Abstract
Over the years the field of neuroanatomy has evolved considerably but unraveling the extraordinary structural and functional complexity of the brain seems to be an unattainable goal, partly due to the fact that it is only possible to obtain an imprecise connection matrix of the brain. The reasons why reaching such a goal appears almost impossible to date is discussed here, together with suggestions of how we could overcome this anatomical problem by establishing new methodologies to study the brain and by promoting interdisciplinary collaboration. Generating a realistic computational model seems to be the solution rather than attempting to fully reconstruct the whole brain or a particular brain region.
Keywords: neuron doctrine, electron microscopy, connectome, synaptome, choice of species for studying the brain, interdisciplinary approaches
The Magnitude of the Problem
“Gentlemen, instead of promising to satisfy your curiosity about the anatomy of the brain, I intend here to make the sincere, public confession that this is a subject on which I know nothing at all.”
—Opening words of the “Discours sur l’anatomie du cerveau” delivered by Nicolaus Steno (1638–1686) in 1669 (Steno, 1669).
The words used by Nicolaus Steno to make his point could be taken as an introductory sentence to describe the magnitude of the problem in dealing with the anatomy of the brain, not only due to the complexity of its organization, but also because our knowledge of the brain is far from complete. The central nervous system works as a whole (Figure 1), and it is well established that the principles of structural design (spatial distribution, number and types of neurons, and synapses per volume, etc.) differ considerably in the different parts of the nervous system, as well as between species and strains. There is also considerable variation associated with age. Indeed, the early postnatal brain is structurally and functionally different from adolescent, young adult and older brain (Jacobs and Scheibel, 1993; Kolb et al., 1998; Marner et al., 2003; Stark et al., 2007; Feldmeyer and Radnikow, 2009; Workman et al., 2013; Luebke et al., 2015). Moreover, there is great interindividual variability in brain size, cortical thickness, number of cells, differences in dendritic trees, etc. (Jacobs et al., 1993; Uylings et al., 2005; Caspers et al., 2006; DeFelipe, 2011), as well as gender differences in multiple regions of the brain (Jacobs et al., 1993; Cahill, 2006; Alonso-Nanclares et al., 2008; Jazin and Cahill, 2010; Luders and Toga, 2010; Semaan and Kauffman, 2010). Therefore, the data obtained in one structure will not necessarily be applicable to another and thus, molecular, genetic and anatomical patterns must be examined separately in particular regions, species and strains, and for different ages and genders. When considering the magnitude of the problem further, solely from the neuroanatomical point of view, we must bear in mind the following considerations. Bota et al. (2003) suggests that in the mammalian central nervous system there are around 500–1000 different gray matter regions (e.g., the retina, dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus, and primary visual cortex); 2500–5000 neuron classes (e.g., retinal photoreceptors, bipolar cells, and ganglion cells); and 25,000–100,000 macroconnections between neuron classes (e.g., from retinal ganglion cells to dorsal lateral geniculate). The neuroanatomical information currently available in the literature provides data about 10% of all the possible long-range projections between the roughly 500 brain regions identified in the rat (Bota and Swanson, 2007). In addition, the vast majority of these studies only provide a qualitative vision of the projections. Thus, we are very far from obtaining a quantitative connectome map. In fact, we do not yet even have a complete map, let alone a quantitative one. For example, since the seminal study of Felleman and Van Essen (1991) of the cortical projections to areas V1, V2 and V4 in the primate cerebral cortex, there has been a major increase in the number of areas reported to project to these areas (see e.g., Markov et al., 2011). As we will see below, this problem is several orders of magnitude higher when we consider the information available using electron microscopy and indeed there is virtually no quantitative electron microscopy data.
Choice of Species for Studying the Brain
Understanding the human brain is the ultimate goal but this is extremely challenging—not only because of its complexity (Figure 2) and the technical difficulties involved, but also because ethical limitations do not allow all of the necessary datasets to be acquired directly from human brains. Consequently, most of our present knowledge of brain structure and behavior has been obtained from experimental animals. The problem is that data from nonhuman brains cannot fully substitute information on humans since there are fundamental structural and behavioral aspects that are unique to humans as well as to any other species (see e.g., Oberheim et al., 2009; DeFelipe, 2011; Sherwood et al., 2012; Geschwind and Rakic, 2013; Kaas, 2013; Hofman, 2014; Rilling, 2014). Accordingly, the question remains as to how much of this nonhuman brain information can be reliably extrapolated to humans, and indeed it is important to establish what the best strategy currently is for obtaining the missing data.
It is a common belief that the use of large mammals, like monkeys or cats, as experimental animals gives more information about the human brain than would be obtained by using small mammals, like mice or rats. Some scientists propose that research using nonhuman primates is essential for bridging a hypothetical gap between the mouse and the human brain (see e.g., Geschwind and Rakic, 2013; Homman-Ludiye and Bourne, 2014). However, there is no bridge between brains; all species have different brains and we will never fully understand how the human brain is functioning by understanding, for example, how the macaque brains do. Certainly, the study of the brain of nonhuman primates has provided us with some very valuable insights about the functional organization of brain regions where generalizations can be made (see e.g., Passingham, 2009). Indeed, comparisons are useful if we want to study certain attributes that humans alone share with certain species—like the ocular dominance columns or orientation columns of the visual cortex, which are present, for instance, in monkeys and cats but not mice and rats (Horton and Adams, 2005). It is also interesting to observe that there are clear differences in the cyto- and myeloarchitecture and chemical neuroanatomy of the thalamus between different mammalian species, indicating differences in their synaptic organization (Jones, 2007b). For example, GABAergic neurons are very numerous in the ventrobasal nucleus of the thalamus of cats, monkeys and humans, but are absent in mice and rats (Arcelli et al., 1997). Thus, macaques, for example, may be considered “more similar” to humans than rodents in this respect. However, from the microanatomical and neurochemical points of view, their brains show many important differences compared to humans, which probably reflects the obvious cognitive differences between macaques and humans (Elston, 2003; Raghanti et al., 2010b; DeFelipe, 2011). For instance, there are remarkable differences between humans and other species in various aspects of the patterns of cholinergic, serotonergic and dopaminergic innervation of the cerebral cortex, like significant differences in the density and laminar distribution of fibers expressing these neurotransmitters (for a review, see Raghanti et al., 2010a). A good example is that, in humans and other primates, all cortical areas receive dopaminergic inputs, whereas in rodents there is little or no dopaminergic innervation in many cortical areas (e.g., the motor, premotor, and supplementary motor areas, and the parietal, temporal, and posterior cingulate cortex; see Berger et al., 1991). This is important to keep in mind since these neurotransmitters exert modulatory effects on a variety of cognitive functions and are involved in many neuropathologies, such as Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, depression, schizophrenia and drug addiction. In other words, these differences probably indicate evolutionary adaptations to particular functions. Thus, the functional significance of the human-specific structure should be dealt with by employing a range of specific strategies.
One such strategy is the creation of a “complete” Brain Atlas, which will be particularly useful for better understanding brain structure and function as we will be able to integrate key information about the human brain, including multi-scale anatomy [macroscale (mm), microscale (μm) and nanoscale (nm) data collection] and data from fMRI, MRI, DTI and MEG. This integrative atlas will also be an excellent tool to study brain diseases by integrating clinical data (Toga et al., 2006). Nevertheless, it should be noted that most studies on human brain structure have been carried out at the light microscopic level and for good reason; studying the human brain via electron microscopy techniques presents certain problems. The scarcity of human brain tissue that is suitable for the study of synaptic circuitry is one of the most important issues to be overcome. Autopsy may be the sole source of control tissue (i.e., from individuals without known psychiatric illnesses or brain pathologies). Since there can be a long delay between death and tissue collection (often over 5 h), the ultrastructure of the post-mortem brain tissue is generally not well preserved, which makes the tissue unsuitable for detailed quantitative analysis. This is one of the principal reasons why synaptic circuitry data for the normal human brain is so lacking. Thus, a major goal would be to use human brain tissue with post-mortem times of less than 2 h and improve the current technologies for the microanatomical analysis of the human brain by adapting methodologies that are normally used to examine the brain of experimental animals. One example of this would be improving the ultrastructural preservation of autopsy human tissue using microwave oven fixation and recently developed automated electron microscopy techniques (see e.g., Blazquez-Llorca et al., 2013; Kuwajima et al., 2013).
By contrast, the human brain shares many common features with other nonhuman mammals that might be considered as basic bricks of brain organization which by definition are common to all mammalian species. Therefore, choosing appropriate experiments to obtain strategic data that could be extrapolated to the human brain will be another major goal. For this general purpose, many neuroscientists suggest that the ideal experimental animals at present are rodents because they can be manipulated to study many aspects from genes to behavior. Furthermore, we can use relatively large numbers of animals at a relatively low cost. What follows is a discussion of the further problems that we have to face from the anatomical point of view, based on a previous publication of the author (see DeFelipe, 2010). An attempt will also be made to clarify frequent misunderstandings and wrong assumptions about certain aspects of the brain organization.
Starting Point: The Neuron Doctrine
The great enigma in the organization of the brain revolves around our need to ascertain how the nervous ramifications end and how neurons are mutually connected. Referring to a simile already mentioned, the idea was to inquire how the roots and branches of the trees in the gray matter terminate, so that in such a dense jungle, in which there are no gaps thanks to its refined complexity, the trunks, branches and leaves touch everywhere.
—Cajal (1917, p. 100; Recuerdos de mi vida)
Our schemes of how neurons function and interchange information with other neural elements is based on the central principle of neuroscience established by the Neuron Doctrine, that is, that the neuron is an independent cellular unit with an overall polarization that mediates its input-output functions (Shepherd, 1991; Jones, 1994). In general, neurons can be divided into distinct morphological and functional regions: a receptor apparatus (formed by the dendrites and cell body or soma), the emission apparatus (the axon), and the distribution apparatus (terminal axonal arborization).
Nevertheless, there are many exceptions that challenge the neuron doctrine. Concerning synaptic relationships, axons have been found to form synapses with other axons and presynaptic elements can be dendrites or somata. Thus a variety of synapses exist in addition to the “classical” axo-dendritic and axo-somatic synapses: axo-axonic, dendro-dendritic, somato-somatic, somato-dendritic, dendro-somatic, dendro-axonic and somato-axonic synapses (Peters et al., 1991). Furthermore, neurons are not only connected by point-to-point chemical synapses, but may also be coupled electrically, and the direction of transmission may be bidirectional through small channels known as gap junctions. A gap of approximately 2 nm separates the plasma membranes of neighboring neurons, and it is the presence of these gap junctions that allows the diffusion of small molecules as well as the flow of electric current (Bennett, 2000; Bennett and Zukin, 2004). Electrical interaction also occurs between neurons that are in very close proximity, even in the absence of specialized membrane structures (Anastassiou and Koch, 2015). Moreover, the transmitter released at synaptic or non-synaptic sites may diffuse and act on other synaptic contacts, or on extrasynaptic receptors (Fuxe et al., 2007). It is also known that neuromodulators (e.g., serotonin, acetylcholine and dopamine) greatly influence neuronal circuit activity. These neuromodulators are secreted by a small group of neurons, and reach large regions of the nervous system by diffusion (see below). In addition, neurosecretory cells release neurohormones, which exert their effects on many regions of the brain via the circulatory system (Fuxe et al., 2010; Marder, 2012). Glial cells have also been proposed as having a role in information processing via bidirectional glial cell-neuron signaling (Araque et al., 2014) and it is thought that neuron-astrocyte metabolic interactions play a critical role in the coupling between neuronal activity and energy metabolism (Magistretti and Allaman, 2015). In spite of this ever-increasing complexity and massively tangled organization, it should be noted that general principles or rules for the design of brain circuits do exist. In the words of Cajal (1917) when referring to the cerebral cortex: “Ese desorden aparente de la maraña cerebral, tan alejada de la regularidad y simetría de la médula espinal y cerebelo, esconde un orden profundo, sutilísimo, actualmente inaccesible”. “That apparent disorder of the cerebral tangle, so different from the regularity and symmetry of the spinal cord and of the cerebellum, hides an extremely subtle, profound organization which is at present inaccessible”. Discovering these rules is clearly of critical importance. For instance, chemical axo-dendritic synapses are by far the most common type of synapse (followed by axo-somatic synapses), at least in mammals. Other types of synapses are not found in all regions of the nervous system and when they are present, they are usually only established between certain types of neurons.
A further aspect to consider is the functional significance of the various types of overall brain connectivity. For example, a wide range of functions which need information to be transmitted quickly from one point to another rely on chemical synapses as the anatomical basis for brain wiring. Reflexes are a good example of this—their neuronal circuits give rise to quick and simple actions, which proceed automatically and subconsciously. Other functions based on point-to-point synaptic wiring are not so simple, however, including the processing of information in large but discrete circuits in the motor and sensory systems, and in those regions of the brain that are involved in complex functions such as reasoning, calculation, language and writing. Modulatory systems, on the other hand, exert their effect on many areas of the brain via many different neuronal circuits, and it is this kind of diffuse action that is involved in more “general” brain states and moods, such as sleep, attentiveness and anxiety. It can therefore be concluded that, while there are exceptions and complexities such as those described above, the neuron doctrine continues to be one of the foundations on which our concept of nervous activity is based (Shepherd, 1991; Jones, 1994).
A First Step Forward: The Connection Matrix of the Brain
As discussed above, one of the first steps towards understanding how neuronal circuits contribute to the functional organization of the brain is to define its detailed structural design and to map its connection matrix. In the words of Swanson and Bota (2010), “the wiring diagram of the nervous system’s structural connectivity provides an obligatory foundational model for understanding functional localization at molecular, cellular, systems, and behavioral organization levels”. The connectivity of the brain can be analyzed at three quite distinct levels (Sporns et al., 2005; DeFelipe, 2010):
Macroscopically, focusing on major tract connectivity, for example by examining images of the whole brain (or of large brain regions), which can even be performed in vivo by MRI or other techniques.
At an intermediate resolution as can be achieved by light microscopy, which also allows putative synaptic contacts to be mapped.
At the ultrastructural level, which can only be studied using electron microscopy and serves to map true synaptic contacts.
Thus, it has been proposed that the term “connectome” be used to refer to the map of connections at the macroscopic and mesoscopic levels and “synaptome” for the map at the ultrastructural level (DeFelipe, 2010).
Powerful methods are currently available that allow the connectome to be traced in meso- (intrinsic or local connections) and macrocircuits (long distance connections). Classical tracing methods (Jones, 2007a) can be used for this purpose, as can molecular/genetic/physiological approaches and imaging techniques, including two-photon imaging and ontogenetic techniques. The development of these techniques with the aim to include large brain structures and cell-type specificity is already providing very important advances in the knowledge of the anatomical and functional connectome in animal models (see Osten and Margrie, 2013). Furthermore, the development of retrograde and anterograde trans-synaptic tracers to directly study cell-to-cell connectivity and the combination of these tracers with in vivo imaging and optogenetic methods and/or inducible gene expression in transgenic mice will represent major advances in the anatomical/functional study of the neural circuits at the mesoscopic level (see Osakada et al., 2011).
Nevertheless, it should be noted that, in general, connectivity visualized at the light microscopic level is rather basic (e.g., connections between brain regions) and, in most cases, point-to-point connections between local neurons and between neurons or afferent fibers cannot be accurately determined (see DeFelipe, 2010). The reason for this is that when a given labeled axon is seen in contact with another labeled neuronal element, it does not necessarily mean that there is a synaptic junction as axonal boutons are adjacent to several possible synaptic targets of which only those that are labeled are visible. In addition, not all axonal boutons establish synaptic contacts and indeed a large proportion of certain axonal systems are non-synaptic, like the axons containing dopamine, noradrenaline, serotonin and acetylcholine. These axonal systems have been examined in several cortical areas of the rat, cat, monkey and human, and in a number of other regions of the central nervous system of the rat, showing a similar low frequency of synaptic contacts although this frequency varies between brain regions (reviewed in Descarries and Mechawar, 2000). However, the axonal boutons from other types of neurons may establish more than one synapse (multiple synapses). For example, it is relatively common to observe the establishment of multiple synapses by: (i) the axonal boutons from thalamocortical afferents—in the visual cortex of both cat and macaque (Freund et al., 1985, 1989); (ii) interneurons like double bouquet cells—in several areas of the macaque and human cerebral cortex (DeFelipe et al., 2006); and (iii) basket cells and dendritic-targeting cells—in the cat visual cortex (Tamás et al., 1997). Furthermore, the studies of White et al. (2004), using serial section reconstructions at the electron microscopic level of thalamocortical axons in mouse barrel cortex, have shown that although the vast majority of synapses are established by the axonal boutons or varicosities (88%), they also occurred at cylindrically shaped regions of the axonal segments (12%). Thus, the presence of a labeled terminal in close apposition with a given neuronal element can only be considered as a putative synaptic contact, whereas an inter-varicose segment of an axon may establish a synapse with an adjacent neuronal element. Keeping all of these points in mind, it is therefore clear that the available connectome diagrams are imprecise.
Electron microscopy with serial section reconstruction is the favored method for tracing the synaptome, and this technology has a proven track record for acquiring 3D data from ultrathin sections. However, it is exceedingly time-consuming and challenging to obtain long series of such sections. As a result, the reconstruction of large tissue volumes is usually not possible. The recent development of automated or semi-automated electron microscopy techniques (which require much less labor-intensive human interaction and training than conventional electron microcopy) represents an important advance in the study of the synaptome (Denk and Horstmann, 2004; Smith, 2007; Helmstaedter et al., 2008; Knott et al., 2008; Merchán-Pérez et al., 2009). For example, the 3D reconstruction method involving the combination of focused ion beam milling and scanning electron microscopy (FIB/SEM; Figures 3D–G) permits the rapid and automatic serial reconstruction of relatively large tissue volumes (Knott et al., 2008; Merchán-Pérez et al., 2009). Nevertheless, even using this FIB/SEM technology, full reconstruction of whole brains will only be possible in some invertebrates or for relatively simple nervous systems. Indeed, even for a small mammal like the mouse, it is impossible to fully reconstruct the brain at the ultrastructural level since the magnification needed to visualize synapses yields relatively small images (in the order tens of μm2).
It is perhaps useful to take the cerebral cortex as an example, and consider what would be required to fully reconstruct just one minicolumn (Figure 3)—defined as a vertical column through the cortical layers made up of the regular columnar disposition of the radial bundles of myelinated axons (radial fasciluli) or vertical aggregates of somata of pyramidal neurons or vertical bundles of apical dendrites of pyramidal cells (see e.g., Fleischhauer et al., 1972; Peters and Walsh, 1972; reviewed in DeFelipe, 2005; Rockland, 2010). In order to fully reconstruct a typical diameter of 50 μm and a height from the pial surface to the white matter of 2000 μm, using sections of 100 μm2 at a thickness of 20 nm, we would need 1.9625 × 106 sections.
The next step would be the huge task of following each of the millions of neuronal and glial processes of the image stacks to fully reconstruct all the elements that make up the minicolumn. If this were possible, then we would have to face another big problem which is that the volume occupied by the whole dendritic and axonal arbors of neurons within the minicolumn exceeds the boundaries of this tissue volume, and consequently a substantial proportion of their dendrites and axons would be cut, in particular the axonal arborizations.
In order to better appreciate the importance of the problem, it is sufficient to visualize the long trajectory and bifurcations of individual pyramidal cell axons across the whole mouse brain (Gong et al., 2013; Figure 4) or the complex axonal arborization patterns of single pyramidal cells in the rat brain (Kita and Kita, 2012; Figure 5).
If we were to follow the extrinsic axons entering the minicolumn that establish synapses with postsynaptic elements of the minicolumn, like motor thalamocortical axons in the rat (Kuramoto et al., 2009) for instance, we would have to face a similar difficulty due to the complexity and widespread axonal arborizations of these neurons (Figure 6). This problem would be even greater if we were to follow an extrinsic axon originating in the basal forebrain. For example, the studies of Wu et al. (2014)—using genetically-directed sparse labeling to examine the full morphologies of individual basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in the mouse—have shown that individual arbors innervate multiple cortical columns, and have >1000 branch points and total axon lengths of up to 50 cm. These authors have also estimated that basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in humans have a mean axon length of ~100 meters. Furthermore, the axons of most cortical neurons (i.e., pyramidal cells) give rise to local axonal arborizations (near the cell body of origin) but the number of axonal synaptic boutons is relatively low (in the order of a few hundreds; see e.g., DeFelipe et al., 1986; Figure 7). Thus, the majority of other synapses within the minicolumn are of extrinsic origin (i.e., axon terminals coming from neurons with a distant origin, like cortico-cortical neurons, thalamo-cortical neurons, etc.). Table 1 outlines the feasibility and non-feasibility of obtaining some critical quantitative anatomical data of the minicolumn that is relevant for connectomics and models.
Table 1.
Light microscopy | Electron microscopy | ||
---|---|---|---|
Feasible | Non-feasible | Feasible | Non-feasible |
Density and spatial distribution of neurons and glia (e.g., cells labeled with NeuN, DAPI) | 1Volume occupied by dendrites, axons and glial processes | Proportion of different types of synapses (e.g., asymmetric and symmetric synapses) | Absolute number and distribution of different types of synapses |
Density and spatial distribution of different types of neurons and glia (e.g., cells that are immunoreactive for GABA, PV, SOM, Iba1) | Inference of number of synapses established by an axon based on the assumption that one varicosity equals one synapse | 2Identification and characterization of long-range synaptic connections | Complete synaptic connections of the axonal arborization of identified neurons |
Volume fraction occupied by neurons, glia, neuropil and blood vessels | 3Number of cells projecting to a given brain region (long-range outputs) | General identification of the postsynaptic targets to a given axon terminal (non-characterized postsynaptic dendritic shafts, dendritic spines, somata, axon initial segments) | Full identification and characterization of the postsynaptic targets (i.e., origin and characteristics of all the postsynaptic dendritic shafts, dendritic spines, somata, axon initial segments) |
Detailed single cell 3D reconstructions of dendritic trees of neurons (cell filling; e.g., intracellular injections of Lucifer Yellow) | 3Number of cells from other brain regions projecting to the minicolumn (long-range inputs) | Complete synaptic input maps on single identified neurons | 4Absolute number of mitochondria in the neuropil |
Detailed single cell 3D reconstructions of axonal arborization of interneurons (cell filling; e.g., intracellular biocytin labeling) | Spatial relationships of synapses with glial processes | ||
2Detailed single cell 3D reconstruction of axonal arborization of projecting neurons (e.g., single neuron-tracing using viral vectors) | |||
Surface area and volume of dendritic shafts, dendritic spines, somata and axon initial segments of identified cells (e.g., intracellularly injected cells) | |||
Density of different types of axon terminals (e.g., immunostained for vGlut1, vGAT) | |||
Complete putative synaptic maps on identified neurons (i.e., characterization of axonal boutons in apposition to neuronal somata, dendrites and axon initial segments of identified neurons |
Abbreviations: DAPI, 4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole; GABA, γ-Aminobutyric acid; PV, Parvalbumin; NeuN, Neuron-Specific Nuclear Protein; SOM, somatostin; VGlut1, Vesicular glutamate transporter 1; vGAT, Vesicular GABA Transporter. 1This data can be inferred from the total number of neurons, glia and volume fraction of the neuropil coupled with electron microscopy data of the neuropil. 2This data can only be obtained in experimental animals. 3Only a rough estimate can be obtained and only in experimental animals. 4Density and spatial distribution of blood vessels coupled with the density of neurons, glia, mitochondria and synapses are fundamental data to model oxygen delivery and energy consumption of local neural circuits in order to better understand how local synaptic activity is regulated and limited by blood flow (see e.g., Blinder et al., 2013; Magistretti and Allaman, 2015).
In conclusion, complete reconstructions of small samples of the mammalian brain (on the nm scale) are feasible, while structures even of relatively small dimensions like the cortical minicolumns cannot be fully reconstructed. As a result, it is only possible to obtain incomplete synaptomes (DeFelipe, 2010; see also Budd and Kisvárday, 2012; da Costa and Martin, 2013).
The Solution: Discrete Sampling and an Integrative Approach
What follows is a discussion of how to deal with the problem of imprecise connectomes and incomplete synaptomes focusing on the cerebral cortex (unless otherwise specified). Nevertheless, the proposed strategy based on a combined light and electron microscopy approach could be applied to any brain region.
While the synaptic density within a given cortical area and layer may vary, this variability remains within a relatively narrow window (DeFelipe et al., 1999). In addition, there are billions of chemical synapses but there are basically only two main types, asymmetric (excitatory; mostly glutamatergic) and symmetric (inhibitory; mostly GABAergic; Houser et al., 1984; Peters et al., 1991; Figure 3C). Other important general microanatomical rules (Feldman, 1984; White, 1989, 2007; DeFelipe and Fariñas, 1992; Nieuwenhuys, 1994; Somogyi et al., 1998; DeFelipe et al., 2002; Shepherd, 2004; Harris and Weinberg, 2012) are that the majority of synapses in the neuropil are of the asymmetric type, and that the percentage of asymmetric and symmetric synapses only varied between 80–95% and 20–5%, respectively, in all the cortical layers, cortical areas and species examined. The main postsynaptic targets of excitatory axon terminals are dendritic spines of pyramidal cells (Figure 3C), which in turn are the most abundant type of neuron (about 75–85% of the total population) and the main source of asymmetric synapses. The whole surface of the dendritic tree of pyramidal cells is covered by dendritic spines except the proximal portion (approximately 10–15 μm from the soma), which is either devoid of dendritic spines or they are found only occasionally. Moreover, the vast majority of dendritic spines establish at least one synapse (Arellano et al., 2007)—most dendritic spines establish a single synapse whereas some dendritic spines establish an additional synapse that is either asymmetric or symmetric (Figure 3C). In addition, symmetric synapses are established mainly on dendritic shafts and are the only type of synapse found on the axon initial segment, soma and proximal dendrites of pyramidal cells (i.e., the dendritic portion devoid of dendritic spines). Furthermore, the axon initial segment of most interneurons (which represent 15–25% of the total population) are devoid of synapses and their soma and proximal dendrites establish both asymmetric and symmetric synapses.
Considering the above-mentioned rules, together with the fact that it is possible to model the statistical distribution of the variation, it can be concluded that, in order to obtain the number and types of synapses present, it is not necessary to reconstruct the entire layer of a particular area. It is possible to determine the range of variability by sampling relatively small regions of the area multiple times instead. To tackle the complexity of brain organization and attempt to better understand it, the most practical approach would seem to involve integrating these data with light microscopy data, e.g., gray matter thickness, the volume fraction of cortical elements (neuropil, neurons, glia and blood vessels), neuron and glia density per volume, neuron microanatomy (i.e., patterns of dendritic arbors, distribution and density of dendritic spines, dendritic length, etc.), together with the patterns of intrinsic (intralaminar, translaminar) and long-range (cortico-cortical, thalamo-cortical, cortico-thalamic, subcortical extra-thalamic) connections (see DeFelipe, 2010). For example, for a given cortical layer, it is clearly not feasible to determine the contribution of its pyramidal cells synapses by reconstructing all the dendritic trees of these cells using an electron microscopy-based approach. However, an estimation of this could be achieved through the pooling of: (i) light microscopy data concerning the total cell number and their microanatomical characteristics with: (ii) FIB/SEM-derived data on axo-spinous and axo-dendritic synapse density. Another approach might be to identify and map the nature of the axon terminals, their spatial distribution etc. by using correlative light and electron microscopy combined with immunocytochemistry or other techniques to label the axon terminals (e.g., conjugate light-electron array tomography; see Collman et al., 2015). For example, it is possible to construct a variety of pyramidal cell-like elements (virtual neurons generated by modeling the quantitative morphometric measures of a given population of 3D reconstructed pyramidal cells) with realistic synaptic weights for computational models based on the morphological parameters found in real pyramidal cells and the general anatomical rules as follows:
Number of synapses on dendrites (N_sy_d):
N_sy_d is calculated using the following data:
L_ap = length of apical dendritic tree;
L_ba = length of basal dendritic tree;
D_sp = density of dendritic spines per μm;
Sy_sp = number of axospinous synapses (asymmetric and symmetric synapses);
Sy_sh = number of synapses on the dendritic shaft (asymmetric and symmetric synapses).
L_ap, L_ba and D_sp can be obtained from 3D reconstructions of pyramidal cells at the light microscopic level (e.g., using 3D confocal microscopy) labeled with markers that allow full visualization of their dendritic arbors (e.g., intracellular injections of Lucifer Yellow or biocytin). Sy_sp and Sy_sh can be estimated from three key synaptic rules. Rule 1 is based on the characteristics of the types and number of synapses on dendritic spines. Rules 2 and 3 are based on the proportion of the symmetric and asymmetric synapses, respectively, found in the neuropil that is on dendritic spines and dendritic shafts. These three rules are derived from electron microscopy data obtained from 3D reconstructions of the neuropil (Figures 3D–G) in the same brain tissue used to obtain the light microscopy data. In these reconstructions, the proportion of the asymmetric and symmetric synapses that are on dendritic spines and dendritic shafts can be established, as can the proportion of dendritic spines that establish one, two or more synapses and the type of these synapses. Shown below is an example of how we can apply these rules to obtain the synaptic weights based on general data obtained in several laboratories, which have not necessarily examined the same species, cortical layers or regions. The density of dendritic spines is not uniform in all regions of the dendritic arbor, and this density as well as the number of synapses on the dendrites, soma and axon initial segment might be variable—with such variability depending on the type of pyramidal cell and on the cortical layer, area, age, gender and species (e.g., DeFelipe and Fariñas, 1992). Consequently, these values should be considered as approximations of general, mean values and should be adjusted and validated in future studies, taking into account the cortical layer, area, age, gender and species examined.
The three rules are as follows:
Rule 1: All dendritic spines establish at least one asymmetric synapse, and 10% of dendritic spines form two synapses. A dendritic spine with a symmetric synapse also establishes an asymmetric synapse.
Rule 2: 31% of all symmetric synapses are formed on spines and 69% are formed on dendritic shafts.
Rule 3: 79% of all asymmetric synapses are formed on dendritic spines and 21% are formed on dendritic shafts.
Thus, the total number, types and distribution of synapses of, for example, a 100 μm length of pyramidal cell dendrite (excluding the initial portion which is free of dendritic spines) with a density of 15 dendritic spines per 10 μm of dendrite, can be estimated based on the following general estimations and assumptions:
Considering rule 1, the total number of synapses on dendritic spines is 165 (150 + 15). According to rules 1 and 2, there will be 31 × 15/100 = 4.65 symmetric axospinous synapses and 160.35 (165 − 4.65) asymmetric axospinous synapses. According to rules 2 and 3, respectively, there will be 4.65 × 69/31 = 10.35 symmetric synapses and 160.35 × 21/79 = 42.62 asymmetric synapses on the dendritic shaft. Thus, in 100 μm of dendrite there will be 160.35 asymmetric synapses on dendritic spines + 42.62 asymmetric synapses on shafts + 4.65 symmetric synapses on dendritic spines + 10.35 symmetric synapses on shafts = a total of 217.97 synapses.
Number of synapses on the soma (N_sy_so):
where,
Sa = Surface area of the soma;
Sy_so = Number of synapses per μm2.
The surface area of the soma can be obtained from 3D reconstructions of identified pyramidal cells at the light microscopic level (e.g., using 3D confocal microscopy) from either genetically or immunocytochemically marked neurons (e.g., Thy1-eGFP-positive pyramidal cells or SMI 32-immunostained pyramidal cells, respectively). An alternative is to use pyramidal cells labeled by either retrograde tract-tracing techniques (e.g., by injecting Fast Blue) or labeled intracellularly (e.g., with Lucifer Yellow or biocytin). Sy_so can be obtained at the light microscopic level by calculating the number and density of immunoreactive puncta for GABAergic markers [e.g., GABA, the GABA transporter 1 (GAT-1), or the vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT)] found in contact with the 3D reconstructed labeled somata. Examples of this include performing immunostaining for GAT-1 in brain sections of Thy1-eGFP mice; using double immunostaining for SMI 32 and GAT-1; or combining tract tracing techniques and immunocytochemistry for GAT-1. The values obtained are then validated by partial 3D reconstruction of pyramidal cell somata at the electron microscopic level in the same brain tissue used to obtain the light microscopy data.
Number of synapses on the axon initial segment (N_sy_ax):
where,
L_ax = Length of the axonal initial segment;
Sy_ax = Number of synapses per μm.
The axon initial segment of identified pyramidal cells at the light microscopic level (see above) can be visualized by ankyrin G immunostaining or other markers of the axon initial segment (e.g., Antón-Fernández et al., 2015) followed by counting of the number of puncta immunostained for GABAergic markers (see above) in contact with the axon initial segment. The values obtained are then validated by 3D reconstruction of the axon initial segment at the electron microscopic level of the pyramidal cells in the same brain tissue used to obtain the light microscopy data.
Finally, the spatial distribution of the somata and the processes of the cellular components of the minicolumn to be analyzed should be taken into account in order to better interpret the electron microscopy data. For example, since the initial portion of the dendrites of pyramidal cells (approximately 10–15 μm from the soma) is free of dendritic spines (see above), most synapses observed in the 3D reconstructions of the neuropil within the minicolumn would most likely be from collateral branches of pyramidal cells originated at a distance from the minicolumn except for the apical dendritic trunks which are at the core of the minicolumn (Figure 3A). Given that it is possible to determine the length of the apical dendrites of pyramidal cells located in different layers, as well as the density of dendritic spines of the apical dendrites and the number of pyramidal cells, it would be relatively easy to obtain an estimation of the number of dendritic spines belonging to the neurons within the minicolumn. Since the number of dendritic spines within a given volume of neuropil is practically equivalent to the number of axospinous asymmetric synapses within that same volume, it would be possible to estimate how many axospinous synapses originate from the neurons within and outside the minicolumn.
In conclusion, the most appropriate route to follow at this moment in time appears to be to link detailed anatomical structural data with the incomplete light and electron microscopy wiring diagrams to build computational models as simplified abstractions, rather than attempting to fully reconstruct the cerebral cortex or any other brain region. Indeed these models are already being used to reason about the data, make predictions and suggest new hypotheses to discover new aspects of the structural and functional organization of the brain (Swanson and Bota, 2010; Kleinfeld et al., 2011; Hill et al., 2012; Helmstaedter, 2013; Morgan and Lichtman, 2013; Sporns, 2013, 2014; da Costa and Martin, 2013; Egger et al., 2014). Nevertheless, it should be kept in mind that the resulting connection matrix that could be obtained with this combined light and electron microscopy approach should be considered as a realistic statistical connection matrix. This statistical matrix does not directly correspond to the exact details of the real circuit itself since, as discussed above, variability exists in many structural (and neurochemical) aspects of the components of the circuits. In other words, all possible connection matrices are constrained by empirically based numerical rules and axonal-dendritic/somatic geometrical relationships from neuronal reconstructions, and exactly “which neuron connects with what” cannot be addressed with this approach. Thus, the functional interpretation of a given experiment based on this statistical connection matrix might not completely match the experimental findings. However, this mismatch could in fact serve to improve the wiring diagrams, making them more and more realistic by adding new connectivity principles.
The Interdisciplinary and Collaborative Approach
It seems clear that only by combining studies at all three levels (macro-, meso-, and nano-scopic) can we fully understand the structural arrangement of the brain as a whole. However, despite the fact that neuroscience has advanced spectacularly in recent decades from genetic, molecular, morphological and physiological perspectives, the question remains as to why we are still so pessimistic about adopting this kind of combined approach. The simple reason for this is that there are enormous gaps between each of these disciplines—gaps which remain practically unexplored. This is not an easy task as it requires cooperation not only between groups of neuroanatomists with expertise in different techniques, but also close collaboration between those with expertise in quite different areas, like specialists in image analysis, data analysis, theory neuroscience, computation, molecular biology, physiology, among others. This is where large international projects come into play, the idea being to pool the efforts of multiple laboratories with different areas of expertise—coordinated through big worldwide projects like the Human Brain Project (HBP) based in the European Union and the Brain Activity Map based in the United States (Markram, 2013; Jorgenson et al., 2015; Zeki, 2015). Thanks to these and other initiatives that promote interdisciplinary collaboration and data sharing, such as the Allen Institute for Brain Research1 or neuroinformatic platforms like NeuroMorpho.Org (Ascoli et al., 2007) and BAMS2 Workspace (Bota et al., 2014), the tempo of the development of new technologies and new strategies to study the brain can be extraordinarily increased giving us cause for optimism.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The Associate Editor Kathleen S. Rockland declares that, despite currently hosting a Frontiers Research Topic with the author Javier DeFelipe, the review was handled objectively. The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to Lidia Alonso-Nanclares, Lidia Blazquez, Ruth Benavides-Piccione, Angel Merchán-Pérez, Alberto Muñoz and José Rodrigo-Rodriguez for insightful discussions and comments on earlier versions of this manuscript, and to all the remaining members of my laboratory for their support. I would like to thank Kathleen S. Rockland for her critical reading of the manuscript and suggestions and Nick Guthrie for editing the text. This work was supported by grants from the following entities: Centro de Investigación en Red sobre Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas (CIBERNED, CB06/05/0066, Spain); the Spanish Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (grant BFU2012-34963 and the Cajal Blue Brain Project, Spanish partner of the Blue Brain Project initiative from EPFL); and the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement no. 604102 (Human Brain Project).
Footnotes
References
- Alonso-Nanclares L., Gonzalez-Soriano J., Rodriguez J. R., DeFelipe J. (2008). Gender differences in human cortical synaptic density. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 14615–14619. 10.1073/pnas.0803652105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anastassiou C. A., Koch C. (2015). Ephaptic coupling to endogenous electric field activity: why bother? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 95–103. 10.1016/j.conb.2014.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antón-Fernández A., Rubio-Garrido P., DeFelipe J., Muñoz A. (2015). Selective presence of a giant saccular organelle in the axon initial segment of a subpopulation of layer V pyramidal neurons. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 869–884. 10.1007/s00429-013-0689-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Araque A., Carmignoto G., Haydon P. G., Oliet S. H., Robitaille R., Volterra A. (2014). Gliotransmitters travel in time and space. Neuron 81, 728–739. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcelli P., Frassoni C., Regondi M. C., De Biasi S., Spreafico R. (1997). GABAergic neurons in mammalian thalamus: a marker of thalamic complexity? Brain Res. Bull. 42, 27–37. 10.1016/s0361-9230(96)00107-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arellano J. I., Espinosa A., Fairén A., Yuste R., DeFelipe J. (2007). Non-synaptic dendritic spines in neocortex. Neuroscience 145, 464–469. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ascoli G. A., Donohue D. E., Halavi M. (2007). Neuromorpho.org: a central resource for neuronal morphologies. J. Neurosci. 27, 9247–9251. 10.1523/jneurosci.2055-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker L. F. (1899). The Nervous System and its Constituent Neurones. New York: D. Appleton and Company. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett M. V. (2000). Electrical synapses, a personal perspective (or history). Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 32, 16–28. 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00065-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett M. V., Zukin R. S. (2004). Electrical coupling and neuronal synchronization in the mammalian brain. Neuron 41, 495–511. 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00043-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berger B., Gaspar P., Verney C. (1991). Dopaminergic innervation of the cerebral cortex: unexpected differences between rodents and primates. Trends Neurosci. 14, 21–27. 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90179-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blazquez-Llorca L., Merchán-Pérez A., Rodríguez R., Gascón J., DeFelipe J. (2013). FIB/SEM technology and alzheimer’s disease: three-dimensional analysis of human cortical synapses. J. Alzheimers Dis. 34, 995–1013. 10.3233/JAD-122038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blinder P., Tsai P. S., Kaufhold J. P., Knutsen P. M., Suhl H., Kleinfeld D. (2013). The cortical angiome: an interconnected vascular network with noncolumnar patterns of blood flow. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 889–897. 10.1038/nn.3426 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bota M., Dong H. W., Swanson L. W. (2003). From gene networks to brain networks. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 795–799. 10.1038/nn1096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bota M., Swanson L. W. (2007). Online workbenches for neural network connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 500, 807–814. 10.1002/cne.21209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bota M., Talpalaru S., Hintiryan H., Dong H. W., Swanson L. W. (2014). BAMS2 workspace: a comprehensive and versatile neuroinformatic platform for collating and processing neuroanatomical connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 3160–3176. 10.1002/cne.23592 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Budd J. M. L., Kisvárday Z. F. (2012). Communication and wiring in the cortical connectome. Front. Neuroanat. 6:42. 10.3389/fnana.2012.00042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cahill L. (2006). Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 477–484. 10.1038/nrn1909 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cajal S. R. (1917). Recuerdos de mi Vida, Vol. 2, Historia de mi Labor Cientıfica. Madrid: Moya. [Google Scholar]
- Caspers S., Geyer S., Schleicher A., Mohlberg H., Amunts K., Zilles K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. Neuroimage 33, 430–448. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collman F., Buchanan J., Phend K. D., Micheva K. D., Weinberg R. J., Smith S. J. (2015). Mapping synapses by conjugate light-electron array tomography. J. Neurosci. 35, 5792–5807. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4274-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- da Costa N. M., Martin K. A. (2013). Sparse reconstruction of brain circuits: or, how to survive without a microscopic connectome. Neuroimage 80, 27–36. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J. (2005). “Reflections on the structure of the cortical minicolumn,” in Neocortical Modularity and the Cell Minicolumn, ed. Casanova M. F. (New York: Nova Science Publishers; ), 57–91. [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J. (2010). From the connectome to the synaptome: an epic love history. Science 330, 1198–1201. 10.1126/science.1193378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J. (2011). The evolution of the brain, the human nature of cortical circuits and intellectual creativity. Front. Neuroanat. 5:29. 10.3389/fnana.2011.00029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J. (2014). El Jardín de la Neurología: Sobre lo Bello, El Arte y el Cerebro. Madrid: Boletín Oficial del Estado and Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas. [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J., Alonso-Nanclares L., Arellano J. I. (2002). Microstructure of the neocortex: comparative aspects. J. Neurocytol. 31, 299–316. 10.1023/A:1024130211265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J., Ballesteros-Yáñez I., Inda M. C., Muñoz A. (2006). Double-bouquet cells in the monkey and human cerebral cortex with special reference to areas 17 and 18. Prog. Brain Res. 154, 15–32. 10.1016/s0079-6123(06)54002-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J., Conley M., Jones E. G. (1986). Long-range focal collateralization of axons arising from corticocortical cells in monkey sensory-motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 6, 3749–3766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J., Fariñas I. (1992). The pyramidal neuron of the cerebral cortex: morphological and chemical characteristics of the synaptic inputs. Prog. Neurobiol. 39, 563–607. 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90015-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J., Marco P., Busturia I., Merchán-Pérez A. (1999). Estimation of the number of synapses in the cerebral cortex: methodological considerations. Cereb. Cortex 9, 722–732. 10.1093/cercor/9.7.722 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denk W., Horstmann H. (2004). Serial block-face scanning electron microscopy to reconstruct three-dimensional tissue nanostructure. PLoS Biol. 2:e329. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Descarries L., Mechawar N. (2000). Ultrastructural evidence for diffuse transmission by monoamine and acetylcholine neurons of the central nervous system. Prog. Brain Res. 125, 27–47. 10.1016/s0079-6123(00)25005-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger R., Dercksen V. J., Udvary D., Hege H. C., Oberlaender M. (2014). Generation of dense statistical connectomes from sparse morphological data. Front. Neuroanat. 8:129. 10.3389/fnana.2014.00129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elston G. N. (2003). Cortex, cognition and the cell: new insights into the pyramidal neuron and prefrontal function. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1124–1138. 10.1093/cercor/bhg093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman M. L. (1984). “Morphology of the neocortical pyramidal neuron,” in Cerebral Cortex, vol. 1. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex, eds Peters A., Jones E. G. (New York: Plenum press; ), 123–200. [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer D., Radnikow G. (2009). Developmental alterations in the functional properties of excitatory neocortical synapses. J. Physiol. 587, 1889–1896. 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.169458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felleman D. J., Van Essen D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 1, 1–47. 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleischhauer K., Petsche H., Wittkowski W. (1972). Vertical bundles of dendrites in the neocortex. Z. Anat. Entwicklungs. 136, 213–223. 10.1007/bf00519179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund T. F., Martin K. A., Soltesz I., Somogyi P., Whitteridge D. (1989). Arborisation pattern and postsynaptic targets of physiologically identified thalamocortical afferents in striate cortex of the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 289, 315–336. 10.1002/cne.902890211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund T. F., Martin K. A., Whitteridge D. (1985). Innervation of cat visual areas 17 and 18 by physiologically identified X- and Y- type thalamic afferents I. Arborization patterns and quantitative distribution of postsynaptic elements. J. Comp. Neurol. 242, 263–274. 10.1002/cne.902420208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuxe K., Dahlström A., Höistad M., Marcellino D., Jansson A., Rivera A., et al. (2007). From the Golgi-Cajal mapping to the transmitter-based characterization of the neuronal networks leading to two modes of brain communication: wiring and volume transmission. Brain Res. Rev. 55, 17–54. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuxe K., Dahlström A. B., Jonsson G., Marcellino D., Guescini M., Dam M., et al. (2010). The discovery of central monoamine neurons gave volume transmission to the wired brain. Prog. Neurobiol. 90, 82–100. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geschwind D. H., Rakic P. (2013). Cortical evolution: judge the brain by its cover. Neuron 80, 633–647. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gong H., Zeng S., Yan C., Lv X., Yang Z., Xu T., et al. (2013). Continuously tracing brain-wide long-distance axonal projections in mice at a one-micron voxel resolution. Neuroimage 74, 87–98. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris K. M., Weinberg R. J. (2012). Ultrastructure of synapses in the mammalian brain. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 4:a005587. 10.1101/cshperspect.a005587 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmstaedter M. (2013). Cellular-resolution connectomics: challenges of dense neural circuit reconstruction. Nat. Methods. 10, 501–507. 10.1038/nmeth.2476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmstaedter M., Briggman K. L., Denk W. (2008). 3D structural imaging of the brain with photons and electrons. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 633–641. 10.1016/j.conb.2009.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill S. L., Wang Y., Riachi I., Schürmann F., Markram H. (2012). Statistical connectivity provides a sufficient foundation for specific functional connectivity in neocortical neural microcircuits. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, E2885–E2894. 10.1073/pnas.1202128109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofman M. A. (2014). Evolution of the human brain: when bigger is better. Front. Neuroanat. 8:15. 10.3389/fnana.2014.00015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homman-Ludiye J., Bourne J. A. (2014). Mapping arealisation of the visual cortex of non-primate species: lessons for development and evolution. Front. Neural Circuits 8:79. 10.3389/fncir.2014.00079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horton J. C., Adams D. L. (2005). The cortical column: a structure without a function. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 837–862. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1623 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houser C. R., Vaughn J. E., Hendry S. H. C., Jones E. G., Peters A. (1984). “GABA neurons in cerebral cortex. Functional properties of cortical cells,” in Cerebral Cortex, eds Jones E. G., Peters A. (New York: Plenum Press; ), 63–89. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs B., Schall M., Scheibel A. B. (1993). A quantitative dendritic analysis of wernicke’s area in humans. II. gender, hemispheric, and environmental factors. J. Comp. Neurol. 327, 97–111. 10.1002/cne.903270108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs B., Scheibel A. B. (1993). A quantitative dendritic analysis of wernicke’s area in humans. I. Lifespan changes. J. Comp. Neurol. 327, 83–96. 10.1002/cne.903270107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jazin E., Cahill L. (2010). Sex differences in molecular neuroscience: from fruit flies to humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 9–17. 10.1038/nrn2754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones E. G. (1994). The neuron doctrine 1891. J. Hist. Neurosci. 3, 3–20. 10.1080/09647049409525584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones E. G. (2007a). Neuroanatomy: cajal and after cajal. Brain Res. Rev. 55, 248–255. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones E. G. (2007b). The thalamus. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jorgenson L. A., Newsome W. T., Anderson D. J., Bargmann C. I., Brown E. N., Deisseroth K., et al. (2015). The BRAIN Initiative: developing technology to catalyse neuroscience discovery. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 370:20140164. 10.1098/rstb.2014.0164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas J. H. (2013). The evolution of brains from early mammals to humans. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 33–45. 10.1002/wcs.1206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kita T., Kita H. (2012). The subthalamic nucleus is one of multiple innervation sites for long-range corticofugal axons: a single-axon tracing study in the rat. J. Neurosci. 32, 5990–5999. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5717-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleinfeld D., Bharioke A., Blinder P., Bock D. D., Briggman K. L., Chklovskii D. B., et al. (2011). Large-scale automated histology in the pursuit of connectomes. J. Neurosci. 31, 16125–16138. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4077-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knott G., Marchman H., Wall D., Lich B. (2008). Serial section scanning electron microscopy of adult brain tissue using focused ion beam milling. J. Neurosci. 28, 2959–2964. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3189-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolb B., Forgie M., Gibb R., Gorny G., Rowntree S. (1998). Age, experience and the changing brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22, 143–159. 10.1016/s0149-7634(97)00008-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuramoto E., Furuta T., Nakamura K. C., Unzai T., Hioki H., Kaneko T. (2009). Two types of thalamocortical projections from the motor thalamic nuclei of the rat: a single neuron-tracing study using viral vectors. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2065–2077. 10.1093/cercor/bhn231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuwajima M., Mendenhall J. M., Harris K. M. (2013). Large-volume reconstruction of brain tissue from high-resolution serial section images acquired by SEM-based scanning transmission electron microscopy. Methods Mol. Biol. 950, 253–273. 10.1007/978-1-62703-137-0_15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luders E., Toga A. W. (2010). Sex differences in brain anatomy. Prog. Brain Res. 186, 3–12. 10.1016/b978-0-444-53630-3.00001-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luebke J. I., Medalla M., Amatrudo J. M., Weaver C. M., Crimins J. L., Hunt B., et al. (2015). Age-related changes to layer 3 pyramidal cells in the rhesus monkey visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1454–1468. 10.1093/cercor/bht336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magistretti P. J., Allaman I. (2015). A cellular perspective on brain energy metabolism and functional imaging. Neuron 86, 883–901. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder E. (2012). Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron 76, 1–11. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markov N. T., Misery P., Falchier A., Lamy C., Vezoli J., Quilodran R., et al. (2011). Weight consistency specifies regularities of macaque cortical networks. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1254–1272. 10.1093/cercor/bhq201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markram H. (2013). Seven challenges for neuroscience. Funct. Neurol. 28, 145–151. 10.11138/FNeur/2013.28.3.144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marner L., Nyengaard J. R., Tang Y., Pakkenberg B. (2003). Marked loss of myelinated nerve fibers in the human brain with age. J. Comp. Neurol. 462, 144–152. 10.1002/cne.10714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merchán-Pérez A., Rodriguez J., Alonso-Nanclares L., Schertel A., DeFelipe J. (2009). Counting synapses using FIB/SEM microscopy: a true revolution for ultrastructural volume reconstruction. Front. Neuroanat. 3:18. 10.3389/neuro.05.018.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan J. L., Lichtman J. W. (2013). Why not connectomics? Nat. Methods. 10, 494–500. 10.1038/nmeth.2480 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuys R. (1994). The neocortex. an overview of its evolutionary development, structural organization and synaptology. Anat. Embryol. (Berl) 190, 307–337. 10.1007/bf00187291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberheim N. A., Takano T., Han X., He W., Lin J. H., Wang F., et al. (2009). Uniquely hominid features of adult human astrocytes. J. Neurosci. 29, 3276–3287. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4707-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osakada F., Mori T., Cetin A. H., Marshel J. H., Virgen B., Callaway E. M. (2011). New rabies virus variants for monitoring and manipulating activity and gene expression in defined neural circuits. Neuron 71, 617–631. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osten P., Margrie T. W. (2013). Mapping brain circuitry with a light microscope. Nat. Methods 10, 515–523. 10.1038/nmeth.2477 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passingham R. (2009). How good is the macaque monkey model of the human brain? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 6–11. 10.1016/j.conb.2009.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters A., Palay S. L., de Webster H. F. (1991). The fine structure of the nervous system. Neurons and their Supporting Cells. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Peters A., Walsh T. M. (1972). A study of the organization of apical dendrites in the somatic sensory cortex of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 144, 253–268. 10.1002/cne.901440302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raghanti M. A., Hof P. R., Sherwood C. C. (2010a). “The evolution of cortical neurotransmitter systems among primates and their relevance to cognition,” in The Human Brain Evolving: Paleoneurological studies in honor of Ralph L. Holloway, eds Broadfield D., Yuan M., Schick K., Toth N. (Gosport: Stone Age Institute Press; ), 195–212. [Google Scholar]
- Raghanti M. A., Spocter M. A., Butti C., Hof P. R., Sherwood C. C. (2010b). A comparative perspective on minicolumns and inhibitory GABAergic interneurons in the neocortex. Front. Neuroanat. 4:3. 10.3389/neuro.05.003.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling J. K. (2014). Comparative primate neuroimaging: insights into human brain evolution. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 46–55. 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rockland K. S. (2010). Five points on columns. Front. Neuroanat. 4:22. 10.3389/fnana.2010.00022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semaan S. J., Kauffman A. S. (2010). Sexual differentiation and development of forebrain reproductive circuits. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 424–431. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd G. M. (1991). Foundations of the neuron doctrine. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd G. M. (2004). “Introduction to synaptic circuits,” in The Synaptic Organization of the Brain, ed. Shepherd G. M. (New York: Oxford University Press; ), 1–38. [Google Scholar]
- Sherwood C. C., Bauernfeind A. L., Bianchi S., Raghanti M. A., Hof P. R. (2012). Human brain evolution writ large and small. Prog. Brain Res. 195, 237–254. 10.1016/b978-0-444-53860-4.00011-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S. J. (2007). Circuit reconstruction tools today. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 601–608. 10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somogyi P., Tamás G., Lujan R., Buhl E. H. (1998). Salient features of synaptic organisation in the cerebral cortex. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 26, 113–135. 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00061-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O. (2013). Making sense of brain network data. Nat. Methods. 10, 491–493. 10.1038/nmeth.2485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O. (2014). Contributions and challenges for network models in cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 652–660. 10.1038/nn.3690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O., Tononi G., Kötter R. (2005). The human connectome: a structural description of the human brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 1:e42. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0010042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark A. K., Toft M. H., Pakkenberg H., Fabricius K., Eriksen N., Pelvig D. P., et al. (2007). The effect of age and gender on the volume and size distribution of neocortical neurons. Neuroscience 150, 121–130. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.06.062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steno N. (1669). Discours de Monsieur Sténon sur l’anatomie du cerveau. Paris: Robert de Ninville. [Google Scholar]
- Swanson L. W., Bota M. (2010). Foundational model of structural connectivity in the nervous system with a schema for wiring diagrams, connectome and basic plan architecture. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 20610–20617. 10.1073/pnas.1015128107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamás G., Buhl E. H., Somogyi P. (1997). Fast IPSPs elicited via multiple synaptic release sites by different types of GABAergic neurone in the cat visual cortex. J. Physiol. Lond. 500, 715–738. 10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp022054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toga A. W., Thompson P. M., Mori S., Amunts K., Zilles K. (2006). Towards multimodal atlases of the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 952–966. 10.1038/nrn2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uylings H. B., Rajkowska G., Sanz-Arigita E., Amunts K., Zilles K. (2005). Consequences of large interindividual variability for human brain atlases: converging macroscopical imaging and microscopical neuroanatomy. Anat. Embryol. (Berl) 210, 423–431. 10.1007/s00429-005-0042-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White E. L. (1989). Cortical Circuits: Synaptic Organization of the Cerebral Cortex. Structure, Function and Theory. Boston: Birkhäuser. [Google Scholar]
- White E. L. (2007). Reflections on the specificity of synaptic connections. Brain Res. Rev. 55, 422–429. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White E. L., Weinfeld E., Lev D. L. (2004). Quantitative analysis of synaptic distribution along thalamocortical axons in adult mouse barrels. J. Comp. Neurol. 479, 56–69. 10.1002/cne.20300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Workman A. D., Charvet C. J., Clancy B., Darlington R. B., Finlay B. L. (2013). Modeling transformations of neurodevelopmental sequences across mammalian species. J. Neurosci. 33, 7368–7383. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5746-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu H., Williams J., Nathans J. (2014). Complete morphologies of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in the mouse. Elife 3:e02444. 10.7554/elife.02444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuan J., Gong H., Li A., Li X., Chen S., Zeng S., et al. (2015). Visible rodent brain-wide networks at single-neuron resolution. Front. Neuroanat. 9:70. 10.3389/fnana.2015.00070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeki S. (2015). Introduction to the theme issue ‘Cerebral cartography: a vision of its future’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 370:20140163. 10.1098/rstb.2014.0163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]