Abstract
Marital strain confers risk of cardiovascular disease (CVD), perhaps though cardiovascular reactivity (CVR) to stressful marital interactions. CVR to marital stressors may differ between middle-age and older adults, and types of marital interactions that evoke CVR may also differ across these age groups, as relationship contexts and stressors differ with age. We examined cardiovascular responses to a marital conflict discussion and collaborative problem solving in 300 middle-aged and older married couples. Marital conflict evoked greater increases in blood pressure, cardiac output and cardiac sympathetic activation than did collaboration. Older couples displayed smaller heart rate responses to conflict than did middle-aged couples, but larger blood pressure responses to collaboration–especially older men. These effects were maintained during a post-task recovery period. Women did not display greater CVR than men on any measure or in either interaction context, though they did display greater parasympathetic withdrawal. CVR to marital conflict could contribute to the association of marital strain with CVD for middle-aged and older men and women, but other age-related marital contexts (e.g., collaboration among older couples) may also contribute to this mechanism.
Keywords: marital conflict, cardiovascular reactivity, collaboration, impedance cardiography, heart rate variability, aging
Marriage is an important influence on physical health across the lifespan. Married individuals live longer than the unmarried, and they are at reduced risk of cardiovascular disease (CVD) (Johnson, Backlund, Sorlie, & Loveless, 2000; Kaplan & Kronick, 2006), although this benefit is often stronger for men than women (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001). Marital quality also influences health. Divorce, separation, and marital strain or conflict are associated with reduced longevity (Tucker et al, 1996), increased risk of hypertension and related cardiovascular complications (Baker et al., 2000), atherosclerosis (Gallo et al., 2003), coronary heart disease (De Vogli, Chandola, & Marmot, 2007; Matthews & Gump, 2002; Zhang & Hatward, 2006), and poor medical prognosis among persons with heart disease (Orth-Gomer et al, 2000; Rohrbaugh, Shoham, & Coyne, 2006). Similarly, for men low marital quality reduces otherwise beneficial effects of frequent spouse contact on progression of atherosclerosis (Janicki et al., 2005).
Physiological effects of stress are an important mechanism in models of health effects of marriage. Social support is associated with reduced physiological responses to stressful stimuli (Uchino, 2006), and this effect could contribute to health benefits of supportive relationships generally and marriage in particular (Uchino et al., 2007). Reports of marital strain are associated with larger cardiovascular and neuroendocrine responses during marital disagreements, as is hostile behavior during such interactions (Robles & Kiecolt-Glaser, 2003). These responses could contribute to health consequences of low marital quality, as heightened cardiovascular reactivity (CVR)–increases in heart rate and blood pressure in response to stressors–are hypothesized to confer increased risk of CVD.
Specifically, unlike metabolically appropriate increases in heart rate and blood pressure during physical activity (e.g., exercise), CVR during psychological stressors exceeds metabolic demands (Sapolsky, 2004; Sherwood, Allen, Obrist, & Langer, 1986). Over time, repeated and prolonged episodes of metabolically excessive CVR are hypothesized to promote sustained increases in blood pressure, development of atherosclerosis, and the precipitation of cardiovascular events (i.e., stroke; myocardial infarction) (Obrist, 1981; Schwartz et al., 2003). Consistent with this view, CVR during psychological stressors is associated with increased risk of hypertension (Matthews et al., 2004; Matthews, Salomon, Brady, & Allen, 2003), atherosclerosis (Barnett et al., 1997; Jennings et al., 2004; Matthews et al., 2006), stroke (Everson et al., 2001; Waldstein et al., 2004) and manifestations of coronary heart disease (Krantz et al., 1991). Given its potential role in associations between marital processes and CVD, the present study examined CVR during marital interactions. Specifically, we tested the effects of age, sex, and type of marital interaction on CVR. We also examined similar effects on post-interaction levels of CVR, as poor recovery following psychological stressors also contributes to CVD (e.g., Steptoe & Marmot, 2005; Stewart, Janicki & Kamark, 2006).
Aging and Adult Development
The cardiovascular diseases influenced by marital quality develop over many years, usually becoming clinically apparent during middle and later adulthood. Hence, it is important to examine mechanisms linking marital processes to disease across a wide age range. CVR to psychological stressors is generally higher in later adulthood (Jennings et al, 1997; Uchino et al., 2005; Uchino et al., 2006). Hence, marital stressors might evoke greater CVR among older adults.
The nature of marital stressors
However, marital interactions differ across adulthood in ways that might reduce CVR (Gagnon, Hersen, Kabacoff, & Van Hasselt, 1999; Rook, Mavandadi, Sorkin, & Zettel, 2007). For example, older couples sometimes display less hostility and greater affection during marital conflicts than do younger couples, and reduced physiological reactivity during these interactions (Carstensen, Gottman, & Levenson, 1995; Levenson, Carstensen, & Gottman, 1993). Therefore, physiological effects of conflict might be a less important mechanism linking marital difficulties and health among older couples.
Yet, stress may occur in other interaction contexts for older couples. Older couples engage in more conjoint efforts to manage routine tasks and challenges (i.e., collaborative problem solving) than younger couples (Baltes & Staudinger, 1996; Berg et al., 2006), and they may benefit more from such efforts (Johansson, Andersson, & Ronnberg, 2000). Couples of all ages collaborate, but it is a more important means of adapting to demands of daily life in later adulthood, in compensating for declining cognitive or physical abilities, or simply as spouses spend more time together in retirement (Dixon & Gould, 1998). Increased collaboration could be stressful for older couples, especially if it raised concerns regarding independence, competence, or control in the relationship (Ehrensaft et al., 1999; Gray-Little & Burks, 1983; Martire, Stephens, Druley, & Wojno, 2002).
Determinants of CVR
Physiological mechanisms underlying CVR may also differ with age (Cooper, Katzel, & Waldstein, 2007). Heart rate (HR) reactivity is influenced by both sympathetic and parasympathetic mechanisms, and these underlying autonomic responses may differ with age. Sympathetic influences on HR can be measured as cardiac pre-ejection period, the time elapsed between the beginning of the contraction of the heart and the moment when ejected blood forces the aortic valve to open. Shorter pre-ejection periods reflect more vigorous (i.e., rapid and forceful) ventricular contraction, an indication of increased sympathetic input to the heart. Parasympathetic influences on HR can be assessed as high frequency heart rate variability, an index of respiratory sinus arrhythmia (Cacioppo, Uchino, & Bernston, 1994). Heart rate increases during inspiration and decreases during exhalation. The proportion of HR variability that corresponds to this respiratory cycle is related to the strength of parasympathetic input to the heart, such that greater high frequency heart rate variability indicates greater parasympathetic activity.
Blood pressure reactivity is influenced by cardiac and vascular mechanisms (Sherwood et al., 1990). Cardiac influences refer to cardiac output–the amount of blood pumped from the left heart each minute. Holding other factors constant (e.g., dilation vs. constriction of the vasculature), increases in cardiac output raise blood pressure. Vascular influences on blood pressure are reflected in total peripheral resistance–the aggregate resistance to blood flow through the systemic circulation. Short-term changes in peripheral resistance generally reflect the constriction versus dilation of the vasculature. Holding cardiac output constant, increased in peripheral resistance through vascular constriction raises blood pressure, whereas dilation lowers blood pressure.
Aging is associated with increased sympathetic activity and tonic peripheral resistance, and decreased heart rate variability (Cooper et al., 2007). Psychological influences on these mechanisms might also vary with age. For example, the psychological experience of challenge evokes pre-ejection period shortening (i.e., increased sympathetic activity) and increased cardiac output, whereas threat evokes increased peripheral resistance (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996). Similarly, active coping and effortful task engagement evoke increased cardiac output and sympathetic cardiac activity, whereas watchful observation of the environment evokes increased peripheral resistance (Brownley, Hurwitz, & Schniederman, 2000). Older couples might find marital conflict less threatening or engaging than their middle-aged counterparts (Carstensen et al., 1995), perhaps altering these underlying mechanisms. Determinants of CVR during marital interaction have not been widely studied. However, a study of young couples found that HR reactivity during negative interactions reflected increased sympathetic activity (i.e., pre-ejection period shortening) rather than parasympathetic withdrawal, and blood pressure reactivity reflected increased cardiac output, suggesting a sympathetically-mediated response (Nealey-Moore et al., 2007) as in challenge.
Sex Differences in Marital Stress and Cardiovascular Reactivity
Effects of marital interactions on CVR may differ for men and women. Women are generally more troubled by problems in close relationships than are men (Davis, Matthews, & Twamley, 1999), perhaps because relationships are more central in their self-definitions (Cross & Madson, 1997). Women often display greater CVR to marital stressors than do men, and this toll from everyday relationship stressors could contribute to their reduced health benefit from marriage (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001). However, some studies do not find this sex difference (e.g., Broadwell & Light, 1999; Nealey-Moore et al., 2007). Further, women’s greater reactivity might reflect their tendency to identify potential marital disagreements and to initiate and pursue conflict discussions (Denton, Burleson, Hobbs, Von Stein, & Rodriguez, 2001; Newton & Sanford, 2003). That is, women’s greater CVR during negative marital interactions might reflect greater levels of speaking, task engagement, or active coping (Smith, Ruiz, & Uchino, 2000).
Models of gender-linked vulnerabilities to stress suggest that women may be susceptible to stressors related to relationship harmony versus discord, whereas men may be more reactive to issues of control, power, or status in the relationship (e.g., Helgeson, 2003; Smith, Gallo, Goble, Ngu, & Stark, 1998). In this view, female sex roles emphasize a broad motive complex labeled communion, such as concerns regarding the maintenance of relationship quality. In contrast, male sex roles emphasize a set of motives called agency, such as concerns about independence, achievement, status and control (Bakan, 1966; Wiggins, 1996). Hence, women may display greater reactivity when marital interactions threaten communion, as during conflict. In contrast, men may be more reactive when the interaction is related to agency, as during achievement-oriented tasks. In prior research, women tend to display greater CVR during marital interactions involving communal stressors such as disagreement, whereas men display greater reactivity when marital interactions emphasize dominance or achievement (e.g., Brown & Smith, 1992; Smith, Gallo et al., 1998).
These sex differences may also be moderated by age. As noted above, over the course of adulthood conflict could decrease in importance as a context for potentially stressful marital interaction, perhaps reducing women’s physiological reactivity to such stressors. In contrast, collaborative problem solving may increase in importance. Especially for older adults, collaboration could evoke issues related to agency, such as control, competence, and independence, as collaboration takes on added importance as a means to manage daily tasks in the face of growing concerns about declining cognitive abilities and functional activity levels. If so, collaboration could be particularly stressful for older men, given sex differences in CVR during agency and communion challenges (e.g., Smith et al., 1996; 1998).
The Present Study
To examine the effects of age, sex, and interactional context on CVR and recovery in response to marital interaction, we recorded HR and blood pressure as well as determinants of these responses in middle-aged and older couples during and after two tasks - a marital conflict discussion and collaborative problem solving. We predicted that marital disagreement would evoke greater CVR and less recovery than would collaborative problem solving, given its more aversive nature. Prior studies suggested that women would respond to disagreement with greater CVR than men (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001), although not all studies support this conclusion. If collaborative problem solving evokes concerns related to agency, men should be more responsive than women to this task (Brown & Smith, 1992; Smith et al., 1998). Prior findings of increased CVR with age (Jennings et al, 1997; Uchino et al, 2005) suggest that responses to stressful marital interactions would be larger among older than middle-aged couples. However, theory and research on age differences in interpersonal stress (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999) suggests that CVR in older individuals might be smaller than for middle-aged couples, especially during disagreement. We also predicted that collaborative problem solving would evoke greater CVR among older couples, given its greater relevance for older couples (Berg et al., 2006; Dixon & Gould, 1998). Finally, we examined affective responses, spouse appraisals, and overt behavior during these interactions (Smith et al., 2009) as mediators of age differences in CVR in response to conflict and collaboration.
Method
Participants
The Utah Health and Aging Study enrolled 146 middle-aged and 154 older couples during 2001–2005. Details of the sample, recruitment, and eligibility criteria are provided in a companion paper (Smith et al., 2009). Mean age for middle-aged wives and husbands was 43.9 (SD = 3.8, range = 32–54 years) and 45.8 years (SD = 4.0, range = 37–59), respectively, and 62.2 (SD = 4.5, range = 50–71) and 64.7 years (SD = 4.3, range = 52–76) for older wives and husbands. Mean marriage length was 18.4 years (range = 2.5 –31) for the middle-aged group and 36.4 years (range = 5 – 53) for the older group. All participants were free of pre-existing cardiovascular disease and not taking medications for cardiovascular disease (i.e., beta-blockers, calcium-blockers, anti-anginals).
Procedure
For details for the study protocol, see Smith et al. (2009). Prior to the initial laboratory session, participants completed questionnaires, including the Marital Adjustment Test (MAT; Locke & Wallace, 1959). During that session couples participated in a disagreement discussion and a collaborative problem-solving task in a counter-balanced order. Tasks were videotaped for behavioral coding. Before both tasks, couples sat quietly, facing each other at a table but separated from view by a movable partition. For 10 minute baselines, participants completed a minimally involving task procedure (Jennings et al., 1992; Smith et al., 2000) in which an audiotape recording prompted them to turn pages in a booklet to examine pairs of landscape pictures. At one minute intervals they silently indicated which picture they preferred and turned to the next pair. After both baselines, participants completed a state affect questionnaire indicating how they felt at that moment. Blood pressure readings were taken at 90 sec. intervals during baseline; the last three readings were averaged to compute baseline values. Impedance cardiography measures were recorded continuously during baseline, and values for the time period corresponding to the final three blood pressure readings were averaged for baseline values. After baseline, the partition was removed and participants received instructions for either the disagreement or collaborative problem solving task. After the task, the partition was replaced and two blood pressure readings were recorded during a 3 minute recovery period. Participants then underwent a second baseline period identical to the first. They then completed the second task, again followed by two blood pressure readings. A schematic of this procedure is presented in Figure 1.
Disagreement Task
As described elsewhere (Smith et al., 2009), for the disagreement task a mutually identified topic was selected for discussion. Prior to the first session couples separately completed the Areas of Disagreement Questionnaire adapted from Fincham (1985), rating their level of disagreement on 13 topic areas (e.g., money, household responsibilities, etc.). Topics with the greatest combined rating of disagreement were suggested for discussion, with the instruction that, “it needs to be something that is currently an issue rather than something that has been resolved in the past.” They were also informed that the topic should be an issue that, “you can discuss for the full time period with both of you contributing to the discussion, and both of you need to feel comfortable discussing it here.” After the topic was selected, couples were told, “we are not expecting you to solve the particular issue right now; you can think of this as an opportunity to work toward making progress on the issue.”
Participants began a 6-minute unstructured conversation regarding this topic and BP was measured three times at equally spaced intervals; impedance cardiography output was measured continuously. After the 6-minute discussion, audiotaped instructions led participants through a 6-minute turn-taking section in which spouses alternated speaking for 1-minute intervals; speaking order (i.e., husband versus wife first) was counterbalanced across couples. During this segment, one BP measurement was taken during each speaking turn; impedance cardiography recording continued continuously. The turn-taking portion was followed by another unstructured conversation during which couples were instructed to discuss the topic together for 3 minutes. Two additional BP measurements were taken, along with continuous impedance cardiography recording. The structured turn-taking conversation was used to control the effects of speaking on the physiological measurements. Following the discussion task, participants completed an affect measure, indicating how they felt during the task. They then rated their partner’s behavior during the interaction. As participants completed post-task questionnaires, two additional blood pressure readings were recorded and HR and other cardiovascular measures were recorded continuously for three minutes.
Collaborative Problem Solving Task
The collaborative task is also described in the companion report (Smith et al., 2009). Couples were given a map of a fictitious town and a list of errands, and asked to plan a route and schedule to complete the errands by the shortest distance and in a designated time period. The task had the same structure as the disagreement task, and physiological measures, self-reports of state affect and ratings of spouse’s behavior were recorded on the same schedule.
Measures
Affective, cognitive, and behavioral measures
We assessed state anger and anxiety after baseline and after the interaction tasks, and participants also rated their spouses’ levels of warmth (vs. hostility) and dominance (vs. submissiveness) during the tasks. We coded task behavior, using a system that captured variations in warmth versus hostility and aspects of control or dominance and assertion (see Smith et al., 2009).
Cardiovascular responses
Dinamap 8100 monitors (Critikon; Tampa, FL) were used to assess systolic and diastolic blood pressure (SBP, DBP) and heart rate (HR). An occluding cuff was attached to the upper portion of the non-dominant arm. To assess the determinants of HR and blood pressure responses, electrocardiogram (ECG), basal thorasic impedance (Zo), and the first derivative of the impedance signal (dZ/dt) were measured continuously using Minnesota Impedance Cardiographs (Model HIC 2000, Surcom; Minneapolis, MN). This allows for the measurement of stroke volume and pre-ejection period, and the estimation of cardiac output, total peripheral resistance, and high frequency heart rate variability as an index of respiratory sinus arrhythmia (Berntson, Cacioppo, & Quigley, 1993; Sherwood et al., 1990). Four band electrodes were placed consistent with guidelines (Sherwood et al., 1990). Voltage bands were placed at the base of the neck and at the bottom of the sternum, and current electrodes were placed at least 3 cm distal to the voltage electrodes. A 4-mA AC current at 100 kHz was passed through the two current bands, and the Zo and dZ/dt signals were recorded from the voltage electrodes. The ECG, Zo, and dZ/dt signals were digitized at 500 Hz. These data were ensemble-average in 1-min epochs, and the waveforms were verified or edited before conducting analyses. Using the Kubicek equation, stroke volume (SV) was estimated and cardiac output was calculated in l/min as HR X (SV/1000) (Sherwood et al., 1990). On the basis of mean arterial pressure (MAP) and cardiac output, total peripheral resistance (TPR) was calculated in resistance units (dynes-s X cm−5) as TPR = MAP/CO X 80 (Sherwood et al., 1990). Pre-ejection period was calculated as the time interval in msec between the Q-point of the ECG and the B-point of the dZ/dt signal. In this manner, HR, pre-ejection period, cardiac output, and total peripheral resistance values were determined for each 1-min interval of all experimental periods.
High frequency heart rate variability was calculated on the basis of the digitized inter-beat intervals from the ECG, initially checked and edited for artifacts using the detection algorithm of Berntson, Quigley, Jang, and Boysen (1990). A heart period time series was created from the inter-beat intervals using a weighted beat algorithm (Berntson, Cacioppo, & Quigley, 1995). Sharp transitions in the heart period time series (e.g., movement and heart rhythm artifacts) were detected and removed by smoothing (Berntson et al., 1990). A linear polynomial was fit to, and subtracted from, the heart period time series (Litvack, Oberlander, Carney, & Saul, 1995). This linear detrending acted as a high pass filter, removing very large ultralow frequency trends (including the DC component) from the input signal. The heart period time series was then band-pass filtered from 0.12 to 0.40 Hz (Neuvo, Cheng,-yu, & Mitra, 1984). The power spectrum of the heart period time series was calculated via a fast Fourier transformation and scaled to msec2/Hz. Heart rate variability was calculated for each minute as a natural log of the area under the heart period power spectrum within the corner frequencies of the bandpass filter (Litvack et al., 1995).
Overview of Analyses
Mixed ANOVA was used for the main analyses. To accommodate dependency of spouses’ responses, Sex was a repeated factor (Kenny, 1995). Task (collaboration vs. disagreement) and Period (i.e., initial unstructured discussion, structured interaction, second unstructured interaction within both tasks) were repeated factors.1 Age Group was a between subjects factor. For CVR, task minus baseline change scores were calculated (Llabre et al., 1991), using the baseline immediately preceding each task and average values within periods. For recovery, a Task x Sex x Age mixed ANOVA was used on recovery–baseline change scores. To follow-up significant interactions, mean comparisons with the appropriate error term from the ANOVA were used (Bernhardson, 1975). Effect sizes are reported as eta-squared. SBP, DBP, and HR were considered primary dependent variables; the remaining measures were used to explicate effects on primary outcomes.
Variations in degrees of freedom reflect missing data, mostly from poor quality impedance cardiography signals. Because couples were the unit of analysis (Kenny, 1995), missing data for either member resulted in a missing case. Percentages of missing cases were as follows: SBP, DBP, and HR −–4.7%; pre-ejection period –35.3%; heart rate variability –21.3%; cardiac output –35.7%; total peripheral resistance –39%. Given these levels of missing data, the main measures of CVR (i.e., HR, SBP, DBP) should be interpreted with more confidence than measures of underlying determinants. Age groups did not differ in amount of missing data, and no psychosocial factors (e.g., marital satisfaction, affect, etc.) were related to missing versus non-missing status.
Results
Overview of Task Differences in Affect, Spouse Appraisals, and Behavior
Before presenting results for CVR and recovery, it is important to summarize the affective, cognitive, and behavioral distinctions between the two marital interaction contexts, reported elsewhere (Smith et al., 2009). Compared to collaboration, disagreement evoked larger increases in anger and anxiety, and spouses rated each other as more hostile (i.e., less friendly) and more controlling. For objectively coded behavior, couples displayed more control, hostile control, and assertiveness during disagreement than collaboration. During disagreement couples also displayed more warmth, primarily in the form of disclosing information and affirming the partner’s comments. Behavior during collaboration primarily involved leading and following (i.e., warm control and submission). Hence, the tasks evoked distinct interactions and experiences, resembling closely the two marital contexts we intended to model as influences on CVR. Also, women displayed more anger during disagreement than did men, and they rated their spouses as more hostile. Older couples reported less anger in response to the disagreement than did middle-aged couples, and they perceived their partners as more friendly (i.e., less hostile) overall.
Baseline Cardiovascular Levels
Baseline levels of cardiovascular function and results of 2 (Age) x (2) (Sex) mixed ANOVAs of average baseline levels are presented in Table 1. Compared to men, women displayed lower baseline SBP, DBP, and cardiac output, and higher HR and heart rate variability. Consistent with prior research (Cooper et al., 2007), compared to the middle-aged group older participants displayed higher SBP, DBP, and total peripheral resistance; lower cardiac output and heart rate variability; and shorter pre-ejection periods.
Table 1.
Women | Men | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Middle-Aged | Older | Middle-Aged | Older | Age Effect |
Sex Effect |
Sex x Age |
||||
Measure | Mean (SE) | Mean (SE) | Mean (SE) | Mean (SE) | F | eta2 | F | eta2 | F | eta2 |
SBP (mm Hg) | 108.2 (1.2) | 122.8 (1.1) | 118.2 (1.1) | 125.5 (1.1) | 83.49** | .219 | 30.86** | .094 | 10.42** | .034 |
DBP (mmHg) | 66.5 (.71) | 69.5 (.70) | 73.9 (.72) | 75.4 (.71) | 9.15** | .030 | 99.64** | .251 | ||
CO (l/min) | 5.9 (.16) | 4.7 (.17) | 5.9 (.17) | 5.5 (.18) | 22.25** | .091 | 8.17* | .035 | 5.75* | .025 |
TPR | 1265 (57) | 1761 (60) | 1340 (55) | 1562 (58) | 41.42** | .157 | 5.40* | .024 | ||
HR (bpm) | 69.4 (.81) | 71.5 (.80) | 67.8 (.82) | 67.8 (.80) | 13.19** | .043 | ||||
PEP (msec) | 101.1 (1.9) | 94.5 (2.0) | 98.0 (2.3) | 94.8 (2.4) | 4.23* | .018 | ||||
RSA | 5.70 (.10) | 4.85 (.11) | 5.24 (.10) | 4.81 (.11) | 37.86** | .146 | 5.79* | .025 | 4.08* | .018 |
SBP = systolic blood pressure; DBP = diastolic blood pressure; CO = cardiac output; TPR = total peripheral resistance; HR = heart rate; PEP = pre-ejection period; RSA = respiratory sinus arrhythmia
p<.05,
p<.001, degrees of freedom range from 1 and 284 to 1 and 181
An Age x Sex interaction on SBP indicated that the age group difference was somewhat larger for women than men, and the sex difference was larger for middle-aged than older participants. An Age Group x Sex interaction for baseline cardiac output indicated that middle-aged men and women did not differ but older women displayed lower resting cardiac output than did older men and middle-aged women. A significant Age Group x Sex interaction on baseline total peripheral resistance indicated that the age-related difference in women’s total peripheral resistance was greater than that for men, though significant for both. Further, middle-aged men and women did not differ, but older women displayed higher resting total peripheral resistance than older men. Finally, an Age Group x Sex interaction for baseline heart rate variability indicated that the age–related difference was larger for women than for men. Overall, age–related differences in blood pressure and heart rate variability tended to be larger for women than for men. The larger age difference in women’s blood pressure likely reflects their greater age-related difference in total peripheral resistance, because older women actually showed less cardiac output.
Given the repeated nature of the tasks, it is important to note that analyses of order indicated that baseline levels before the second task were equivalent or somewhat below the initial baseline, for all measures. Hence, the second baseline was successful in returning participants to initial resting levels, and equally so for older and middle-aged participants, and for men and women.
Blood Pressure Reactivity and Determinants
Results are presented for each individual cardiovascular outcome in the following text. To provide a summary of common effects across measures, main effects for Task and Age Group are presented in Tables 2 and 3, respectively.
Table 2.
Measure | Disagreement Mean (SE) | Collaboration Mean (SE) | F | eta-squared |
---|---|---|---|---|
Task Period Reactivity | ||||
SBP (mm Hg) | 9.3 (.40) | 4.5 (.35) | 119.81** | .297 |
DBP (mmHg) | 5.6 (.21) | 3.4 (.19) | 77.04** | .213 |
CO (l/min) | .34 (.04) | −.09 (.05) | 50.56** | .210 |
TPR | 12.5 (16.0) | 73.8 (16.8) | 8.98** | .047 |
HR (bpm) | 2.5 (.15) | 3.7 (.17) | 43.93** | .134 |
PEP (msec) | −1.89 (.58) | −.29 (.59) | 5.35* | .027 |
RSA | .016 (.03) | −.144 (.04) | 16.16** | .074 |
Recovery Period | ||||
SBP (mm Hg) | 5.2 (.36) | 4.2 (.37) | 4.67* | .016 |
SBP = systolic blood pressure; DBP = diastolic blood pressure; CO = cardiac output; TPR = total peripheral resistance; HR = heart rate; PEP = pre-ejection period; RSA = respiratory sinus arrhythmia
p<.05,
p<.001, degrees of freedom range from 1 and 284 to 1 and 181
Table 3.
Measure | Middle-aged Mean (SE) | Older Mean (SE) | F | eta-squared |
---|---|---|---|---|
Task Period Reactivity | ||||
SBP (mm Hg) | 6.1 (.42) | 7.7 (.43) | 6.60* | .023 |
CO (l/min) | .045 (.053) | .202 (.052) | 4.51* | .023 |
TPR | 71.2 (17.9) | 15.0 (18.4) | 4.82* | .026 |
HR (bpm) | 3.6 (.18) | 2.6 (.18) | 13.80** | .046 |
Recovery Period | ||||
SBP (mm Hg) | 3.7 (.40) | 5.7 (.40) | 12.47** | .042 |
CO (l/min) | −.24 (.055) | −.04 (.056) | 6.33* | .032 |
HR (bpm) | 2.1 (.18) | 1.2 (.17) | 13.78** | .046 |
RSA | −.345 (.05) | −.189 (.05) | 4.85* | .024 |
SBP = systolic blood pressure; CO = cardiac output; TPR = total peripheral resistance; HR = heart rate; RSA = respiratory sinus arrhythmia
p<.05,
p<.001, degrees of freedom range from 1 and 284 to 1 and 181
A 2(Age) x (2) (Sex) x (2) (Task) mixed ANOVA on SBP change scores revealed a significant task main effect. As presented in Table 2, participants displayed a larger increase in SBP during disagreement than during collaboration. As presented in Table 3, an Age main effect indicated that older participants responded with a larger increase in SBP than did middle-aged participants. These main effects were qualified by a significant Sex x Task interaction, F(1,284) = 16.73, p<.001, eta-squared = .056, and a Sex x Task x Age interaction, F(1,284) = 7.45, p<.01, eta-squared = .026. Means for these effects on SBP reactivity are presented in Figure 2. Within the Sex x Task interaction, both men and women showed greater SBP reactivity during disagreement than collaboration (men: 8.9 mmHg vs. 5.9 mmHg, SE = .52, .50; women: 9.7 mmHg vs. 3.1 mmHg, SE = .53, .49), both t(284)>3.0, p<.01. During disagreement, men and women did not differ, but during collaboration men displayed larger increases in SBP than did women, t(284) >3.0, p<.01. Within the Sex x Task x Age interaction (see Figure 2), comparisons indicated that middle-aged and older men and women did not differ during disagreement. However, during collaboration, older men displayed greater SBP reactivity than did each of the other three groups, all t(284) > 3.0, p<.01.
The mixed ANOVA of DBP change scores revealed a significant Task main effect. As presented in Table 2, disagreement evoked greater DBP reactivity than collaboration. This effect was qualified by a Task x Age interaction, F(1,284) = 9.27, p<.005, eta-squared = .032 (see Figure 3). Follow-up comparisons indicated that for both middle-aged and older participants, disagreement evoked greater DBP reactivity than collaboration, both t(284) > 3.0, p<.01. Further, middle-aged and older groups did not differ during disagreement, but older participants displayed greater DBP reactivity during collaboration than middle-aged participants, t(284) > 3.0, p<.01.
The mixed ANOVA of cardiac output indicated disagreement evoked a significantly larger increase than collaboration, as presented in Table 2. As seen in Table 3, older participants responded with a larger increase in cardiac output across tasks than did middle-aged participants. This effect was qualified by an Age x Sex interaction, F(1,190) = 7.89, p<.005, eta-squared = .040. Follow-up comparisons indicated that the effect of Age Group was significant for men (older = .254 l/min, SE = .079; middle-aged = −.101 l/min, SE = .080), t(190) > 3.0, p<.01, but not women (older = .150 l/min, SE = .063; middle-aged = .191 l/min, SE = .064).
The mixed ANOVA of total peripheral resistance indicated a main effect for task, such that participants demonstrated little change during disagreement whereas during collaboration they displayed a significantly larger increase (see Table 2). Older participants displayed little task-related change in total peripheral resistance overall, whereas younger participants displayed a significantly larger increase (see Table 3). This latter effect was qualified by a significant Sex x Age interaction, F(1,181) = 6.08, p<.02, eta-squared = .033. Middle-aged (28.52, SE = 21.69) and older women (31.67, SE = 22.29) did not differ in their total peripheral resistance response, whereas middle-aged men (113.95, SE = 27.05) displayed a larger increase than older men (−1.68, SE = 27.80) and than both groups of women, all t(181) > 3.0, p<.01. No other effects approached significance.
To summarize, the greater SBP and DBP reactivity evoked by disagreement than collaboration reflected increases in cardiac output and occurred despite the fact that collaboration evoked greater increases in total peripheral resistance. Also, the greater blood pressure reactivity of older participants reflects their larger increases in cardiac output, and occurred despite their smaller increases in peripheral resistance. However, it is important to note again that as expected initial resting levels of total peripheral resistance were higher in the older group.
Heart Rate Reactivity and Determinants
A 2 (Age) x (2) (Task) x (2) (Sex) mixed ANOVA of HR change indicated greater HR reactivity during collaboration than disagreement (see Table 2). Middle-aged participants displayed greater HR reactivity across tasks than older participants (see Table 3). In a Task x Age interaction, F(1,284) = 10.47, p<.001, eta-squared = .036 (see Figure 4), follow-up comparisons indicated that collaboration evoked greater HR reactivity than disagreement for both middle-aged (3.90 bpm, SE = .239; 3.25 bpm, SE = .219), t(284) = 2.41, p<.05, and older participants (3.56 bpm, SE = .240; 1.66 bpm, SE = .220), t(284) >3.0, p<.01. Further, middle-aged and older participants did not differ during collaboration, but middle-aged participants displayed greater HR reactivity during disagreement than older participants, t(284) > 3.0, p<.01.
A mixed ANOVA of pre-ejection period indicated that disagreement evoked more cardiac sympathetic activation (i.e., pre-ejection period shortening) than collaboration (see Table 2). No other effects were significant.
The three-way mixed ANOVA of changes in heart rate variability indicated that collaboration evoked a larger decrease in cardiac parasympathetic activity than did disagreement (see Table 2). Women displayed a larger decrease in heart rate variability across tasks (−.130, SE = .035) than did men (.002, SE = .038), F(1,203) = 6.98, p<.001, eta-squared = .033. No other effects were significant for heart rate variability change.
To summarize, collaboration evoked a larger increase in HR than disagreement, likely reflecting greater decrease in parasympathetic activity (i.e., heart rate variability) during collaboration. In fact, this larger and parasympathetically-mediated HR response to collaboration than disagreement occurred despite the fact that disagreement evoked greater sympathetic cardiac activation (i.e., pre-ejection period shortening) than collaboration.
Recovery Period Effects
Blood pressure and determinants
Participants displayed higher recovery period SBP following disagreement than collaboration (see Table 2), and older couples displayed greater SBP than middle-aged couples (see Table 3). In an Age x Task interaction on DBP, F(1,286) = 4.75, p = .03, eta-squared = .016, older couples displayed larger increases over baseline DBP than did middle-aged couples after collaboration but not disagreement (see Figure 5). Further, recovery period DBP was higher following disagreement than collaboration for middle-aged but not older couples.
During recovery middle-aged couples showed lower cardiac output–below baseline levels–than did older couples (see Table 3), and there were no significant effects on total peripheral resistance during recovery. Hence, older participants’ greater blood pressure during recovery reflected higher cardiac output relative to the middle-aged group.
Heart rate and determinants
Middle-aged participants displayed higher recovery period HR than older participants, and lower levels of heart rate variability (see Table 3). Age groups did not differ in pre-ejection period during recovery. Hence, the higher recovery period HR of the middle-aged group reflected greater parasympathetic withdrawal.
Exploratory Analyses of Psychosocial Mediators of Age Differences
To identify mediators of age group differences in CVR, we evaluated affective, spouse appraisal and behavioral variables that demonstrated parallel age differences (cf. Smith et al., 2009) using Sobel tests (MacKinnon et al., 2002). Although we identified several significant correlates of CVR, in no cases were these factors significant mediators of age group differences in CVR. For example, while controlling age, wives’ state anger during disagreement and their ratings of husbands’ affiliation during this task were associated with wives’ HR reactivity, partial r(290) = .12 and −.18, both p<.05. Husbands’ state anger was not significantly related to their HR reactivity during disagreement, partial r(290) = .10, p = .084, but wives’ state anger was related to husbands’ HR reactivity, partial r(290) = .16, p<.01. Further, higher scores on couples’ combined marital adjustment scores were associated with reduced HR reactivity during disagreement but not collaboration, Task x MAT interaction F(1,282) = 6.13, p<.015, eta-squared = .021. However, control of these psychosocial variables did not alter the age difference in HR responses to disagreement, and in none of these cases were Sobel tests of mediation significant. Similarly, husbands’ levels of warm control were significantly associated with their SBP and DBP reactivity during collaboration, partial r(290) = .12 and .15, both p < .04, but the age group difference in husbands’ SBP and DBP reactivity during collaboration remained significant when their levels of warm control were controlled, and tests of mediation were again non-significant. Tests of recovery period responses revealed no significant mediators.
Discussion
CVR during stressful marital interactions may link marital strain with the development and course of CVD. Such effects likely occur over many years during middle and later adulthood. Hence, marital interaction contexts in addition to conflict are important potential influences on CVR, because conflict may decline in later adulthood and collaborative problem solving increases in importance. Further, men and women may differ in CVR during marital interaction, or in their relative responsiveness to different aspects of marital interaction. Thus, the present study tested age and sex differences in CVR during marital disagreement and collaboration.
Relative to collaboration, disagreement evoked greater SBP and DBP reactivity, driven by cardiac sympathetic activation. That is, compared to collaboration, disagreement evoked larger increases in cardiac output, larger decreases in pre-ejection period, and smaller increases in total peripheral resistance. Effects of disagreement on blood pressure persisted into a post-task recovery period. In contrast, collaboration evoked greater HR reactivity than disagreement, despite the smaller increase in cardiac sympathetic activity (i.e., less pre-ejection period shortening). The greater HR response to collaboration was apparently due to parasympathetic withdrawal, reflected in the larger decrease in heart rate variability during collaboration, and these effects also persisted into the recovery period. Hence, cardiovascular response to collaboration was characterized by a parasympathetically-mediated increase in HR and increased peripheral resistance, whereas marital disagreement evoked greater blood pressure reactivity through cardiac sympathetic activation.
The pattern of CVR evoked by disagreement suggests greater task engagement, challenge, and/or effortful coping compared to collaboration (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996; Brownley et al., 2000). To the extent that these aspects of CVR confer increased risk of CVD, the present findings support models in which marital strain contributes to CVD through this mechanism (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001; Rhobles & Kiecolt-Glaser, 2003). In contrast, collaboration evoked greater HR reactivity through reduced parasympathetic activation, and increased total peripheral resistance. The psychological determinants of this pattern could involve cognitive processing demands of this task (Brownley et al., 2000; Gianaros, Van Der Veen, & Jennings, 2004). The negative health consequences of the pattern of CVR evoked by disagreement are better established than the consequences of the pattern evoked by collaboration. However, increased HR reactivity, heightened total peripheral resistance, and reduced heart rate variability have been implicated in the adverse effects of stress on health (Gianaros et al., 2005; Treiber et al., 2003; Uchino et al., 2007).
Age Differences in Response to Marital Interaction
Previous theory and research suggests that older adults differ from their middle-aged counterparts in ways that could alter effects of marital processes on CVR. For example, in prior research marital conflicts were characterized by more warmth and less hostility among older couples, as well as less physiological reactivity (e.g., Carstensen et al., 1995; Levenson, Carstensen, & Gottman, 1994). In other results from the present study (see Smith et al., 2009), disagreement evoked less anger from older couples than middle-aged participants and older participants rated their spouses as warmer across both tasks. In results reported here, there was mixed evidence of reduced CVR. Older couples demonstrated less HR reactivity during conflict than middle-aged couples. However, their blood pressure responses during conflict were similar to middle-aged couples. Further, during and after collaboration older participants–especially men–displayed greater blood pressure reactivity. Hence, results for HR reactivity supported the notion that physiological costs of marital conflict are reduced among older persons, but blood pressure responses indicated that the physiological cost of a different marital interaction context (i.e., collaboration) may be greater.
Older couples engage in collaboration more frequently than middle-aged couples (Berg et al., 2006), and in the present study they reacted to it with greater CVR. To the extent that CVR contributes to CVD, collaboration during marital interactions could be an important influence on cardiovascular health among older individuals, particularly for men. These age- related differences could reflect issues surrounding agency. Older individuals often construe collaboration as necessitated by a need to compensate for their declining cognitive abilities (Berg et al., 2006), raising concerns about their level of competence, independence, and control in daily activities. Or, one spouse may feel the need to direct such efforts, a task made more difficult by the concurrent desire to manage the emotional quality of the interaction (Blanchard-Fields, 2007; Carsetnsen et al., 1999). Such agency-related challenges or threats can heighten CVR, and in the context of marital interactions this is especially true for men (Brown & Smith, 1992; Smith et al., 1998).
We attempted to find mediators of age group differences in CVR, by examining associations with affective, appraisal, and behavioral variables where we found similar age differences (Henry et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2009). For example, older participants reported higher levels of marital satisfaction and less anger during disagreement, and appraised their spouses as friendlier during the interaction tasks, factors that might have contributed to older participants’ reduced HR reactivity during disagreement. Wives who reported larger increases in anger during disagreement displayed greater HR reactivity, as did their husbands. Wives who rated their husbands as more hostile during disagreement also displayed greater HR reactivity. Further, couples who reported higher marital satisfaction displayed smaller HR responses to disagreement. However, control of these factors did not attenuate the age group difference in HR reactivity during disagreement, and formal tests of mediation (MacKinnon et al., 2002) were not significant. Similarly, the greater levels of warm control displayed by older men during collaboration might reflect efforts to accomplish the instrumental task without straining relationship quality - a more salient and important goal among older couples (Blanchard-Fields, 2007; Carstensen et al., 1999). Greater engagement in this effort could evoke greater CVR. Men who displayed more warm control during collaboration had greater SBP reactivity during this task, but this behavioral variable did not mediate the age group difference in blood pressure reactivity. Hence, psychosocial variables were related to CVR in meaningful ways, but they did not mediate age differences in CVR.
The determinants of blood pressure and HR clarified the nature the CVR during marital conflict and collaboration. They also suggested additional age group differences. Across both tasks older men responded with larger increases in cardiac output and smaller increases in total peripheral resistance than did middle-aged men. This may suggest different psychological reactions, such that older men may have experienced the tasks as challenging while the middle-aged men experienced them as more threatening (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996). Smaller changes in peripheral resistance could also reflect age-related increases in vascular rigidity (Cooper et al., 2007).
The age differences in resting cardiovascular function are consistent with prior research (Cooper et al., 2007), including higher blood pressure and peripheral resistance, lower cardiac output and heart rate variability, and shorter pre-ejection period. These differences likely reflect underlying age-related increases in vascular stiffness and sympathetic tone, increased ventricular stiffness, and reduced parasympathetic tone. The greater SBP reactivity among older participants before and following marital interaction was accompanied by higher cardiac output rather than peripheral resistance, and therefore does not seem simply to reflect the general reduction in arterial compliance (i.e., less vascular dilation to accommodate increased blood pressure) commonly associated with aging. Further, the greater blood pressure response of older participants during and after martial interactions was more pronounced for collaboration than disagreement. This suggests that psychological effects of collaboration rather than more general age-related differences in cardiovascular function may have contributed to older participants’ greater blood pressure response to collaboration. The smaller HR responses of older participants were similarly task-specific, occurring during disagreement but not collaboration. Hence, this difference might reflect a psychological aspect of the task rather than a more general age-related difference in cardiac function. However, it is important to note again that we identified no such psychological mediators of these effects among the affective, appraisal, and overt behavioral responses we measured.
Sex Differences in Responses during Disagreement and Collaboration
Results reported elsewhere (Smith et al., 2009) provide evidence consistent with the hypothesis that women find relationship stressors more upsetting than do men; during disagreement they reported a larger increase in anger and rated their spouses as less friendly. However, despite these sex differences in subjective experiences, there was no evidence that women responded to disagreement with greater CVR. Both men and women displayed larger increases in SBP, DBP, cardiac output, and cardiac sympathetic activity (i.e., pre-ejection period shortening) in response to disagreement than to another form of marital interaction–collaboration, and the magnitude of these responses did not differ for men and women, even though the present study was quite well-powered to detect such sex differences. The only task-specific sex difference in CVR was a Sex x Task interaction for SBP reactivity, in which men and women did not differ in their response to disagreement but men displayed a larger response than women during collaboration.
To the extent that CVR during marital conflict contributes to effects of marital strain on CVD, these results suggest that this mechanism is important for both men and women. Consistent with prior studies in which men displayed greater CVR than women during marital interaction tasks that involve challenges other than conflict or disagreement (Brown & Smith, 1992; Smith et al., 1998), the present results also suggest that some types of common marital interactions –such as collaborative problem solving – may be more taxing for men than for women. Interestingly, women did display a larger decrease in parasympathetic activity (i.e., heart rate variability) across the two tasks, and stress-related decreases in heart rate variability confer increased risk of CVD in older women (Gianaros et al., 2005). Hence, parasympathetic mechanisms could contribute to greater vulnerability to marital stress or less benefit from marriage overall for women.
Limitations
The cross-sectional design precludes the attribution of age group differences to aging, per se, rather than cohort differences. For example, the cohort of older men studied here may be less familiar or comfortable with collaboration than the middle-aged group, resulting in greater blood pressure reactivity during collaboration. Similarly, middle-aged and older couples differ strongly in length of marriage (point-biserial r(300) = .73, p<.001). When non-manipulated variables are highly correlated, separation through analysis of covariance or similar techniques can produce highly misleading results and is not recommended (Miller & Chapman, 2001). Hence, we cannot attribute the observed age group effects on CVR specifically to age, rather than length of marriage.2
The two interaction tasks produced expected affective, cognitive, and behavioral differences, but it is possible that they differ from naturally occurring marital conflict or collaborative problem solving. Ambulatory assessment of cardiovascular responses to naturally occurring marital interactions would be useful in this regard. The levels of CVR to the marital interaction tasks were not as large as those often seen with other laboratory stressors, raising questions as to the relevance for disease risk of changes of this magnitude. However, it may also be possible that cardiovascular responses to potentially stressful laboratory marital interactions underestimate the magnitude of these responses in natural environments.
Although several studies support the hypothesis that CVR is associated with future risk of CVD, the status of this mechanism is best considered tentative and in need of additional research (Treiber et al., 2003). Also, our sample was predominantly White, and middle or upper middle socioeconomic status, and consisted of heterosexual couples. Future research should examine other demographic groups. Speech rate or loudness might have contributed to CVR effects observed here.3 Also, the specific cognitive demands of the collaborative problem solving task may have contributed to the pattern of cardiovascular effects. Future research should include tasks with the different demands in order to identify the physiological consequences of collaboration. Also, although we identified meaningful affective, appraisal, and overt behavioral correlates of CVR, these factors did not mediate the age group differences. Future research should include efforts to not only identify but also explain age differences in CVR in marital interactions.
Finally, our findings suggested that women experienced marital disagreement as more aversive than men (Smith et al, 2009), but they did not display greater responses on traditional aspects of CVR. This null result could reflect aspects of our methodology. Sex differences in physiological response to marital conflict may be more likely when couples are instructed to resolve the issue, rather than simply to discuss it. Further, sex differences may be more likely when individuals are freer to identify conflict topics on their own, initiate interactions regarding such difficulties, and pursue them in an unrestrained manner (Denton et al, 2001; Newton & Sanford, 2003). These aspects of task procedures would maximize sex differences in the tendency to identify, initiate, and pursue such issues (Christensen & Heavey, 1990). That is, the greater physiological reactivity of women often observed during marital conflict might reflect greater levels of speech or engagement in the discussion, rather than a greater tendency to respond physiologically to marital strain (c.f., Nealey-Moore et al., 2007). In the more structured interaction procedures used here, couples discussed a disagreement that they both felt was an on-going problem. This could have attenuated sex differences in task engagement that otherwise contribute to sex differences in CVR during marital disagreement.4
Equating husbands’ and wives’ levels of task involvement when testing sex differences in physiological response to marital stress could be seen as, “placing procrustean experimental restrictions on otherwise naturally occurring systems” and thereby creating, “the danger that such experimentally controlled conditions will not generalize well outside the laboratory” (Coan & Gottman, 2007, p.78).5 Yet, relaxation of experimental controls creates interpretive ambiguities, as described above. Hence, in designing and interpreting psychophysiological studies of marital interaction, researchers face a complex trade-off between control or precision on the one hand and realism, generalizability or ecological validity on the other (Nealey-Moore et al., 2007).
Summary, Conclusions and Future Directions
Compared to another common type of marital interaction (i.e., collaboration), marital disagreement evoked a pattern of heightened CVR characterized by larger increases in blood pressure, cardiac output, and cardiac sympathetic activation. These findings support models suggesting that CVR contributes to associations between marital strain and heightened CVD risk (Rhobles & Kiecolt-Glaser, 2003; Smith & Glazer, 2006), and this mechanism is relevant for both men and women. Although we found no evidence that women are more reactive to marital stressors than are men on traditional measures of CVR (i.e. SBP, DBP, HR), across both tasks women displayed greater reductions in heart rate variability. This stress response has been found to be associated with greater CVD risk among older women (Gianaros et al., 2005). Hence, in future efforts to understand sex differences in marriage and health, parasympathetic responses to marital interactions seem worthy of additional research.
Marital conflict evoked greater blood pressure reactivity and increased cardiac sympathetic activation than did collaboration - generally for both middle-aged and older couples. Therefore, this relationship stressor could influence disease risk though this mechanism for both middle-aged and older individuals. Yet, older couples were less reactive to marital conflict than middle-aged couples, at least in terms of HR responses. Further, episodes of marital conflict may be less frequent for older couples (Gagnon et al, 1997). Hence, the psychophysiology of marital conflict could play a reduced role in associations between marital strain and CVD among older couples. However, a second marital context quite common among older couples – collaborative problem-solving - evoked greater CVR and recovery period blood pressure among older couples than for middle-aged couples, particularly for men. Hence, marital interactions other than conflict may play a role in cardiovascular risk for older adults. The age, sex, and task differences in CVR demonstrated here underscore the importance of the aging and life-span development perspective in explicating associations of social relationships with physical health, as the specific aspects of close relationships that influence physical health may wax and wane in importance across the decades of adulthood.
Acknowledgments
The research reported here was supported by NIH grant AG018903
Footnotes
The Period main effect (i.e., initial unstructured discussion, structured discussion, second unstructured discussion) was significant for most cardiovascular measures, indicating greater reactivity while participants were known to be talking (i.e., structured period) than when they could have been talking (i.e., unstructured discussions). However, in no cases did the Period factor interact with Task, Age, or Sex (or combinations of these factors) in such as way that one of those primary effects was found during some but not all of the levels of the Period factor. Hence, results for the period factor are not presented further.
When length of marriage (LOM) as a continuous variable replaces age group in the analysis, there are significant main effects of LOM on SBP (p<.05) and HR (p<.03) reactivity, but they are weaker than the effects obtained with the age group factor. The Task x Sex x LOM interaction on SBP reactivity is significant and similar in magnitude to that obtained with the Age Group factor, as is the Task x LOM interaction on HR reactivity. However, the Task x LOM interaction on DBP is not significant. Effects of LOM during recovery were not significant. Hence, replacing age with LOM reproduces some but not all effects of age group.
The total number of speech acts during the 6-minutes of both tasks was used as a general measure of the amount of speech. Middle-aged participants (mean = 65.7, SE = 1.18) produced more speech acts than did older couples (mean = 59.6, SE = 1.16), F(1,280) = 13.6, p<.001, eta-squared = .046; women (65.4, 1.07) produced more than men (60.0, 0.98), F(1, 280) = 20.53, p<.001, eta-squared = .068; and participants produced more coded speech acts during collaboration (65.9, 1.03) than during disagreement (59.4, 0.89), F(1,280) = 45.06, p<.001, eta-squared = .139. Hence, speech production as measured by number of coded speech acts did not did not correspond to the pattern of Age, Sex, or Task differences in blood pressure reactivity, CO, or PEP response. Although the pattern of Age, Sex and Task differences on HR, RSA, and TPR responses were similar to the results for the number of coded speech acts, this measure of speech activity did not correlate with any of the CVR measures during the initial 6-minute interaction period. Hence, at least by this measure of speech activity, the observed results for CVR are not due to this factor. However, it is possible that other aspects of speech could contribute to the observed effects. It is also important to note that as described previously (see footnote 1) in no cases were effects involving Task, Age and Sex on CVR found within some periods within the task but not others. Hence, similar effects during unstructured interaction periods and periods when speaking was controlled further reduce the likelihood that speech artifacts explain the observed effects.
It is important to note that when analyses of CVR were conducted on only the unstructured portions of the disagreement task, we still found no evidence that women were more reactive than men.
The thoughtful discussion of this issue by Coan and Gottman (2007) concerned effects of conflict task procedures on behavioral processes and their associations with marital outcomes, rather than psychophysiological responses. However, their comments are relevant to this methodological challenge in psychophysiological studies of marriage (c.f., Nealey-Moore et al., 2007).
Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/journals/pag.
References
- Bakan D. The Duality of Human Existence. Chicago: Rand McNally; 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Baker B, Paquette M, Szalai JP, Driver H, Perger T, Helmers K, O’Kelly B, Tobe S. The influence of marital adjustment on 3-year left ventricular mass and ambulatory blood pressure in mild hypertension. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2000;160:3453–3458. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.22.3453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baltes PB, Staudinger UM. Interactive minds: Life-span perspectives on the social foundation of cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Barnett PA, Spence JD, Manuck SB, Jennings JR. Psychological stress and the progression of carotid artery disease. Journal of Hypertension. 1997;15:49–55. doi: 10.1097/00004872-199715010-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berg C, Smith T, Skinner M, Ko K, Story N, Allen N, Beveridge R, Pearce G, Uchino B, Florsheim P. Cognitive and interpersonal factors involved in couples’ perceptions of collaborative problem solving. Paper presented at the Gerontological Society of America Meetings; Dallas, TX. Nov, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Bernhardsen CS. Type I error rates when multiple comparisons follow a significant F test in AVOVA. Biometrics. 1975;31:229–232. [Google Scholar]
- Berntson GG, Cacioppo JT, Quigley KS. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Autonomic origins, physiological mechanisms, and psychophysiological implications. Psychophysiology. 1993;30:183–196. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb01731.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson GG, Cacioppo JT, Quigley KS. The metrics of cardiac chronotropism: Biometriic perspectives. Psychophysiology. 1995;31:44–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb03308.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson CG, Quigley KS, Jang J, Boysen ST. An approach to artifact identification: Application to heart period data. Psychophysiology. 1990;27:586–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb01982.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard-Fields F. Everyday problem-solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;16:26–31. [Google Scholar]
- Blascovich J, Tomaka J. The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1996;28:1–51. [Google Scholar]
- Broadwell SD, Light KC. Family support and cardiovascular responses in married couples during conflict and other interactions. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1999;6:40–63. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0601_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown PC, Smith TW. Social influence, marriage, and the heart: Cardiovascular consequences of interpersonal control in husbands and wives. Health Psychology. 1992;11:88–96. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.11.2.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brownley KA, Hurwitz BE, Schneiderman N. Cardiovascular psychophysiology. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary L, Berntson GG, editors. Handbook of psychophysiology. 2. Cambridge Press; 2000. pp. 224–264. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Uchino BN, Bernston GG. Individual differences in the autonomic origins of heart rate reactivity: The psychometrics of respiratory sinus arrhythmia and pre-ejection period. Psychophysiology. 1994;31:412–419. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02449.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Gottman JM, Levenson RW. Emotional behavior in long-term marriage. Psychology and Aging. 1995;10:140–149. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.10.1.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Isaacowitz DM, Charles CT. Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist. 1999;54:165–181. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.3.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen A, Heavey C. Gender and social structure in the demand / withdraw pattern of marital conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59:73–81. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.59.1.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coan J, Gottman J. Sampling, experimental control, and generalizability in the study of marital process models. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2007;69:73–80. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper DC, Katzel LI, Waldstein SR. Cardiovascular reactivity in older adults. In: Aldwin CA, Park CL, Spiro A, editors. Handbook of health psychology and aging. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 142–164. [Google Scholar]
- Cross SE, Madson L. Models of the self: self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;122:5–37. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis MC, Matthews KA, Twamley EW. Is life more difficult on Mars or Venus? A meta-analytic review of sex differences in major and minor life events. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1999;21:83–97. doi: 10.1007/BF02895038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denton WH, Burleson BR, Hobbs BV, Von Stein M, Rodriguez CP. Cardiovascular reactivity and initiate/avoid patterns of marital communication: A test of Gottman’s psychophysiologic model of marital interaction. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2001;24:401–421. doi: 10.1023/a:1012278209577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vogli R, Chandola T, Marmot MG. Negative aspects of close relationships and heart disease. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2007;167:1951–1957. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.18.1951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dixon RA, Gould ON. Younger and older adults collaborating on retelling everyday stories. Applied Developmental Science. 1998;2:160–171. [Google Scholar]
- Ehrensaft MK, Langhinrichsen-Rohling J, Heyman RE, O’Leary KD, Lawrence E. Feeling controlled in marriage: A phenomenon specific to physically aggressive couples? Journal of Family Psychology. 1999;13:20–32. [Google Scholar]
- Everson SA, Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Lakka TA, Sivenius J, Salonen JT. Stress-induced blood pressure reactivity and incident stroke in middle-aged men. Stroke. 2001;32:1263–1270. doi: 10.1161/01.str.32.6.1263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fincham FD. Attribution processes in distressed and nondistressed couples: Responsibility for marital problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1985;94:183–190. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.94.2.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagnon MD, Hersen M, Kabacoff RI, Van Hasselt VB. Interpersonal and psychological correlates of marital dissatisfaction in late life: a review. Clinical Psychology Review. 1999;19:359–378. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(97)00048-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallo LC, Troxel WM, Kuller LH, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Edmundowicz D, Matthews KA. Marital status, marital quality, and atherosclerotic burden in postmenopausal women. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65:952–962. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097350.95305.fe. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gianaros PJ, Van Der Veen FM, Jennings JR. Regional cerebral blood flow correlates with heart period and high-frequency heart period variability during working-memory tasks: Implications for the cortical and subcortical regulation of cardiac autonomic activity. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:521–30. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00179.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gianaros PJ, Salomon K, Zhou F, Owens J, Edmundowicz D, Kuller L, Matthews K. A greater reduction in high-frequency heart rate variability to a psychological stressor is associated with subclinical coronary and aortic calcification in postmenopausal women. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67:553–560. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000170335.92770.7a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray-Little B, Burks N. Power and satisfaction in marriage: A review and critique. Psychological Bulletin. 1983;93:513–538. [Google Scholar]
- Helgeson VS. Gender-related traits and health. In: Suls J, Wallston K, editors. Social Psychological Foundations of Health and Illness. UK: Blackwell Publishers; 2003. pp. 367–394. [Google Scholar]
- Janiciki DL, Kamarck TW, Shiffman S, Stton-Tyrrell K, Gwaltney CJ. Frequency of spousal interaction and 3-year progression of carotid artery intima medial thickness: the Pittsburgh Healthy Heart Project. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67:889–896. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000188476.87869.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jennings JR, Kamarck T, Stewart C, Eddy M, Johnson P. Alternate cardiovascular baseline assessment techniques: Vanilla or resting baseline. Psychophysiology. 1992;24:474–475. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb02052.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jennings JR, Kamarck T, Manuck S, Everson S, Kaplan G, Salonen J. Aging or disease? Cardiovascular reactivity in Finnish men over the middle years. Psychology and Aging. 1997;12:225–38. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jennings JR, Kamarck TW, Everson-Rose SA, Kaplan GA, Manuck SB, Salonen JT. Exaggerated blood pressure responses during mental stress are prospectively related to enhanced carotid atherosclerosis in middle-aged Finnish men. Circulation. 2004;110:2198–2203. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000143840.77061.E9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson O, Andersson J, Ronnberg J. Do elderly couples have a better prospective memory than other elderly people when they collaborate? Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2000;14:121–133. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson N, Backlund E, Sorlie P, Loveless C. Marital status and mortality: The National Longitudinal Mortality Study. Annals of Epidemiology. 2000;10:224–238. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(99)00052-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan RM, Kronick RG. Marital status and longevity in the United States population. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2006;60:760–765. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.037606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenny DA. Design and analysis issues in dyadic research. Review of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;11:164–184. [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Newton TL. Marriage and health: his and hers. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:472–503. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krantz DS, Helmers KF, Bairey CN, Nebel LE, Hedges SM, Rosanski A. Cardiovascular reactivity and mental stress-induced myocardial ischemia in patients with coronary artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1991;53:1–12. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199101000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levenson RW, Carstensen LL, Gottman JM. Long-term marriage: Age, gender, and satisfaction. Psychology and Aging. 1993;8:301–313. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.8.2.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levenson RW, Carstensen LL, Gottman JM. The influence of age and gender on affect, physiology, and their interrelations: A study of long-term marriages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67:56–68. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.67.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litvack DA, Oberlander TF, Carney LH, Saul JP. Time and frequency domain methods for heart rate variability analysis: A methodological comparison. Psychophysiology. 1995;32:492–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb02101.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llabre M, Spitzer S, Saab P, Ironson G, Schneiderman N. The reliability and specificity of delta versus residualized change as measures of cardiovascular reactivity to behavioral challenges. Psychophysiology. 1991;28:701–711. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01017.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locke JJ, Wallace KM. Short marital adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living. 1959;21:251–255. [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Huffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test the significance of the mediated effect. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martire LM, Stephens MAP, Druley JA, Wojno WC. Negative reactions to received spousal care: Predictors and consequences of miscarried support. Health Psychology. 2002;21:167–176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews KA, Gump BB. Chronic work stress and marital dissolution increase risk of post-trial mortality in men from the Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2002;162:309–315. doi: 10.1001/archinte.162.3.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews KA, Katholi CR, McCreath H, Whooley MA, Williams DR. Blood pressure reactivity to psychological stress predicts hypertension in the CARDIA study. Circulation. 2004;110:74–78. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000133415.37578.E4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews KA, Salomon K, Brady SS, Allen MT. Cardiovascular reactivity to stress predicts future blood pressure in adolescence. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65:410–415. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000057612.94797.5f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews KA, Zhu S, Tucker DC, Whooley MA. Blood pressure reactivity to psychological stress and coronary calcification in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study. Hypertension. 2006;47:391–395. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000200713.44895.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GM, Chapman JP. Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:40–48. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nealey-Moore JB, Smith TW, Uchino BN, Hawkins MW, Olson-Cerny C. Cardiovascular reactivity during positive and negative marital interactions. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2007;30:505–519. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9124-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuvo Y, Cheng-Yu D, Mitra S. Interpolated finite impulse response filters. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing. 1984;32:563–570. [Google Scholar]
- Newton TL, Sanford JM. Conflict structure moderates associations between cardiovascular reactivity and negative marital interaction. Health Psychology. 2003;22:270–278. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.3.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orth-Gomer K, Wamala SP, Horsten M, Schenck-Gustafsson K, Schneiderman N, Mittleman MA. Marital stress worsens prognosis in women with coronary heart disease: The Stockholm female coronary risk study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2000;284:3008–3014. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.23.3008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robles TF, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. The physiology of marriage: pathways to health. Physiology and Behavior. 2003;79:409–416. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00160-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohrbaugh MJ, Shoham V, Coyne JC. Effect of marital quality on eight-year survival of heart patients with heart failure. American Journal of Cardiology. 2006;98:1069–1072. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2006.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rook KS, Mavandadi S, Sorkin DH, Zettel LA. Optimizing social relationships as a resource for health and well-being in later life. In: Aldwin CA, Park CL, Spiro A, editors. Handbook of health psychology and aging. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 267–285. [Google Scholar]
- Sapolsky RM. Why zebras don’t get ulcers. New York: Henry Holt; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Sherwood A, Allen MT, Fahrenberg J, Kelsey RM, Lovallo WR, van Doornen LJP. Methodological guidelines for impedance cardiography. Psychophysiology. 1990;27:1–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb02171.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherwood A, Allen MT, Obrist PA, Langer AW. Evaluation of beta-adrenergic influences on cardiovascular and metabolic adjustments to physical and psychological stressors. Psychophysiology. 1986;23:89–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00602.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, Berg CA, Florsheim P, Uchino BN, Pearce G, Hawkins M, Henry NJM, Beveridge R, Skinner M, Olson-Cerny C. Conflict and collaboration in middle-aged and older couples: I. Effects on self-reported affect, appraisals of spouses, and observed behavior. 2009 Manuscript under review. [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, Gallo LC, Goble L, Ngu LQ, Stark KA. Agency, communion, and cardiovascular reactivity during marital interaction. Health Psychology. 1998;17:537–545. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.17.6.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, Glazer K. Handbook of families and health: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2006. Hostility, marriage, and the heart: The social psychophysiology of cardiovascular risk in close relationships; pp. 19–39. [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, Limon JP, Gallo LC, Ngu LQ. Interpersonal control and cardiovascular reactivity: Goals, behavioral expression, and the moderating effects of sex. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;70:1012–1024. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.5.1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, Ruiz J, Uchino BN. Vigilance, active coping, and cardiovascular reactivity during social interaction in young men. Health Psychology. 2000;19:382–92. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.19.4.382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steptoe A, Marmot M. Impaired cardiovascular recovery following stress predicts 3-year increases in blood pressure. Journal of Hypertension. 2005;23:529–546. doi: 10.1097/01.hjh.0000160208.66405.a8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart JC, Janicki DL, Kamarck TT. Cardiovascular reactivityto and recovery from psychosocial challenge as predictors of 3-year change in blood pressure. Health Psychology. 2006;25:111–118. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strough J, Patrick JH, Swenson KM, Cheng S, Barnes KA. Collaborative everyday problem solving: Interpersonal relationships and problem dimensions. International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2003;56:43–66. doi: 10.2190/Y3XN-RW1A-7EWT-KXTC. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treiber FA, Kamarck T, Schneiferman N, Sheffield D, Kapuku G, Taylor T. Cardiovascular reactivity and development of preclinical and clinical disease states. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65:46–62. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200301000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker JS, Friedman HS, Wingard DL, Schwartz JE. Marital history at midlife as a predictor of longevity: Alternative explanations to the protective effect of marriage. Health Psychology. 1996;15:94–101. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.15.2.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchino BN. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2006;29:377–387. doi: 10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchino BN, Berg CA, Smith TW, Pearce G, Skinner M. Age-related differences in ambulatory blood pressure during daily stress: Evidence for greater blood pressure reactivity with age. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21:231–239. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchino BN, Holt–Lunstad J, Bloor L, Campo R. Aging and cardiovascular reactivity to stress: longitudinal evidence for changes in stress reactivity. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20:134–143. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchino BN, Smith TW, Holt-Lunstad J, Campo R, Reblin M. Stress and illness. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Bertson GG, editors. Handbook of Psychophysiology. 3. Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 608–632. [Google Scholar]
- Waldstein SR, Siegel EL, Lefkowitz D, Maier KJ, Brown JR, Obuchowski AM, et al. Stress–induced blood pressure reactivity and silent cerebrovascular disease. Stroke. 2004;35:1294–1298. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000127774.43890.5b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiggins J. An informal history of the interpersonal circumplex. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1996;66:361–382. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6602_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Z, Hayward MD. Gender, the marital life course, and cardiovascular disease in late midlife. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2006;68:639–657. [Google Scholar]