Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Aug 21.
Published in final edited form as: Appl Neuropsychol. 2011 Apr;18(2):95–106. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2010.547774

Designing rehabilitation programs for neglect: Could 1+1 be more than 2?

Styrmir Saevarsson 1,2,3,*, Ulrike Halsband 1, Árni Kristjánsson 2,4
PMCID: PMC4544767  NIHMSID: NIHMS291699  PMID: 21660761

Abstract

Unilateral neglect is a multimodal neuropsychological disorder which has puzzled scientists for a long time. Many interventions have been developed, but only a handful have proved to be effective. This review examines whether applying different therapeutic techniques in combination will increase therapeutic benefits. Studies were reviewed where therapies are applied sequentially or in combination with other techniques. The results indicate that combining different interventions leads to increased general improvement compared with other non-combined designs, even when the number of treatment sessions is not constant. Practical and theoretical aspects of different treatments are discussed. The combined approach to treatment may have direct relevance to other disorders than neglect. This report introduces a new classification scheme for different interventions with the aim of facilitating more focused therapy. Finally, suggestions are made as to what the focus of future studies of neglect therapy should be and how therapeutic benefits might be maximized.

Keywords: Unilateral neglect, classification scheme, therapeutic designs, sequential and combined designs

Introduction

Unilateral neglect is a multimodal neuropsychological disorder where patients fail to respond to stimuli in their contralesional hemifield (Heilmann, Watson, & Valenstein, 2003; Mesulam, 1985). A patient with neglect may fail to read the left part of a sentence, read only the right pages of a book, or fail to notice that someone passes them by on their left side (Weinstein, 1981). Despite being severely disabling for patients, effective treatment strategies for neglect have remained elusive.

One out of three stroke victims show symptoms of neglect and new cases of neglect are estimated to be three to five million per year worldwide (Appelros, Karlsson, Seiger, & Nydevik, 2002; Corbetta, Kincade, Lewis, Snyder, & Sapir, 2005; Pedersen, Jørgensen, Nakayama, Raaschou, & Olsen, 1997). Right hemisphere stroke patients with neglect are more likely to show deficits in daily functioning compared with right hemisphere stroke patients without the disorder, even when lesion size is taken into account (Buxbaum et al., 2004; Denes, Semenza, Stoppa, & Lis, 1982; Freund, 1987; Katz, Hartman-Maeir, Ring, & Soroker, 1999). Effective treatment would clearly be of value, for patients and their families, in addition to reducing the costs of health care. Note that only a decade ago, some standard textbooks argued that neglect was essentially untreatable (see Robertson, 2002).

A number of therapeutic interventions for neglect have been developed in the last few decades, but very few have conclusively been shown to be of value in treating the syndrome. Some have speculated that stronger and more durable intervention effects might result from combining different treatments or applying different treatments repeatedly (e.g. Kerkhoff, 2003; Rossetti & Rode, 2002; Singh-Curry & Husain, 2008). This raises the important question of whether the particular therapeutic design may play as large a role in neglect rehabilitation as the individual intervention techniques,

The aim is to compare the effects of combining different therapies (either simultaneously or sequentially) with other therapy approaches. Also, to understand what benefits might be gained by using combinations of therapies to treat neglect, as compared to single-session and sequential applications of the same rehabilitation techniques. This topic has not been systematically addressed in earlier reviews. The review also introduces, a new classification scheme for different types of therapy, with the aim of assisting clinicians in making informed decisions about which therapies are best suited to the treatment of individual patients with idiosyncratic symptoms.

Multimodality of neglect

Neglect is a multimodal syndrome where various types of dysfunction vary greatly between patients (e.g. Verdon, Schwartz, Lovblad, Hauert, & Vuilleumer, 2010) and the critical neural tissue damage varies between individuals. The affliction is “at its core” a disorder of attention, and the neural systems damaged correspond well with regions that have been shown to play key roles in attentional orienting (Awh & Jonides, 2001; Geng et al., 2006; Jovicich and others, 2001; Kristjánsson, Vuilleumier, Schwartz, Macaluso, & Driver, 2007; Ruff, Kristjánsson, & Driver, 2007; Yantis & Serences, 2003). Neglect is therefore not connected with damage to sensory areas of the brain. For example, not necessarily confined to vision, and manifests itself in many different types of deficit. The best intervention will therefore involve treatment of the multifaceted symptoms of neglect with different interventions; each of which could target the individual deficits. For example, the syndrome seems to involve dysfunctional attentional orienting towards one hemifield, other researchers have argued that the deficits are not necessarily confined to one visual field (Duncan et al., 1999; Husain et al., 2001, but see Kristjánsson & Vuilleumier, 2010). Furthermore, some studies have shown that many aspects of the perceptual process remain intact for the affected hemifield (see e.g. Conci et al., 2009; Driver & Vuilleumier, 2001; Finke et al., 2009; Kristjánsson, Vuilleumier, Malhotra, Husain, & Driver, 2005; Mattingley, Davis, & Driver, 1997; Saevarsson, Jóelsdottir, Hjaltason, & Kristjánsson, 2008a). Interventions that only focus on rectifying the rightwards bias, or only address the deficiencies in search behavior, might then fail to address crucial aspects of the syndrome. A therapeutic approach which attacks both these aspects could be much more effective (e.g. Saevarsson, Kristjánsson, & Halsband, in press).

Prognosis and recovery

Full or partial spontaneous recovery of neglect may occur for some patients after onset, while the syndrome remains chronic for a sizable number. Stone, Halligan, and Greenwood (1993) have reported that 82% of right hemisphere, and 65% of left hemisphere stroke patients show acute neglect symptoms, while Halligan and Marshall (1989) found that 48% of right hemisphere and 15% of left hemisphere patients show symptoms of the disorder for at least two months following stroke. The degree to which this recovery was confounded with post-stroke therapy is not known. How the effects of neglect therapy may interact with spontaneous recovery is unclear, and is a notoriously difficult issue to address experimentally. Some degree of recovery usually occurs spontaneously in the weeks or months following stroke, typically within the first three months, while at other times recovery is not seen until a few years following the brain injury. Different aspects of the syndrome may also recover at different rates (such as motor dysfunction and anosognosia; Cramer, 2008).

Corbetta and colleagues (2005) studied changes in neural plasticity connected with recovery in neglect. They found that recovery from symptoms caused by right frontal lesions was correlated with structural changes of this area and physiological changes in intact dorsal and ventral networks of attention that are functionally related to, but anatomically distinct from the locus of the brain injury. However, they did not explicitly address the extent to which this recovery was therapy based versus spontaneous. Both behavioral measurements and functional imaging were performed at an acute stage of the syndrome (three to four weeks following stroke) and repeated more than six months post-stroke, when the symptoms had become more stable. Some of the patients received some kind of neglect therapy during this period. Robertson, McMillan, Edgeworth, & Brock (2002) speculated that the plasticity changes produced by recovery following rehabilitation and spontaneous recovery could be quite similar, making any distinct effects of the two difficult to disentangle. Many questions remain unanswered regarding recovery rates for different therapies (Cramer, 2008).

Interventions for neglect

There appears to be a general consensus that interventions of clinical value for neglect should lead to benefits lasting at least a day or longer. There is, however, much less agreement on how the syndrome should be treated. Long term clinical benefits have not been sufficiently evaluated for all methods (Robertson, 2002). Nevertheless, those treatments that have not yet been found to be clinically useful in terms of the duration of beneficial effects, might still be found to play a role in treating neglect; for instance, if they can facilitate any benefits from using another intervention by shrinking the neglected space at the time of intervention and strengthen the overall clinical effects when used in conjunction with other therapies. Caloric vestibular stimulation, to cite one example, might increase the alertness of neglect patients as other interventions, such as PA (prism adaptation) or eye-patching, when simultaneously applied (see e.g. Saevarsson, Kury, Cho, Kristjánsson, & Halsband, 2008d, for some preliminary findings).

Only a few functional brain imaging studies have been performed to explore the neurophysiological bases of therapies for neglect (see Arene & Hillis, 2007 for an overview). For instance, a PET study by Luauté et al. (2006b) showed that low-level visuomotor adaptation (i.e. automatic modifications of visuomotor correspondence following a prism-exposure phase) resulted in modulations of neuronal activity in the right cerebellum, the right posterior parietal cortex, the left thalamus, the left temporo-occipital cortex, and the left medial temporal cortex (see also Luauté et al., 2009). Similar rehabilitation studies on animals show how behavioral recovery is correlated with plasticity changes in the brain (e.g. Nudo, Wise, SiFuentes, & Milliken, 1996). These studies give some indication of what sort of reorganization of the dysfunctional neural networks takes place following the application of these interventions. How these findings on the neurophysiological underpinnings of different methods relate to the selection of interventions and therapeutic designs for patients as a function of their individual brain lesions remains to be systematically studied.

What determines the success of therapy for neglect?

A large number of unrelated factors can affect the outcome of a particular treatment. As with many other health problems, the symptoms of neglect can vary over time. Since different interventions have different limitations and merits, some interventions can be more therapeutically relevant than others for different subgroups of patients, or for similar groups at different times. For instance, one therapy might be more relevant when rapid effects of intervention are important (Saevarsson, Kristjánsson, Hildebrandt, & Halsband, 2009b). However, what kind of therapy is most effective at different points in time following the onset of neglect remains unclear.

Regrettably, a sizeable number of patients do not seem to receive any benefits from therapy. The patients’ level of functioning may be too severely disrupted for them to undergo treatment, or the selected therapies may not target their clinical symptoms sufficiently (see e.g. Vuilleumier, 2007). Since neglect is multifaceted and some interventions seem to be quite task specific, the choice of intervention is of great importance (Rossetti & Rode, 2002). Visual scanning training has for instance been found to help patients with reading, but the benefits of this training are remarkably specific to this single task (Manly & Mattingley, 2004). Some medical problems can exclude certain interventions, therapy combinations, or therapy sequences. For instance, neck vibration is not recommended for patients wearing a pacemaker or for epileptic patients without medication. Eye-patching should be avoided if a patient suffers from hemianopia, and PA should be avoided if the patient suffers from dizziness. A few guidelines can be pointed out which are useful in designing individualized therapy sessions. For one, it is important to consider the nature and degree of the patient's cognitive impairment since impairments can vary greatly between patients, in terms of arousal levels, sustained attention, and spatial orienting (Robertson, 2002). For instance, some patients show improvements from visuospatial motor cueing training but and not from visual scanning training (Samuel et al., 2000).

The symptoms of the vast majority of neglect patients fall under more than one subcategorization (e.g. motor, visual, tactile, olfactory, or auditory), and different interventions seem to affect different subtypes of the disorder (see Rossetti & Rode, 2002 for an overview). Thus, when an individual therapy is designed, such differences in the individual symptomology of differing patients must be taken into account. When a patient's fitness, or general ability level, is too low, sequential application of interventions in which patients can rest between sessions might be more advisable than simultaneous combined interventions, since the patient may simply cope more easily with the therapy (although the results of Keller, Lefin-Rank, Lösch, & Kerkhoff, 2009 may suggest some important caveats to this, see below). Simultaneous combined interventions may be preferable when the patient's fitness level is high, since the total intervention time is shorter as compared to the sequential approach, and some research indicates that this may be a more effective intervention type overall (see below). Future research should address differences in therapeutic effects when two interventions are applied in combination or in sequence.

Classifying the different intervention designs

Systematic classification of the different rehabilitation schemes may lead to more informed choices of therapy designs. The following three classification criteria are proposed: 1) passive vs. active therapies, 2) restorative vs. compensatory therapies and 3) top-down vs. bottom-up therapies. Passive interventions do not necessarily require the patient's attention and collaboration. This type of intervention can be used in combination with any other type of intervention if the patient's fitness level is high enough to allow this. Neck vibration is an example of such a passive intervention. For instance, PA is an active intervention technique which requires the patient's collaboration, attention, and vigilance. While passive intervention techniques can be combined with other passive or active techniques (see e.g. Saevarsson et al., in press), there may be problems with applying two or more active intervention techniques simultaneously (see e.g. Keller et al., 2009). Interestingly, Luauté, Halligan, Rode, Rossetti, and Boisson (2006a) recommended mostly active neglect interventions (such as PA and visual scanning training) in their comprehensive review of therapeutic techniques for neglect. Interestingly, only one of the interventions recommended in their review was a combination of active and passive techniques (neck vibration combined with an extensive training program). More research involving combinations of passive and active therapy types might reveal a more important role for passive therapy approaches. To provide an example, Saevarsson et al. (in press, 2009b) recently found more improvement in neglect symptoms when neck vibration and PA were used in combination, as compared with neck vibration or PA applied on their own.

Compensatory interventions involve feedback upon performance or some form of support provided by the therapist. Interventions of this sort can be both passive and active. For instance, visual scanning (Weinberg et al., 1977) is an active compensatory training technique, while limb activation is passive compensatory. Limb activation might be categorized as passive for hemiplegic (paralyzed on the left side of their body) patients and active for those without hemiplegia. A restorative intervention is aid- or medicine- based and is not necessarily dependent on help from a therapist. Neck vibration (NV; Karnath, Christ, & Hartje, 1993) and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (Oliveri et al., 2001) are examples of passive-restorative interventions, and PA is an example of an active-restorative intervention.

The aim with top-down approaches is to get the patient to voluntary reduce or compensate for their neglect symptoms by providing them with strategies to improve their functioning. Such approaches do not always require the use of external devices. Top-down methods can be passive-compensatory (e.g. music training) or active-compensatory (e.g. mental imagery; Smania, Bazoli, Piva, & Guidetti, 1997). Passive-restorative and active restorative top-down approaches that have been used for the treatment of neglect were not found. The aim with bottom-up approaches is to alter underlying causes, normally through the use of a device that manipulates stimuli in the patient's environment or enhances their awareness of their neglected side. Bottom-up interventions do not necessarily require the patient's cooperation and can, with relative ease, be combined simultaneously with many other interventions. Bottom-up approaches can be subdivided into passive-compensatory (such as dimming the lights; Hjaltason & Tégner, 1992), active-compensatory (limb activation; Halligan & Marshall, 1989), passive-restorative (NV; Karnath et al., 1993) and active-restorative (caloric stimulation; Rubens, 1985).

Table 1 presents brief descriptions of ten interventions that have been developed for neglect. The table categorizes each rehabilitation technique in terms of the three criteria that have been explained here. The focus is on interventions that have either been used in combination with other methods or sequentially with the same or different therapies. This categorization of therapy techniques can be used as a guide when a rehabilitation program for a particular patient is designed, and may aid in the development and design of future studies of the efficacy of neglect interventions, particularly as they are applied with other methods. While evidence for the efficacy of each of these methods is quite variable, the main point here is that it is important to attack the symptoms from different angles. Although the efficacy of a specific method may not be supported by strong evidence, it may, nevertheless be of value when used in combination with other therapy types (see e.g. Saevarsson et al. 2008d).

Table 1.

The table gives a brief rationale and description of specialized neglect interventions that have been used in combination and sequentially with other methods.

Name of intervention Procedure / rationale Mechanism Attentional load Compensatory / restorative Can be used simultaneously with

1. CVS Injection of cold water in the external ear canal produces correction of spatial coordinate frame Bottom-up Passive Restorative 2-10
2. EP & RHB Eye patches are worn to produce Sprague effect (visually-guided behavior to the contralateral space) Bottom-up Passive Restorative 1, 3-10
3. LAT Active and passive movements with a left limb to the left to increase the function of the right hemisphere Bottom-up Active / Passive Compensatory 1-2,4-10
4.NV Vibration applied to posterior part of left neck muscles to recalibrate egocentric/spatial coordinates Bottom-up Passive Restorative 1-3, 5-10
5. OPKS Leftward moving background of a screen that causes recalibration of spatial coordinates Bottom-up Active Restorative 1-4, 9
6. PA Successive pointing movements while wearing prisms that produces visuo-motor adaptation Bottom-up Active Restorative 1-4, 9
7. SAT Examiner touches different locations on a patients back and the patient has to indicate the touched location on a mannequin in front of them Top-down Active Compensatory 1-4, 9
8. SATG Sequential sound stimulation to alert spatial attention to left Top-down Active Compensatory 1-2, 9
9. TENS Surface stimulation with electrical impulses that generate tingling sensation Bottom-up Passive Compensatory 1-8, 10
10. VST Uncued and cued voluntary scanning eye movement training to the left side of a scanning board Top-down Active Compensatory 1-3, 9

Abbreviations for interventions: CVS: Caloric vestibular stimulation; EP: Eye patching; LAT: Limb activation training; NV: Neck vibration; OPKS: Optokinetic stimulation; RHB: Right hemiblinding blinding; SAT: Sensory awareness training; SATG: Sustained attention training; TENS: Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation; VST: Visual scanning training

Can the use of extant therapy types be improved?

Only a handful of therapies have been developed that have been shown to result in significant, relatively long-lasting reductions in symptoms, in terms of neuropsychological testing and functional effectiveness (see e.g. Luauté et al., 2006a, for a comprehensive overview; see also Bowen & Lincoln, 2002). Although some single session interventions have shown relatively long term clinical benefits on neglect symptoms, many authors have argued that combined applications of different interventions (even applied repeatedly) could increase the therapeutic effects of various interventions that have already been developed (e.g. Kerkhoff, 2001, 2003; Manly & Mattingley, 2004; Rossetti & Rode, 2002). In this context, Kerkhoff (2001; p. 21) asked: “How can different treatment approaches be combined effectively to reach a maximal outcome for the patient?”. Singh-Curry and Husain (2008; p. 449) echoed this question: “Both spatial and nonspatial mechanisms contribute to neglect and may represent different targets for treatments aimed at rehabilitating the condition”. In the future, it is likely that new interventions, new combinations of extend methods, and new sequences of known methods will be developed and may become standard tools for treating neglect. It is suspected that the design of such methods will (and should) be the focus of research in the coming years since current interventions have not proved to be sufficiently effective at relieving neglect symptoms, while evidence that combining therapies can lead to additive therapeutic benefits upon neglect continues to emerge.

Recent years have seen an increased number of studies where different therapies are used both in combination and sequentially. In an attempt to measure the effects of such approaches, a literature search aimed at identifying and classifying studies of this sort was performed. The Medline, PsychInfo, ScienceDirect, and Web of Science databases were used to identify studies where one of four types of therapeutic design was employed (see tables 2 to 5 below). 39 relevant studies using this search criteria were found. An attempt to estimate the success of different therapeutic designs in neglect was made based on the results of these identified studies.

Table 2.

Studies that have applied simultaneously combined therapeutic methods for one session in neglect rehabilitation.

Study Interventions Number of patients Duration of intervention Therapeutic results

Butter & Kirsch (1992) EP, VST 31 Unspecified The group that received both interventions showed stronger improvement than either of the two cols that only received training on one of the treatments
Saevarsson et al. (in press) PA, NV 12 20 min More improvement from combined NV and PA compared to PA and NV applied in isolation
Saevarsson et al. (2008c) PA, RHB 4 10-15 min Some indication of improvement in the PA-RHB group, but not in the PA-LHB group

Abbreviations in the table: EP: Eye patching;; NV: Neck vibration;; PA: Prism adaptation; RHB: Right hemifield blinding; VST: Visual scanning training

Table 5.

Studies that have used different interventions applied at different times to explore neglect rehabilitation.

Study Interventions Number of patients Duration of intervention Therapeutic results

Antonucci et al. (1995) GCT, VST, RT, CT, FD 20 2 months Experimental and control (no special intervention) cols showed similar improvement of neglect symptoms
Hildebrandt et al. (1998) VCT, CS 25 4 weeks The approach resulted in additive therapeutic effects
Hildebrandt et al. (1999) VCT, TENS, AS >32 4 weeks Additive effects were revealed with the approach
Karnath et al. (1995) TENS, NV 4 Unspecified Only short-term improvement was found after neck vibration at the time of intervention
Keller et al. (2009) PA, OPKS 10 Ca. 30 minutes OPKS alone resulted in more improvement compared to when PA was also applied
Pizzamiglio et al. (2004) OPKS, VST 3 6 weeks The mixed sequential application of VST and OPKS was not found to produce additive therapeutical effects
Robertson et al. (1992) PCL, LAT, AVP 3 5 days All sequences of intervention showed improvement
Samuel et al. (2000) VSMC, VST 2 2 days Patients showed improvement of neglect symptoms one month after intervention
Weinberg et al. (1979) SAT, SOT, VST 53 4 weeks The experimental cols showed stronger improvement compared to the control group.
Wilson et al. (2000) LAT, SATG 1 20 days Additional sequential benefits from sequential mixed therapy compared with LAT alone
Wilson et al. (2003) SATG, EL 1 20 days Improvements were found two weeks following intervention

Abbreviations of the table:AS: Acoustical stimulation; AVP: Avoidance conditioning procedure; COT: Computerized alertness training; CS: Contralesional stimulation; CT: Copying training; EL: Errorless learning; FD: Figure description; GCS: General cognitive stimulation LAT: Limb activation training; NV: Neck vibration; OPKS: Optokinetic stimulation; PA: Prism adaptation; PCL: Perceptual anchoring training; RT: Reading training SAT: Sensory awareness training; SATG: Sustained attention training; SOT: Spatial organization training; TENS: Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation; VCT: Visual compensatory training VFT: Visuomotor feedback training; VSMC: Visuo-spatial-motor cueing; VST: Visual scanning training

Table 2 lists the studies where two different treatment approaches (combined design) have been applied simultaneously in one session. Table 3 shows approaches where a combination of treatments (sequential combined design) is applied for more than one session. Table 4 shows studies that have used sequential designs, where a single therapeutic approach is applied repeatedly. Table 5 shows sequentially mixed designs, where dissimilar treatments are applied at different times.

Table 3.

Studies that used combined therapeutic designs in neglect rehabilitation with treatment applied more than once.

Study Interventions Number of patients Duration of intervention Therapeutic results

Brunila et al. (2002) LAT, VT 4 6 weeks Combined approach suggests additive therapeutical effects
Polanowska et al. (2009) LSS, VST 40 1 month LSS combined with VST showed more benefits than VST alone
Schindler et al. (2002) LAT, ET 10 1 week The group that received both treatments showed superior therapeutic effects compared to VST
Schröder et al. (2008) ET, TENS 30 1 week Combined approach led to larger improvement than exploration training on its own
Wiart et al. (1997) VST, TR 25 1 month Combined approach resulted in more improvement than the control group which received regular neurorehabilitation
Wiebecke et al. (2007) PA, RPMS 26 3-4 weeks Combined approach showed the same improvement as the PA on standard tests. Only the PA group showed improvement in daily function.

Abbreviations of the table: ET: Exploration training; LAT: Limb activation training; LSS: Left hand somatosensory stimulation; PA: Prism adaptation; RPMS: Repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation; TENS: Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation; TR: Trunk rotation; VST: Visual scanning training; VT: Visual training

Table 4.

Studies that have used sequential interventions (same method applied repeatedly) in neglect rehabilitation.

Study Intervention(-s) Number of patients Duration of intervention Therapeutic results

Bailey et al. (2002) VST or LAT 7 3 weeks Patients in both cols showed improvements three weeks after intervention
Cherney et al. (2003) VST, AT, or OR 4 8-9 weeks No clear conclusions could be drawn about the success of the intervention
Dijkermann et al. (2004) PA 1 1 month Results are not reported on whether the second PA session produced stronger improvement than the first session of PA
Frassinetti et al. (2002) PA 13 5 weeks Improvement was found on many different neglect tests following five weeks of repeated application of PA
Harvey et al. (2003) VFT 14 6 weeks Significantly more improvement than that seen for a control group of neglect patients
Humphreys et al. (2006) PA 1 14 weeks Results indicate long-term improvement following prolonged PA.
Johannsen et al. (2003) NV 10 10 days Improvement from symptoms of neglect 1,4 years following therapy
Katz et al. (2005) IVET 19 1 month Some indication of improvement
Kerkhoff et al. (2001) OPKS 3 2 weeks Improvement of neglect symptoms 18-24 months following therapy
Kerkhoff et al. (2006) OPKS or VST 10 2 weeks Significant improvement in the OPKS group unlike the VST group
Maddicks et al. (2003) LAT 1 10 days Improvements were found in the locomotor space but not in peripersonal space
McCarthy et al. (2002) IT 2 Unspecified Imagined movement of left arm improved neglect, and the effect was specific to the arm that was imagined
Nys et al. (2008) PA 16 1 month No clear difference between the PA and the control group was found
Robertson et al. (2002) LAT 39 3 months The intervention was linked to long term improvement of neglect
Serino et al. (2009) PA 20 2 weeks Stronger effects compared to a control group of patients. Benefical effects were confirmed one month following the PA.
Sturm et al. (2006) CAT, OPKS 14 3 weeks The CAT and the OPKS cols showed both three weeks improvement following three weeks of training
Shiraishi et al. (2008) PA 7 2 months Long-term improvement of eye-movements to the neglected side
Song et al. (2009) rTMS 14 2 weeks Some indication of improvement of neglect symptoms
Thimm et al. (2006) COT 7 3 months Limited improvement of neglect symptoms

Abbreviations of the table:AS: Acoustical stimulation; AT: Attention training AVP: Avoidance conditioning procedure; CAT: Computerized alertness training; COT: Computerized alertness training; CS: Contralesional stimulation; CT: Copying training; FD: Figure description; GCS: General cognitive stimulation; IT: Imagination training; IVET: Interactive virtual imaging training; LAT: Limb activation training; NV: Neck vibration; OPKS: Optokinetic stimulation; OR: Oral reading; PA: Prism adaptation; PCL: Perceptual anchoring training; RT: Reading training; rTMS: Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; SAT: Sensory awareness training; SATG: Sustained attention training; SOT: Spatial organization training; TENS: Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation; VCT: Visual compensatory training VFT: Visuomotor feedback training; VSMC: Visuo-spatial-motor cueing; VST: Visual scanning training

What can be learned from the findings on combined and sequential approaches to neglect therapy tabulated here? The most important conclusion is that combined sequential designs (as shown in table 3) seem to be the most effective form of treatment.1 All sequentially combined therapy studies (see table 3) appeared to result in larger improvement for the experimental group as compared to a control group. In addition, the three studies in which the single-session combined approach was used show some evidence of additive therapeutic effects, as compared to the control groups (see table 2). The results from tables 2 and 3 suggest that combining two or more treatment methods and applying them simultaneously, preferably with repeated application, is a quite promising avenue for neglect therapy and rehabilitation.

On the other hand, the results from the 19 studies in which only a single therapy type was used (see table 4) are rather heterogeneous. While some studies show evidence of moderate improvement, others do not. Finally, the 11 studies that used sequentially mixed designs (see table 5) show improvements in symptoms for most patients, in basic agreement with the findings in tables 2 and 3.

The most important conclusion from tables 2 to 5 is that the three approaches (tables 2, 3, and 5) which mix different therapy types appear to result in larger improvements of neglect symptoms than sequential designs where a single therapy type is applied repeatedly (table 4). What is particularly interesting is that in some cases the design seems to play as large a role as the intervention itself, regardless of the number of therapeutic sessions that are applied for both types of designs. This indicates that the particular therapy type employed is not the only important factor with regard to neglect rehabilitation, but that the therapeutic design and how it interacts with a particular therapy type are equally important. If true, this finding should transform the agenda for future studies on neuropsychological rehabilitation.

Although the vast amount of single session studies on the different interventions were not reviewed here (i.e. therapies not used in combination with other methods as in table 2), all four categories of design tabulated in tables 2 to 5 seem to show superior therapeutic effects compared to single session baseline interventions (most of the studies provide such baselines). The main limitation to the information given in these tables is the relatively low number of studies2. Furthermore, neglect was assessed in many non-uniform ways across these studies, which may have affected the outcome evaluations. Additionally, since only a small number of studies have used sequentially combined approaches (table 3) the tables are incomplete, and a large majority of the studies that were reviewed here has been devoted to the study of therapeutic benefits of repeated individual sessions of a single treatment method (table 4). Also, a large portion of the studies covered in tables 2 to 5 are focused on the visual modality in neglect. Therefore, any conclusions that can be drawn from the review with regard to other modalities must come with this caveat.

It is particularly interesting to see that some of the interventions that have been found to be successful when used in combined or sequential designs, have not been found to be particularly successful when used in isolation (such as neck vibration, Luauté et al., 2006a). This suggests that therapeutic methods which may not be particularly effective on their own can still be of clinical value when used in conjunction with other therapy types. The importance of this point should not be underestimated. Such synergistic effects are of particular interest, and deserve further study.

One of the main suggestions made here is that more studies of various designs of therapeutic techniques are strongly needed for an in-depth analysis and interpretation of different approaches. The claim is that the results suggest that the importance of the design of neglect therapy is underestimated in the literature and that scientists appear to be magnetically drawn to single session applications of individual methods. Much more research is needed to explore the interactive relationship between therapy designs on the one hand and the different therapies on the other. The current results (in tables 2, 3, and 5) do nevertheless give some hope that the number of studies of therapy designs where different methods are applied in combination is increasing.

There are other arguments for using combinations of therapies applied simultaneously rather than single therapeutic techniques. If combined interventions work better in terms of therapeutic effects and speed of both application and therapeutic success, it is not unreasonable to assume that the amount of time the patients need to spend in rehabilitation clinics might be reduced and used in a more efficient manner. This may then be an important and useful strategy since simultaneous combined therapies are less likely to tire out patients and consequently diminish their fitness levels. The results of a recent study conducted by Keller et al. (2009) are particularly telling. The researchers found that applying two active interventions (optokinetic stimulation and PA) at different times, albeit with almost no break in-between, resulted in lesser therapeutic effects than optokinetic stimulation applied on its own. This might suggest that long and difficult therapy sessions tire patients out, leading to lessened clinical effects from the combination of methods. Fatigue may lead to a strengthening of neglect symptoms which could overwrite any beneficial effects from the combination of therapies 3 (e.g. Saevarsson, Kristjánsson, & Hjaltason, 2008a).

Different patients with a diagnosis of neglect show diverse symptoms, and these symptoms may have various degrees of overlap. In theory, this point could, be employed to argue for the use of very strategically picked single therapies based on patients’ particular symptoms. However, the vast majority of neglect patients do not have a single group of symptoms that are particularly sensitive to a single intervention. Furthermore, the current state of knowledge of specific aspects of neglect and their interaction for individual patients is not sufficient to serve as a basis for selecting a particular therapy.

Many possible combinations and sequences of interventions have not been studied experimentally and could be of great interest. For instance, studies of the simultaneous combined use of two active therapies are for the most part missing, and the same is true for combinations of two passive therapy types. Also, repetitive TMS has neither been explored in combination nor sequentially with neck vibration. Furthermore, new neuropsychological interventions, such as transcranial direct current stimulation, remain unexplored for neglect (e.g. Baker, Rorden, & Fridriksson, 2010). Future research could include neuroimaging during a combined intervention study. Such research might reveal greater activity modulations in the human brain than have already been found in functional brain imaging studies on isolated or sequential therapies, especially in light of the fact that combined approaches have shown larger behavioral improvements from rehabilitation than single therapy designs. Meanwhile, it is important to continue the development of new interventions for neglect, due to the relatively limited success of currently available therapy techniques. This continued development should be guided by an increased understanding of the disorder and the underpinnings of current interventions. Although current therapeutic techniques for neglect have regrettably shown only limited benefits, the progress that has nevertheless been made in recent years affords us some cautious optimism for the future.

The principal claim here is that combined or sequentially combined applications of different therapies are the most promising avenues for future intervention procedures for neglect. Apart from the growing scientific evidence (as seen in tables 2, 3, and 5), another reason for this claim is that neglect is a multimodal, multi-component disorder that can be caused by dysfunction or damage in several cortical and subcortical neural circuits. Note that discussion of the importance of therapeutic designs and other practical suggestions in the current review might be equally applicable to other health problems, such as aphasia, which shares the diverse and heterogeneous symptomology of neglect. However, the authors are not aware of any scientific reviews on therapeutic designs for other disorders.

Conclusions

While neglect continues to be an immense burden for patients, their families and the worlds’ health care systems, effective therapies and designs for the disorder remain elusive. The main argument in the current review is that more attention should be paid to the design of rehabilitation programs. Different therapeutic techniques used in combination that are applied repeatedly may currently be the most promising approach to treating the disorder, and most likely produce the strongest and longest-lasting effects. The primary reason for this is that neglect symptoms tend to vary considerably between individuals and the clinical symptoms can normally be seen in different modalities, heterogeneous behaviors, and on various types of tasks.

Acknowledgements

SS was supported with a grant from the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD), and AK was supported by a grant from The Research Fund of the University of Iceland and the Human Frontiers Science Program. This work was also supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health to Prof. Chris Rorden (R01-NS054266). The authors would like to express their gratitude to Mrs. Stephanie Saskia Schneider for her assistance with data collection. Chris Rorden, Dr. Arthur MacNeill Horton, Jr., and five anonymous reviewers are thanked for helpful comments. Portions of this research were presented at the meeting of the Nordic-Baltic doctoral network in Helsinki, Finland, in August, 2009.

Footnotes

1

At present, the low number of relevant studies prevents us from drawing strong conclusions about individual combinations of therapies and designs. Our aim is rather to highlight some trends in the findings – in particular that combinations of approaches hold the greatest promise -and to point the way forward in terms of research into different therapeutic designs.

2

This scarcity of studies precludes a meta-analysis at this point.

3

Note that the results of this pilot study of Keller et al. could, on the surface, be considered as speaking against the combined therapy approach that we advocate here. However, all the other evidence that we present here points in the other direction.

References

  1. Antonucci G, Guariglia C, Judica A, Magnotti L, Paolucci S, Pizzamiglio L, Zoccolotti Effectiveness of neglect rehabilitation in a randomized group study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1995;17:383–389. doi: 10.1080/01688639508405131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Appelros P, Karlsson GM, Seiger A, Nydevik I. Neglect and anosognosia after first-ever stroke: incidence and relationship to disability. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine. 2002;34:215–220. doi: 10.1080/165019702760279206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arene NU, Hillis AE. Rehabilitation of unilateral spatial neglect and neuroimaging. Europa Medicophysica. 2007;43:255–269. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Awh E, Jonides J. Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5:119–126. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01593-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bailey JB, Riddoch MJ, Crome P. Treatment of Visual Neglect in Elderly Patients With Stroke: A Single-Subject Series Using Either a Scanning and Cueing Strategy or a Left-Limb Activation Strategy. Physical Therapy. 2002;82:782–797. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baker JM, Rorden C, Fridriksson J. Using Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation to Treat Stroke Patients With Aphasia. Stroke. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.576785. ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bowen A, Lincoln N. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2002;18:CD003586. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brunila T, Lincoln N, Lindell A, Tenovuo O, Hämäläinen H. Experiences of combined visual training and arm activation in the rehabilitation of unilateral neglect: A clinical study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2002;12:27–40. [Google Scholar]
  9. Butter CM, Kirsch NK. Combined and separate effects of eye patching and visual stimulation on unilateral neglect following stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1992;73:1133–1139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, Farne A, Whyte J, Ladavas E, et al. Hemispatial neglect: Subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability. Neurology. 2004;62:749–756. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000113730.73031.f4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cherney LR, Halper AS, Papachronis D. Two approaches to treating unilateral neglect after right hemisphere stroke: a preliminary investigation. Top Stroke Rehabilitation. 2003;9:22–33. doi: 10.1310/BWY3-7GQL-596Y-V17J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Conci M, Böbel E, Matthias E, Keller I, Müller HJ, Finke K. Preattentive surface and contour grouping in Kanizsa figures: evidence from parietal extinction. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:726–732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Lewis C, Snyder AZ, Sapir A. Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention in spatial neglect. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1603–1610. doi: 10.1038/nn1574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cramer SC. Repairing the human brain after stoke: I. Mechanisms of spontaneous recovery. Annals of Neurology. 2008;62:272–287. doi: 10.1002/ana.21393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Denes G, Semenza C, Stoppa E, Lis A. Unilateral spatial neglect and recovery from hemiplegia: a follow-up study. Brain. 1982;105:543–552. doi: 10.1093/brain/105.3.543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dijkerman HC, Webeling M, ter Wal JM, Groet E, van Zandvoort MJ. A long lasting improvement of somatosensory function after prism adaptation, a case study. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1697–1702. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Driver J, Vuilleumier P. Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction. Cognition. 2001;79:39–88. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00124-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Duncan J, Bundesen C, Olson A, Humphreys G, Chavda S, Shibuya H. Systematic analysis of deficits in visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1999;128:450–478. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.128.4.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Finke K, Bucher L, Kerkhoff G, Keller I, von Rosen F, Geyer T, Müller H, Bublak P. Inhibitory and facilitatory location priming in patients with left-sided visual hemi-neglect. Psychological Research. 2009;73:177–185. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0209-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frassinetti F, Angeli V, Meneghello F, Avanzi S, Làdavas E. Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. Brain. 2002;125:608–623. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Freund H-J. Differential effects of cortical lesions in humans. Motor areas of the cerebral cortex. In: Porter R, editor. Motor areas of the cerebral cortex. John Wiley and Sons; Chichester: 1987. pp. 269–281. [Google Scholar]
  22. Geng JJ, Eger E, Ruff C, Kristjánsson Á, Rotshtein P, Driver J. On- line attentional selection from competing stimuli in opposite visual fields: Effects on human visual cortex and control processes. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;96:2601–2612. doi: 10.1152/jn.01245.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Halligan PW, Marshall JC. Laterality of motor response in visuo-spatial neglect: A case study. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27:1301–1317. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90042-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Harvey M, Hood B, North A, Robertson IH. The effects of visuomotor feedback training on the recovery of hemispatial neglect symptoms: assessment of a 2-week and follow-up intervention. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:886–893. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00003-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E. Neglect: clinical and anatomic issues. In: Feinberg TE, Farah MJ, editors. Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. McGraw-Hill; NY: 2003. pp. 303–311. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hildebrandt H, Benetz J, Schröder A, Sachsenheimer W. Behandlungserfolge bei Gesichtsfeldausfall und Neglect durch kompensatorisches Training und sensible Anbahnung. Neurologische Rehabilitation. 1998;4:132–136. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hildebrandt H, Brasse M, Pfefferkorn D, Fecht AVD, Sachsenheimer W. Intersensorische Kombinationsbehandlung bei unilateralen Sehstörungen: Eine Replikation ihrer Effektivität und eine Spezifikation ihres Aufbaus. Neurologische Rehabilitation. 1999;5:328–334. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hjaltason H, Tégner R. Darkness improves line bisection in unilateral spatial neglect. Cortex. 1992;28:353–358. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80145-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Humphreys GW, Watelet A, Riddoch MC. Long-term effects of prism adaptation in chronic visual neglect: a single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2006;23:463–478. doi: 10.1080/02643290500202755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Husain M, Mannan S, Hodgson T, Wojciulik E, Driver J, Kennard C. Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. Brain. 2001;124:941–952. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.5.941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Johannsen L, Ackermann H, Karnath H-O. Lasting amelioration of spatial neglect by treatment with neck muscle vibration even without concurrent training. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine. 2003;35:249–253. doi: 10.1080/16501970310009972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jovicich J, Peters RJ, Koch C, Braun J, Chang L, Ernst T. Brain areas specific for attentional load in a motion-tracking task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2001;15:1048–1058. doi: 10.1162/089892901753294347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Karnath H-O. Transcutaneous electrical stimulation and vibration of neck muscles in neglect. Experimental Brain Research. 1995;105:321–324. doi: 10.1007/BF00240969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Karnath H-O, Christ K, Hartje W. Decrease of contralateral neglect by neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline. Brain. 1993;116:383–396. doi: 10.1093/brain/116.2.383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Katz N, Hartman-Maeir A, Ring H, Soroker N. Functional disability and rehabilitation outcome in right hemisphere damaged patients with and without unilateral spatial neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1999;80:379–384. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(99)90273-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Katz N, Ring H, Naveh Y, Kizony R, Feintuch U, Weiss PL. Interactive virtual environment training for safe street crossing of right hemisphere stroke patients with unilateral spatial neglect. Disability and Rehabilitation. 2005;27:1235–1243. doi: 10.1080/09638280500076079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Keller I, Lefin-Rank G, Lösch J, Kerkhoff G. Combination of pursuit eye movement training with prism adaptation and arm movements in neglect therapy: a pilot study. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2009;23:58–66. doi: 10.1177/1545968308317438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kerkhoff G. Spatial hemineglect in humans. Progress in Neurobiology. 2001;63:1–27. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(00)00028-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kerkhoff G. Modulations and rehabilitation of spatial neglect by sensory stimulation. Progress in Brain Research. 2003;142:257–271. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)42018-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kerkhoff G, Keller I, Ritter V, Marquardt C. Repetitive optokinetic stimulation induces lasting recovery from visual neglect. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2006;24:357–369. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kerkhoff G, Marquardt C, Jonas M, Ziegler W. Repetitive optokinetische Stimulation (R-OKS) zur Behandlung des multimodalen Neglects. Neurologie & Rehabilitation. 2001;7:179–184. [Google Scholar]
  42. Kristjánsson Á, Vuilleumier P. Disruption of spatial memory in visual search in the left visual field in patients with hemispatial neglect. Vision Research. 2010;50:1426–1435. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.001. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kristjánsson Á, Vuilleumier P, Malhotra P, Husain M, Driver J. Priming of color and position during visual search in unilateral spatial neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;17:859–873. doi: 10.1162/0898929054021148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kristjánsson Á, Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S, Macaluso E, Driver J. Neural basis for priming of pop-out revealed with fMRI. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:1612–1624. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Luauté J, Halligan P, Rode G, Rossetti Y, Boisson D. Visuo-spatial neglect: A systematic review of current interventions and their effectiveness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006a;30:961–982. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Luauté J, Michel C, Rode G, Pisella L, Jacquin-Courtois S, Costes N, et al. Functional anatomy of the therapeutic effects of prism adaptation on left neglect. Neurology. 2006b;66:1859–1867. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219614.33171.01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Luauté J, Schwartz S, Rosetti Y, Spiridon M, Rode G, Boisson D, Vuilleumier P. Dynamic changes in brain activity during prism adaptation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:169–178. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3054-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Maddicks R, Marzillier S, Parker G. Rehabilitation of unilateral neglect in the acute recovery stage: The efficacy of limb activation therapy. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2003;13:391–408. doi: 10.1080/09602010244000264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Manly T, Mattingley JB. Visuospatial and attentional disorders. In: Goldstein LH, McNeil JE, editors. Clinical Neuropsychology: A Practical Guide to Assessment and Management for Clinicians. John Wiley and Sons Ltd.; West Sussex: 2004. pp. 229–252. [Google Scholar]
  50. Mattingley JB, Davis G, Driver J. Preattentive filling-in of visual surfaces in parietal extinction. Science. 1997;31:671–674. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5300.671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. McCarthy M, Beaumont JG, Thompson R, Pringlea H. The role of imagery in the rehabilitation of neglect in severely disabled brain-injured adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2002;17:407–422. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mesulam MM. Principles of behavioral neurology. F. A. Davis; Philadelphia: 1985. [Google Scholar]
  53. Nudo RJ, Wise BM, SiFuentes F, Milliken GW. Neural substrates for the effects of rehabilitative training on motor recovery alter ischemic infarct. Science. 1996;272:1791–1794. doi: 10.1126/science.272.5269.1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Nys GMS, de Haan EHF, Kunneman A, de Kort PLM, Dijkerman HC. Early cognitive impairment predicts long-term depressive symptoms and quality of life after stroke. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2008;26:1–12. [Google Scholar]
  55. Oliveri M, Bisiach E, Brighina F, Piazza A, La B, Buffa D, Fierro B. rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional visuospatial hemineglect. Neurology. 2001;57:1338–1340. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.7.1338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pedersen PM, Jørgensen HS, Nakayama H, Raaschou HO, Olsen TS. Hemineglect in acute stroke-incidence and prognostic implications. The Copenhagen Stroke Study. American Journal of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1997;76:122–127. doi: 10.1097/00002060-199703000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Pizzamiglio L, Fasotti L, Jehkonen M, Antonucci G, Magnotti L, Boelen D, Asa S. The use of optokinetic stimulation in rehabilitation of the hemineglect disorder. Cortex. 2004;40:441–450. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70138-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Polanowska K, Seniow J, Paprot E, Leśniak M, Członkowska A. Left-hand somatosensory stimulation combined with visual scanning training in rehabilitation for post-stroke hemineglect: A randomised, double blind study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2009;19:364–382. doi: 10.1080/09602010802268856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Robertson IH. Cognitive neuroscience and brain rehabilitation: a promise kept. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 2002;73 doi: 10.1136/jnnp.73.4.357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Robertson IH, McMillan TM, Edgeworth J, Brock D. Rehabilitation of unilateral neglect by limb activation: A randomised single blind controlled trial. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2002;12:439–454. [Google Scholar]
  61. Robertson IH, North NT, Geggie C. Spatiomotor cueing in unilateral left neglect: three case studies of its therapeutic effects. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 1992;55:799–805. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.9.799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rossetti Y, Rode G. Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: Noncognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder. In: Karnath HO, Milner D, Vallar G, editors. The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect. Oxford University Press; New York: 2002. pp. 375–396. [Google Scholar]
  63. Rubens AB. Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect. Neurology. 1985;35:1019–1024. doi: 10.1212/wnl.35.7.1019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Ruff CC, Kristjánsson Á, Driver J. Readout from iconic memory involves similar neural processes as selective spatial attention. Psychological Science. 2007;18:901–909. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01998.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Saevarsson S, Jóelsdóttir S, Hjaltason H, Kristjánsson Á. Repetition of context improves visual search performance in hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2008a;46:1161–1169. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Saevarsson S, Kristjánsson Á, Halsband U. Strength in numbers: Combining neck vibration and prism adaptation produces additive therapeutic effects in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. doi: 10.1080/09602011003737087. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Saevarsson S, Kristjánsson Á, Halsband H. Prism adaptation affects the impaired hemifield in neglect. Second International Symposium on Visual Search and Selective Attention. 2008c:126. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  68. Saevarsson S, Kury S, Cho A, Droll H, Kristjánsson Á, Halsband U. Die Effekte von Eye Patching und vestibulärer Stimulation auf die Linien-Teilung bei gesunden Teilnehmern. Journal of Neuropsychology. 2008d;196 [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  69. Saevarsson S, Kristjánsson Á, Hjaltason H. Unilateral neglect: A review of causes, anatomical localization, theories and interventions. Icelandic Medical Journal. 2009a;95:27–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Saevarsson S, Kristjánsson Á, Hildebrandt H, Halsband U. Prism adaptation improves visual search in neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2009b;47:717–725. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Samuel C, Louis-Dreyfus A, Kaschel R, Makiela E, Troubat M, Anselmi N, et al. Rehabilitation of severe unilateral neglect by visuo-spatial cueing: Two single case studies. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2000;10:385–399. [Google Scholar]
  72. Schindler I, Kerkhoff G, Karnath H-O, Keller I, Goldenberg G. Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery in spatial neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2002;73:412–419. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.73.4.412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schröder A. TENS and optokinetic stimulation in neglect therapy after cerebrovascular accident: a randomized controlled study. European Journal of Neurology. 2008;15:922–927. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02229.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Serino A, Barbiani M, Rinaldesi ML, Làdavas E. Effectiveness of prism adaptation in neglect rehabilitation. A controlled trial study. Stroke. 2009;40:1392–1398. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.530485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Shiraishi H, Yamakawa Y, Itou A, Muraki T, Asada T. Long-term effects of prism adaptation on chronic neglect after stroke. NeuroRehabilitation. 2008;23:137–151. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Singh-Curry V, Husain M. Rehabilitation of neglect. In: Stuss DT, Winocur G, Robertson IH, editors. Cognitive Neurorehabilitation, Second Edition: Evidence and Application. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2008. pp. 449–463. [Google Scholar]
  77. Smania N, Bazoli F, Piva D, Guidetti G. Visuomotor imagery and rehabilitation of neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1997;78:430–436. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(97)90237-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Song WQ, Du B, Xu Q, Hu J, Wang M, Luo Y. Low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation for visual spatial neglect: a pilot study. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine. 2009;41:162–165. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Stone SP, Halligan PW, Greenwood RJ. The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age Ageing. 1993;22:48–52. doi: 10.1093/ageing/22.1.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sturm W, Thimm M, Küst J, Karbe H, Fink GR. Alertness-training in neglect: Behavioral and imaging results. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2006;24:371–384. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Thimm M, Fink GR, Küst J, Karbe H, Sturm W. Impact of alertness training on spatial neglect: a behavioral and fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:1230–1234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Verdon V, Schwartz S, Lovblad K-O, Hauert C-A, Vuilleumier P. Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: A study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Brain. 2009;133:880–894. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Vuilleumier P. Hemispatial neglect. In: Godefroy O, Bogousslavsky J, editors. The Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology of Stroke. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2007. pp. 148–197. [Google Scholar]
  84. Weinberg J, Diller L, Gordon WA, Gerstman LJ, Lieberman A, Lakin P, et al. Visual scanning training effect on reading-related tasks in acquired right brain damage. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1977;58:479–486. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Weinberg J, Diller L, Gordon WA, Gerstman LJ, Lieberman A, Lakin P, et al. Training sensory awareness and spatial organization in people with right brain damage. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1979;60:491–496. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Weinstein EA. Woodrow Wilson: A medical and psychological biography. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 1981. [Google Scholar]
  87. Wiart L, Côme AB, Debelleix X, Petit H, Joseph PA, Mazaux JM, Barat M. Unilateral neglect syndrome rehabilitation by trunk rotation and scanning training. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1997;78:424–429. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(97)90236-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wiebecke J. Neglectherapie durch Prismenadaptation und repetitive periphere Magnetstimulation. “Diplomarbeit” thesis. Ruhr-Universität Bochum; Germany: 2007. Unpublished. [Google Scholar]
  89. Wilson FC, Manly T. Sustained attention training and errorless learning facilitates self-care functioning in chronic ipsilesional neglect following severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2003;13:537–548. [Google Scholar]
  90. Wilson FC, Manly T, Coyle D, Robertson IH. The effect of contralesional limb activation training and sustained attention training for self-care programmes in unilateral spatial neglect. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2000;16:1–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Yantis S, Serences JT. Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2003;13:187–193. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00033-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES