completed items. Responses with more than 2 missing GHQ-12 items were omitted from the analysis (n = 47 in South Yorkshire, n = 114 in Worcestershire).
TABLE 1—
Variable | Resident of Flooded Property, No. (%) or Mean ±SD | Resident of Nonflooded Property, No. (%) or Mean ±SD | P for Trend |
Experienced disrupted servicesa | 201 (52.76) | 232 (15.33) | < .001 |
Perceived home to be at risk of floodinga | 110 (28.50) | 204 (12.55) | < .001 |
Had previous flood experiencea | 90 (24.39) | 267 (17.01) | < .001 |
Years in areab | 19.90 ±17.38 | 22.28 ±19.31 | .03 |
Family or friends nearbyb | 337 (88.68) | 1388 (85.89) | .18 |
Social cohesionb | 29.85 ±1.47 | 29.83 ±1.75 | .85 |
Note. For illustrative purposes, we dichotomized the level of flooding and categorized “flooding” as any water entering the house.
Factors loading on the vulnerability latent construct.
Factors loading on the resilience latent construct.
Social cohesion was measured with the Neighborhood Cohesion Scale.16,17 Each of the 8 questions consisted of a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” Across the literature pertaining to social cohesion and mental health, the use of cross-sectional methods has raised issues about reverse causality13,14 or same-source bias because of the nature of the constructs. Those experiencing poorer mental health may in turn be more likely to view themselves as more isolated and be more pessimistic about the cohesion in their communities, and vice versa. To address this point, we calculated an area social cohesion score using a standardized census geographical unit, the lower super output area (LSOA), with an average population size of approximately 1500 individuals. Each individual had a score aggregated at the LSOA level for all individuals in that area, other than themselves.
Analysis
We used a Bayesian version (BSEM) of a structural equation modeling (SEM) approach for the statistical analysis.18 SEM was chosen because of its ability to explore both direct and indirect effects as well as allowing the production of latent variables derived through the correlation structure of directly observed variables. Through the use of SEM, it was possible to explore the latent concepts of resilience and vulnerability, which were not directly measured at the time of data collection. Risk factors identified by Paranjothy et al.8 were the level of flood waters, reported damage to property, disruption to services, health concerns relating to the floods, and perceived negative impact on finances.
The present analysis initially explored the covariance structure through factor analysis, an iterative principle factor design with varimax rotation. Factor analysis identified 2 clusters of variables conceptualized as resilience and vulnerability. We then regressed these factors upon psychological distress using the BSEM method. We addressed missing values using a Bayesian method in Mplus 6 (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA)19 and Markov chain Monte Carlo simulation.
We assessed the goodness of fit of the models by the positive predictive checking method,20 which generates a positive predicative P value for the overlapping distributions of the real and replicated data—that is, it answers the question, Does the replicated data have a distribution similar to that of the real data? The more similar the data, the better the fit; therefore, a well-fitted model is expected to have 95% confidence intervals for the difference between the observed and the replicated χ2 values that cross zero and a positive predicative P value greater than .05.18
RESULTS
Survey response rates were 38% in South Yorkshire and 14% in Worcestershire. The age range for the sample was 16 to 96 years. The mean age of responders was 50 years (SD = 17 years) in South Yorkshire and 57 years (SD = 17 years) in Worcestershire. On the basis of 2001 census data, the age profile of responders was similar to that of the sample populations, although people younger than 36 years were underrepresented and people older than 50 years were overrepresented. Most respondents to the survey were female (72% in South Yorkshire and 57% in Worcestershire). According to data from the Office for National Statistics,21 52% of the population in the sample areas were female.
Residents who were flooded were similar to those who were not flooded in terms of the length of time they had lived in the area (20 vs 22 years), the presence of family and friends nearby, and the mean area-level social cohesion (Table 1). The distribution of social cohesion values ranged from 8 to 40 (mean = 29.32; SD = 5.61; median = 30.0; interquartile range = 26.0–33.0). The 8-item Neighborhood Cohesion Scale demonstrated good internal consistency: the Cronbach α was 0.820, with all items loading on to 1 factor, and the Eigenvalue was 3.61, with item loads ranging from 0.64 to 0.78. The Cronbach α for the GHQ-12 was 0.72. The level of reported psychological distress in the flooded group was 68.9%, compared with 19% in the unaffected group.
We used SEM to explore the relationship between community-level factors (vulnerability and resilience) and poor mental health (psychological distress). The results from the factor and path analysis are shown in Figure 1. The figure shows both the loadings of the items onto the latent variable, represented by λ, and the parameter estimates, represented by B. Factor loadings represent the degree to which each of the variables correlates with each of the factors, revealing the extent to which each of the variables contributes to the meaning of each of the factors. The boxes refer to directly observed variables, whereas the ovals are latent constructs. The factor loadings for the latent variable of vulnerability were flood risk (λ = 0.345), disruption of services (λ = 0.484), and previous flood experience (λ = 0.301); for resilience, they were years in area (λ = 0.339), family nearby (λ = 0.419), and perceived social cohesion (λ = 0.401). All factor loadings were significant (P > .005).
Flood level was positively associated with both vulnerability (b = 0.460; 95% confidence interval [CI] = 0.336, 0.616) and resilience (b = 0.093; 95% CI = 0.008, 0.174), as well as with psychological distress (b = 0. 342; 95% CI = 0.205, 0.444). The standardized BSEM model estimates for the latent constructs regressed on the outcome of psychological distress showed a significant decrease for resilience (b = −0.162; 95% CI = −0.244, −0.079) and a positive but nonsignificant effect on vulnerability (b = 0.744; 95% CI = −0.194, 1.641).
The 95% confidence interval for the difference between the observed and the replicated χ2 values indicated a good fit (95% CI = −1.565, –60.963; positive predicative P = .07).
DISCUSSION
In this study, we sought to examine the influence of concepts of vulnerability and resilience, which are often mooted as important in population health but are inherently hard to measure. The use of SEM allows the exploration of potential causal mechanisms. Our findings suggest that variables relating to the construct of resilience and, in particular, social cohesion are more strongly associated with psychological distress than variables related to vulnerability. In developing a method for assessing social cohesion, we have addressed one aspect of reverse causality in cross sectional studies; where social cohesion survey measures are based on individuals’ perceptions, it is possible that false-positive associations are found because those experiencing poorer mental health may be more likely to view themselves as more isolated and may be more pessimistic in their answers about the level of cohesiveness in their communities.
Social cohesion is thought to influence health in a number of ways: by promoting the adoption and reinforcement of health-related behaviors and by increasing access to resources and amenities.22,23 Social cohesion also influences the psychological processing of the individual by providing meaningful contact and increasing the sense of purpose and connection with other residents, contributing to more favorable mental health outcomes.24–26 During times of adversity, when disasters affect the community as a whole, an increase in levels of social cohesion allows for greater interaction and communication, which reduce individual self-reliance and perceived inequity.
Interventions to increase social cohesion have been shown to be effective when they encourage the creation of informal social ties while reinforcing commitment to positive social values.27 Ecologically based investigations focusing on self-efficacy and enhancing a positive self-concept28,29 may also be effective. These methods seek to enhance the individual within his or her neighborhood, to develop friendships, and to show the availability of others to help. It needs to be borne in mind, however, that communities with greater levels of cohesion will still have marginalized groups on the periphery that may be significantly disadvantaged.30
Strengths and Limitations
We have attempted to quantify, through correlation, the higher-level community constructs from real data gathered soon after a disaster. The levels of reported psychological distress in flooded and unaffected groups were comparable to those of other studies, providing evidence of the external validity of these data.31 Furthermore, the 8 items on the Neighborhood Cohesion Scale demonstrated good internal consistency, in line with other studies.11,32,33
However, there are a number of limitations. The response rate to the survey was 38% in South Yorkshire and 14% in Worcestershire. Although these figures are similar to those reported in other studies of this type,34 it is possible that those experiencing a more profound reaction to the flooding were more likely to respond, producing an overestimate of psychological distress in the flooded group. However, the prevalence of psychological distress in the flooded group in our study is similar to the reported levels in other studies of flooding victims,35,36 suggesting that our results are unlikely to be biased in this way.
Conclusions
Our data suggest that societal adaptation to climate change and the increasing frequency of disasters cannot be adequately addressed just by agencies responsible for disaster planning and management. Purely focusing on the physical safety of populations and the resilience of infrastructure and services will lessen the acute physical harm of disasters. However, to protect and strengthen the psychological health and well-being of communities, attention needs to be given to strengthening social cohesion.
Acknowledgments
This study, which was undertaken without funding, analyzed a data set originally commissioned by the UK Health Procreation Agency.
We thank Daniel Farewell for his statistical advice and John Gallacher and David Fone for their invaluable insights into the concept of cohesion. We also thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments.
Human Participant Protection
The analysis, which reported a secondary analysis of a data set collected by the UK Health Procreation Agency, was not deemed a research project but a Health Impact Assessment; therefore, ethical review was not needed.
References
- 1.Tang Q, Zhang X, Francis JA. Extreme summer weather in northern mid-latitudes linked to a vanishing cryosphere. Nat Clim Chang. 2014;4:45–50. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Aerts JC, Botzen WW, Emanuel K, Lin N, de Moel H, Michel-Kerjan EO. Evaluating flood resilience strategies for coastal megacities. Science. 2014;344(6183):473–475. doi: 10.1126/science.1248222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ahern M, Kovats RS, Wilkinson P, Few R, Matthies F. Global health impacts of floods: epidemiologic evidence. Epidemiol Rev. 2005;27(1):36–46. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxi004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Davis TE, III, Grills-Taquechel AE, Ollendick TH. The psychological impact from Hurricane Katrina: effects of displacement and trauma exposure on university students. Behav Ther. 2010;41(3):340–349. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2009.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Heir T, Sandvik L, Weisæth L. Hallmarks of posttraumatic stress: symptom Z-scores in a tsunami-affected tourist population. Psychopathology. 2009;42(3):157–164. doi: 10.1159/000207457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mason V, Andrews H, Upton D. The psychological impact of exposure to floods. Psychol Health Med. 2010;15(1):61–73. doi: 10.1080/13548500903483478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McFarlane AC, Clayer JR, Bookless CL. Psychiatric morbidity following a natural disaster: an Australian bushfire. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1997;32(5):261–268. doi: 10.1007/BF00789038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Paranjothy S, Gallacher J, Amlôt R et al. Psychosocial impact of the summer 2007 floods in England. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):145. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Aldrich DP. Building Resilience: Social Capital in Post-Disaster Recovery. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Erikson K. Everything in Its Path: Destruction of Community in the Buffalo Creek Flood. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fone D, Dunstan F, Lloyd K, Williams G, Watkins J, Palmer S. Does social cohesion modify the association between area income deprivation and mental health? A multilevel analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(2):338–345. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gapen M, Cross D, Ortigo K et al. Perceived neighborhood disorder, community cohesion, and PTSD symptoms among low‐income African Americans in an urban health setting. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2011;81(1):31–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01069.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Iwase T, Suzuki E, Fujiwara T, Takao S, Doi H, Kawachi I. Do bonding and bridging social capital have differential effects on self-rated health? A community based study in Japan. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2012;66(6):557–562. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.115592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Diez Roux AV. Neighborhoods and health: where are we and where do we go from here? Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique. 2007;55(1):13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.respe.2006.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Goldberg D, Williams P. A User’s Guide to the GHQ. Windsor, UK: NFER Nelson; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Buckner JC. The development of an instrument to measure neighborhood cohesion. Am J Community Psychol. 1988;16(6):771–791. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Robinson D, Wilkinson D. Sense of community in a remote mining town: validating a neighborhood cohesion scale. Am J Community Psychol. 1995;23(1):137–148. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Muthén B, Asparouhov T. Bayesian structural equation modeling: a more flexible representation of substantive theory. Psychol Methods. 2012;17(3):313–335. doi: 10.1037/a0026802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus: Statistical Analysis With Latent Variables: User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stern HS, Cressie N. Posterior predictive model checks for disease mapping models. Stat Med. 2000;19(17–18):2377–2397. doi: 10.1002/1097-0258(20000915/30)19:17/18<2377::aid-sim576>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Office for National Statistics. Neighbourhood statistics. 2014. Available at: http://www.neighbourhood.statistics.gov.uk. Accessed June 19, 2014.
- 22.Kawachi I, Berkman L. Social cohesion, social capital, and health. In: Berkman L, Kawachi I, editors. Social Epidemiology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 174–190. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rogers EM. Diffusion of Innovations. 4th ed. New York, NY: The Free Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Driessen G, Gunther N, Van Os J. Shared social environment and psychiatric disorder: a multilevel analysis of individual and ecological effects. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1998;33(12):606–612. doi: 10.1007/s001270050100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Silver E, Mulvey EP, Swanson JW. Neighborhood structural characteristics and mental disorder: Faris and Dunham revisited. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55(8):1457–1470. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00266-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Weich S, Blanchard M, Prince M, Burton E, Erens B, Sproston K. Mental health and the built environment: cross-sectional survey of individual and contextual risk factors for depression. Br J Psychiatry. 2002;180(5):428–433. doi: 10.1192/bjp.180.5.428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hawkins JD, Catalano RF. Communities That Care. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Durlak JA, Weissberg RP. The impact of after-school programs that promote personal and social skills. Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning, 2007. Available at: http://www.casel.org/s/the-impact-of-after-school-programs-that-promote-personal-and-social-skills.pdf. Accessed March 2, 2015.
- 29.Lenzi M, Vieno A, Perkins DD, Pastore M, Santinello M, Mazzardis S. Perceived neighborhood social resources as determinants of prosocial behavior in early adolescence. Am J Community Psychol. 2012;50(1–2):37–49. doi: 10.1007/s10464-011-9470-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Greene G, Turley R, Mann M, Amlôt R, Page L, Palmer S. Differing community responses to similar public health threats: a cross-disciplinary systematic literature review. Sci Total Environ. 2014;470–471:759–767. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2013.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Knott C. General mental and physical health. In: Craig R, Mindell J, editors. Health Survey for England 2012: Health, Social Care and Lifestyles, Vol. 1. Leeds, UK: Health and Social Care Information Center; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Elliott J, Gale CR, Parsons S, Kuh D HALCyon Study Team. Neighbourhood cohesion and mental wellbeing among older adults: a mixed methods approach. Soc Sci Med. 2014;107:44–51. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Van Dijk HM, Cramm JM, Nieboer AP. Social cohesion as perceived by community-dwelling older people: the role of individual and neighbourhood characteristics. Int J Ageing Later Life. 2014;8(2):9–31. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rubin GJ, Brewin CR, Greenberg N, Simpson J, Wessely S. Psychological and behavioural reactions to the bombings in London on 7 July 2005: cross sectional survey of a representative sample of Londoners. BMJ. 2005;331(7517):606. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38583.728484.3A. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Parker D, Tapsell S, McCarthy S. Enhancing the human benefits of flood warnings. Nat Hazards (Dordr) 2007;43(3):397–414. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tunstall S, Tapsell S, Green C, Floyd P, George C. The health effects of flooding: social research results from England and Wales. J Water Health. 2006;4(3):365–380. doi: 10.2166/wh.2006.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]