Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2015 Aug 21;10(8):e0135954. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135954

Cool Headed Individuals Are Better Survivors: Non-Consumptive and Consumptive Effects of a Generalist Predator on a Sap Feeding Insect

Orsolya Beleznai 1,2,#, Gergely Tholt 1,3,#, Zoltán Tóth 4,#, Vivien Horváth 1,5, Zsolt Marczali 2, Ferenc Samu 1,*
Editor: Csaba Moskát6
PMCID: PMC4546593  PMID: 26295476

Abstract

Non-consumptive effects (NCEs) of predators are part of the complex interactions among insect natural enemies and prey. NCEs have been shown to significantly affect prey foraging and feeding. Leafhopper's (Auchenorrhyncha) lengthy phloem feeding bouts may play a role in pathogen transmission in vector species and also exposes them to predation risk. However, NCEs on leafhoppers have been scarcely studied, and we lack basic information about how anti-predator behaviour influences foraging and feeding in these species. Here we report a study on non-consumptive and consumptive predator-prey interactions in a naturally co-occurring spider–leafhopper system. In mesocosm arenas we studied movement patterns during foraging and feeding of the leafhopper Psammotettix alienus in the presence of the spider predator Tibellus oblongus. Leafhoppers delayed feeding and fed much less often when the spider was present. Foraging movement pattern changed under predation risk: movements became more frequent and brief. There was considerable individual variation in foraging movement activity. Those individuals that increased movement activity in the presence of predators exposed themselves to higher predation risk. However, surviving individuals exhibited a ‘cool headed’ reaction to spider presence by moving less than leafhoppers in control trials. No leafhoppers were preyed upon while feeding. We consider delayed feeding as a “paradoxical” antipredator tactic, since it is not necessarily an optimal strategy against a sit-and-wait generalist predator.

Introduction

The consumption of prey by a predator is one of the most important ecological interactions, but costly anti-predator behaviour of prey, called non-consumptive effects (NCEs), also have considerable impact on ecological systems [1, 2]. The wide ranging behavioural responses of prey to the presence or cues of predators may result in reduced feeding [3], a lower quality diet [4, 5], physiological stress [6] or may be detrimental to reproduction [7]. A meta-analysis of studies in terrestrial systems showed substantial and significant decrease in foraging effort as a consequence of increased predation risk [8]. Such negative impacts of NCEs have the potential to cascade through ecological systems, affecting productivity and diversity at lower trophic levels [1].

Under predation risk, prey animals face a problem of optimization: they have to trade off foraging, feeding and reproduction against anti-predator vigilance, refuge seeking or fleeing [9, 10]. While a number of experimental results demonstrate that reduced activity enhances survival of the prey [11, 12], there are contrasting models which suggest increased activity resulting in higher elusiveness might also be a good tactic to avoid predation [13]. More recently, as variation in anti-predator behaviour at the level of the individual is gaining attention, it has been documented that responses to predator encounters interact with behavioural syndromes [14]. Sap feeding hemipteran insects are especially vulnerable to predation when their stylet is inserted in the plant tissues. Consequently, their decision whether to feed or for how long to feed should markedly depend on predation risk.

Here we report a study on the behavioural aspects of non-consumptive predator-prey interactions and possible consumptive outcome in a leafhopper-spider system. The grass-feeding leafhopper Psammotettix alienus (Dahlbom) (Hemiptera: Cicadellidae) is an important pest of cereals, and the only known vector of Wheat Dwarf Virus (WDV, Geminiviridae) [15], which causes serious damage to cereal crops. P. alienus has a lengthy phloem feeding phase [16], during which leafhoppers are essentially anchored to the plant by the opened serrate tip of their stylet [17], which–even if a potential predator is detected–can only be removed after several seconds. Virus acquisition and inoculation takes place during phloem feeding [18, 19]. Thus, the length of phloem feeding can be critical both for predator avoidance and for the effectiveness of virus transmission.

In NCE studies, spiders are frequently used as model predators because they are handled and manipulated easily [20], leave chemical cues in form of silk [3], and their NCEs often cascade through several trophic levels [21]. In this study we used a natural enemy of the leafhopper, the common agrobiont spider Tibellus oblongus (Walckenaer) (Araneae: Philodromidae) [22]. T. oblongus is a generalist sit-and-wait predator, active mostly during daytime. Without a web, it ambush hunts for a wide variety of prey [2325]. The species mostly occurs in the foliage of grassy vegetation [26], perching on grass or cereal leaves, precisely in the microhabitat where P. alienus feeds.

In order to understand the complex interactions among predators and sap feeding vector herbivores, it is first critical to document how predators impact herbivores. Focusing on NCEs, we hypothesized that the leafhoppers would alter their foraging activity and feeding behaviour when exposed to predation risk. We predicted that, compared to a control situation, under predation risk foraging will be reduced, which can be detected by change in the movement pattern, and we also expected that phloem feeding, detected by the production of honeydew, will be reduced by delaying and/or shortening of feeding events. We were also interested in individual variation in anti-predator behaviour and its bearing on prey survival. We predicted that under identical predation risk the individual with lower foraging movement and feeding activity will have a higher probability of survival. To test this, we exposed two leafhoppers to one spider, and compared foraging movement activity and feeding rate of victims and survivors facing the same spider.

Materials and Methods

Experimental animals

Collection and keeping

To initiate laboratory populations, T. oblongus and P. alienus were collected from fallow and grassland areas near Budapest and Székesfehérvár, Hungary (47°32'58.94"N, 18°55'48.81"E; 47°14'40.73"N, 18°25'40.45"E, respectively) by sweep net and suction sampling. After collection, P. alienus were kept in a climate room (L/D: 16/8 h, 23/16°C regime) on potted young barley (Hordeum vulgare L. cv MV Jubilant); pots caged with fine meshing. Juveniles were kept until adulthood; only adults were used for the experiments. Animals, randomly selected for the trials, were withheld from food for 30 min and their sex was determined. Spider individuals were kept under the same conditions, in individual plastic vials with moistened Plaster of Paris base to maintain humidity, fed ad libitum with Drosophila melanogaster (Meigen) twice a week. For the predator treatment we selected juvenile spiders randomly from the laboratory stock. Small spiders were not selected; the body mass range was 5–15 mg. Prior to the experiment, spiders received no food for one week to standardize hunger level. Their body mass was measured to the nearest 0.1 mg before the trials. Individual spiders and leafhoppers were only used once in our experiments.

Ethics statement

None of the arthropod species used in the present experiment are protected in Hungary. Under Hungarian law (act 348/2006, paragraph 10 (3)), unprotected arthropods can be freely collected in fields and their use in laboratory experiments requires no permit or approval by ethics committee. Collecting sites (crop fields and grassland patches) were either owned by the research institute (Plant Protection Institute, Centre for Agricultural Research, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest) or were publicly accessible, therefore no permit was needed to access them. During experimentation we avoided causing any unnecessary harm, suffering or distress to the study subjects.

Behavioural observations

Procedures of observations

Observational trials of P. alienus were conducted in mesocosm arenas in the presence and absence of the spider. Each arena consisted of a pot (10 cm diam.) pre-planted with 3 stems of barley in sterilised soil. Pots were fitted with plexiglass cylinders (6 cm diameter, 20 cm high), covered with fine meshing. Arenas received uniform lighting, temperature was 23°C, relative humidity 30–50%. Trials were performed when plants were at 2–3 leaves stage.

During a trial, after placing the animals into the arena, their behaviour was monitored visually for the prescribed period of the trial. We used the Observer XT software (version 11, Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Netherlands) to record the timing and duration of behavioural events.

Movement activity observations

We made observations of movement activity in 29–42 min trials (Table 1). These were run in two series, in August-November 2012 and in October-November 2013. We recorded the following behavioural events: (i) the occurrence of predation; (ii) moving events, observed separately both for spiders and leafhoppers, which were more or less continuous change of position by walking, or repositioning by jumping; (iii) stationary events, being mutually exclusive to moving events; (iv) honeydew production by the leafhopper; (v) feeding by the spider. If a behavioural event ended artificially (the leafhopper was preyed on or observation ended), then we considered the last behaviour as incomplete (censored). Such incomplete behaviours were included in count type variables (they were counted), but their durations were not taken into account in statistical tests.

Table 1. Subjects present in observational trials.
Trials
Observation Leafhopper number Spider treatment Leafhopper control Spider control
Feeding (3 h) 2L L L S (N = 44) L L (N = 44)
Movement activity (c. 30 min) 1L L S (N = 96) L (N = 96) S (N = 96)
2L L L S (N = 100) L L (N = 100) S (N = 100)

L = leafhopper, S = spider, in brackets the number of trials run.

In movement activity observations we had three trials (arenas) per session: a spider treatment, a leafhopper control and a spider control (Table 1). We ran two setup variants, one with one leafhopper per trial (1L), and another with two leafhoppers (2L).We refer to this as ‘leafhopper number’ variable. In the case of 2L setups, one of the individuals was marked with a small dot of silver glue, the same applied in electrical penetration graph studies [16]. We found no effect of this marking on the probability of becoming a survivor or victim during the trials (test of independence; χ 2 1 = 0.15, P = 0.695). The main reason for having 2L situations was that we could compare the behaviour of prospective victim and survivor individuals and could follow the behaviour of a survivor leafhopper after the spider attacked the other individual.

The sequence of moving and stationary events (pattern of movement activity) was compared in various time windows, or “periods”, dictated by the research question (Fig 1). Questions regarding movement activities during foraging were studied in the ‘foraging period’, that is the period before a leafhopper started to feed. Honeydew production always occurred in the course of a stationary event. Since a variable but often lengthy period is needed for the leafhoppers to reach the phloem with their stylets [16], we regarded the whole stationary event during which honeydew was produced as part of the feeding process, and therefore excluded from the foraging period (Fig 1). If the leafhopper did not feed, then all events of the trial were counted as foraging period.

Fig 1. Schematic representation of the movement activity observations in a two-leafhopper session.

Fig 1

Activity pattern and feeding events are depicted for subjects in the trials, foraging, pre-attack and post-attack periods are additionally marked. In two leafhopper trials, when the victim was attacked, if the current behaviour of the survivor extended beyond the attack on the victim, then this extension was included in the pre-attack period.

Questions regarding the effect of predation were studied during ‘pre-attack’ and ‘post-attack periods’ comprising two levels of the variable ‘predation period’ (Fig 1). Victim behaviour could only be studied during the pre-attack period, but survivor behaviour could be compared between pre- and post-attack periods. During post-attack periods spiders were effectively feeding on their prey for the whole, or nearly the whole period (spider feeding events did not end until the end of observation in 113 cases out of the total 127; the mean duration of uncensored feeding events was 18.1 min). We compared trials where predation occurred (N = 53) to trials where spider was present but no predation occurred (N = 47). We have artificially created predation periods also in the no predation trials by randomly assigning one of the attack times of the predation trials to no predation trials. We thereby created artificial pre- and post-attack periods with nearly identical duration distributions in both predation and no predation trials.

Feeding observations

Feeding observations were carried out in 3 h trials and focused on the duration of P. alienus feeding; movement activity was not recorded here. These longer observations were needed because often both the initiation of feeding and the total feeding time were longer than the duration of the movement activity observations. We only ran 2L sessions (Table 1). Since spider behaviour (apart from predation) was not observed, we had no spider control trials. There was one series of the observation sessions in April 2013.

Statistical analysis

Statistical procedures

Response variables, random terms, fixed predictors and observed variables of subject states are described below, and listed in detail in Table 2. We applied linear mixed-effect models (LME) to analyse the effect of predators on various aspects of foraging movements using the ‘nlme’ package of R 3.1.0 [27, 28]. Subjects and experimental units were used as random blocking factors where applicable. The necessity of each random factor was investigated by comparing the models with and without the given factor using likelihood ratio tests. When random effects were found negligible, LME models shrank to linear models (LM). In all model fittings we used backward removal procedure, starting with the full models containing all explanatory variables (main effects and specific interactions), then dropped the predictor with the highest P-value in each step until only P ≤ 0.05 effects remained (if there were any) in the final models [29]. AIC values and Akaike weights of the final and other candidate models are presented in S1 Table. Requirements of the fitted models were checked by plot diagnosis. For significant predictors of the final models we estimated effect sizes [30] as the proportion of variance explained by each trait (η 2 and its 95% CI) [31]. Parameter estimates of the predictors in the final models together with their standard error are shown in S2 Table.

Table 2. Behavioural responses (D) and potential predictors (I) used in the fitted models as dependent and independent variables, and observed variables (O) used in proportions and Chi square tests.
Variable name Description Role / Type 1
Number of movements number of moving events D / count
Duration of moving events duration of moving events D / cont
Duration of stationary events duration of stationary events D / cont
Duration of feeding duration of leafhopper feeding events D / cont
Moving % foraging period total movement duration as % of foraging period D / cont
Time starting time of an event (relative to start of trial) I / cont
Time to feed starting time of the first feeding event in a trial I / cont
Subject observed individual I / rnd block
Trial observations of one arena I / rnd block
Session trials observed in parallel I / rnd block
Series sessions run in the same period of year I / rnd block
Spider presence of spider in a trial (present/control) I / nominal
Predation whether predation occurred in a trial (yes/no) I / nominal
Prey whether the individual became prey (yes/no) I / nominal
Status leafhopper status (control/victim/survivor) I / nominal
Sex leafhopper sex (male/female) I / nominal
Movement state prey movement state (at time of predation) O / nominal
Leafhopper number number of Psammotettix individuals in a trial (1L/2L) I / nominal
Feeding status whether a leafhopper started to feed (produce honeydew) during a trial (fed/not fed) O / nominal
Predation period period within a trial (pre-attack/post-attack) I / nominal
Observation duration duration of an observed period or a whole trial I / cont
Spider weight weight (mg) of Tibellus individuals I / cont

1 cont = continuous, rnd = random

It can be assumed that a leafhopper placed on a host plant will eventually start to feed; likewise, when feeding started it will inevitably end at some time point. Therefore we analysed time to the first feeding (‘time to feeding’) and ‘duration of feeding’ events on a survival analysis platform in JMP 6.0 [32] using the feeding observation data-set. We applied a Cox Proportional-hazards (CPH) model [33], used effectively for event–time data in foraging studies [34]. If feeding did not start or end until the end of an observation or a predatory event, those cases were marked as censored.

We applied proportion tests for the investigation of the equality of proportions (case of moving/stationary states of prey and non-prey leafhoppers at the moment of predation), and Chi square tests to test for independence of frequency data (number of individuals feeding, not feeding, becoming victims or survivor). Variables were subjected to logarithm (duration variables) or square-root transformation (movement number variable) to improve their fit to normal distribution. All tests were two-tailed with α = 0.05. We numbered each statistical model, listed in Table 3 and described briefly below. Models are grouped by basic questions they try to answer.

Table 3. The effect of different variables in statistical models and tests on leafhopper foraging movement activity and feeding.
{Model No.} Dependent variable (Observation a ) Predictors df Test statistics F/χ2 η 2 (95% CI) P
Q1 d : Does spider presence affect leafhoppers’ activity?
{1} Duration of moving events b (M) Observation duration b 1,442 12.70 0.03 [0.01–0.07] 0.0004
Time b 1,1339 27.34 0.02 [0.01–0.04] <0.0001
Sex 1,442 1.26 0 [0–0.02] 0.263
Spider 1,442 16.33 0.04 [0.01–0.08] <0.0001
Spider × Sex 1,442 7.47 0.02 [0–0.05] 0.007
Leafhopper number 1,441 0.08 - 0.784
Spider × Time b 1,1338 0.06 - 0.808
{2} Duration of stationary events b (M) Observation duration b 1,445 91.86 0.17 [0.11–0.23] <0.0001
Time b 1,1481 87.20 0.06 [0.04–0.08] <0.0001
Spider 1,445 31.69 0.07 [0.03–0.12] <0.0001
Leafhopper number 1,445 10.00 0.02 [0–0.06] 0.002
Sex 1,444 0.16 - 0.692
Spider × Sex 1,443 0.09 - 0.766
Spider × Time b 1,1480 0.92 - 0.337
{3} Number of movements c (M) Observation duration 1,128 245.59 0.66 [0.56–0.72] <0.0001
Spider 1,326 10.09 0.03 [0.004–0.07] 0.002
Leafhopper number 1,326 5.14 0.02 [0–0.05] 0.024
Sex 1,127 1.53 - 0.218
Spider × Sex 1,126 2.16 - 0.145
{4} Moving % foraging period b (M) Observation duration b 1,123 33.56 0.21 [0.10–0.33] <0.0001
Spider 1,321 1.31 - 0.254
Leafhopper number 1,321 2.32 - 0.128
Sex 1,122 2.71 - 0.103
Spider × Sex 1,121 0.71 - 0.403
Q2: Are nearby leafhoppers’ activity affected by act of predation and consumption?
{5} Number of movements c (M) Observation duration b 1,196 86.74 0.31 (0.20–0.40) <0.0001
Predation period 1,196 6.19 0.03 (0.001–0.09) 0.014
Predation 1,195 0.06 - 0.815
Sex 1,195 0.35 - 0.553
Predation period × Predation 1,194 1.68 - 0.196
{6} Duration of moving events b (M) Sex 1,127 13.14 0.09 (0.02–0.20) 0.0004
Predation period 1,126 3.25 - 0.074
Observation duration b 1,126 0.45 - 0.504
Predation 1,126 0.02 - 0.902
Predation period × Predation 1,124 0.01 - 0.941
Q3: Is leafhopper feeding affected by spider presence?
{7} Time to feed (F) Spider 1 13.68 - 0.0002
Sex 1 12.03 - 0.0005
Spider × Sex 1 4.8 - 0.028
{8} Duration of feeding (F) Spider 1 1.55 - 0.212
{9} Feeding status/ Spider (F) - 1 27.18 - <0.0001
{10} Feeding status/ Spider (M) - 1 61.57 - <0.0001
{11} Feeding status/ Prey (F) - 1 6.43 - 0.011
{12} Feeding status/ Prey (M) - 1 1.57 - 0.209
Q4: Is leafhopper activity a predictor of becoming victim?
{13} Number of movements c (M) Observation duration b 1,138 51.31 0.27 [0.15–0.38] <0.0001
Prey 1,138 29.94 0.18 [0.08–0.29] <0.0001
Sex 1,137 0.27 0.601
Sex × Prey 1,136 0.03 - 0.858
{14} Duration of moving events b (M) Observation duration b 1,68 8.27 0.11 [0.01–0.25] 0.005
Sex 1,34 0.21 - 0.646
Prey 1,34 0.34 - 0.561
Sex × Prey 1,32 1.57 - 0.220
{15} Moving % foraging period b (M) Observation duration b 1,261 35.84 0.12 [0.06–0.20] <0.0001
Status 2,261 3.74 0.03 [0–0.07] 0.025
Sex 1,260 3.25 - 0.073
Sex × Status 2,258 0.49 - 0.613
{16} Movement state/Prey (M) - 1 61.07 - <0.0001
Q5: Is leafhopper activity correlated with spider activity?
{17} Number of movements c (M) Observation duration b 1,265 72.47 0.21 [0.13–0.30] <0.0001
Leafhopper number 1,265 27.55 0.09 [0.04–0.16] <0.0001
Prey 1,265 38.62 0.13 [0.06–0.20] <0.0001
Spider activity 1,265 3.91 0.01 [0–0.06] 0.049
Spider weight 1,264 3.19 - 0.075
Prey × Spider activity 1,264 0.13 - 0.715
{18} Duration of moving events b (M) Observation duration b 1,167 5.53 0.03 [0.001–0.10] 0.020
Spider activity 1,167 7.32 0.04 [0.003–0.11] 0.008
Leafhopper number 1,166 0.08 - 0.774
Prey 1,50 1.40 - 0.242
Spider weight 1,166 0.63 - 0.428
Prey × Spider activity 1,49 2.23 - 0.142

Test statistics and P-values for the non-significant predictors were obtained by including them one by one into the final model (in bold). Test statistics is F for all LME/LM models {1–6}, {13–15}, {17–18} and χ 2 for CPH models {7–8}, Chi square test {9–12} and the proportion test {16}.

aObservations: M = Movement activity observation, F = Feeding observation

blog-transformed

csquare-root transformed

dnumbered question referring to questions in Material and Methods

Statistical models and tests of predation risk effects

Question 1: Does spider presence affect leafhoppers’ activity? The effect of predation risk (variable ‘spider’; presence of spider levels: present/control) on the movement activity pattern of leafhoppers during the foraging period was modelled for the duration of moving events {1}, the duration of stationary events {2}, the number of movements {3} and the percent time leafhoppers spent moving during the foraging period {4} (Table 3).

Question 2: When predation and ensuing consumption of prey happens, are the activities of a nearby leafhopper affected? The effect of actual predation (variable ‘predation’; levels: predation yes/no) on survivors was tested in 2L trials by comparing movement activity pattern between the two levels (pre- / post-attack periods) of ‘predation period’. Since predation periods were defined for no predation trials as well (see Movement activity observations section), we established a two-way arrangement, where the effect of ‘predation period’ and ‘predation’ was analysed by LME models (Table 3) on the number of movements {5} and the average duration of moving events {6} of the leafhoppers.

Question 3: Is leafhopper feeding affected by spider presence? We modelled time to feeding and duration of feeding events by CPH models on data from the 3 h long feeding observations. Spider and sex were included in time to feeding model {7}, whereas sex was not considered in feeding duration model {8} because in spider treatment trials only 2 female and 8 male leafhoppers started feeding, compared to 28 female and 22 male individuals feeding in control trials. Separately for both the feeding observation and the movement activity observation datasets, we also tested for independence between leafhopper feeding status (fed/not fed) and spider presence (present/control) {9,10}, and leafhopper feeding status and prey status (victim/survivor) {11,12}.

Question 4: Is leafhopper activity a predictor of becoming victim? We examined which movement activity pattern proved better at avoiding predation and which made the leafhopper more prone to it, by analysing the potential behavioural differences between victim and survivor leafhoppers. In one approach, considering the pre-attack period, we compared number of movements {13} and the average duration of moving events {14} between survivors and victims (control leafhoppers excluded). Then, considering the respective foraging periods, we compared the percent time spent moving during foraging among leafhoppers of control, survivor and victim status {15}. In another approach we analysed whether moving or not moving (levels of the variable ‘movement state’) was the more likely behaviour occurring at the time of the predation, analysed by a proportion test {16}.

Question 5: Is leafhopper activity correlated with spider activity? The potential relationship between the activity of spiders and leafhoppers within the same trial was analysed both for the number of movements {17} and for the average duration of moving events {18} of leafhoppers during the pre-attack period (Table 3). Since movement number and movement durations (log-transformed to improve its fit to normal distribution) of the spiders were highly correlated, we performed principal component analyses (PCA) on these two variables and considered the first principal component as a proxy for spider activity in models {17–18}. The first component (spider activity, henceforward) was positively correlated with both movement measures and accounted for 66% of the total variance.

Results

Effect of perceived risk on movement patterns

Question 1: Leafhoppers changed their pattern of movement activity during foraging if they were exposed to predation risk. We found that the average duration of both moving and stationary events became slightly, but significantly shorter if a spider was present (Fig 2, Table 3 {1,2}). The shortening of all event types should result in more events, and indeed there were significantly more movement events in spider treatments (Table 3 {3}). However, shorter but more numerous events did not change the overall duration leafhoppers spent moving in the presence of a spider (Table 3 {4}).

Fig 2. Change in moving and stationary event durations as a function of time.

Fig 2

Relationship between the start of event and event duration (both log-transformed) during moving (a) and being stationary (b) in the foraging period.

We also observed a “background” shortening of the duration of moving and stationary events over the course of the trials that happened independently of spider presence (Fig 2). The duration of both moving and stationary events weakly but significantly decreased with time (Table 3 {1,2}). Also, the duration of moving events was longer in males compared to females when a spider was present (Table 3 {1}), and the duration of stationary events was longer where there were two leafhoppers present (Table 3 {2}). In all instances, Spider × Time interactions were not significant (Table 3 {1,2}), thus the slope of the “background” temporal changes in event durations was independent of spider presence, as the temporal trends found were similar between spider and control trials.

Question 2: We found that in 2L trials where one leafhopper fell as victim, the survivor individual moved more frequently in the post-attack period (during which the victim individual was fed on by the spider) than in the pre-attack period. However, if we designate artificial pre- and post-attack periods to trials where spider was present but no predation occurred, then leafhoppers compared within the same periods showed no difference either in the number of movements (Fig 3, Table 3 {5}) or in the duration of moving events (Table 3 {6}). Thus, predation on the victim individual did not exert a significant effect on movement activity pattern of survivor individuals.

Fig 3. Number of movement events in survivor leafhoppers in trials with or without predation during the pre-attack and post-attack periods.

Fig 3

The same letters signify that none of the groups were not significantly different (at P = 0.05) according to a Tukey HSD test.

Effect of risk on leafhopper feeding

Question 3: Spider presence affected leafhopper feeding mostly by delaying it; n the spider treatment the time lag until the leafhoppers started to feed was significantly longer than in the control (Table 3 {7}, Fig 4). According to the survival analysis model, the “hazard” that a leafhopper starts to feed in the absence of spider was 3.82 times (95% CI: 1.79–10.39) greater than in the presence of spider. Male leafhoppers started feeding earlier, and the interaction between sex and spider treatment indicated that male time lag to feed was significantly less prolonged in the presence of spider than that of the females (Table 3 {7}). The duration of the feeding events was not significantly influenced by spider presence (Table 3 {8}).

Fig 4. The instantaneous rate of leafhopper feeding initiation as a function of observation time.

Fig 4

Blue line represents ‘Spider treatment’, red line ‘Leafhopper control’, shaded areas are 95% CI.

In the control trials of the movement observations, which lasted only c. 30 mins, 27% of the leafhoppers started to feed, whereas in the feeding observation controls, which lasted 3 h, 45.5% of the leafhoppers started to feed. In both movement and feeding observations, in the spider treatments these figures were highly significantly lower, 3.7% and 10.2%, respectively (Table 3 {9,10}).

Individual variation in predator-prey interactions

Leafhopper activity, including feeding, did affect the chances of becoming victim. While fewer leafhoppers fed in the spider treatments, those which did feed consistently had a lower rate of predation (Table 4), which in the feeding observations proved to be significant (Table 3 {11,12}). In those four cases when both feeding and predation occurred, the spider attacked the leafhopper after it finished feeding and started to move. No leafhopper was preyed upon while actually feeding.

Table 4. Number of leafhoppers started to feed, and fell as prey in the spider and control treatments during sessions of feeding and movement observations.

Feeding status Feeding observations Movement observations
Spider treatment Control Spider treatment Control
victim Survivor victim survivor
Fed 1 8 40 3 8 80
Not fed 44 35 48 132 152 216
Total 45 43 88 135 160 296

Question 4: Leafhoppers that became victims moved more frequently than survivors (Table 3 {13}), but average movement duration was not affected (Table 3 {14}). Comparing victims and survivors to control animals revealed that the percentage of time spent moving during foraging was significantly different among these groups (Fig 5, Table 3 {15}). Survivors spent less time moving than controls (P = 0.018), and victims tended to spend more time moving than controls (P = 0.051) (Fig 5).

Fig 5. Total time spent moving expressed as percent of foraging time in control, victim and survivor leafhoppers.

Fig 5

Groups marked with different letters are significantly different at P = 0.05 according to Tukey HSD test.

Leafhoppers’ movement activity was not independent of the likelihood of becoming a prey (Table 3 {16}): 74.7% of the victims were moving at the moment of predation, whereas the proportion of moving individuals at the same time point was only 9.9% among the survivors.

Question 5: Leafhopper movement number was significantly correlated with the spiders’ activity (Table 3 {17}). After controlling for observation duration, leafhopper movement number significantly increased if the spiders moved more (Fig 6, Table 3 {17}). The effect of the variable ‘prey’ (levels: victim/survivor) was significant in this model, as well, meaning that prospective victims and survivors differed in their movements even after accounting for spider activity (Fig 6). Surprisingly, spider size had a weak negative effect on leafhoppers’ activity. The duration of leafhoppers’ moving events was only affected by the number of movement events of spiders (Table 3 {18}).

Fig 6. The relationship between spider activity and leafhopper movement number in prey and non-prey individuals.

Fig 6

Spider activity was expressed as PCA first component calculated from spiders’ number and duration of movement events.

Discussion

The behavioural observations clearly demonstrated that leafhoppers reacted to predation risk by altering both their foraging and feeding behaviour. However, analysing movement data during foraging revealed that, independently of predator-induced stress, there is also a baseline temporal change which has to be controlled for if predation risk effects are to be detected. The baseline change manifested in both moving and stationary events becoming gradually shorter, signifying an accelerating number of movements. An acceleration of movement activity might arise from the perception of environmental gradients, e.g. chemical or tactile cues on the leaf surface that indicate the vicinity of a favourable feeding spot. Modelling studies demonstrated that even sparse sensory signals may lead the forager to concentrate search effort near feeding targets [35]. We can presume that energetic state, i.e. hunger, may also act as a driver that can be responsible for accelerated movement activity during foraging [36].

Predation risk seems to have interfered with the baseline trend by shifting the foraging towards even more frequent movements. As a result, P. alienus had more, but shorter, moving events when a spider was present in the arena. Contrary to this finding, our initial hypothesis predicted reduced activity, which was found to be a common type of anti-predator behaviour ranging from less movement [12] to death feigning [37]. However, increased movement is not necessarily dangerous for prey [13] and it can be expected if, for instance, the detection of a motionless prey increases with time [38], or if prey waste cues accumulate increasing the probability of detection over time [39]. Besides, predator presence could have interacted with physiological stress responses in the prey, as reviewed by Hawlena and Schmitz [40]. In particular, internal stress from starvation has been shown to alter anti-predator behaviour in another hemipteran, the aphid Acyrthosiphon pisum (Harris) [41].

Sensing killed congeners, possibly through alarm cues, are known to play a role in the detection of the predator and modulating anti-predator reaction in aphids [42]. However, the experiments reported here gave no indication of the latter mechanism in P. alienus, since leafhoppers gave the same reaction to spiders, whether they were present in the arenas or had already captured a conspecific.

Movement activity during the foraging phase was decisive for the fate of leafhopper individuals. There was a dynamic relationship between leafhopper and spider movements. Leafhoppers gave a gradated response to spiders, as they moved the least in the control trials, and when a spider was present their activity correlated with the spider’s activity. However, even after controlling for spider activity, victims still showed significantly more movements. The same phenomenon was revealed by directly comparing victims and survivors facing the same spider in the same trial. Leafhoppers are stationary while feeding, and those leafhoppers which engaged in feeding in spite of spider presence had a lower probability of predation, and in particular, no leafhopper actually feeding was preyed upon by spider. Thus, paradoxically, those individuals which showed the “typical response” of increased movement frequency to predator fell victims, and those which were”cool headed” and gave a muted response often survived.

So, why did certain individuals increase time spent moving, while others decreased it in the presence of a spider? The imperfect behaviour of victims under predation threat could be one explanation. As Welton et al. [43] argue, this might arise because there is often no second chance in learning about predation risk. Possibly, the observed behaviours might be adaptive outside the laboratory, e.g. under different predator density [44, 45]. Furthermore, our observed reaction may be generic, representing an adaptation to multiple predator species [46] with a variety of predatory tactics [47]. Leafhoppers have various spider predators [23, 48] which employ variations of sit-and-wait strategy, but also have many actively searching insect predators, like predatory bugs [49], lacewing larvae [50] and ladybird beetles [51]. Adaptation to more active predators is also hinted by the observation, that leafhopper activity was positively correlated to spider activity. If generic reaction would be the cause for the observed imperfect behaviour, that would suggest that increased predator diversity may increase prey vulnerability through limitations in adapting to various predatory tactics.

Another possible explanation for paradoxical increased activity is the risk taken by active foraging is traded off against some benefits or avoidance of other adverse effects. Frequent feeding is a necessity for sap feeding insects. They have a low endurance of starvation, because without access to sap fluids they are prone to dehydration [52]. Studies have found that starvation elicited a gradated increase in foraging movements; for instance foraging flight in the case of Aphis fabae (Scopoli) [53]. Poorer body condition resulting from access only to inferior host plants led the monophagous aphid Uroleucon jaceae (Linnaeus) to continue feeding and thus exhibit a more risk prone strategy of reduced escape behaviour in the presence of predators [54]. The time scale that starvation changes sap feeders’ behaviour is as short as 1.5 hours, during which significant changes in foraging behaviour were already detected [55]. These examples highlight the possible trade-off between condition and predation risk avoidance, so we propose that a likely mechanism for the paradoxical increased movement response of P. alienus lies in the interaction between predation risk and prey condition.

Our basic prediction that predation risk would negatively affect feeding was partly supported, as leafhoppers considerably delayed their feeding and fed much less often in the presence of a spider, although duration of feeding was not affected. Sacrificing food for safety seems to be the most common NCE of predators [8, 56], ranging from the rejection of flowers by bumble bees if a crab spider is present [57] to reduced feeding by elk after the re-introduction of wolves into Yellowstone National Park [58]. For sap feeders, patch exploitation needs an initial investment of mouthpart insertion and finding suitable vascular elements within the plant tissues. We argue that since insertion carries both a metabolically high cost and the risk of decreased escape opportunity, it is mostly the decision whether to start feeding that should depend on predation risk. The observed delayed feeding might fit a successful avoidance strategy against an average predator; however, individuals that were actually feeding during the observations experienced a substantially lower risk of predation, indicating that this strategy might be suboptimal when facing a specific sit-and-wait generalist predator.

The experiments reported here demonstrated that the presence of a spider triggers important changes in the foraging and feeding behaviour of a leafhopper, that is also vector of a plant virus and other plant diseases [18, 19]. When spider was present, feeding was delayed by P. alienus, resulting in significantly fewer feeding events. This may have important implications on virus transmission and epidemics. Details of whether, and how, the process of stylet insertion is modified under predation risk, remains to be investigated and could be important in the spread of plant pathogens such as the Wheat Dwarf Virus.

Our study showed that generalist predators, like the spider T. oblongus, exert significant non-consumptive effects on sap feeding insects. We observed the strongest reaction in reducing the number of feeding events of the leafhoppers by significantly delaying them, but the pattern of foraging movement activity was also affected. Studying individual variation in prey movements showed the interesting phenomenon of certain individuals not ceasing movement activity in the presence of predator, thus exposing themselves to higher predation risk. The observed generic strategy of leafhoppers to delay feeding when spider was present, also did not help avoiding predation. We argue that these phenomena might be due to a generic adaptation to a suite of natural enemies, which may prove imperfect facing a particular predator. A possible implication of non-consumptive effects on vector organisms is that they may cascade to further levels of the vector-pathogen-plant system, which we anticipate as an important area for future research.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. AIC values and the corresponding Akaike weights calculated for the LME models in Table 3.

Candidate models for each dependent variable are sorted by their AIC value from the lowest to highest. Models in bold are the final models obtained during the applied model selection procedure. In most cases, final models had the lowest AIC value among the candidates. In model {4} and {15} the best model and the consecutive one cannot be differentiated according to their AIC value (i.e. ΔAIC<2). In model {6}, the model including ‘Predation period’ had lower AIC value (but ΔAIC was <2 between the two models) than our final model. In all these cases, we took uncertainty regarding our final model into account during the interpretation of the results.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Parameter estimates of the significant predictors in the final LME models.

For each factor, the effects of additional levels (i.e. compared to the first level of the given predictor) are shown (the name of the level is written in brackets).

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Ms. Erika Botos for field and laboratory assistance. Previous versions of the manuscript were helpfully commented on by Ann Rypstra, James Harwood and Jamin Dreyer. We are grateful for linguistic revision by Jamin Dreyer.

Data Availability

All relevant data are available via Figshare (http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1472880).

Funding Statement

Funding for the study was provided by British Ecological Society Research grant (http://www.britishecologicalsociety.org) 4768/5806 to FS and Országos Tudományos Kutatási Alapprogramok (K81971; www.otka.hu/en/) to FS. During the study, ZT was supported by the ‘Lendület’ programme of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (MTA, LP2012-24/2012; www.mta.hu), Országos Tudományos Kutatási Alapprogramok grant (PD108938; www.otka.hu/en/) and the postdoctoral research programme of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (SZ-029/2013; www.mta.hu). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Schmitz OJ, Grabowski JH, Peckarsky BL, Preisser EL, Trussell GC, Vonesh JR. From individuals to ecosystem function: Toward an integration of evolutionary and ecosystem ecology. Ecology. 2008;89(9):2436–45. 10.1890/07-1030.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Lima SL. Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions—What are the ecological effects of anti-predator decision-making? Bioscience. 1998;48(1):25–34. 10.2307/1313225 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Rypstra AL, Buddle CM. Spider silk reduces insect herbivory. Biol Lett. 2013;9(1). 20120948 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0948 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Barnier F, Valeix M, Duncan P, Chamaille-Jammes S, Barre P, Loveridge AJ, et al. Diet quality in a wild grazer declines under the threat of an ambush predator. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;281(1785):5 10.1098/rspb.2014.0446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Jones EI, Dornhaus A. Predation risk makes bees reject rewarding flowers and reduce foraging activity. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65(8):1505–11. 10.1007/s00265-011-1160-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Slos S, Meester LD, Stoks R. Behavioural activity levels and expression of stress proteins under predation risk in two damselfly species. Ecol Entomol. 2009;34(3):297–303. 10.1111/j.1365-2311.2008.01077.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Li DQ, Jackson RR. Influence of diet-related chemical cues from predators on the hatching of egg-carrying spiders. J Chem Ecol. 2005;31(2):333–42. 10.1007/s10886-005-1344-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Verdolin JL. Meta-Analysis of Foraging and Predation Risk Trade-Offs in Terrestrial Systems. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60(4):457–64. 10.2307/25063837 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Rothley KD, Schmitz OJ, Cohon JL. Foraging to balance conflicting demands: novel insights from grasshoppers under predation risk. Behav Ecol. 1997;8(5):551–9. 10.1093/beheco/8.5.551 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Embar K, Kotler BP, Mukherjee S. Risk management in optimal foragers: the effect of sightlines and predator type on patch use, time allocation, and vigilance in gerbils. Oikos. 2011;120(11):1657–66. 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2011.19278.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Skelly DK. Activity level and the susceptibility of anuran larvae to predation. Anim Behav. 1994;47(2):465–8. 10.1006/anbe.1994.1063 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Bell RD, Rypstra AL, Persons MH. The effect of predator hunger on chemically mediated antipredator responses and survival in the wolf spider Pardosa milvina (Araneae: Lycosidae). Ethology. 2006;112(9):903–10. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01244.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Lima SL. Putting predators back into behavioral predator-prey interactions. Trends Ecol Evol. 2002;17(2):70–5. 10.1016/s0169-5347(01)02393-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. McDermott DR, Chips MJ, McGuirk M, Armagost F, DiRienzo N, Pruitt JN. Boldness is influenced by sublethal interactions with predators and is associated with successful harem infiltration in Madagascar hissing cockroaches. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2014;68(3):425–35. 10.1007/s00265-013-1657-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Vacke J. Wheat dwarf virus disease. Biol Plant. 1961;3(3):228–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Tholt G, Samu F, Kiss B. Penetration and feeding behaviour on host and non-host plants in a virus vector sap feeding insect: an Electrical Penetration Graph study. Entomol Exp Appl. 2015;155:123–36. doi: 0.1111/eea.12290. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Zhao L, Dai W, Zhang C, Zhang Y. Morphological characterization of the mouthparts of the vector leafhopper Psammotettix striatus (L.) (Hemiptera: Cicadellidae). Micron. 2010;41(7):754–9. Epub 2010/07/02. 10.1016/j.micron.2010.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Fereres A, Moreno A. Behavioural aspects influencing plant virus transmission by homopteran insects. Virus Res. 2009;141(2):158–68. 10.1016/j.virusres.2008.10.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Stafford CA, Walker GP, Creamer R. Stylet penetration behavior resulting in inoculation of beet severe curly top virus by beet leafhopper, Circulifer tenellus . Entomol Exp Appl. 2009;130(2):130–7. 10.1111/j.1570-7458.2008.00813.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Schmidt-Entling MH, Siegenthaler E. Herbivore release through cascading risk effects. Biol Lett. 2009;5(6):773–6. 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Hawlena D, Strickland MS, Bradford MA, Schmitz OJ. Fear of Predation Slows Plant-Litter Decomposition. Science. 2012;336(6087):1434–8. 10.1126/science.1220097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Samu F, Szinetár C. On the nature of agrobiont spiders. J Arachnol. 2002;30(2):389–402. 10.1636/0161-8202(2002)030[0389:Otnoas]2.0.Co;2. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC0.626. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Wisniewska J, Prokopy RJ. Do spiders (Araneae) feed on rose leafhopper (Edwardsiana rosae; Auchenorrhyncha: Cicadellidae) pests of apple trees? Eur J Entomol. 1997;94(2):243–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Zrubecz P, Tóth F, Nagy A. Pókfajok (Xysticus kochi Thorell; Tibellus oblongus Walckenaer) lárváinak hatékonyságvizsgálata virágtripszek (Frankliniela spp.) ellen hajtatott paprikában [Study into the effectiveness of larvae of spider species (Xysticus kochi Thorell; Tibellus oblongus Walckenaer) against flower thrips (Frankliniela spp.) in greenhouse pepper]. Növényvédelem. 2004;40(10):527–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Wegener C. Predation on the grassbug species Notostira elongata (Heteroptera: Miridae) by Nabidae (Heteroptera) and selected non-webbuilding spiders (Araneae). Entomol Gen. 1998;22(3–4):295–304. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Samu F, Beleznai O, Tholt G. A potential spider natural enemy against virus vector leafhoppers in agricultural mosaic landscapes–Corroborating ecological and behavioral evidence. Biol Contr. 2013;67(3):390–6. 10.1016/j.biocontrol.2013.08.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-PLUS. New York: Springer Verlag; 2000. 528 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing Available: http://www.R-project.org/. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Grafen A, Hails R. Modern Statistics for the Life Sciences. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002. 368 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Garamszegi LZ. Comparing effect sizes across variables: generalization without the need for Bonferroni correction. Behav Ecol. 2006;17(4):682–7. 10.1093/beheco/ark005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. SAS Institute. JMP Statistics and Graphics Guide, Release 6. Cary, NC, USA: SAS Institute Inc.; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Cox DR. Regression Models and Life-Tables. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol. 1972;34(2):187–220. 10.2307/2985181 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Wajnberg E, Gonsard PA, Tabone E, Curty C, Lezcano N, Colazza S. A comparative analysis of patch-leaving decision rules in a parasitoid family. J Anim Ecol. 2003;72(4):618–26. 10.1046/j.1365-2656.2003.00731.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Hein AM, McKinley SA. Sensing and decision-making in random search. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(30):12070–4. 10.1073/pnas.1202686109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Zhang T, Branch A, Shen P. Octopamine-mediated circuit mechanism underlying controlled appetite for palatable food in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(38):15431–6. 10.1073/pnas.1308816110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Honma A, Oku S, Nishida T. Adaptive significance of death feigning posture as a specialized inducible defence against gape-limited predators. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;273(1594):1631–6. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3501. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1634928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Martín J, Luque-Larena JJ, López P. When to run from an ambush predator: balancing crypsis benefits with costs of fleeing in lizards. Anim Behav. 2009;78(4):1011–8. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Banks PB, Norrdahl K, Korpimaki E. Nonlinearity in the predation risk of prey mobility. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2000;267(1453):1621–5. 10.1098/rspb.2000.1187 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Hawlena D, Schmitz OJ. Physiological stress as a fundamental mechanism linking predation to ecosystem functioning. Am Nat. 2010;176(5):537–56. 10.1086/656495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Villagra CA, Ramirez CC, Niemeyer HM. Antipredator responses of aphids to parasitoids change as a function of aphid physiological state. Anim Behav. 2002;64:677–83. 10.1006/anbe.2002.4015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Fievet V, Lhomme P, Outreman Y. Predation risk cues associated with killed conspecifics affect the behavior and reproduction of prey animals. Oikos. 2008;117(9):1380–5. 10.1111/j.2008.0030-1299.16629.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Welton NJ, McNamara JM, Houston AI. Assessing predation risk: optimal behaviour and rules of thumb. Theor Popul Biol. 2003;64(4):417–30. 10.1016/s0040-5809(03)00097-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Englund G. Importance of spatial scale and prey movements in predator caging experiments. Ecology. 1997;78(8):2316–25. 10.2307/2265894 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Bowler DE, Yano S, Amano H. The non-consumptive effects of a predator on spider mites depend on predator density. J Zool. 2013;289(1):52–9. 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2012.00961.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Lone K, Loe LE, Gobakken T, Linnell JDC, Odden J, Remmen J, et al. Living and dying in a multi-predator landscape of fear: roe deer are squeezed by contrasting pattern of predation risk imposed by lynx and humans. Oikos. 2014;123(6):641–51. 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2013.00938.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Preisser EL, Orrock JL, Schmitz OJ. Predator hunting mode and habitat domain alter nonconsumptive effects in predator-prey interactions. Ecology. 2007;88(11):2744–51. 10.1890/07-0260.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Hogg BN, Daane KM. The roles of top and intermediate predators in herbivore suppression: contrasting results from the field and laboratory. Ecol Entomol. 2014;39(2):149–58. 10.1111/een.12079 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Straub CS, Simasek NP, Gapinski MR, Dohm R, Aikens EO, Muscella S. Influence of nonhost plant diversity and natural enemies on the potato leafhopper, Empoasca fabae, and pea aphid, Acyrthosiphon pisum, in alfalfa. J Pest Sci. 2013;86(2):235–44. 10.1007/s10340-012-0465-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Daane KM, Yokota GY, Zheng Y, Hagen KS. Inundative release of common green lacewings (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae) to suppress Erythroneura variabilis and E. elegantula (Homoptera: Cicadellidae) in vineyards. Environ Entomol. 1996;25(5):1224–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Barcos AKS, Velasco LRI, Medina CD. Foraging Behavior of the Yellow Netted Ladybeetle Heteroneda billardieri Crotch (Coleoptera: Coccinellidae). Philipp Agric Sci. 2014;97(2):155–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Pompon J, Quiring D, Giordanengo P, Pelletier Y. Role of xylem consumption on osmoregulation in Macrosiphum euphorbiae (Thomas). J Insect Physiol. 2010;56(6):610–5. 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2009.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Hardie J, Schlumberger A. The early appearance of foraging flight associated with starvation in an aphid. Entomol Exp Appl. 1996;80(1):73–5. 10.1007/978-94-009-1720-0_16 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Stadler B, Weisser WW, Houston AI. Defense reactions in aphids—the influence of state and future reproductive success. J Anim Ecol. 1994;63(2):419–30. 10.2307/5559 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Klingauf FA. Host Plant Finding and Acceptance In: Minks AK, Harrewijn P, editors. Aphids: Their Biology, Natural Enemies and Control. World Crop Pests Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1987. p. 209–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Brown JS, Kotler BP. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecol Lett. 2004;7(10):999–1014. 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00661.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Reader T, Higginson AD, Barnard CJ, Gilbert FS. The effects of predation risk from crab spiders on bee foraging behavior. Behav Ecol. 2006;17(6):933–9. 10.1093/beheco/arl027 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Laundre JW, Hernandez L, Altendorf KB. Wolves, elk, and bison: reestablishing the "landscape of fear" in Yellowstone National Park, USA. Can J Zool. 2001;79(8):1401–9. 10.1139/cjz-79-8-1401 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Table. AIC values and the corresponding Akaike weights calculated for the LME models in Table 3.

Candidate models for each dependent variable are sorted by their AIC value from the lowest to highest. Models in bold are the final models obtained during the applied model selection procedure. In most cases, final models had the lowest AIC value among the candidates. In model {4} and {15} the best model and the consecutive one cannot be differentiated according to their AIC value (i.e. ΔAIC<2). In model {6}, the model including ‘Predation period’ had lower AIC value (but ΔAIC was <2 between the two models) than our final model. In all these cases, we took uncertainty regarding our final model into account during the interpretation of the results.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Parameter estimates of the significant predictors in the final LME models.

For each factor, the effects of additional levels (i.e. compared to the first level of the given predictor) are shown (the name of the level is written in brackets).

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are available via Figshare (http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1472880).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES