Abstract
Background
Substantial impairment in digit-symbol substitution task performance in schizophrenia is well established, which has been widely interpreted as denoting a specific impairment in processing-speed ability. However, other higher-order cognitive functions might be more critical to performance on this task. To date, this has not been rigorously investigated in schizophrenia.
Methods
One-hundred and twenty-five schizophrenia cases and 272 controls completed neuropsychological measures of processing speed, memory and executive functioning. We implemented a series of confirmatory factor and structural regression modeling in order to build an integrated model of processing speed, memory and executive function with which to deconstruct digit-symbol substitution task and characterize discrepancies between cases and controls.
Results
The overall structure of the processing speed, memory and executive function model was the same across groups (χ2 = 208.86, p>.05) but the contribution of the specific cognitive domains to coding task performance differed significantly. When completing the task controls relied on executive function and, indirectly, on working memory ability; while schizophrenia cases utilized an alternative set of cognitive operations whereby they relied on the same processes required to complete verbal fluency tasks.
Conclusions
Successful coding task performance is predominantly reliant on executive function, rather than processing-speed or memory abilities. Schizophrenia patients perform poorly on this task due to an apparent lack of appropriate executive function input, they rely instead on an alternative cognitive pathway.
Keywords: Keywords: schizophrenia, neuropsychology, processing speed, information processing, structural equation modeling, digit-symbol substitution
Introduction
Neuropsychological research has established that individuals with schizophrenia exhibit impaired performance when completing numerous cognitive tasks (1). The task on which patients appear to be most impaired is the digit-symbol substitution task (2-4). This finding has been interpreted as denoting a substantial impairment in processing speed in schizophrenia. However, this interpretation only holds true if those cognitive processes, which are required to complete the digit-symbol substitution task, are truly synonymous with processing-speed ability. Indeed evidence is now accruing which suggests that perhaps digit-symbol substitution performance is reliant on more complex cognitive abilities such as memory and executive function (5-9). Were this the case it would necessitate a re-evaluation of the meaning of the digit-symbol substitution impairment in schizophrenia. The goal of the present study was to disentangle both the cognitive underpinnings of the digit-symbol substitution task in healthy individuals and to postulate a model for understanding the impairments exhibited by schizophrenia patients when completing this task. In so doing we hoped to appropriately frame the digit-symbol substitution impairment in schizophrenia and moreover generate testable hypotheses for the origins of the impairment.
Salthouse (1996) defined processing-speed as the number of correct responses an individual is able to make within a finite amount of time. Thus, on the face of it at least, the digit-symbol substitution task indexes processing-speed ability where participants are required to correctly substitute symbols and digits using a key under timed conditions (10). However, several lines of evidence suggest that coding tasks may, in fact, overlap with tests of memory and executive function. In factor analytic research, processing speed factors typically comprise the digit-symbol substitution task alongside the Trail Making Tests and verbal fluency both of which encapsulate distinct elements of executive function and memory 6. Knowles and colleagues (2012) extended this finding. Using a confirmatory factor model of processing speed comprising factors of varying degrees of cognitive complexity they demonstrated that the digit-symbol substitution task completion is reliant not on basic psychomotor speed but instead on processes more associated with memory and also executive functioning 7. This finding is in line with studies using linear regression models which have shown that both memory and executive function make significant contributions to performance 5 and also with work from the field of experimental psychology which has highlighted memory deficits in schizophrenia as a contributing factor to the coding-task impairment (11). These lines of evidence suggest that the digit-symbol substitution task may be much more than a mere test of processing speed, but no study has made a detailed examination of the role of processing speed along with memory and executive function in task completion. Doing so would allow an integrated model of the cognitive impairment in schizophrenia to be developed.
We report data from a large sample of schizophrenia cases and community controls that completed multiple measures of processing speed, memory and executive function. We applied confirmatory factor analyses to these measures, followed by structural regression modeling techniques. The aims of this study were threefold: (a) to establish the structure of processing speed, memory and executive function abilities and also, where the digit-symbol substitution best fitted within that structure; (b) to investigate the hierarchical relationships within the structure and the digit symbol-coding task; and (c) examine whether differences between controls and cases in the structure and/or the hierarchy could explain the coding-task deficit in schizophrenia. The present investigation is, to our knowledge, the largest rigorous study of the causes of the processing-speed impairment in schizophrenia using a comprehensive model of cognition.
Methods
Participants
Participants comprised 125 schizophrenia patients (average age = 31.58 (range 20–67) years, 68% males) and 272 controls (average age = 40.96 (range 17–65) years, 34% male). For the schizophrenia group the mean SAPS (scale for the assessment of positive symptoms) score was 10.97 (SD=16.11) and the SANS (scale for the assessment of negative symptoms) was 28.89 (SD=23.56). The sample used in the present paper is the same as used in a previous paper of which the present paper is an extension (7).
Neuropsychological Assessment
A comprehensive neuropsychological battery was administered to all participants including: visual scanning (12), simple motor speed (12), number sequencing (12), letter sequencing (12), number-letter switching (12), letter and category fluency (12), spatial span forward and backward (10), visual pattern task (10), listening memory span (10), letter-number sequencing (10), digit span forward (10), tower test (10), Wisconsin card sorting task (10), switching fluency (12), and the digit symbol substitution task.
Working memory measures were included if they assessed the maintenance and manipulation of verbal and visuospatial information (13-15); and executive function measures were included that assessed shifting, updating and inhibition. Tests of processing speed were selected in accordance with the theoretical and empirical work of Salthouse (16) and which covered abilities previously suggested to be involved in DSST completion including psychomotor speed, visual scanning, sustained attention and coordination of elementary operations (17). Further details of the processing-speed measures chosen for inclusion can be found in (7).
Statistical Analysis and Model Building
Data were carefully screened for outliers and there was no evidence of potential bias. In terms of statistical power, Monte Carlo simulations of structural equation modelling suggest that samples of at least one-hundred cases are required to confer enough power for models to converge with a good degree of accuracy (18).
Confirmatory factor analysis was implemented using Analysis of Momentary Structure (AMOS) 18.0 software (19) to test a six-factor theory-based model of processing speed, memory and executive function. The processing-speed portion of this model was established previously (7) while the memory and executive portion, which was built specifically for this paper, was based on theoretical and experimental work by Baddeley which has consistently shown that memory is composed of separate verbal and visuospatial elements alongside an executive component. The executive component was modelled on the work of Miyake and colleagues, which demonstrated that executive function ability incorporates three fundamental elements: shifting, updating and inhibition (20). Full details of model building, assessment and comparison can be found in the supplemental materials. We used structural equation modeling (SEM) to examine the degree to which each of the latent variables contributes to performance on the digit-symbol substitution task by comparing alternative nested models (models with different combinations of paths from each latent variable to the digit-symbol substitution task) using the specification search function in AMOS (19). This analysis was done separately in cases and controls. This analysis is akin to a complex multiple regression analysis where the latent variables can be conceived as predictor variables and the digit-symbol substitution task the dependent variable. The specification search selects the model that fits the data best, which is analogous to selecting the most parsimonious and best-fitting multiple regression model i.e. that model that can account for a significant portion of the variance in the dependent variables using the fewest predictor variables in the equation (20).
Results
Descriptive Statistics and Group Comparisons
Table 1 presents the performance of cases and controls on the measures of processing speed, memory and executive function. Cases performed significantly worse than controls on all measures (all p-values <0.001). The most severe impairment was on the digit symbol-coding task (d = -1.51), followed by the semantic fluency measure (d = -1.18). The observed effect sizes are similar to those reported in recent meta-analyses (2-3).
Table 1.
Control (N=272) | Schizophrenia (N=125) | Difference (Controlmean-Schizophreniamean) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Task | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Digit Symbol Substitution | |||||
Task | 0.37 | 0.79 | -0.85 | 0.93 | 1.21 |
Semantic Fluency | 0.28 | 0.90 | -0.77 | 0.84 | 1.05 |
Number Sequencing | -0.32 | 0.61 | 0.73 | 1.29 | -1.05 |
Listening Memory Span | 0.28 | 0.90 | -0.77 | 0.81 | 1.04 |
Switching Fluency | 0.30 | 0.86 | -0.68 | 0.95 | 0.99 |
Letter Fluency | 0.25 | 0.91 | -0.71 | 0.90 | 0.96 |
Letter Sequencing | -0.28 | 0.75 | 0.68 | 1.19 | -0.96 |
Letter Number | |||||
Sequencing | 0.26 | 0.87 | -0.67 | 0.98 | 0.93 |
Visual Scanning | -0.28 | 0.64 | 0.64 | 1.33 | -0.92 |
Number-Letter | |||||
Sequencing | -0.23 | 0.82 | 0.64 | 1.16 | -0.88 |
Spatial Span Backward | 0.23 | 0.89 | -0.63 | 1.03 | 0.86 |
Visual Pattern Task Span | 0.19 | 0.96 | -0.52 | 0.92 | 0.71 |
Tower Task | 0.21 | 0.85 | -0.55 | 1.15 | 0.76 |
WCST | 0.15 | 0.99 | -0.45 | 0.88 | 0.60 |
Spatial Span Forward | 0.14 | 0.98 | -0.39 | 0.96 | 0.53 |
Digit Span Forward | 0.16 | 0.99 | -0.38 | 0.90 | 0.55 |
Simple Motor Speed | -0.14 | 0.85 | 0.36 | 1.24 | -0.50 |
Based on Table 2 in Knowles and colleagues (2012).
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
The memory, executive function and processing-speed model (see blue portion of Figure 1, and also see Table 2 for the correlations between factors) fit was good in both controls (χ2 =92.8883, p=0.22; RMSEA=0.021; CFI=.99 and NFI=0.94) and cases (χ2 =106.9688, p=0.08; RMSEA=0.044; CFI=.97 and NFI=0.86). Furthermore, an exploratory factor analysis suggested a similar factor structure (Table S1). Multigroup comparison indicated that neither the factor structure (χ2 = 196.45170, p=0.080, RMSEA =.020, CFI =.99, NFI =.961) nor the factor loadings (χ2 = 208.86178, p=0.057, RMSEA =.021, CFI =.98, NFI =.91) differed significantly between controls and cases, suggesting that the basic structure is the same across groups.
Table 2.
Psychomotor Speed | Visuo-Spatial Memory | Phonological Memory | Executive Functioning | Shifting and Sequencing | Verbal Fluency | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Psychomotor Speed | 1 | -.59 | -.43 | -.94 | .86 | -.79 |
Visuo-Spatial Memory | -.49 | 1 | .84 | .88 | -.78 | .58 |
Phonological Memory | -.42 | .86 | 1 | .75 | -.63 | .53 |
Executive Functioning | -.65 | .64 | .76 | 1 | -.92 | .85 |
Shifting and Sequencing | .83 | -.65 | -.58 | -.82 | 1 | -.77 |
Verbal Fluency | -.50 | .42 | .56 | .32 | -.63 | 1 |
Structural Model Specification
Next we examined which factors of the processing speed, memory and executive function model best predicted coding task performance. Table 3 presents the results of the specification search used to select the best combination of factors in controls and cases. In the control group (χ2 = 90.4573, p=0.081, BCC0 = 0.00, AIC0 = 0.00) the best fitting model was a two path model with a path from Executive Function (β-coefficient =.39, p<.05) and a path from Shifting and Sequencing (β-coefficient = -.35, p<.05) to the digit-symbol coding task. In cases (χ2 = 90.6877, p=0.136, BCC0 = 0.00, AIC0 = 0.00) the best fitting model was also a two-path model but with a path from Shifting and Sequencing (β-coefficient = -.37, p<.05) and a path from Verbal Fluency (β-coefficient =.51, p<.01) to the digit-symbol coding task (see yellow portion of Figure 1).
Table 3.
Model | df | χ2 | p | BCC0 | AIC0 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Executive Functioning & Shifting and Sequencing | 73 | 90.45 | .081 | 0.000 | 0.000 | |
Controls | Executive Functioning | 74 | 92.79 | .069 | 0.207 | 0.334 |
Executive Functioning, Shifting and Sequencing & Verbal Fluency | 72 | 88.82 | .076 | 0.491 | 0.364 | |
| ||||||
Executive Functioning & Verbal Fluency | 77 | 90.68 | .136 | 0.000 | 0.000 | |
Cases | Visuo-Spatial Memory, Phonological Memory & Verbal Fluency | 76 | 89.13 | .144 | 0.782 | 0.452 |
Visuo-Spatial Memory, Shifting and Sequencing & Verbal Fluency | 76 | 90.24 | .127 | 1.887 | 1.557 |
Exploratory Structural Model of Processing Speed, Memory and Executive Function
Structural regression modelling was implemented to develop a hierarchical model of processing speed, memory and executive function where the digit-symbol substitution task was placed at the top of the hierarchy. A hierarchical model of processing speed, memory and executive function is shown in Figure 2, this model fitted the data well in both controls (χ2 = 106.38110 p =.36, AIC = 242.38, CFI = 1.00, NFI =.93, RMSEA =.013) and cases (χ2 = 132.33110 p =.07, AIC = 252.330, CFI =.97, NFI =.87, RMSEA =.040). The upper portion of the model (including the digit-symbol coding task) differed between controls and cases. Nested model comparisons showed that in controls the best fitting model was a two-path model (χ2 = 106.38102 p =.36, AIC = 242.38, CFI = 1.00, NFI =.93, RMSEA =.035), with a path from Executive Function (β-coefficient =.33, p<.05) and one from Shifting and Sequencing (β-coefficient = -.39, p<.01). In cases the best fitting model was a one-path model (χ2 = 132.33110 p =.07, AIC = 252.33, CFI =.97, NFI =.87, RMSEA =.040), with a path from Verbal Fluency (β-coefficient =.89, p<.001). Thus the paths in the upper portion of the model where factors connected directly to the digit-symbol substitution task differed in controls and cases. However, the characterisation of the model below these directly connecting factors was the same in both groups (including the relationships between psychomotor speed, visuo-spatial memory, verbal memory, shifting and sequencing and verbal fluency) except that in cases the path between Phonological Memory and Verbal Fluency was dropped because it was not significant.
Discussion
It is well established that digit-symbol substitution task performance is substantially impaired in schizophrenia, a finding that has been widely interpreted as denoting a specific impairment in processing-speed ability. However, the results of the present study challenge this view. By building an integrated model of processing speed, memory and executive function this study has revealed that the digit-symbol substitution task is best described as a measure of executive function (rather than processing speed or memory) and by developing an exploratory model the present study generates a tentative but testable hypothesis for why schizophrenia patients underperform on this task, namely that in cases (as compared to controls) there is a discrepancy in the level of executive function input.
The confirmatory factor model shown in Figure 1 demonstrates that the same basic structure of executive, memory and processing speed cognitive functions existed in healthy controls and schizophrenia patients with a factor model comprising psychomotor speed, visuo-spatial memory, phonological memory, executive function, shifting and sequencing, and verbal fluency fitting equally well in both groups. It is of note that this model is very similar to one generated by an exploratory factor solution (Table S1) – the digit-symbol substitution task was not included in this analysis as this end goal was to use this factor structure to deconstruct the task. This is important to note as it supports the idea that this model is truly one that represents the data rather than representing, for example, the authors' expectations for how the measures should group together. Upon using the confirmatory factor analysis to deconstruct the primary cognitive influences on digit-symbol substitution task performance it became apparent that the way in which the constituent cognitive functions were applied to the digit-symbol coding task completion differed between groups. Healthy controls appeared to rely on executive function and shifting and sequencing abilities to complete the task, while schizophrenia patients relied on sequencing and shifting and verbal fluency. First, this finding mirrors that of Knowles and colleagues, which was that digit-symbol substitution performance is more closely allied to those dimensions of processing speed associated with memory and executive function rather than with basic psychomotor speed. Secondly, the findings of the present study extend those of Knowles and colleagues (7) by showing that in schizophrenia the level of executive function input is insufficient for successful digit-symbol coding task performance.
The exploratory model presented in this study, where the confirmatory factor model was re-specified to as hierarchical, is not being proposed as the definitive model of cognition in schizophrenia rather it represents an attempt to disentangle the underlying cognitive underpinnings of the digit-symbol substitution deficit in schizophrenia and moreover an attempt to generate hypotheses for future work in the field. What it proposes is that given a hierarchical structure of cognition there appears to be one or more pathways in schizophrenia which are defunct and this leads to patients employing an alternative strategy to controls, one which seemingly leads to a decrement in digit-symbol substitution task performance. Of course, this hypothesis should be thoroughly tested in future, perhaps using experimental psychology techniques where digit-symbol themed paradigms could be employed, for example by relaxing the time constraints for digit-symbol testing; suggesting strategies; using dual task designs which might be used to partial out the working memory or executive function components of the task, and so on. Nonetheless the results here suggest that when completing the digit-symbol substitution task, schizophrenia patients depend upon cognitive abilities typically associated with verbal fluency rather than executive functioning. Successful performance on executive function measures is characterised by on-line, seamless integration of multiple abilities including planning, inhibition, set-shifting and updating, all of which must be adaptively applied as the task progresses. Verbal fluency requires participants to make laboured searches of long-term memory under timed conditions with application of inhibition (17). Given this it is unsurprising that controls outperform schizophrenia patients on the digit-symbol substitution task, a measure which does not require access to long-term memory but which necessitates flexible co-ordination of multiple cognitive sub-processes – procedures akin to executive functioning. The implication here is not that schizophrenia patients utilize verbal fluency ability volitionally, but that due to physical or psychological constraints they must rely on what might be considered the next best alternative to executive function or perhaps even a diluted version of it. It is true that verbal fluency has been grouped under the umbrella of Speed of Processing in comprehensive cognitive batteries such as the MATRICS (21), and as such it might be argued here that in schizophrenia patients the digit-symbol substitution task is reliant on processing speed by proxy. However, the Verbal Fluency factor encapsulated by the modelling in the present paper appears to correlate strongly with Shifting and Sequencing and Executive Functioning as well as Psychomotor Speed (Table 2). Moreover, verbal fluency has traditionally been considered a measure of executive functioning and is used primarily to index frontal lobe deficits (22). Thus, while Psychomotor Speed, of course, plays a part in digit-symbol substitution task completion it is shown here to also measure of executive function in controls and in cases, the primary difference between groups is in the level of executive functioning input to digit-symbol substitution completion.
The results of the present study tie in neatly with the idea of a cognitive-control deficit in schizophrenia (23). Cognitive control refers to the ability in humans to adaptively update those processes and behaviours that are necessary for the completion of a task at hand (24). The cognitive-control deficit in schizophrenia is owing to inadequate recruitment and coordination of multiple brain regions (in particular the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) when completing tasks (25, 26). Cognitive control networks are thought to support executive functions (27), that is the same cognitive ability found to support digit-symbol substitution task completion in healthy controls (and which is lacking in patients) in the present paper. Future research might extend the present findings by investigating whether this apparent lack of cognitive control, which is also present in neuroimaging investigations of cognition in schizophrenia (28, 29), might be extrapolated to explain impairments in tasks in addition to the digit-symbol substitution task.
Traditionally the focus of research has been on impairments in memory and executive functioning (30-36). Furthermore, the idea that digit-symbol substitution task performance is reliant on executive function is not an entirely new one, but the present study is the first to present an integrated model of cognition in order to demonstrate this. The first clue that symbol coding tasks were more than a measure of basic psychomotor speed comes from a series of experiments utilising the digitised tablet paradigm (37-40) which led the authors to conclude that ‘the SDST [DSST] mainly provide [s] information about the subjects' cognitive functioning and their speed of information processing and far less about the progression of psychomotor processes’ (41). Subsequent work, which used coding task subtests to deconstruct the digit-symbol substitution task, seemed to suggest that the task measured psychomotor speed (as represented by the symbol copy subtest) rather than memory (measured by the incidental learning subtest) (42, 43); but this result was likely due to shared method variance between symbol copy and the task proper (40). Indeed, a study that examined digit symbol coding performance using a neuropsychological test battery, rather than the digit symbol subtests, showed that a motor task accounted for 20% of the variance and general memory measures made a significant and almost equal contribution accounting for approximately 15% of the variance. Furthermore a measure of executive functioning significantly predicted coding task performance after the effects of motor speed, perceptual speed, and visual scanning had been partialled out (5). Other experimental work examining the conceptual and procedural overlap and differences between fluency tasks and coding tasks under variable time constraints would be valuable.
This study has some potential limitations. It might be argued that the model presented in the current paper is not necessarily the definitive model of processing speed, memory and executive function as alternative specifications may fit the data equally well. Although both the confirmatory factor and hierarchical models explained a substantial amount of the variance in digit-symbol coding task performance in both controls (49% and 50% for the confirmatory and hierarchical models respectively) and in cases (70% and 78%) as denoted by the squared multiple correlations of the digit-symbol coding task (44). It could also be argued that the results presented here are a reflection of testing methodology rather than underlying cognitive structure. However, the current model specification is supported by an exploratory factor analysis which resulted in a six factor solution (see Table S1). Moreover the final groupings of measures were a result of exploratory and also confirmatory factor analysis whereby alternative groupings were carefully considered. Another potential limitation of the present study is the difference in gender distribution in controls and cases (34% male and 68% male respectively) which may affect the results given that previous research has shown that brain structure may vary between genders. However, the fit of the six-factor model was equally as good when using data residualised for the effects of age and sex. A caveat of the results of the present paper is that the hierarchical model is exploratory. However, this model serves to support the findings of the confirmatory factor analyses rather than as a stand alone finding. Another potential limitation of the present study is that it is possible that in different, perhaps more symptomatic patient groups the model may be characterized differently, for example in a first-episode versus a chronic group; this could be explored in future studies. Furthermore, it would be of interest to attempt to replicate these findings under an experimental psychology paradigm. For example, Elahipanah and colleagues demonstrated that schizophrenia patients adopt an inefficient visual search strategy when completing the digit-symbol substitution task (45) – a finding that is compatible with the overall view presented in the present manuscript, which is that patients are not adopting an optimal executive strategy when completing the task.
In conclusion, the results of the present study that the digit-symbol substitution task is better characterised as a measure of executive function than processing speed or memory; and the pronounced digit-symbol substitution task impairment in schizophrenia appears to be due to a lack of appropriate executive function input.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by NIMH grant MH066105, and by the UK Department of Health via the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Specialist Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health award to South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust (SLaM) and the Institute of Psychiatry at King's College London. The authors thank James Gold and Dwight Dickinson for their assistance with the preparation of the article.
Footnotes
Financial Disclosures: The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Reichenberg A, Harvey PD. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: Integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:833–58. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dickinson D, Ramsey ME, Gold JM. Overlooking the obvious: A meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:532–42. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Knowles EEM, David AS, Reichenberg A. Processing speed deficits in schizophrenia: Reexamining the evidence. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:835. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09070937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schaefer J, Giangrande E, Weinberger DR, Dickinson D. The global cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: Consistent over decades and around the world. Schizophr Res. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Joy S, Fein D, Kaplan E. Decoding digit symbol: Speed, memory, and visual scanning. Assessment. 2003;10:56–65. doi: 10.1177/0095399702250335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Knowles EE, Weiser M, David AS, Dickinson D, Glahn D, Gold J, et al. Dedifferentiation and substitute strategy: Deconstructing the processing-speed impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;142:129–36. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cella M, Wykes T. Understanding processing speed-its subcomponents and their relationship to characteristics of people with schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2012 doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.730038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Savla GN, Twamley EW, Delis DC, Roesch SC, Jeste DV, Palmer BW. Dimensions of executive functioning in schizophrenia and their relationship with processing speed. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:760–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wechsler D. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bachman P, Reichenberg A, Rice P, Woolsey M, Chaves O, Martinez D, et al. Deconstructing processing speed deficits in schizophrenia: Application of a parametric digit symbol coding test. Schizophr Res. 2010;118:6–11. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.02.1029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer J. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Scale. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Baddeley AD. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nat Neurosci. 2003;4:829–39. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1980;19:450–66. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Miyake A, Shah P. Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. 1996;103:403–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press; Oxford; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Loehlin JC. Latent variable models: an introduction to factor, path, and structural equation analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, N.J.; London: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Arbuckle JL. Amos. 2006;7.0Generic
- 20.Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000;41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.National Institute of Mental Health. Neuropharmacology Committee. Measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia: MATRICS. 2004 Jun; 2003. Conference Proceedings. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lesh TA, Niendam TA, Minzenberg MJ, Carter CS. Cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia: Mechanisms and meaning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:316–38. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science. 1998;280:747–9. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5364.747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Verchinski BA, Marenco S, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Complexity of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: More than up or down. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Glahn DC, Ragland JD, Abramoff A, Barrett J, Laird AR, Bearden CE, et al. Beyond hypofrontality: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of working memory in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:60–9. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Niendam TA, Laird AR, Ray KL, Dean YM, Glahn DC, Carter CS. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012;12:241–68. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Verchinski BA, Marenco S, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Complexity of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: More than up or down. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:2209–15. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Goldman-Rakic PS, Castner SA, Svensson TH, Siever LJ, Williams GV. Targeting the dopamine D1 receptor in schizophrenia: Insights for cognitive dysfunction. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;174:3–16. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1793-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–45. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Laws KR. A meta-analytic review of wisconsin card sort studies in schizophrenia: General intellectual deficit in disguise? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 1999;4:31–5. doi: 10.1080/135468099396025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Johnson-Selfridge M, Zalewski C. Moderator variables of executive functioning in schizophrenia: Meta-analytic findings. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:305–16. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Li CS. Do schizophrenia patients make more perseverative than non-perseverative errors on the wisconsin card sorting test? A meta-analytic study. Psychiatry Res. 2004;129:179–90. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lee J, Park S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:1358–66. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Pelletier M, Achim AM, Montoya A, Lal S, Lepage M. Cognitive and clinical moderators of recognition memory in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2005;74:233–52. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Forbes NF, Carrick LA, McIntosh AM, Lawrie SM. Working memory in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2009;39:889–905. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hoof JJv, Jogems-Kosterman BJ, Sabbe BG, Zitman FG, Hulstijn W. Differentiation of cognitive and motor slowing in the digit symbol test (DST): Differences between depression and schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 1998;32:99–103. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(98)00057-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jogems-Kosterman BJ, Zitman FG, Hoof JJv, Hulstijn W. Psychomotor slowing and planning deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;48:317–33. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00097-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Morrens M, Hulstijn W, Hecke JV, Peuskens J, Sabbe BG. Sensorimotor and cognitive slowing in schizophrenia as measured symbol digit substitution test. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40:200–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Grootens KP, Vermeeren L, Verkes RJ, Buitelaar JK, Sabbe BG, Veelen Nv, et al. Psychomotor planning is deficient in recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2009;107:294–302. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Morrens M, Hulstijn W, Matton C, Madani Y, Bouwel Lv, Peuskens J, et al. Delineating psychomotor slowing from reduced processing speed in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:457–71. doi: 10.1080/13546800802439312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kreiner DS, Ryan JJ. Memory and motor skill components of the WAIS-III digit symbol-coding subtest. Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;15:109–13. doi: 10.1076/clin.15.1.109.1906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Joy S, Kaplan E, Fein D. Speed and memory in the WAIS-III digit symbol-coding subtest across the adult lifespan. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2004;19:759–67. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bentler PM, Raykov T. On measures of explained variance in nonrecursive structural equation models. J Appl Psychol. 2000;85:125–31. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Elahipanah A, Christensen BK, Reingold EM. What can eye movements tell us about symbol digit substitution by patients with schizophrenia? Schizophr Res. 2011;127:137–43. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.