Skip to main content
American Journal of Public Health logoLink to American Journal of Public Health
. 2015 Sep;105(9):1784–1791. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302704

Vulnerable Bullies: Perpetration of Peer Harassment Among Youths Across Sexual Orientation, Weight, and Disability Status

Marla E Eisenberg 1,, Amy L Gower 1, Barbara J McMorris 1, Michaela M Bucchianeri 1
PMCID: PMC4539843  PMID: 26180987

Abstract

Objectives. We examined perpetration of bullying among youths in vulnerable groups relative to youths in peer groups not categorized as vulnerable.

Methods. Data were collected in 2013 from a large school-based survey of adolescents conducted in Minnesota (n = 122 180). We used the χ2 test and logistic regression to compare measures of perpetration of physical and relational bullying, as well as experiences of victimization and perpetration (or both), across categories of sexual orientation, weight status, and disability status.

Results. Rates of physical and relational bullying perpetration were significantly higher among youths in vulnerable groups than among those not in vulnerable groups. With respect to context of victimization experiences, young men and women from vulnerable groups were overrepresented in the group comprising both perpetrators and victims. For example, odds of being both a perpetrator and a victim were 1.41 to 3.22 times higher among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths than among heterosexual youths.

Conclusions. Vulnerable youths, who are prone to peer harassment, may also act as perpetrators of bullying. Prevention strategies should address the particular needs of these populations; targeted programming may be appropriate.


Bullying or peer harassment is defined as any behavior that is aggressive, repetitive, and involves an imbalance of power, and it can include acts that are physical, verbal, or relational.1 Nationally, 1 in 5 high school students have been bullied on school property.2 More than half of sixth-grade students in a large statewide sample reported that another student or group of students made fun of them or excluded them socially, including approximately 18% who reported that such experiences occurred about once per week or more frequently.3 Young people who are victimized are also commonly perpetrators of peer harassment.4,5 A robust literature demonstrates that involvement with bullying, as the victim, perpetrator, or both, has lasting negative educational, physical, social, and emotional consequences.6–16

Young people from particular groups, such as those who are gay, lesbian, or bisexual (GLB); those who are overweight; and those who have a physical disability or emotional or behavioral problem, report grossly higher levels of victimization than other young people.17–21 Weight, perceived sexual orientation, and ability in school are the most commonly observed “reasons” for harassment among students.22,23 Research has shown that these populations are especially vulnerable to poor psychosocial outcomes resulting from harassment.24–30

Very little research, however, has examined perpetration of peer harassment among young people from these vulnerable groups, and findings from the existing studies conflict. One study showed that rates of bullying perpetration were significantly lower among GLB American youths than among their heterosexual peers, whereas another study showed that rates were higher among GLB Australian college students than their heterosexual counterparts.17,31 Janssen et al. found that overweight and obese Canadian youths 15 to 16 years old, but not those in the 11- to 14-year age group, were more likely to report bullying perpetration than their normal-weight classmates.32

Young people with disabilities or chronic health conditions have received the most attention in the bullying perpetration literature, and the focus has been on conditions with a behavioral component such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.33 For example, Swearer et al. found that students with behavioral disorders and those with observable disabilities reported bullying others more than students in general education classes.34

Youths who may be particularly vulnerable to harassment owing to their real or perceived sexual orientation, weight status, or disability status may be predominantly thought of as victims, and therefore their roles, experiences, and needs as perpetrators may be overlooked. Understanding bullying perpetration among these young people, including in the context of victimization, is essential to the development of effective prevention and intervention programs. In this study, we used a large statewide survey of adolescents to examine rates of perpetration of peer harassment among youths in vulnerable groups and those in peer groups not categorized as vulnerable.

METHODS

We derived our data from the 2013 Minnesota Student Survey (MSS), a statewide survey of 5th-, 8th-, 9th-, and 11th-grade students. All public school districts were invited to take part, and 280 of 334 districts in the state (84%) participated in 2013. Across all grades, 162 034 students took part in the survey (approximately 67% of enrolled students). Parents were notified of the administration of the survey and could choose not to provide consent. Data collection was anonymous. The survey was administered via paper or pencil (65%) or via computer (35%) according to schools’ preferences; items were identical in the 2 formats.

As with previous waves of the MSS, information on gender was missing, responses were highly inconsistent, or there was a pattern of likely exaggeration in approximately 2% of surveys, and these surveys were eliminated from our analyses. The fifth-grade version did not include all items relevant to our analyses, and thus the data from this survey were excluded. The remaining sample included 61 341 male (50.2%) and 60 839 female (49.8%) students in grades 8, 9, and 11. In addition, the eighth-grade survey did not include sexual orientation items, so data from that survey were excluded from analyses specifically focusing on sexual orientation.

Survey and Measures

The MSS includes a wide variety of health behavior measures as well as related measures focusing on risk and protective factors. The survey was revised in 2013 with input from experts in multiple fields to incorporate items addressing issues not previously assessed but that have come to be of interest in recent years. When possible, items were drawn from other large youth health surveys or validated scales.35

Vulnerable groups.

Two items were used to group students according to sexual orientation: one focusing on sexual identity (i.e., students described themselves as heterosexual, gay or lesbian, bisexual, or not sure) and one focusing on behavior (i.e., students reported the gender of their sexual partners in the preceding year). Because many in this age group were not yet sexually active, preference was given to sexual identity in defining groups. However, participants who identified themselves as heterosexual but also reported same-sex experiences were categorized as “discordant heterosexuals” on the basis of previous research demonstrating that the health profile of this group is more similar to nonheterosexuals than to heterosexuals.36–39 Participants who were not consistently heterosexual are referred to here as LGBQ (lesbian, gay, bisexual, or questioning).

Students’ self-reported height in inches and weight in pounds were used to determine weight status, and these measurements were converted to body mass indexes (BMIs) via the standard formula. Students with BMIs in the 85th to less than the 95th percentile and those with BMIs at or above the 95th percentile (according to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s recommended cutpoints for gender and age) were classified as overweight and obese, respectively.40,41

Two survey items were used to determine disability status.42,43 First, students were asked whether they had any physical disability or long-term (≥ 6 months) health problem (e.g., asthma, cancer, diabetes, epilepsy). Second, they were asked whether they had any long-term mental health, behavioral, or emotional problems. Those with a physical disability, a mental health or behavioral–emotional problem, or both were compared with students who reported neither type of problem.

Perpetration of peer harassment.

Harassment experiences were categorized in several ways. Four items asked students to report how many times in the past 30 days they had engaged in physical bullying of another student (i.e., “pushed, shoved, slapped, hit, or kicked someone when you weren’t kidding around” or “threatened to beat someone up”) or had been involved in relational bullying of another student (i.e., “spread mean rumors or lies about someone else” or “excluded someone from friends, other students, or activities”). Response options ranged from never to every day. Those reporting any perpetration of bullying were compared with those who reported never having engaged in bullying given our previous work indicating that even infrequent bullying involvement (1–2 times per month) is associated with increased levels of a variety of adjustment problems in adolescence.3 A third peer harassment variable (any bullying) was created to compare those who perpetrated physical or relational bullying and those who did not report either type of bullying.

Because of existing literature indicating that youths involved in bullying as both victims and perpetrators are at elevated risk of poor psychosocial outcomes, we also examined perpetration of peer harassment in conjunction with victimization experiences.44–47 Parallel questions regarding victimization as a result of physical and relational harassment were used, and measures of any type of victimization and any type of perpetration were combined to group participants into one of 4 mutually exclusive categories: no involvement, victim, perpetrator, or both perpetrator and victim (hereafter perpetrator/victim).

Demographic covariates.

Self-reported data were used to assess grade in school, racial/ethnic group, and poverty status. Participants reported all that applied of 5 race groups; race categories were combined with a separate item assessing Hispanic ethnicity to create a single 7-category variable (Hispanic and non-Hispanic American Indian, Asian, Black, Pacific Islander, White, and multiple race). We used 3 items to determine poverty status: whether participants received free or reduced-price lunches; whether, in the preceding year, they had stayed in a shelter, somewhere not intended as a place to live, or someone else’s home because they had no other place to stay; and whether they had been forced to skip meals in the preceding 30 days because their family did not have enough money to buy food. Students who responded affirmatively to one or more of these items were included in the poverty group. We also used school location in the Minneapolis/St. Paul metropolitan area versus school location elsewhere in the state as a control variable.

Data Analysis

The percentage of missing data on items addressing sexual orientation, disability status, and physical and relational bullying victimization and perpetration ranged from 1.7% to 2.1% and was considered negligible. However, 12% of students had missing data on BMI and could not be categorized according to weight status. To determine the representativeness of our analytic sample, we used the χ2 test to compare those who were and were not included in analyses of weight status. The members of the analytic sample did not differ from those with missing data on overweight status with respect to bullying or victimization experiences (χ2 = 3.8; P = .281). However, significant differences were found for all demographic variables. For example, the percentage of missing data on weight ranged from 9.0% among White students to 24.3% among African American students (χ2 = 2762.0; P < .001). Other significant differences were more modest; for example, 11.3% of male participants and 12.6% of female participants were missing data on weight (χ2 = 45.5; P < .001).

We used the χ2 test of association to detect differences in the prevalence of physical, relational, or any bullying perpetration across categories of vulnerable youths. To examine perpetration in the context of victimization experiences, we conducted similar bivariate analyses focusing on the combination of perpetration and victimization across vulnerable groups.

Finally, we used logistic regression to generate odds ratios of membership in each bullying involvement group for each category of vulnerable youths (relative to students in peer groups not categorized as vulnerable) after adjustment for our demographic covariates. Interaction tests indicated that gender significantly modified the association between vulnerability and bullying involvement in a majority of the models (i.e., more than expected owing to chance); thus, we stratified all of our analyses by gender. To ascertain whether certain analytic choices affected the results, we conducted supplemental analyses in which we repeated all of our tests with different categorizations of independent and dependent variables (e.g., weekly bullying involvement vs any involvement, all LGBQ participants combined). Because of the very large sample size, we used an alpha level of .001 to infer significant associations.

RESULTS

The sample was approximately evenly distributed across the 8th, 9th, and 11th grades, and a majority of the participants (73.7%) were non-Hispanic White. The sample included similar percentages of non-Hispanic Asian (5.4%), non-Hispanic Black (5.3%), multiple-race (6.9%), and Hispanic (7.3%) youths. Almost one third (30.6%) of the participants reported 1 or more poverty indicators, and slightly more than half (53.1%) resided in the Minneapolis/St. Paul metropolitan area.

The majority of both male and female participants were heterosexual, were not overweight, and did not have a disability (Table 1). Almost one quarter of the participants reported any bullying perpetration in the preceding 30 days; boys were more likely to engage in physical bullying and girls in relational bullying. Fewer than half of the sample members were involved in bullying in some capacity, most commonly as a victim only or as a perpetrator/victim. Approximately one third of male participants and half of female participants reported victimization.

TABLE 1—

Vulnerable Groups and Bullying Perpetration: Minnesota Student Survey Participants, 2013

Male, No. (%) Female, No. (%)
Vulnerable group
Sexual orientationa
 Heterosexual 36 166 (92.6) 35 549 (91.4)
 Discordant heterosexual 956 (2.5) 271 (0.7)
 Bisexual 528 (1.4) 1 722 (4.4)
 Gay/lesbian 395 (1.0) 269 (0.7)
 Unsure 1 016 (2.6) 1 066 (2.7)
Overweight
 Not overweight 40 262 (74.0) 43 758 (82.3)
 85th–< 95th percentile 7 734 (14.2) 6 229 (11.7)
 ≥ 95th percentile 6 400 (11.8) 3 202 (6.0)
Disability
 None 47 649 (79.8) 43 988 (73.5)
 Physical 6 380 (10.7) 7 014 (11.7)
 Mental health/emotional 3 978 (6.7) 6 088 (10.2)
 Both 1 673 (2.8) 2 781 (4.6)
Bullying perpetration/involvement
Bullying perpetration
 Physical 9 932 (16.6) 5 539 (9.2)
 Relational 7 895 (13.2) 11 226 (18.7)
 Any 14 229 (23.8) 14 431 (24.1)
Bullying involvement
 None 34 474 (58.2) 28 486 (47.8)
 Victim only 10 695 (18.1) 16 736 (28.1)
 Perpetrator only 4 614 (7.8) 2 718 (4.6)
 Perpetrator/victim 9 481 (16.0) 11 635 (19.5)
Any victimization 20 176 (34.1) 28 371 (47.6)
a

Questions on sexual orientation were not asked of eighth-grade students.

In almost all cases, rates of bullying perpetration were significantly higher among young people in vulnerable groups than among those not in vulnerable groups (Table 2). For example, 14.3% of heterosexual male participants reported perpetrating physical bullying, whereas rates in LGBQ groups ranged from 18.9% to 27.8% (χ2 = 211.5; P < .001). About 1 in 5 female students without a disability (21.5%) reported engaging in any bullying perpetration, as compared with more than a third of female students with a mental health or behavioral–emotional problem (36.4%) or both a physical disability and a mental health or behavioral problem (38.1%; χ2 = 962.0; P < .001). The only exception was that overweight and obese youths were not disproportionately involved in perpetration of relational bullying.

TABLE 2—

Perpetration of Bullying by Vulnerability Group: Minnesota Student Survey Participants, 2013

Male Students
Female Students
Vulnerability Group Physical, % or χ2 (P) Relational, % or χ2 (P) Any, % or χ2 (P) Physical, % or χ2 (P) Relational, % or χ2 (P) Any, % or χ2 (P)
Sexual orientation 211.5 (< .001) 237.8 (< .001) 191.9 (< .001) 749.7 (< .001) 69.4 (< .001) 277.4 (< .001)
 Heterosexual 14.3 11.7 21.2 6.9 16.5 20.6
 Discordant heterosexual 27.8 20.4 34.4 16.5 26.2 31.7
 Bisexual 23.6 20.6 32.9 23.2 22.1 35.5
 Gay/lesbian 18.9 27.4 31.4 23.9 22.4 35.4
 Unsure 22.8 20.4 30.4 13.5 20.6 26.9
Weight status 109.3 (< .001) 0.8 (.677) 39.4 (< .001) 312.1 (< .001) 3.5 (.170) 84.8 (< .001)
 Not overweight 15.2 13.3 23.0 7.9 19.0 23.5
 85th–< 95th percentile 18.1 13.0 25.0 12.0 20.0 27.2
 ≥ 95th percentile 19.9 13.1 26.2 15.6 19.5 29.1
Disability 681.8 (< .001) 273.1 (< .001) 576.7 (< .001) 1257.2 (< .001) 388.0 (< .001) 962.0 (< .001)
 None 15.0 12.4 22.2 7.2 17.2 21.5
 Physical 17.4 14.0 25.0 8.8 19.3 24.2
 Mental health/emotional 25.7 18.4 34.3 18.3 26.0 36.4
 Both 34.0 23.4 41.0 20.7 26.7 38.1

Table 3 shows the distribution of perpetration and victimization experiences across vulnerable groups, and the results reveal significant differences among both male and female participants. With few exceptions, perpetration-only rates differed minimally across groups; however, vulnerable youths were consistently overrepresented in the perpetrator/victim group. For example, 4% to 5% of female youths reported perpetration only, regardless of disability status, but rates of perpetration/victimization were significantly higher among female youths with a mental health or emotional problem (31.7%) or both types of disability (34.0%) than among those with no disability (16.9%; χ2 = 2242.9; P < .001).

TABLE 3—

Involvement in Peer Harassment by Vulnerability Group: Minnesota Student Survey Participants, 2013

Male Participants
Female Participants
Vulnerability Group None, % Victim, % Perpetrator, % Perpetrator/Victim, % χ2 (P) None, % Victim, % Perpetrator, % Perpetrator/Victim, % χ2 (P)
Sexual orientation 380.7 (< .001) 450.8 (< .001)
 Heterosexual 62.9 15.9 7.7 13.5 52.4 27.0 4.4 16.2
 Discordant heterosexual 49.0 16.5 11.2 23.3 35.0 33.5 3.4 28.1
 Bisexual 40.7 26.9 8.3 24.1 31.9 32.6 5.8 29.7
 Gay/lesbian 43.8 24.9 7.4 23.9 29.9 34.9 3.5 31.8
 Unsure 48.9 20.8 6.8 23.5 45.0 28.1 6.2 20.6
Weight status 40.2 (< .001) 110.7 (< .001)
 Not overweight 58.9 18.1 7.5 15.5 48.3 28.3 4.3 19.2
 85th–< 95th percentile 57.0 18.0 8.5 16.5 43.9 29.0 5.2 22.0
 ≥ 95th percentile 56.6 17.2 8.7 17.5 42.0 28.7 6.0 23.3
Disability 1384.7 (< .001) 2242.9 (< .001)
 None 61.0 16.9 7.8 14.3 52.5 26.1 4.6 16.9
 Physical 55.4 19.6 7.4 17.5 45.5 30.4 4.1 20.1
 Mental health/emotional 39.4 26.3 7.8 26.5 27.1 36.5 4.7 31.7
 Both 33.1 26.0 7.8 33.1 25.2 36.7 4.2 34.0

Logistic regression models showed that, after adjustment for sociodemographic covariates, youths in vulnerable groups had greater odds of bullying involvement, as perpetrators, victims, and particularly perpetrator/victims, than those not in vulnerable groups (Table 4). For example, odds of being a perpetrator/victim were 1.41 to 3.22 times higher among LGBQ youths than among heterosexual youths. The exception to the overall pattern was that odds of bullying involvement were not significantly higher among overweight and obese boys after adjustment for covariates. Supplemental analyses involving different categorizations of bullying and vulnerability showed a pattern of results very similar to that described here (the findings of these analyses are available on request).

TABLE 4—

Odds of Bullying Involvement Adjusted for Grade, Race/Ethnicity, Poverty Status, and Metropolitan School Location: Minnesota Student Survey, 2013

Vulnerability Group Victim Only,a OR (99.9% CI) Perpetrator Only,a OR (99.9% CI) Perpetrator/Victim,a OR (99.9% CI)
Male participants
Sexual orientation
 Heterosexual (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Discordant heterosexual 1.31 (0.96, 1.81) 1.74* (1.20, 2.53) 2.06* (1.55, 2.74)
 Bisexual 2.60* (1.78, 3.78) 1.68 (0.95, 2.98) 2.56* (1.72, 3.82)
 Gay/lesbian 2.24* (1.44, 3.47) 1.33 (0.67, 2.66) 2.52* (1.61, 3.95)
 Unsure 1.65* (1.23, 2.21) 1.16 (0.74, 1.81) 2.13* (1.61, 2.84)
Weight status
 Not overweight (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00
 85th–< 95th percentile 1.01 (0.90, 1.13) 1.12 (0.96, 1.31) 1.06 (0.94, 1.19)
 ≥ 95th percentile 0.97 (0.85, 1.09) 1.13 (0.96, 1.34) 1.11 (0.98, 1.27)
Disability
 None (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Physical 1.25* (1.11, 1.40) 1.03 (0.86, 1.22) 1.30* (1.15, 1.47)
 Mental health/emotional 2.38* (2.20, 3.44) 1.47* (1.18, 1.83) 2.77* (2.39, 3.20)
 Both 2.38* (2.06, 2.74) 1.68* (1.20, 2.36) 3.93* (3.17, 4.86)
Female participants
Sexual orientation
 Heterosexual (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Discordant heterosexual 1.80* (1.09, 2.97) 1.03 (0.30, 3.50) 2.61* (1.54, 4.44)
 Bisexual 1.90* (1.54, 2.34) 1.94* (1.33, 2.85) 2.68* (2.15, 3.34)
 Gay/lesbian 2.22* (1.31, 3.76) 1.35 (0.42, 4.35) 3.22* (1.87, 5.56)
 Unsure 1.20 (0.93, 1.55) 1.63* (1.03, 2.57) 1.41* (1.06, 1.88)
Weight status
 Not overweight (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00
 85th–< 95th percentile 1.07 (0.96, 1.19) 1.21* (0.98, 1.50) 1.14* (1.01, 1.29)
 ≥ 95th percentile 1.08 (0.93, 1.26) 1.33* (1.01, 1.76) 1.18* (1.00, 1.39)
Disability
 None (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Physical 1.32* (1.19, 1.46) 1.02 (0.81, 1.27) 1.33* (1.19, 1.50)
 Mental health/emotional 2.69* (2.39, 3.02) 1.98* (1.57, 2.50) 3.59* (3.17, 4.06)
 Both 2.83* (2.39, 3.36) 1.76* (1.24, 2.50) 3.93* (3.29, 4.69)

Note. CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio.

a

Versus the no bullying involvement group.

*P < .001.

DISCUSSION

The primary aim of this study was to examine rates of bullying perpetration among young people who are often considered vulnerable to harassment victimization as a result of their sexual orientation, weight, or ability status. Our analyses indicated that youths in these groups are more likely than their peers without these characteristics to be involved in bullying, particularly as perpetrator/victims. With limited exceptions, overall patterns of results were similar among male and female participants and for each type of vulnerable group.

Our findings are not entirely consistent with the existing literature on perpetration of harassment among youths. First, overall rates of bullying involvement differed from estimates reported in other research.2,3,17,45 These differences are likely attributable to the way in which bullying perpetration and victimization were measured. In our study, we combined 2 separate items regarding physical bullying and 2 items regarding relational bullying to categorize harassment experiences. Other researchers have used single-item measures,2,3,17 limited their assessments to bullying experiences on school property,2,45 or used broad or narrow definitions of bullying (e.g., saying or doing nasty or unpleasant things45 vs teasing, threatening, spreading rumors about, hitting, shoving, or hurting another student repeatedly2). These nuances can result in substantial differences in prevalence estimates. Furthermore, we used a cutpoint of once or twice a month to indicate bullying involvement; in other studies, different response categories have been used to characterize involvement in bullying.

Second, several associations between perpetration and vulnerability status found in the present study differed from previous work. For example, in a large study of US adolescents, Berlan et al. found lower rates of perpetration among GLB youths than among their heterosexual counterparts.17 These differences could be attributable to measurement (as just described), sample characteristics, or the time at which data were collected. In particular, during the 12-year span (2001–2013) between the data collection periods of the Berlan et al. study and our investigation, news coverage and public discourse on the subject of bullying and its consequences increased dramatically, and there was a substantive shift in the social narrative around bullying, from portraying it as mischief to characterizing it as a criminally liable act.48 Similarly, there have been radical changes in social acceptance of the GLB community over the past decade, as well as related changes in policy.49 Differences in these sociocultural factors may contribute to the social standing of LGBQ youths and, therefore, to a different role for this group in terms of peer harassment.

Interestingly, our bivariate analyses showed that overweight and obese youths were more likely than nonoverweight youths to be involved in physical bullying but not relational bullying. The physical size of these youths may be intimidating to their peers, resulting in a physical advantage that could be used to bully others. By contrast, relational bullying relies on an individual’s position within his or her social group. Because overweight and obese youths, on average, have fewer friends than their nonoverweight peers and are rarely nominated as popular,50–52 they may have little opportunity to spread rumors or exclude others from desirable social groups or activities.

Students with emotional and behavioral disorders tend to engage in bullying at higher levels than other subgroups of students with disabilities.53,54 Their bullying involvement may be exacerbated by poor social skills, including difficulty interpreting social cues and attributing hostile characteristics to peers’ behaviors,55,56 resulting in more reactive aggression.57 A recent study showed that levels of anger and fighting were positively associated with bullying involvement among students with emotional or behavioral disabilities. In fact, bullying behaviors may be a manifestation of certain emotional and behavioral disabilities, given that anger and externalizing behaviors are included in the diagnostic criteria for behavioral disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder.58

Our finding that some LGBQ groups and youths with disabilities have higher odds of bullying perpetration than their peers who are heterosexual and do not have disabilities raises questions about the visibility of particular vulnerabilities. Unlike body size, sexual orientation and behavioral or emotional disability are not necessarily known to others. Youths with “hidden” vulnerability characteristics may bully in part to ensure that their vulnerable status remains hidden or to jockey for social position within their peer group to stave off harassment. Although we were unable to examine these distinctions, they do fit conceptually with some of our results. For example, we found that male and female participants with a discordant heterosexual orientation (i.e., those who self-identified as heterosexual but reported same-gender sexual partners) had the highest prevalence of physical and relational bullying perpetration, respectively, of any sexual orientation group. Young people in this category may harass others as a means of asserting power or social dominance and further masking their sexual orientation.

Elevated odds of perpetration among youths with disabilities may be the result of a similar mechanism: asserting one’s social position to hide a perceived weakness. Some research suggests that students with disabilities engage in bullying as a learned behavior, possibly as a reaction to prolonged victimization.54,59,60 Because of poor interpersonal relationship skills, such students engage in aggression to avoid long-term victimization. This may explain the higher rates of both perpetration and victimization among students with multiple types of disabilities.54,61

Strengths and Limitations

Our study offers several contributions to the small body of research on bullying perpetration among vulnerable youths. The very large sample size allowed for identification and statistically valid analyses of youths in small or hard-to-reach groups (e.g., LGBQ youths). Our inclusion of 3 types of vulnerable groups in the same study permitted a broader view of vulnerability in relation to peer harassment than that of previous studies and made it possible to detect patterns of associations that were common across groups. Similarly, our use of multiple items to assess sexual orientation and disability status resulted in a more accurate classification of participants into vulnerability groups than has been the case in previous research.17,20

However, our study is also subject to certain limitations. First, the cross-sectional nature of the surveillance data set precluded conclusions about causation. Although our findings might indicate that young people in vulnerable groups are striking out against others, we also speculate that, on the basis of their experiences as victims, youths with these vulnerable characteristics are more attuned to harassment overall. Greater awareness of bullying may then contribute to overreporting among these youths in comparison with young people without vulnerable characteristics, who are less commonly victimized.

The use of self-report measures of height and weight to generate weight categories was a second limitation. Although this was the only feasible approach with a sample the size of ours, studies have demonstrated that self-reported height and weight may lead to misclassification of weight status,62,63 which could bias associations toward the null. There was also an unusually high level of missing data on weight status relative to the other variables we assessed. Thus, our findings regarding overweight may be most relevant to particular students (those who were White, older, not experiencing poverty, and living in the Minneapolis/St. Paul metropolitan area) who were more likely than others to provide BMI data. In addition, our self-reported measures of disability status were not validated with secondary sources such as parents’ reports or formal diagnoses.64 Students’ self-reported disability status may not be recognized by schools with an official label, and such students may not qualify for special education services.

Third, although our use of a large surveillance instrument allowed for identification of young people with various types of vulnerability, the instrument did not include in-depth questions about these characteristics. In particular, information on the extent to which vulnerability characteristics were known to others (e.g., through disclosure of sexual orientation or a visible disability) could allow for deeper inquiry into the social dynamics surrounding peer harassment among these youths. Finally, the use of school-based data collection procedures may underrepresent youths who drop out of school, attend alternative schools, or have frequent absences owing to a chronic health condition or experiences with peer harassment.

Conclusions

Existing research has established that the well-being of perpetrators of peer harassment can be severely compromised, and those who are both perpetrators and victims are particularly at risk. However, research involving vulnerable adolescent populations has focused almost exclusively on their experiences as victims. The findings of this study deepen our understanding of bullying involvement among vulnerable youths and indicate that they may be acting as perpetrators in this social dynamic as well or that perpetration is more salient to them than to young people without a particular vulnerability. Health care providers and other youth service professionals, particularly those working with vulnerable populations, should inquire about young people’s involvement with bullying as perpetrators as well as victims. Bullying prevention programs implemented in schools or other community settings should address the particular needs of vulnerable populations. Regardless of the direction of the associations observed here, targeted programming for specific groups may be appropriate.

Further research is also needed to replicate and extend our findings regarding peer harassment experiences among youths in vulnerable groups. Understanding who is being targeted by perpetrators from vulnerable groups (e.g., someone else from the same group or a younger person with no particular vulnerability characteristics) will further inform intervention activities. In addition, there is a need for further research on the physical and mental health implications of perpetration among youths in vulnerable groups. Longitudinal studies building on our cross-sectional findings are needed to shed light on the temporal ordering of bullying involvement and ways in which visibility of particular vulnerabilities may influence victimization or perpetration among these youths. Understanding similarities or differences in the effects of bullying perpetration on youths in vulnerable groups and youths in other groups will guide recommendations for care of young people and prevention of bullying.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by grant R40 MC 26815 (M. E. Eisenberg, PI) from the Maternal and Child Health Research Program, Health Resources and Services Administration, US Department of Health and Human Services. A. L. Gower was supported by a National Research Service Award in Primary Medical Care (grant T32HP22239).

Minnesota Student Survey data were provided by public school students in Minnesota via local public school districts and managed by the Minnesota Student Survey Interagency Team.

Human Participant Protection

No protocol approval was needed for this study because existing anonymous data were used.

References

  • 1.US Department of Health and Human Services. Bullying Prevention Campaign. Available at: http://mchb.hrsa.gov/programs/bullying. Accessed April 30, 2015.
  • 2.Kann L, Kinchen S, Shanklin SL et al. Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2013. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2014;63(suppl 4):1–168. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gower AL, Borowsky IW. Associations between frequency of bullying involvement and adjustment in adolescence. Acad Pediatr. 2013;13(3):214–221. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2013.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wang J, Iannotti RJ, Nansel TR. School bullying among adolescents in the United States: physical, verbal, relational, and cyber. J Adolesc Health. 2009;45(4):368–375. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Borowsky IW, Taliaferro L, McMorris BJ. Suicidal thinking and behavior among youth involved in verbal and social bullying: risk and protective factors. J Adolesc Health. 2013;53(suppl 1):S4–S12. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.10.280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Copeland WE, Wolke D, Angold A, Costello EJ. Adult psychiatric outcomes of bullying and being bullied by peers in childhood and adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(4):419–426. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hensley V. Childhood bullying: a review and implications for health care professionals. Nurs Clin North Am. 2013;48(2):203–213. doi: 10.1016/j.cnur.2013.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Fekkes M, Pijpers FIM, Fredriks M, Vogels T, Verloove-Vanhorick SP. Do bullied children get ill, or do ill children get bullied? A prospective cohort study on the relationship between bullying and health-related symptoms. Pediatrics. 2006;117(5):1568–1574. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Vernberg EM, Nelson TD, Fonagy P, Twemlow SW. Victimization, aggression, and visits to the school nurse for somatic complaints, illnesses, and physical injuries. Pediatrics. 2011;127(5):842–848. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-3415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ragatz LL, Anderson RJ, Fremouw W, Schwartz R. Criminal thinking patterns, aggression styles, and the psychopathic traits of late high school bullies and bully-victims. Aggress Behav. 2011;37(2):145–160. doi: 10.1002/ab.20377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kim MJ, Catalano RF, Haggerty KP, Abbott RD. Bullying at elementary school and problem behaviour in young adulthood: a study of bullying, violence and substance use from age 11 to age 21. Crim Behav Ment Health. 2011;21(2):136–144. doi: 10.1002/cbm.804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sourander A, Jensen P, Ronning JA et al. Childhood bullies and victims and their risk of criminality in late adolescence: the Finnish from a Boy to a Man Study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(6):546–552. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.6.546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ttofi MM, Farrington DP, Lösel F. The predictive efficiency of school bullying versus later offending: a systematic/meta-analytic review of longitudinal studies. Crim Behav Ment Health. 2011;21(2):80–89. doi: 10.1002/cbm.808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Olweus D. Bullying at school and later criminality: findings from three Swedish community samples of males. Crim Behav Ment Health. 2011;21(2):151–156. doi: 10.1002/cbm.806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hemphill SA, Kotevski A, Herrenkohl TI et al. Longitudinal consequences of adolescent bullying perpetration and victimisation: A study of students in Victoria, Australia. Crim Behav Ment Health. 2011;21(2):107–116. doi: 10.1002/cbm.802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Løhre A, Lydersen S, Paulsen B, Mæhle M, Vatten LJ. Peer victimization as reported by children, teachers, and parents in relation to children’s health symptoms. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):278. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Berlan ED, Corliss HL, Field AE, Goodman E, Austin SB. Sexual orientation and bullying among adolescents in the Growing Up Today Study. J Adolesc Health. 2010;46(4):366–371. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Neumark-Sztainer D, Falkner N, Story M, Perry C, Hannan PJ, Mulert S. Weight-teasing among adolescents: correlations with weight status and disordered eating behaviors. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002;26(1):123–131. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Puhl RM, Heuer C. The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2009;17(5):941–964. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sterzing PR, Shattuck PT, Narendorf SC, Wagner M, Cooper BP. Bullying involvement and autism spectrum disorders: prevalence and correlates of bullying involvement among adolescents with an autism spectrum disorder. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(11):1058–1064. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Blake JJ, Lund EM, Zhou Q, Kwok O, Benz MR. National prevalence rates of bullying victimization among students with disabilities in the United States. Sch Psychol Q. 2012;27(4):210–222. doi: 10.1037/spq0000008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Puhl RM, Luedicke J, Heuer C. Weight-based victimization toward overweight adolescents: observations and reactions of peers. J Sch Health. 2011;81(11):696–703. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2011.00646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rose CA, Monda-Amaya LE, Espelage DL. Bullying perpetration and victimization in special education: a review of the literature. Remedial Spec Educ. 2011;32(2):114–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D, Haines J, Wall M. Weight-teasing and emotional well-being in adolescents: longitudinal findings from Project EAT. J Adolesc Health. 2006;38(6):675–683. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M. Associations of weight-based teasing and emotional well-being among adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003;157(8):733–738. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.8.733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Barnes AJ, Eisenberg ME, Resnick MD. Suicide and self-injury among children and youth with chronic health conditions. Pediatrics. 2010;125(5):889–895. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-1814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Almeida J, Johnson RM, Corliss HL, Molnar BE, Azrael D. Emotional distress among LGBT youth: the influence of perceived discrimination based on sexual orientation. J Youth Adolesc. 2009;38(7):1001–1014. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9397-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Birkett M, Espelage DL, Koenig B. LGB and questioning students in schools: the moderating effects of homophobic bullying and school climate on negative outcomes. J Youth Adolesc. 2009;38(7):989–1000. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9389-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Taylor LA, Saylor C, Twyman K, Macias M. Adding insult to injury: bullying experiences of youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child Health Care. 2010;39(1):59–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cappadocia MC, Weiss JA, Pepler D. Bullying experiences among children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(2):266–277. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1241-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wensley K, Campbell M. Heterosexual and nonheterosexual young university students’ involvement in traditional and cyber forms of bullying. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2012;15(12):649–654. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Janssen I, Craig WM, Boyce WF, Pickett W. Associations between overweight and obesity with bullying behaviors in school-aged children. Pediatrics. 2004;113(5):1187–1194. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.5.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kumpulaninen K, Rasanen E, Puura K. Psychiatric disorders and the use of mental health services among children involved in bullying. Aggress Behav. 2001;27(2):101–110. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Swearer SM, Wang C, Maag JW, Siebecker AB, Frerichs LJ. Understanding the bullying dynamic among students in special and general education. J Sch Psychol. 2012;50(4):503–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2012.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.California Department of Education, WestEd. California Healthy Kids Survey. Available at: http://chks.wested.org. Accessed April 30, 2015.
  • 36.Corliss HL, Goodenow CS, Nichols L, Austin SB. High burden of homelessness among sexual-minority adolescents: findings from a representative Massachusetts high school sample. Am J Public Health. 2011;101(9):1683–1689. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cochran SD, Mays VM. Physical health complaints among lesbians, gay men, and bisexual and homosexually experienced heterosexual individuals: results from the California Quality of Life Survey. Am J Public Health. 2007;97(11):2048–2055. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.087254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Cochran SD, Mays VM. Burden of psychiatric morbidity among lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals in the California Quality of Life Survey. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009;118(3):647–658. doi: 10.1037/a0016501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Laska MN, VanKim NA, Lust KD, Erickson DJ, Eisenberg ME, Rosser B. Disparities in weight and weight behaviors by sexual orientation in college students. Am J Public Health. 2015;105(1):111–121. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. About BMI for children and teens. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/childrens_bmi/about_childrens_bmi.html. Accessed April 30, 2015.
  • 41.Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011–2012. JAMA. 2014;311(8):806–814. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Youth Risk Behavior Survey, 2005. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/HealthyYouth/yrbs/index.htm. Accessed April 30, 2015.
  • 43.Edwards TC, Patrick DL, Topolski TD. Quality of life of adolescents with perceived disabilities. J Pediatr Psychol. 2003;28(4):233–241. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gini G, Pozzoli T. Association between bullying and psychosomatic problems: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2009;123(3):1059–1065. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Nansel TR, Overpeck M, Pilla RS, Ruan WJ, Simons-Morton B. Bullying behaviors among US youth: prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. JAMA. 2001;285(16):2094–2100. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.16.2094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Juvonen J, Graham S, Schuster MA. Bullying among young adolescents: the strong, the weak and the troubled. Pediatrics. 2003;112(6):1231–1237. doi: 10.1542/peds.112.6.1231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Brunstein Klomek A, Marrocco F, Kleinman M, Schonfeld IS, Gould MS. Bullying, depression, and suicidality in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(1):40–49. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000242237.84925.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wayne R. The social construction of childhood bullying through US news media. J Contemp Anthropol. 2013;4(1):37–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Pew Research Center. LGBT in changing times: attitudes, experiences, and growing acceptance. Available at: http://www.pewresearch.org/packages/lgbt-in-changing-times. Accessed April 30, 2015.
  • 50.Strauss RS, Pollack HA. Social marginalization of overweight children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003;157(8):746–752. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.8.746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Schaefer DR, Simpkins SD. Using social network analysis to clarify the role of obesity in selection of adolescent friends. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(7):1223–1229. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wang SS, Houshyar S, Prinstein MJ. Adolescent girls’ and boys’ weight-related health behaviors and cognitions: associations with reputation- and preference-based peer status. Health Psychol. 2006;25(5):658–663. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Monchy MD, Pijl SJ, Zandburg T. Discrepancies in judging social inclusion and bullying pupils with behavioural problems. Eur J Spec Needs Educ. 2004;19(3):317–330. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Van Cleave J, Davis MM. Bullying and peer victimization among children with special health care needs. Pediatrics. 2006;118(4):e1212–e1219. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-3034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Crick NR, Dodge KA. Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Dev. 1996;67(3):993–1002. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dodge KA, Lansford JE, Burks VS et al. Peer rejection and social information processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior problems in children. Child Dev. 2003;74(2):374–393. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Rose CA. Bullying among students with disabilities: impact and implications. In: Espelage DL, Swearer SM, editors. Bullying in North American Schools: A Socio-Ecological Perspective on Prevention and Intervention. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Routledge; 2011. pp. 34–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rose CA, Espelage DL. Risk and protective factors associated with the bullying involvement of students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Behav Disord. 2012;37(3):133–148. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Camodeca M, Goossens FA. Aggression, social cognitions, anger and sadness in bullies and victims. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(2):186–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00347.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Nabuzoka D. Teacher ratings and peer nominations of bullying and other behavior of children with and without learning difficulties. Educ Psychol. 2003;23(3):307–321. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kaukiainen A, Salmivalli C, Lagerspetz K et al. Learning difficulties, social intelligence, and self-concept: connections to bully-victim problems. Scand J Psychol. 2002;43(3):269–278. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Fonseca H, Silva M, Matos MG et al. Validity of BMI based on self-reported weight and height in adolescents. Acta Paediatr. 2010;99(1):83–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2009.01518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Flood V, Webb K, Lazarus R, Pang G. Use of self-report to monitor overweight and obesity in populations: some issues for consideration. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2000;24(1):96–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2000.tb00733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Topolski TD, Edwards TC, Patrick DL. Toward youth self-report of health and quality of life in population monitoring. Ambul Pediatr. 2004;4(suppl 4):387–394. doi: 10.1367/A03-131R.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of American Public Health Association

RESOURCES