Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Aug 28.
Published in final edited form as: Epidemiology. 2011 Nov;22(6):877–878. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3182319910

Evaluating Marginal Streuctural Model Assumptions in Assessing Hormonal Contraception on Risk of HIV Acquistion

Pai-Lien Chen a, Stephen R Cole b, Charles S Morrison c
PMCID: PMC4551514  NIHMSID: NIHMS323051  PMID: 21968782

To the Editor

Recently, two analyses were published from the Hormonal Contraception and Risk of HIV Acquisition Study, a multicenter cohort study specifically designed to evaluate whether hormonal contraceptive use (depot-medroxyprogesterone acetate [DMPA] or combined oral contraceptives [COCs]) alters the risk of HIV acquisition among women.1,2 A detailed description of study participants and methods has been published elsewhere.1 The main study analysis used conventional Cox proportional hazard models with time-varying hormonal contraceptive exposure to adjust for demographic and sexual risk factors. This analysis showed little or no evidence of increased risk of HIV acquisition with either DMPA (hazard ratio = 1.25 [95% confidence interval= 0.89 - 1.78]) or COCs (0.99 [0.69 - 1.42]) compared with the non-hormonal group.1 A reanalysis using a marginal-structural-model (MSM) to accommodate hormonal-contraceptive exposure switching and confounding with the same set of demographic and sexual risk factors found evidence for an increased HIV acquisition risk associated with DMPA (1.48 [1.02 - 2.15]) but not COC use (1.19 [0.80 - 1.76]).2 MSMs can appropriately adjust for time-dependent confounding and selection bias using the inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting (IPTW).3 Nevertheless, the model relies on certain assumptions that either cannot be verified (e.g., no unmeasured confounders) or could be problematic in the data from this study4 (e.g.,violation of the positivity assumption due to high association between pregnancy and hormonal contraceptive use).

We conducted several sensitivity analyses to address the above concerns. First, we examined possible model misspecification in the marginal structural model by adding or removing suspected time-varying confounder(s). Second, we investigated the variability of hazard ratios estimated by MSM using a nonparametric bootstrapping procedure to obtain estimates from 1000 with-replacement bootstrap samples.5 Finally, we explored possible violations of the positivity assumption when pregnancy status was included in the MSM analyses.

We found a consistently larger effect of DMPA than COCs on HIV acquisition, compared with the nonhormonal group, across various analyses (Table). DMPA also appeared to have a larger effect with MSM analyses than with the conventional Cox model when similar covariates were included in analyses. Model misspecification can cause estimated hazard ratios to vary from 1.24 (0.85 - 1.80) to 1.51 (1.03 – 2.22) for DMPA, and from 1.00 (0.69 – 1.47) to 1.19 (0.80 – 1.76) for COCs.

Table. Estimated Effect of HC Exposure on Time to HIV Acquisition in HC-HIV Study, Uganda and Zimbabwe, 1999-2004.

Analysis COC DMPA

Time-dependent covariates/confounder included Estimated weights Mean (SD) Hazard ratio 95% Confidence interval d Hazard ratio 95% Confidence interval d
Standard Cox model
Unadjusted None _ 1.06 0.75, 1.48 1.20 0.87, 1.65
Adjusted a primary partner risk, participant behavioral risk, any condom use, and coital frequency _ 1.05 0.73, 1.52 1.25 0.89, 1.77
MSM Analysis ab
 1 primary partner risk 0.98 (0.56) 1.00 0.69, 1.47 1.24 0.85, 1.80
 2 primary partner risk, participant behavioral risk, 0.99 (0.54) 1.08 0.74, 1.57 1.35 0.94, 1.94
 3 primary partner risk, participant behavioral risk, and any condom use 0.99 (0.54) 1.19 0.80, 1.76 1.48 1.02, 2.15
 4 primary partner risk, participant behavioral risk, any condom use, and coital frequency 1.01 (0.55) 1.18 0.78, 1.76 1.51 1.03, 2.22
Bootstrapping abc primary partner risk, participant behavioral risk, and any condom use _ 1.17 0.82, 1.62 1.45 1.04, 1.95
Added pregnant statusa primary partner risk, participant behavioral risk, any condom use, and current pregnant status 1.20 (16.43) 1.12 0.77, 1.64 1.30 0.92, 1.85
a

also adjusted for baseline covariates (site, age, education, STI history, breastfeeding, coital frequency and living with partner)

b

weights computing including site, baseline covariates (age, education, STI history, breastfeeding, coital frequency and living with partner) and time-varying confounders; final model adjusted for baseline covariates only.

c

based on 100 bootstrap samples with variables included in Model 3

d

robust 95% confidence interval for all weighted analyses except for the bootstrapping approach that used the quintiles of 1000 bootstrap sample estimates

Compared with the results in Model 3 (the published results1), the bootstrapped estimates provided similar findings. We also note that the distributions of estimated weights were similar among MSM analyses that did not include pregnancy status; means and standard deviations were around 1 and 0.55, respectively, with reasonable ranges from about 0.01 to 2.64. However, when pregnancy status was included in the analysis, the distribution from untruncated weights clearly indicated the violation of positivity assumption because a large standard deviation (16.4) and a wide range of weights (from <0.01 to 3915) were observed. In general, the estimated effect becomes unstable and attenuated under that situation, with a large deviation of weights.

The validity of the MSM analysis depends on the assumption that all confounders were measured and sufficient to adjust for confounding and selection biases. The causal effect estimates could be biased if these assumptions were incorrectly specified. In these analyses we evaluated the potential issues of model specification and the positivity assumption. These findings parallel the results from a simulation study6 that found that inclusion of more confounders (even in the absence of complete control) resulted in a decrease in bias. This observation suggests that including all possible time-varying confounders in analyses may reduce bias, as long as no finite-sample bias is introduced. In addition, because current pregnancy clearly violated the positivity assumption for computing IPTW in the estimation of the hormonal-contraceptive effect on HIV acquisition, alternative modeling approaches (such as structural-nested-failure-time models) should be considered.7

Details on time-dependent confounder assessment and MSM specification are available in the eAppendix. (http://links.lww.com)

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgments

Funding: This work was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), National Institutes of Health (NIH), Department of Health and Human Services through a contract with Family Health International (FHI) (Contract Number N01-HD-0-3310).

Footnotes

Conflict of interest: none declared.

SDC Supplemental digital content is available through direct URL citations in the HTML and PDF versions of this article (www.epidem.com).

References

  • 1.Morrison CS, Richardson BA, Mmiro F, Chipato T, Celentano DD, Luoto J, et al. Hormonal contraception and the risk of HIV acquisition. AIDS. 2007;21(1):85–95. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e3280117c8b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Morrison CS, Chen PL, Kwok C, Richardson BA, Chipato T, Mugerwa R, Byamugisha J, Padian N, Celentano DD, Salata RA. Hormonal contraception and HIV acquisition: reanalysis using marginal structural modeling. AIDS. 2010;24(11):1778–1781. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32833a2537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Robins JM, Hernan MA, Brumback B. Marginal structural models and causal inference in epidemiology. Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):550–60. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200009000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cole SR, Hernán MA. Constructing inverse probability weights for marginal structural models. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;168(6):656–64. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Efron B, Tibshirani RJ. An introduction to the bootstrap. London, United Kingdom: Chapman-Hall; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Barber JS, Murphy SA, Verbitsky N. Adjusting for Time-Varying Confounding in Survival Analysis. Sociological Methodology. 2004;34:163–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Robins JM. Structural nested failure time models. In: Armitage P, Colton T, editors. Encyclopedia of Biostatistics. Wiley; Chichester: 1998. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES