Abstract
Background. LRRK2 S1647T has been identified as a polymorphic risk variant for Parkinson's disease (PD) in Chinese individuals. As LRRK2 is the most common genetic cause for PD, it has drawn great interest regarding whether cognitive impairments in PD are related with LRRK2. Purpose. This study aimed to explore the effects of LRRK2 S1647T polymorphism on cognitive function in PD. Method. 90 PD patients were randomly recruited. They underwent a series of clinical evaluations and genetic testing for the LRRK2 S1647T polymorphism. Global intellect and five cognitive domains (language fluency, visuospatial function, attention, memory, and executive function) were compared between S1647T carriers and noncarriers. Results. No differences in motor features were found between two groups, but the executive function evaluation showed that Stroop word colour test time (SWCT-TIME) scores were lower in LRRK2 S1647T carriers than in noncarriers (P = 0.017). However, multiple linear regression analysis indicated that the correlation between S1647T polymorphism and SWCT-TIME scores did not reach significant level (P = 0.051). Conclusion. Our findings suggest that cognitive impairments are not correlated with different LRRK2 S1647T polymorphisms in Chinese PD individuals.
1. Introduction
Although the aetiology of Parkinson's disease (PD) remains unclear, a complex interaction between genetic and environmental factors is generally considered to be its cause [1]. Mutations in the gene encoding for leucine-rich repeat kinase 2 (LRRK2) at the PARK8 locus on chromosome 12q12 may be related with PD. LRRK2 S1647T has been identified as a risk variant for PD in the Chinese population [2]. Among our PD samples in southern China, 35.7% were heterozygous and 24.4% were homozygous for this allele [3].
Clinically, LRRK2-related PD is characterized by motor features consistent with idiopathic PD, including asymmetrical tremor with bradykinesia and rigidity that responds to dopamine replacement [4]. However, nonmotor symptoms, especially those related to cognition, are not well characterized in LRRK2-related PD. We previously demonstrated that cognitive impairment is common in the early and middle stages of PD without dementia, and executive function is the most commonly impaired domain in a Chinese population of PD [5]. Mutations in the gene encoding α-synuclein (SNCA) have been associated with cognitive impairments in PD, suggesting that genetic factors influence cognition in patients with PD [6]. As LRRK2 is the most common genetic cause for PD [7], it has drawn great interest concerning whether cognitive impairments in PD are related with LRRK2 [8]. As the S1647T polymorphism is common in Chinese populations, we performed detailed neuropsychological evaluations of 90 PD patients (45 LRRK2 S1647T carriers and 45 noncarriers) to explore the effect of this polymorphism on cognitive function in PD and to achieve a more extensive clinical characterization of patients with this polymorphism.
2. Materials and Methods
The study was carried out in accordance with the Guide for the Experimentation with Humans. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Sun Yat-sen University, and all subjects provided their written informed consent to participate in the study.
2.1. Subjects
PD participants were randomly recruited in the PD clinic and the ward of Department of Neurology at the First Affiliated Hospital, Sun Yat-sen University, China, between November 2008 and April 2010. The diagnosis of PD was made by neurologists using the UK Brain Bank criteria [9]. The study ruled out patients with identified dementia (according to DSM-IV criteria for dementia) and/or definite depression (Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD) ≥ 20 points) [10], definite anxiety (Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAMA) ≥ 22 points) [11], color blindness, and deafness. Illiterate patients were also excluded. Finally, a total of 90 PD patients were included in this study. They underwent a series of clinical evaluations and genetic testing for the LRRK2 S1647T polymorphism after providing informed consent.
2.2. Gene Polymorphism Analysis
DNA was extracted from a peripheral venous blood sample using standard procedures, and LRRK2 S1647T polymorphisms were identified with polymerase chain reaction-restriction fragment length polymorphism analysis.
2.3. Clinical Assessment
All cases were in “on” condition when the evaluation was conducted. They were under stable dopaminergic therapy and did not require booster doses of L-DOPA or dopamine agonists. Clinical and demographic data were collected using standardized case report forms that included the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) [12] and Hoehn and Yahr Scale [13]. The neuropsychological battery was performed in a single session, including the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, used to assess global mental status) [14]. Five cognitive domains involving language fluency, visuospatial function, memory, attention, and executive function were evaluated by the animal fluency test (AFT) [15], clock-drawing test (CDT) [16], Rey auditory verbal learning test (AVLT) [17], Wechsler adult intelligence scale digit span task (DST) [18], and Stroop word colour test (SWCT) [19]. Both SWCT-TIME and SWCT-ERROR scores were calculated by scores in the incongruous Stroop condition minus scores in the basal condition, so that the presence of basal differences in performing the SWCT test between the two groups can be excluded. A single rater who was blind to the clinical characteristics and genotypes of the subjects performed the cognitive evaluations.
2.4. Statistical Analysis
SPSS 17.0 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used. Comparisons between LRRK2 S1647T carriers and noncarriers were computed using chi-square and t-tests. A multiple linear regression model was used to explore the risk factors of cognitive impairments. Independent factors that had a possible association with cognitive impairments were entered into a multiple linear regression model, with the scores of cognitive assessments as the dependent variable. A value of P < 0.05 was considered significant.
3. Results
3.1. General Demographic and Clinical Assessment
The gene polymorphism analysis showed that our cohort included 45 carriers and 45 noncarriers of LRRK2 S1647T. As shown in Table 1, there were no marked differences in general demographic data including sex, age, and education between carriers and noncarriers of LRRK2 S1647T. We also did not identify differences between the two groups regarding clinical data such as age of onset, duration of illness, PD symptoms and severity, or L-DOPA dose.
Table 1.
S1647T carriers | S1647T noncarriers | P (χ 2/t) values | |
---|---|---|---|
Sex ratio (M/F) | 30/15 | 25/20 | 0.280 (1.169) |
Age (years) | 60.79 ± 10.19 | 64.04 ± 8.17 | 0.098 (−1.672) |
Education (years) | 11.49 ± 4.22 | 11.31 ± 3.89 | 0.836 (0.208) |
Age at PD onset (years) | 55.51 ± 10.14 | 59.01 ± 8.04 | 0.073 (−1.814) |
Disease duration (years) | 5.29 ± 3.96 | 4.96 ± 3.86 | 0.691 (0.399) |
Onset side (right/left) | 34/11 | 36/9 | 0.612 (0.257) |
Symptom at PD onset (tremor/not tremor) | 27/18 | 26/19 | 0.830 (0.046) |
Axial symptom (yes/no) | 29/16 | 23/22 | 0.200 (1.640) |
UPDRS III | 23.34 ± 9.88 | 23.56 ± 12.71 | 0.930 (−0.088) |
Hoehn and Yahr | 2.02 ± 0.72 | 2.02 ± 0.83 | 1.000 (0) |
L-DOPA dose (mg/d) | 379.68 ± 264.27 | 400.39 ± 222.44 | 0.689 (−0.402) |
Anticholinergic use (yes/no) | 32/13 | 35/10 | 0.468 (0.526) |
3.2. Neuropsychological Assessment
Neuropsychological examinations did not reveal remarkable differences in language fluency (AFT), visuospatial function (CDT), attention (DST), memory (AVLT), or global cognitive function (MMSE) between LRRK2 S1647T carriers and noncarriers, but there was a significant difference in executive function (SWT-TIME scores) between the two groups (Table 2). A significant difference (P = 0.049) was also found for HAMD, with lower scores in the S1647T carriers, although the patients with definite depression have been excluded (Table 2).
Table 2.
Variables | S1647T carriers (n = 45) | S1647T noncarriers (n = 45) | P (t) values |
---|---|---|---|
MMSE | 27.78 ± 1.58 | 28.20 ± 1.83 | 0.244 (−1.172) |
AVLT (immediate) | 40.29 ± 10.31 | 40.07 ± 11.12 | 0.922 (0.098) |
AVLT (20 min) | 8.87 ± 2.84 | 8.73 ± 3.48 | 0.843 (0.199) |
SWCT-TIME | 44.20 ± 23.06 | 56.98 ± 26.64 | 0.017 (−2.433) |
SWCT-ERROR | 4.67 ± 3.00 | 5.60 ± 4.63 | 0.172 (−1.377) |
Animal fluency test | 16.71 ± 4.25 | 15.91 ± 5.20 | 0.426 (0.800) |
Clock-drawing test | 3.82 ± 0.44 | 3.89 ± 0.38 | 0.446 (−0.765) |
Digit span task | 12.73 ± 1.95 | 12.73 ± 1.88 | 1.000 (0) |
HAMD | 3.44 ± 3.23 | 5.11 ± 4.58 | 0.049 (−1.994) |
HAMA | 3.44 ± 2.58 | 4.11 ± 3.41 | 0.298 (−1.046) |
3.3. Analysis of Multiple Factors for SWCT-TIME Scores
Multiple linear regression analysis indicated that the correlation between S1647T polymorphism and SWCT-TIME scores did not reach significant level (P = 0.051). In addition, the possible risk factors for cognitive impairments were all found not correlated with the SWCT-TIME scores significantly after multiple linear regression analysis (Table 3).
Table 3.
Variable | P value | Beta value | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | |||
S1647T polymorphism | 0.051 | 0.213 | −0.044 | 21.740 |
Age (years) | 0.244 | −3.460 | −25.608 | 6.621 |
Education (years) | 0.776 | −0.030 | −1.496 | 1.121 |
Age at PD onset (years) | 0.220 | 3.615 | −6.105 | 26.072 |
Disease duration (years) | 0.214 | 1.528 | −5.904 | 26.003 |
UPDRS III | 0.087 | 0.217 | −0.072 | 1.053 |
Hoehn and Yahr | 0.883 | −0.020 | −9.811 | 8.454 |
L-DOPA dose (mg/d) | 0.344 | 0.129 | −0.015 | 0.042 |
HAMA | 0.901 | 0.017 | −2.144 | 2.431 |
HAMD | 0.644 | 0.065 | −1.358 | 2.183 |
4. Discussion
LRRK2 mutations were first identified as a cause of autosomal-dominant PD by Zimprich et al. in 2004 [20]; this finding had an instant, significant, and lasting impact on the understanding of the genetic basis of PD. Since then, additional LRRK2 polymorphisms in different loci have been reported [2, 21]. Although they are distributed worldwide, the effects of LRRK2 polymorphisms may vary in different populations. The LRRK2 S1647T polymorphism is found in Chinese population, but its influences on the clinical features of PD remain to be elucidated.
To answer the question of whether the LRRK2 S1647T polymorphism affects PD symptoms in Chinese subjects, we compared the core features of PD between two groups of patients: 45 LRRK2 S1647T carriers and 45 noncarriers. We did not identify any differences in motor symptoms between the two groups, which is consistent with a previous report that motor features in patients with LRRK2-related PD were similar to those with idiopathic PD [4].
In this study, MMSE was used as the global cognitive test but not MOCA which overlaps the animal fluency test and clock-drawing test in the following assessments. The result did not reveal significant differences in MMSE scores between LRRK2 S1647T carriers and noncarriers. Surprisingly, the SWCT-TIME scores of LRRK2 S1647T carriers were significantly lower than those of LRRK2 S1647T noncarriers, but there were no differences in the scores of other scales evaluating other cognitive domains such as language fluency, visuospatial function, memory, or attention. The risk factors for cognitive impairment, such as age, education, disease severity, and anticholinergic use, were also not significantly different between the two groups. Seemingly this finding suggests that executive function impairment is less severe in LRRK2 S1647T carriers. To be cautious, we conducted a multiple linear regression analysis and found that S1647T polymorphism was not the predictor of SWCT-TIME scores. The significant differences found by t-tests between LRRK2 S1647T carriers and noncarriers may be due to the fact that carriers were younger than noncarriers, even though no significant differences emerge between two groups. Another factor which could help to explain the better SWCT-TIME scores of carriers was the significantly lower HAMD scores of carriers, for the exclusion criteria of HAMD ≥ 20 points only excluded the patients with definite depression but those with possible depression were not excluded yet. It is well documented that cognitive scores can be influenced by depression state [22]. However, we cannot gain a conclusion that carriers are less depressive than noncarriers because the exclusion of patients with definite depression made the result not comprehensive.
Although there is no existing study regarding the correlation of nonmotor features and LRRK2 S1647T, some similar reports on relationships between cognitive impairment and LRRK2 polymorphisms in other loci reached different conclusions. In 2004, Paisán-Ruíz et al. [23] studied a small series of LRRK2 R1396G patients and reported that there was lower prevalence of cognitive impairments in LRRK2-related PD compared to subjects with idiopathic PD. In a multicentre study with large samples from across the world, Healy et al. [24] reported that cognition changes in patients with LRRK2-related PD were milder than those in subjects with idiopathic PD. However, the large samples did not include Chinese population. Estanga et al. [25] performed a detailed cognitive study of 60 Spanish PD patients and found that the neuropsychological performance of carriers with another LRRK2 variant (R1441G) was similar to that of LRRK2 R1441G noncarriers. The different conclusions among these studies might be due to different loci and different samples. Our study is the unique study to investigate the relationship between the particular polymorphism S1647T and cognitive function in Chinese PD population and it may contribute to the improvement of evaluation in worldwide PD population.
5. Conclusion
In summary, cognitive function seems to be similar among PD patients with different LRRK2 S1647T polymorphisms. However, these findings need to be confirmed by prospective studies that assess larger samples. Another limitation of this study was that it did not include a demographically matched control group. As a result, we did not have access to normative control data for the assessments and therefore cannot compare the level of cognitive difficulty experienced by the research subjects with control subjects. Further comparative studies between PD patients and healthy individuals with or without this polymorphism are needed to better examine its effect on cognitive function.
Acknowledgment
The authors would like to thank Editage (http://www.editage.cn/english-editing/) for English language editing.
Conflict of Interests
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests regarding the publication of this paper.
Authors' Contribution
Qi Wu, Ling Chen, Zhong Pei, and Yifan Zheng conceived and designed the study. Yifan Zheng, Yanmei Liu, Hongyan Zhou, and Wenbiao Xian performed the study. Yifan Zheng and Yingying Fang analyzed the data. Zhong Pei contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools. Yifan Zheng and Qi Wu wrote the paper. Ling Chen obtained agreement from the patients.
References
- 1.Eriksen J. L., Wszolek Z., Petrucelli L. Molecular pathogenesis of Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology. 2005;62(3):353–357. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.3.353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tan E.-K., Peng R., Teo Y.-Y., et al. Multiple LRRK2 variants modulate risk of Parkinson disease: a Chinese multicenter study. Human Mutation. 2010;31(5):561–568. doi: 10.1002/humu.21225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Zheng Y. F., Liu Y., Wu Q., et al. Confirmation of LRRK2 S1647T variant as a risk factor for Parkinson’s disease in Southern China. European Journal of Neurology. 2011;18(3):538–540. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2010.03164.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Khan N. L., Jain S., Lynch J. M., et al. Mutations in the gene LRRK2 encoding dardarin (PARK8) cause familial Parkinson's disease: clinical, pathological, olfactory and functional imaging and genetic data. Brain. 2005;128(12):2786–2796. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wu Q., Chen L., Zheng Y., et al. Cognitive impairment is common in Parkinson's disease without dementia in the early and middle stages in a Han Chinese cohort. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders. 2012;18(2):161–165. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ikeuchi T., Kakita A., Shiga A., et al. Patients homozygous and heterozygous for SNCA duplication in a family with parkinsonism and dementia. Archives of Neurology. 2008;65(4):514–519. doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.4.514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Skipper L., Li Y., Bonnard C., et al. Comprehensive evaluation of common genetic variation within LRRK2 reveals evidence for association with sporadic Parkinson's disease. Human Molecular Genetics. 2005;14(23):3549–3556. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddi376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kawakami F., Yabata T., Ohta E., et al. LRRK2 phosphorylates tubulin-associated tau but not the free molecule: LRRK2-mediated regulation of the tau-tubulin association and neurite outgrowth. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030834.e30834 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hughes A. J., Ben-Shlomo Y., Daniel S. E., Lees A. J. What features improve the accuracy of clinical diagnosis in Parkinson's disease: a clinicopathologic study. Neurology. 1992;42(6):1142–1146. doi: 10.1212/wnl.42.6.1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 1960;23(2):56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. The British Journal of Medical Psychology. 1959;32(1):50–55. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fahn S., Elton R. Unifined Parkinson's disease rating scale. In: Fahn S., Marsden C. D., Calne D. B., Goldstein M., editors. Recent Developments in Parkinson's Disease. Vol. 2. Florham Park, NJ, USA: Macmillan Health Care Information; 1987. pp. 153–304. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hoehn M. M., Yahr M. D. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17(5):427–442. doi: 10.1212/wnl.17.5.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E., McHugh P. R. ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chan A. S., Poon M. W. Performance of 7- to 95-year-old individuals in a Chinese version of the category fluency test. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1999;5(6):525–533. doi: 10.1017/S135561779956606X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sunderland T., Hill J. L., Mellow A. M., et al. Clock Drawing in Alzheimer's disease. A novel measure of dementia severity. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1989;37(8):725–729. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1989.tb02233.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mungas D. Differential clinical sensitivity of specific parameters of the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1983;51(6):848–855. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.51.6.848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yao S., Chen H., Jiang L., Tam W.-C. C. Replication of factor structure of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Chinese version in Chinese mainland non-clinical and schizophrenia samples. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2007;61(4):379–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01672.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stroop J. R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18(6):643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Zimprich A., Biskup S., Leitner P., et al. Mutations in LRRK2 cause autosomal-dominant parkinsonism with pleomorphic pathology. Neuron. 2004;44(4):601–607. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Berg D., Schweitzer K., Leitner P., et al. Type and frequency of mutations in the LRRK2 gene in familial and sporadic Parkinson's disease. Brain. 2005;128(12):3000–3011. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Farrin L., Hull L., Unwin C., Wykes T., David A. Effects of depressed mood on objective and subjective measures of attention. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2003;15(1):98–104. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.15.1.98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Paisán-Ruíz C., Jain S., Evans E. W., et al. Cloning of the gene containing mutations that cause PARK8-linked Parkinson's disease. Neuron. 2004;44(4):595–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Healy D. G., Falchi M., O'Sullivan S. S., et al. Phenotype, genotype, and worldwide genetic penetrance of LRRK2-associated Parkinson's disease: a case-control study. The Lancet Neurology. 2008;7(7):583–590. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70117-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Estanga A., Rodriguez-Oroz M. C., Ruiz-Martinez J., et al. Cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease related to the R1441G mutation in LRRK2. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders. 2014;20(10):1097–1100. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]