Abstract
We studied if testosterone and estradiol concentrations are associated with specific female waist-to-hip ratios (WHRs) and body mass indices (BMIs). Participants were 187 young women from which waist, hips, weight, and height were measured. In addition, participants informed on which day of their menstrual cycle they were and provided a 6 mL saliva sample. Ninety-one of them were in the follicular phase and 96 in the luteal phase. Only in the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle we found a significant interaction between testosterone and estradiol affecting WHR (b ± s.e. = −0.000003 ± 0.000001; t 94 = −2.12, adjusted R 2 = −0.008, P = 0.03). Women with the highest levels of both hormones had the lowest WHRs, while women with low estradiol and high testosterone showed the highest WHRs. BMI significantly increased as testosterone increased in female in their nonfertile days.
1. Introduction
Given the current worldwide obesity crisis, waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) and body mass index (BMI) are recommended anthropometric measures to physicians to serve as proxies of metabolic and cardiovascular risk in women and men, with WHR being a better predictor of cardiovascular disease and diabetes than BMI ([1–3], but see [4]). It is known that as WHR and BMI increase, there is a higher risk of incurring into a variety of diseases, ranging from hypertension to diabetes, cancer, fertility problems, and depression [1, 4–6].
From a different perspective, evolutionary psychology, knowing that a low female WHR is associated with increased fecundity, Singh [7] showed that men rate female with low WHRs, particularly those equal or close to 0.7, as the most attractive, without BMI having such a strong effect. Low WHRs are attractive to men [7–12], even cross-culturally [13–15]. However, mean WHR differs between human populations; women from non-Western countries have on average higher WHR than women from Western countries [16]. Women with high WHRs have more sons than those with a lower WHR, which in turn have more daughters [17]. Moreover, besides cardiovascular risk, women with high WHRs suffer from anxiety, low self-esteem, and anger and receive less social support [18].
High estradiol concentrations are deemed as responsible for maintaining a low WHR, since this steroid regulates fat accumulation in the buttocks, hip, thighs, and bosom [19, 20]. Estradiol also affects women behavior and personality. Women with low estrone-3-glucuronide (the main metabolite of estradiol) levels exhibit stronger ovulatory shifts in preferences for masculine male voices during the late follicular phase and feminine male voices during the luteal phase than do women with high estrone-3-glucuronide levels [21]. During the late follicular phase, the reported ideal number of children increases with estrone-3-glucuronide levels in young nulliparous women [22], and ratings of the attractiveness of photographs of male faces during this phase are positively correlated with women's estradiol concentrations and the testosterone concentrations of the pictured men [23]. Overbidding (i.e., bidding that exceeds the risk-neutral Nash equilibrium bid) in auction games increases along with estradiol levels in women [24]. Implicit power motivation (a concern for having an impact on others or the world at large) is positively correlated with estradiol levels, and affiliation motivation (a concern for establishing, maintaining, or restoring close, friendly relationships) is high at midcycle [25–27]. High estradiol concentrations are associated with low levels of attachment avoidance and high levels of intimacy motivation, two personality traits involved in looking for and maintaining emotional intimacy [28]. On the other hand, besides high avoidance attachment and high attachment anxiety, women with low basal estradiol levels show an attenuated estradiol response to viewing emotional, nonsexual, and intimacy scenes, such as daughter-father interactions [29]. Concerning female sexuality, estradiol is positively correlated with overall sexual desire and the Sensory Dimension [30] of the Orgasm Checklist [31]. Weak sexual arousal and unsatisfactory orgasmic experiences are common in women with low estradiol concentrations [30]. Women having high estradiol concentrations throughout the late follicular phase experience more jealous feelings to imagined partner's sexual infidelity and more anger than hurt to imagined partner's emotional infidelity [32, 33].
Recent studies have revealed an outstanding role of feminine testosterone in metabolic and psychological processes, including WHR. The action of testosterone on WHR seems contrary to that of estradiol. High WHRs are correlated with high testosterone levels in pre- and perimenopausal women [34–36] or in medical conditions where this hormone is naturally augmented such as in polycystic syndrome or morbid obesity [36, 37]. Concerning sexuality, women with high androgen levels experience more orgasms [38], report more satisfactory orgasmic experiences [39], have greater well-being [40], tend to be promiscuous [41], and exhibit increased attraction to masculine faces in the late follicular days [42]. Socially, they are dominant persons [43], are usually involved in long-term relationships [44], and achieve greater occupational reputation [45]. However, they score low on the “children lover” and “maternal” items of the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) [46]. As entrepreneurs they exhibit less risk aversion in auction games [24], exhibit increased risk seeking for losses, and are more verbally aggressive [16].
We studied the relation between salivary estradiol and testosterone with WHR and BMI in a population of young healthy women. The main idea was to search for a role of these steroids in eliciting interindividual variation in these two anthropometric measures. In particular, we were interested in WHR, which has been correctly identified as an “honest” signal of potential female fertility, supported by gynecological findings (see references above). Considering that in women estrogens promote fat accumulation in hips, buttocks, thighs, and bosom [19, 20], while androgens promote fat accumulation in the abdomen, leading to weight increase [34, 35], we expected that the lowest WHRs would be associated with high levels of estradiol, while both high WHRs and BMIs would be positively correlated with testosterone.
2. Material and Methods
2.1. Subjects
One hundred eighty-seven women voluntarily participated in the present work. Volunteers were students recruited in various faculties at the Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México and the Escuela Nacional de Antropología e Historia in Mexico City. To be eligible, participants were required to be either in the second week of the menstrual phase (late follicular) or in the third to fourth week (luteal phase). All participants signed an agreement stating they had read and understood they were taking part in a nonintrusive research protocol and have no impediment to do so. For taking part in the study, participants received a monetary compensation (~$20.00 USD at the time of the study). Participants were not asked to provide written name or any other confidential data but had to agree on being measured and give age. In addition, participants were required not to be using hormonal or chemical contraceptives, to have had regular menstrual cycles (28–30 days) during the last 6 months, and not to suffer from any sort of gynecological illness. Table 1 shows the characteristics of our sample of participants.
Table 1.
Fertile | Nonfertile | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Mean | Std. deviation | Minimum | Maximum | N | Mean | Std. deviation | Minimum | Maximum | |
Age (years) | 91 | 22.02 | 2.60 | 18.00 | 29.00 | 96 | 22.06 | 2.90 | 18.00 | 36.00 |
Weight (kg) | 59.70 | 10.50 | 43.00 | 92.00 | 61.46 | 12.46 | 44.00 | 107.00 | ||
Height (m) | 1.58 | 0.07 | 1.34 | 1.80 | 1.59 | 0.06 | 1.48 | 1.79 | ||
Waist-to-hip ratio | 0.76 | 0.05 | 0.66 | 0.93 | 0.78 | 0.05 | 0.69 | 0.93‡ | ||
Body mass index | 23.59 | 3.52 | 17.22 | 34.00 | 24.04 | 3.88 | 17.19 | 34.77 | ||
Estradiol (pg/mL) | 233.41 | 184.77 | 86.10 | 1138.10 | 36.10 | 25.28 | 0.00 | 85.71*** | ||
Testosterone (ng/dL) | 72.02 | 29.93 | 20.00 | 160.00 | 56.83 | 21.21 | 27.00 | 123.00*** |
‡ P < 0.1; *** P < 0.0001 compared to the fertile phase.
2.2. Ethical Note
The Bioethics Committee of the Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatría “Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz” approved our research protocol. The study conforms to the guidelines of the Mexican Official Norm and the World Health Organization regulations for research with human beings.
2.3. Procedure
Participants were asked to present at the lab between 0900 and 1100 h when testosterone levels are high [47, 48], to be measured and provide a 6 mL saliva sample (collected in sterile polypropylene vials) for hormonal assessment, and inform if they had or not passed their fertile days. Height, waist, and hip circumference were obtained using a measurement tape and weight was measured using an electronic floor scale (0.1 kg precision). Waist circumference was measured at the narrowest point between the chest and hips. Hip circumference was measured around the hips and buttocks.
Saliva samples were immediately frozen in acetone and dry ice and stored at −70°C. Afterwards samples were freed from mucopolysaccharides by subjecting them to 4 subsequent freeze-thaw cycles and centrifugation (3000 rpm × 30 min), collecting the supernatants in each cycle to proceed to the next freeze-thaw cycle. We measured testosterone by quimioluminiscence (IMMULITE 1000 Siemens counter, ciudad). The minimum testosterone measured was 20 ng/dL and the maximum 160 ng/dL. Interassay and intra-assay coefficients were 8.95% and 8.02%, respectively.
We used RIA to measure estradiol in duplicate, using solid-phase Coat-A-Count 125I RIA. The estradiol standard was diluted 30 times in albumin buffer 4% pH 7.2. Interassay and intra-assay coefficients were 7.84% and 7.01%, respectively.
2.4. Analyses
To ensure that participants did know in which phase of the menstrual cycle (late follicular or luteal) they were, comparisons between measurements obtained from women in the fertile (late follicular) and nonfertile (luteal) days were done by means of t-tests. To test the relation between estradiol and testosterone and WHR and BMI we used Hierarchical Linear Regression. Independent variables were entered in the next order: (1) estradiol, (2) testosterone, and (3) the testosterone × estradiol interaction. Following Aiken and West [49] method to plot and obtain slopes of interactions in Multiple Regression, testosterone and estradiol mean-centered data were used in the Hierarchical Linear Regressions. Analyses were done using SPSS 17 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). All tests are two-tailed, and significance was set at P ≤ 0.05.
3. Results
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of our sample. Participants in the fertile and the nonfertile phase were on average the same age, although the oldest participant pertains to the nonfertile sample. Our sample included 4 undernourished women (BMI < 18.5), 2 in the fertile phase and 2 in the nonfertile phase; 31 overweight participants (25 < BMI < 30), 2 in the fertile phase and 29 in the nonfertile phase; 15 obese (BMI ≥ 30), 6 in the fertile phase and 9 in the nonfertile. The WHR of women in the nonfertile phase was nonsignificantly higher than those from the fertile phase (t 185 = 1.71, P = 0.09), but BMIs were on average equally distributed. Estradiol was significantly higher in the fertile phase (t 185 = 10.4, P < 0.0001) and so was testosterone (t 185 = 4.02, P < 0.0001).
Appreciate in Figure 1 that, in the total sample, WHR and BMI had a significant positive correlation (b ± s.e. = 0.01 ± 0.001; t 185 = 7.05, R 2 = 0.21, P < 0.0001). This positive association did not change with phase (fertile: b ± s.e. = 0.01 ± 0.001; t 89 = 5.8, R 2 = 0.27, P < 0.0001: nonfertile: b ± s.e. = 0.005 ± 0.001; t 94 = 4.2, R 2 = 0.16, P < 0.0001). However, the slope of the fertile days was significantly steeper than the one of the nonfertile days (t 185 = 2.12, P = 0.035).
The BMI was neither correlated with estradiol, testosterone, nor the estradiol × testosterone interaction in women in their fertile days. Yet, in the nonfertile phase, BMI increased significantly as testosterone levels were higher (b ± s.e. = 0.04 ± 0.02; t 94 = 2.1, R 2 = 0.07, adjusted R 2 = 0.05, P = 0.041). Performing the analysis using the raw data yields exactly the same significant effect of testosterone (b ± s.e. = 0.04 ± 0.02; t 94 = 2.32, R 2 = 0.06, adjusted R 2 = 0.045, P = 0.023; Figure 2).
We found a significant interaction between estradiol and testosterone (b ± s.e. = −0.000003 ± 0.000001; t 94 = −2.12, R 2 = 0.065, adjusted R 2 = 0.032, P = 0.03) in the WHR analyses of the fertile phase. Figure 3 shows the Aiken and West [49] plot for the interaction between independent variables. Low levels of testosterone had no effect on WHR at varying levels of estradiol (b = 0.00007 ± 0.00005; t 89 = 1.4, P = 0.2). The high testosterone × estradiol effect showed a significant negative slope (b = −0.0001 ± 0.00005; t 89 = −2.31, P = 0.023). The highest WHRs were found when testosterone was high and estradiol low, but as estradiol increased, WHR decreased, accounting for the minimal WHRs when both steroids were high (Figure 3). The slopes were significantly different (t 180 = 2.8, P = 0.006). No significant effects of estradiol and testosterone on WHR were found in the nonfertile phase.
Finally, we also analyzed the effect of the estradiol/testosterone ratio on BMI and WHR. In neither case we found a significant relationship between the estradiol/testosterone ratio and BMI or WHR controlling for menstrual phase, nor in the total sample (BMI: b ± s.e. = −0.073 ± 0.09; t 185 = −0.80, R 2 = 0.003, P = 0.42; WHR: b ± s.e. = −0.0 ± 0.001; t 185 = −0.33, R 2 = 0.001, P = 0.74).
4. Discussion
Our results show that despite being positively correlated, WHR is not totally dependent on BMI. The coefficient of determination (R 2) in our study shows that only 21% of the datapoints (39/187) accounted for the significant linear relationship. A look at Figure 1 reveals that the highest WHRs do not account for the most extreme BMIs. Moreover, we found that WHR measures, although slightly, vary between the follicular and luteal phases, while BMI does not, a finding that launches a voice of caution when measuring WHR for either clinical or research studies, since menstrual cycle phase might introduce measurement errors in studies, such as water accumulation throughout the luteal phase.
We found that women having the smallest WHRs during the fertile phase also had the greatest salivary concentrations of testosterone and estradiol. Since testosterone is the precursor of estradiol, our finding could suggest these women might have a higher turnover of estradiol, which, in turn, favors fat deposition in hips and buttocks. However, using the estradiol/testosterone ratio as a proxy of testosterone-to-estradiol conversion did not yield significant results. However, high levels of the nonaromatizable androgen dihydrotestosterone and of sex-hormone binding globulin [35, 50] that inhibit fat accumulation in the abdomen might also contribute to reducing the waist. Our findings show that it is the conjoint action of both estradiol and testosterone (or its metabolites) that results in the “attractive” ca. 0.7 WHR [8]. However, more studies are required to understand the metabolic pathways involved.
van Anders and Hampson [34] and Sowers et al. [36] reported a positive correlation between mean bioavailable testosterone levels and female WHR. However, both studies did not measure estradiol. Also, their samples included a large age range, up to 42 years of age, while ours, except for a single case, was restricted to young women going from their eighteens to the late twenties. In women, testosterone decreases monotonically with age, and by 40 women have around half the testosterone concentrations they had by the age of 20 [40]. Therefore, the main difference between the van Anders and the Hampson and the Sowers et al. studies and ours was that our participants were younger; we compared the follicular versus the luteal phase; we measured total estradiol and testosterone levels and included estradiol as a moderator of the testosterone effects. Yet, we cannot attribute our findings to free estradiol or free testosterone effects; therefore, our results are correlative rather than causal. But, considering that our sample encompassed undernourished to obese women (which did not show the highest testosterone levels), we show that high levels of testosterone are not entirely accountable of a high WHR or a high BMI, except in the luteal phase for the latter case. The findings that BMI increased significantly along with increasing testosterone levels in the nonfertile days when estradiol levels are significantly low or that in the fertile days women with the lowest estradiol levels and the highest testosterone levels had the highest WHRs are strong evidence that estrogens levels moderate the testosterone effects. As mentioned above, the metabolic pathways need to be unraveled. Nonetheless, our study shows that testosterone or its metabolites have effects in determining female body shape.
Acknowledgments
This research project was supported by the Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatría “Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz” (Research Grant no. NC093420.0). The authors are grateful to Gemma Estudillo for her valuable help with laboratory work.
Conflict of Interests
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests that could be perceived as prejudicing the impartiality of the research reported.
References
- 1.Browning L. M., Hsieh S. D., Ashwell M. A systematic review of waist-to-height ratio as a screening tool for the prediction of cardiovascular disease and diabetes: 05 could be a suitable global boundary value. Nutrition Research Reviews. 2010;23(2):247–269. doi: 10.1017/S0954422410000144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gharipour M., Sarrafzadegan N., Sadeghi M., et al. Predictors of metabolic syndrome in the Iranian population: waist circumference, body mass index, or waist to hip ratio? Cholesterol. 2013;2013:6. doi: 10.1155/2013/198384.198384 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Liu K., He S., Hong B., et al. Over time, do anthropometric measures still predict diabetes incidence in Chinese Han nationality population from Chengdu community? International Journal of Endocrinology. 2013;2013:10. doi: 10.1155/2013/239376.239376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Emerging Risk Factors Collaboration. Separate and combined associations of body-mass index and abdominal adiposity with cardiovascular disease: collaborative analysis of 58 prospective studies. The Lancet. 2011;377(9771):1085–1095. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(11)60105-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Zheng Y., Sun Q., Chen K., et al. Waist-to-hip ratio, dyslipidemia, glycemic levels, blood pressure and depressive symptoms among diabetic and non-diabetic Chinese women: a cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(10) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109765.e109765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Pantasri T., Norman R. J. The effects of being overweight and obese on female reproduction: a review. Gynecological Endocrinology. 2014;30(2):90–94. doi: 10.3109/09513590.2013.850660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Singh D. Body shape and women's attractiveness: the critical role of waist-to-hip ratio. Human Nature. 1993;4(3):297–321. doi: 10.1007/bf02692203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Singh D. Female mate value at a glance: relationship of waist-to-hip ratio to health, fecundity and attractiveness. Neuroendocrinology Letters. 2002;23:81–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.George H. R., Swami V., Cornelissen P. L., Tovée M. J. Preferences for body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio do not vary with observer age. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology. 2008;6(3):207–218. doi: 10.1556/jep.6.2008.3.4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Perilloux H. K., Webster G. D., Gaulin S. J. C. Signals of genetic quality and maternal investment capacity: the dynamic effects of fluctuating asymmetry and waist-to-hip ratio on men’s ratings of women’s attractiveness. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2010;1(1):34–42. doi: 10.1177/1948550609349514. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rilling J. K., Kaufman T. L., Smith E. O., Patel R., Worthman C. M. Abdominal depth and waist circumference as influential determinants of human female attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2009;30(1):21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.08.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dixson B. J., Grimshaw G. M., Linklater W. L., Dixson A. F. Eye-tracking of men's preferences for waist-to-hip ratio and breast size of women. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2011;40(1):43–50. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9523-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dixson B. J., Li B., Dixson A. F. Female waist-to-hip ratio, body mass index and sexual attractiveness in China. Current Zoology. 2010;56:175–810. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dixson B. J., Sagata K., Linklater W. L., Dixson A. F. Male preferences for female waist-to-hip ratio and body mass index in the highlands of Papua New Guinea. The American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2010;141(4):620–625. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Swami V., Jones J., Furnham A. Men's preferences for women's profile waist-to-hip ratio, breast size, and ethnic group in Britain and South Africa. British Journal of Psychology. 2009;100(2):313–325. doi: 10.1348/000712608X329525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cashdan E. Hormones and competitive aggression in women. Aggressive Behavior. 2003;29(2):107–115. doi: 10.1002/ab.10041. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Singh D., Zambarano R. J. Offspring sex ratio in women with android body fat distribution. Human Biology. 1997;69(4):545–556. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wing R. R., Matthews K. A., Kuller L. H., Meilahn E. N., Plantinga P. Waist to hip ratio in middle-aged women: associations with behavioral and psychosocial factors and with changes in cardiovascular risk factors. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology. 1991;11(5):1250–1257. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.11.5.1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Björntorp P. Adipose tissue distribution and function. International Journal of Obesity. 1991;15:67–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pedersen S. B., Kristensen K., Hermann P. A., Katzenellenbogen J. A., Richelsen B. Estrogen controls lipolysis by up-regulating α2A-adrenergic receptors directly in human adipose tissue through the estrogen receptor α. Implications for the female fat distribution. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2004;89(4):1869–1878. doi: 10.1210/jc.2003-031327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Feinberg D. R., DeBruine L. M., Jones B. C., Little A. C. Correlated preferences for men's facial and vocal masculinity. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2008;29(4):233–241. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Law Smith M. J., Deady D. K., Moore F. R., et al. Maternal tendencies in women are associated with estrogen levels and facial femininity. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61(1):12–16. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Roney J. R., Simmons Z. L. Women's estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men's testosterone. Hormones and Behavior. 2008;53(1):14–19. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schipper B. C. Sex hormones and choice under risk. SSRN Electronic Journal. 2012:1–70. http://www.ssrn.com/abstract=2046324. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schultheiss O. C., Dargel A., Rohde W. Implicit motives and gonadal steroid hormones: effects of menstrual cycle phase, oral contraceptive use, and relationship status. Hormones and Behavior. 2003;43(2):293–301. doi: 10.1016/s0018-506x(03)00003-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Stanton S. J., Schultheiss O. C. Basal and dynamic relationships between implicit power motivation and estradiol in women. Hormones and Behavior. 2007;52(5):571–580. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stanton S. J., Edelstein R. S. The physiology of women's power motive: implicit power motivation is positively associated with estradiol levels in women. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43(6):1109–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Edelstein R. S., Stanton S. J., Henderson M. M., Sanders M. R. Endogenous estradiol levels are associated with attachment avoidance and implicit intimacy motivation. Hormones and Behavior. 2010;57(2):230–236. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Edelstein R. S., Kean E. L., Chopik W. J. Women with an avoidant attachment style show attenuated estradiol responses to emotionally intimate stimuli. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61(2):167–175. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.van Anders S. M., Brotto L., Farrell J., Yule M. Associations among physiological and subjective sexual response, sexual desire, and salivary steroid hormones in healthy premenopausal women. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2009;6(3):739–751. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2008.01123.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mah K., Binik Y. M. Do all orgasms feel alike? Evaluating a two-dimensional model of the orgasm experience across gender and sexual context. Journal of Sex Research. 2002;39(2):104–113. doi: 10.1080/00224490209552129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Geary D. C., DeSoto M. C., Hoard M. K., Sheldon M. S., Cooper M. L. Estrogens and relationship jealousy. Human Nature. 2001;12(4):299–320. doi: 10.1007/s12110-001-1001-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gaulin S. J. C., Silverman I., Phillips K., Reiber C. Activational hormonal influences on abilities and attitudes. Implications for evolutionary theory evolution and cognition. Evolution and Cognition. 1997;3(2):191–199. [Google Scholar]
- 34.van Anders S. M., Hampson E. Waist-to-hip ratio is positively associated with bioavailable testosterone but negatively associated with sexual desire in healthy premenopausal women. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67(2):246–250. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000151747.22904.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Santoro N., Torrens J., Crawford S., et al. Correlates of circulating androgens in mid-life women: the study of women’s health across the nation. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2005;90(8):4836–4845. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-2063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sowers M. F., Beebe J. L., McConnell D., Randolph J., Jannausch M. Testosterone concentrations in women aged 25–50 years: associations with lifestyle, body composition, and ovarian status. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2001;153(3):256–264. doi: 10.1093/aje/153.3.256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ehrmann D. A. Polycystic ovary syndrome. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2005;352(12):1223–1236. doi: 10.1056/nejmra041536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.van Anders S. M., Hamilton L. D., Schmidt N., Watson N. V. Associations between testosterone secretion and sexual activity in women. Hormones and Behavior. 2007;51(4):477–482. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.van Anders S. M., Dunn E. J. Are gonadal steroids linked with orgasm perceptions and sexual assertiveness in women and men? Hormones and Behavior. 2009;56(2):206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Davis S. R., Tran J. Testosterone influences libido and well being in women. Trends in Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2001;12(1):33–37. doi: 10.1016/s1043-2760(00)00333-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.van Anders S. M., Hamilton L. D., Watson N. V. Multiple partners are associated with higher testosterone in North American men and women. Hormones and Behavior. 2007;51(3):454–459. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Welling L. L. M., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., et al. Raised salivary testosterone in women is associated with increased attraction to masculine faces. Hormones and Behavior. 2007;52(2):156–161. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Mehta P. H., Josephs R. A. Testosterone and cortisol jointly regulate dominance: evidence for a dual-hormone hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior. 2010;58(5):898–906. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.08.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.van Anders S. M., Goldey K. L. Testosterone and partnering are linked via relationship status for women and ‘relationship orientation’ for men. Hormones and Behavior. 2010;58(5):820–826. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Al-Ayadhi L. Y. Sex hormones, personality characters and professional status among Saudi females. Saudi Medical Journal. 2004;25(6):711–716. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Deady D. K., Smith M. J. L., Sharp M. A., Al-Dujaili E. A. S. Maternal personality and reproductive ambition in women is associated with salivary testosterone levels. Biological Psychology. 2006;71(1):29–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Rivera-Woll L. M., Papalia M., Davis S. R., Burger H. G. Androgen insufficiency in women: diagnostic and therapeutic implications. Human Reproduction Update. 2004;10(5):421–432. doi: 10.1093/humupd/dmh037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Liening S. H., Stanton S. J., Saini E. K., Schultheiss O. C. Salivary testosterone, cortisol, and progesterone: two-week stability, interhormone correlations, and effects of time of day, menstrual cycle, and oral contraceptive use on steroid hormone levels. Physiology & Behavior. 2010;99(1):8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Aiken L., West S. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. London, UK: Sage; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Blouin K., Veilleux A., Luu-The V., Tchernof A. Androgen metabolism in adipose tissue: recent advances. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology. 2009;301(1-2):97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2008.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]