Skip to main content
Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology logoLink to Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology
. 2015 Sep;7(9):a019075. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a019075

The Influence of Genome and Cell Size on Brain Morphology in Amphibians

Gerhard Roth 1, Wolfgang Walkowiak 2
PMCID: PMC4563707  PMID: 26261281

Abstract

In amphibians, nerve cell size is highly correlated with genome size, and increases in genome and cell size cause a retardation of the rate of development of nervous (as well as nonnervous) tissue leading to secondary simplification. This yields an inverse relationship between genome and cell size on the one hand and morphological complexity of the tectum mesencephali as the main visual center, the size of the torus semicircularis as the main auditory center, the size of the amphibian papilla as an important peripheral auditory structure, and the size of the cerebellum as a major sensorimotor center. Nervous structures developing later (e.g., torus and cerebellum) are more affected by secondary simplification than those that develop earlier (e.g., the tectum). This effect is more prominent in salamanders and caecilians than in frogs owing to larger genome and cells sizes in the former two taxa. We hypothesize that because of intragenomic evolutionary processes, important differences in brain morphology can arise independently of specific environmental selection.


Amphibians have large genomes and correspondingly large cells. This may cause nervous (and nonnervous) tissue to develop slowly and limit morphological complexity in the adult brain.


All vertebrate brains possess fundamental organizational features. Nonetheless, they display great taxonomic variation. Brains not only differ in absolute and relative size, but also in morphological complexity across, as well as within, classes, orders, and even families. Morphological complexity may be characterized by the number of anatomically distinct nuclei, the degree of lamination of specific parts of the brain (e.g., dorsal pallium, mesencephalic tectum, thalamic nuclei), the number of different cells types, and the degree and pattern of neuronal connectivity (Nieuwenhuys et al. 1998).

Traditionally, brain differences are considered to have resulted from environmental selection so that brain complexity may have been favored by the demands imposed by environmental complexity. Alternatively, some of the observed morphological differences may have arisen independently of specific environmental selection. Increases in brain complexity may have allowed certain species to enter new ecological niches or to develop new lifestyles and thereby escape competition (cf. Roth 2013). One rationale for this hypothesis is the fact that substantial changes in brain morphology usually occur too slowly to be the result of rapid adaptation to a new environment. Thus, causes for such changes would be nonselective, at least with respect to the new environment.

Here, we argue that the morphological complexity of brains in amphibians (and in lungfishes) is substantially influenced by genome size and cell size. The changes resulting from these factors, rather than directing environmental selection, “canalize” adaptive processes (Wake and Roth 1989). This process leads to secondary simplification of morphology in the context of “paedomorphosis,” a form of heterochronic development, in which traits that characterize juvenile or “larval” stages of ancestral taxa appear in the adult stage of descendant taxa (Wake 1966; Gould 1977; Alberch et al. 1979).

Modern amphibians are represented by the subclass Lissamphibia (i.e., smooth-skinned amphibians), which, according to present taxonomy, comprises three orders: frogs (Anura, with 55 families and 6439 described species), salamanders and newts (Caudata, with 10 families and 668 described species), and caecilians (Gymnophiona, with 10 families and 200 described species) (amphibiaweb.org). Anurans are found worldwide with the exception of Arctic and Antarctic regions, whereas salamanders are found only in the Northern hemisphere of North America and Eurasia, as well as in Central America and northern parts of South America. Caecilians are found only in the tropics and subtropics of Eurasia, Africa, and America. Molecular evidence shows that Caudata and Anura are sister taxa, and Gymnophiona is sister to them (Shen et al. 2013).

Vertebrates differ dramatically in genome size (i.e., the amount of DNA). The smallest genome is found in teleost fishes, with <1 pg DNA per haploid genome (Olmo 1983; Morescalchi 1990; Gregory 2005a,b). Amniote genome sizes are also relatively small and range from 0.9 to 5.5 pg DNA (Olmo 1983). The largest genome sizes among vertebrates and animals in general are found in lungfishes (Dipnoi), with 111 pg in Lepidosiren, 142 pg in Protopterus, and 80 pg in Neoceratodus (Northcutt 1987). Among anurans, genome size ranges between 1.01 pg (Scaphiopus couchi; Animal Genome Size Database, www.genomesize.com; last accessed August 2014) and 19 pg (Arenophryne rotunda) (Mahony 1986). Salamanders have genome sizes between 13.9 pg (Desmognathus wrighti) and 83 pg (Necturus maculosus), and caecilians have genome sizes ranging from 8 to 26 pg. The plethodontid species Hydromantes italicus (72 pg) appears to have the largest genome of any terrestrial animal, although several tropical plethodontids approach this value (cf. Table 1).

Table 1.

Recalculated estimates of genome and nerve cell for salamanders and frogsa

(Sub)family/tribe Species n References Genome size (c value) Cell size (µm3) Brain size (mm3) Morphological complexity
Salamanders
Plethodontidae
Desmognathinae Desmognathus wrighti 2 Gregory 2005b 13.89 7.7 1.2 1
Dicrurus aeneus 7.5 1.8 1.5
Desmognathus ochrophaeus 4 Gregory 2005b 15.59 7.2 2.2 2.3
Desmognathus monticola 4 Gregory 2005b 16.47 9.2 10.1 4
Desmognathus quadramaculatus 3 Gregory 2005b 17.19 9.5 12.9 5
Plethodontinae
Hemidactyliini Eurycea bislineata 3 Gregory 2005b 26.92 8.8 3 1.5
Plethodonthini Plethodon cinereus 9 Gregory 2005b 23.03 8.9 3.2 3
Plethodon jordani 3 Gregory 2005b 28.97 11.8 6.5 4
Bolitoglossini Hydromantes italicus 2 Gregory 2005b 71.65 15.1 12.5 0
Hydromantes genei 1 Gregory 2005b 42.63 15.1 13.3 0.5
Batrachoseps attenuatus 4 Gregory 2005b 40.13 11.3 2.3 0
Parvimolge townsendi 1 Gregory 2005b 20.8 8.1 1.4 1
Thorius narisovalis 1 Gregory 2005b 25.2 10.5 1.3 0.5
Thorius pennatulus 9 0.5 0.5
Bolitoglossa subpalmata 2 Gregory 2005b 65.21 13.4 7.8 0
Bolitoglossa dofleini 1 Gregory 2005b 59.4 13.2 17.2 1
Ambystomatidae Ambystoma opacum 6 Gregory 2005b 30.69 12.2 8.1 3
Ambystoma mexicanum 5 Gregory 2005b 34.75 11 28.1 4.3
Salamandridae Salamandrina terdigitata 1 Gregory 2005b 20.6 11 1.8 3
Salamandra salamandra 10 Gregory 2005b 35.24 10.1 19.3 3
Pleurodeles waltl 7 Gregory 2005b 20.44 9.7 10.9 3
Triturus alpestris 9 Gregory 2005b 26.9 11
Anura
Leiopelmatidae Leiopelma hochstetteri 1 Gregory 2005b 8.63
Ascaphidae Ascaphus truei 4 Gregory 2005b 4.13
Bombinatoridae Bombina bombina 6 Gregory 2005b 11.38 9.4
Bombina orientalis 3 Gregory 2005b 8.18 9.9 10.9 2
Discoglossidae Discoglossus pictus 4 Fritz et al. 1994; Gregory 2005b 5.29 8 15.6 3.7
Alytes obstetricans 3 Gregory 2005b 9.19 7.6
Pipidae Xenopus laevis 9 Gregory 2005b 3.3 6.2 30.9 4
Ranidae Rana temporaria 10 Gregory 2005b 4.26 7.9 18.5 3
Hyperoliidae Hyperolius quinquevittatus 6.4 8.8 5
Afrixalus fornasinii 1 Gregory 2005b 5.57 6.9 4.5
Hylidae Hyla chrysoscelis 1 Gregory 2005b 4.67
Hyla versicolor 2 Gregory 2005b 4.88
Osteopilus septentrionalis 1 Gregory 2005b 1.95 6.5 20.9 4
Gastrotheca riobambae 1 Schmid et al. 1990 4.25 7.3 19.2 3.5
Bufonidae Bufo bufo 11 Gregory 2005b 6.69 6.4 4
Myobatrachidae Limnodynastes tasmaniensis 1 Gregory 2005b 2.4 6.6 9.8 3.5
Arenophryne rotunda 1 Morescalchi 1990 19 10 5.5 1.5
Dendrobatidae Dendrobates (Oophaga) pumilio ? BGI-G10k top 101 species (genome10k.soe.ucsc.edu) 9 7 5 3
Leptodactylidae Eleutherodactylus coqui ? BGI-G10k top 101 species (genome10k.soe.ucsc.edu) 3.4 5.8 14 4
Eleutherodactylus limbatus 5.1 1.4 3.5
Racophoridae Rhacophorus leucomystax 7.2 18.6 3.8

a22 species of salamanders and 19 species of frogs. (Data from Roth et al. 1994.)

In amphibians, as well as in some other groups of vertebrates, genome size determines cell size (i.e., volume) by the fact that large amounts of DNA appear to require large nuclei and, consequently, large cell volumes (Ohno 1970; Szarski 1970, 1983; Olmo and Morescalchi 1975, 1978; Vinogradov 2004; Gregory 2005a). In salamanders, average cell size varies from 7.2 µm in diameter in Desmognathus ochrophaeus to 15.1 µm in Hydromantes genei. In frogs, the smallest cell size of 5.1 µm is found in the miniaturized Eleutherodactylus (formerly Sminthillus) limbatus, and the largest of 10 µm in A. rotunda. Genome and cell size are assumed to significantly affect, in an inverse manner, (1) metabolic rates, (2) number of cells, (3) speed of growth, and (4) differentiation processes including cell cycle time (Horner and MacGregor 1983; Sessions and Larson 1987; Jokusch 1997; Chipman et al. 2001). Consequently, amphibians with large genome and cell sizes are expected to show low metabolic rates, slow growth and differentiation processes, and a relatively low number of cells, including neurons (Gregory 2005a).

GENOME SIZE, NERVE CELL SIZE, AND BRAIN COMPLEXITY

When compared with the brains of cartilaginous and bony fish and all amniotes, amphibian brains show a relatively simple, “embryonic” or “larval-like” morphology. Within amphibians, frogs generally display a more complex brain morphology than salamanders (Dicke and Roth 2007). In frogs, we find morphologically distinct nuclei that often lie in migrated positions in the diencephalon, the pretectum, torus semicircularis, and mesencephalic tegmentum. In addition, multiple lamination (i.e., an alternation of cellular and fibrous layers) is found in the tectum mesencephali and in a number of diencephalic nuclei, whereas in salamanders, cell migration and lamination is much reduced or even completely absent. Finally, frogs have well-developed and laminated cerebella, whereas in salamanders, the corpus cerebelli is very small, or the auricles form a paired structure without a fusion in the midline (ten Donkelaar 1998b). Caecilians show a situation very similar to that found in salamanders.

Here, we argue that in amphibians the seemingly simple or primitive brain morphology is the result of secondary simplification caused by an increase in genome size, which in turn leads to an increase in cell size and, consequently, retardation of the development of the brain and other traits. Furthermore, we argue that brain traits appearing late in development are more affected by such retardation than those appearing early. In general, secondary simplification is expected to be more prominent in urodeles than in anurans because of the much larger genome and cell sizes in the former.

To support these ideas, we review data that relate genome size, cell size, and the morphological complexity of prominent parts of the amphibian brain, such as the tectum mesencephali (“optic tectum”) and torus semicircularis. These areas are the major brain centers for processing visual and acoustic information, respectively. In addition, we provide estimates for the size of the cerebellum, which is important for sensorimotor coordination and the number of hair cells in a special region of the amphibian papilla, an inner ear organ particular to anurans.

Genome Size and Nerve Cell Size

In Table 1, we show recalculated estimates of genome and nerve cell size from Roth et al. (1994) for 22 species of salamanders and 19 species of frogs. In Figure 4, we plot data from Table 1. In Figure 4A, a significant positive correlation is evident between genome size and nerve cell size in both frogs and salamanders. Thus, species from both groups with larger genome sizes generally have larger nerve cells.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Correlation between genome sizes, cell sizes, and morphological complexity of the mesencephalic tectum. Filled diamond, anurans; open diamond, urodeles. Correlation coefficient (r) and probability of α error (p) are indicated. (A) Correlation with cell size in anurans (r = 0.753, p < 0.01) and urodeles (r = 0.835, p < 0.01). (B) Correlation with morphological complexity in anurans (r = −0.772, p < 0.01) and urodeles (r = −0.552, p < 0.05.)

Tectum Mesencephali

Complexity of the tectum mesencephali was judged on the basis of the degree of lamination (i.e., the number of alternating cell and fiber layers) in the midbrain tectum, the main visual center of these animals (see examples in Fig. 1 and Dicke and Roth 2007). For correlational analyses among nerve cell size, brain size and morphological complexity of the tectum, we defined five classes in frogs and six classes in salamanders. The classes range from class 1, which represents the most undifferentiated state of the tectum, to class 5 or 6, which represent the most differentiated state (for details see Roth et al. 1994).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Morphology of the tectum mesencephali in the toad Bufo bufo (A,B), in the salamander Desmognathus quadramaculatus (C,D), and the salamander Hydromante italicus (E,F). B. bufo is characterized by a relatively small genome and small nerve cells and has a highly laminated tectum. Desmognathus quadramaculatus has a relatively small genome and relatively small cells compared with other salamanders and, accordingly, has one of the most structured salamander tecta. H. italicus has the largest genome among terrestrial vertebrates and very large cells, and its tectal morphology appears to be very simple because of the absence of any lamination. Scale bar, 100 µm. (From Roth et al. 1993; reprinted, with permission, from the authors.)

In frogs, four groups of species were recognized regarding morphological complexity of the tectum. The first group contains frogs with large to very large nerve cells (i.e., A. rotunda) (10 µm average cell diameter) Bombina bombina (9.4 µm), and Bombina orientalis (9.9 µm). Their tecta have an average rank of morphological complexity of 1.5–2.0 according to the criteria mentioned above. Thus, these three species have the least differentiated anuran tecta. Brain size of these species ranges from very small to intermediate sizes (i.e., from 5.5 to 10.9 mm3).

The next group contains species that have intermediate nerve cell sizes (i.e., Gastrotheca riobambae, Dendrobates pumilio, and Discoglossus pictus (average cell diameter 7.4 µm). Morphological complexity of the tectum is intermediate and ranks 3.0–3.7. The brain size of these species ranges between 5.0 and 19.2 mm3.

The next group comprises frogs with medium to small nerve cells (i.e., Mantella cowanii, Rhacophorus leucomystax, Rana temporaria, Limnodynastes tasmaniensis, and Osteopilus (formerly Hyla) septentrionalis) (average cell diameter 6.8 µm). Tectal morphology ranks 3.0–4.2 in complexity. Eleutherodactylus limbatus is included in this group based on its complexity score, but it has the smallest cells found among frogs (cell diameter 5.1 µm). E. limbatus is a true miniaturized species (11.7 mm snout-vent length [SVL]), and it has by far the smallest brain among anurans sampled (1.4 µm3). Brain sizes in the other species range between 5.4 and 20.9 mm3.

The last group comprises frogs that have small to very small nerve cells: Hyperolius quinquevittatus, Xenopus laevis, Eleutherodactylus coqui, and Mantella aurantiaca (average cell diameter 6.1 µm). Their tectal morphology ranks 4–5 in complexity, and their brain size ranges from 5.3 to 30.9 mm3.

Thus, in frogs, morphological complexity of the anuran tectum is significantly negatively correlated with cell size (r = −0.777, p < 0.01). Frogs with smaller cells have more complex tecta (Fig. 4B). However, brain size and body size are uncorrelated with genome or nerve cell size with the exception that species with very large genome and nerve cell sizes tend to have small brains, in addition to low tectal complexity.

In salamanders, four groups were recognized. The most undifferentiated tectum (average rank per species 0–0.5) is associated with nerve cells of medium to large size (diameters 9.0–11.3 µm) and found in small to miniaturized species (13–42 mm SVL)—Thorius narisovalis, Thorius pennatulus, and Batrachoseps attenuates. Brain sizes range from 0 to 2.3 mm3. The second group also has relatively undifferentiated tecta (0–1) and even larger cells (diameters 13.2–15.1 µm); species in this group are medium-sized to large (>45 mm SVL)—Bolitoglossa pesrubra (formerly subpalmata), Bolitoglossa dofleini, H. italicus, and H. genei. Brain size ranges from 7.8 to 17.2 mm3. A third group has more differentiated tecta (rank 1–3), but cells are of small to medium cell size (diameters 7.2–11.0 µm); members of this group are small (<42 mm SVL)—Desmognathus wrighti, Dicrurus aeneus, D. ochrophaeus, Parvimolge townsendi, Salamandrina terdigitata, Plethodon cinereus, and Eurycea bislineata. Brain sizes range from 1.2 to 3.2 mm3. The fourth group has the best developed tecta (rank 3–5) and cells that are of intermediate size (diameters 9.2-12.2 μm); species of this group are large to very large (>64 mm SVL)—Desmognathus monticola, Desmognathus quadramaculatus, Plethodon jordani, Salamandra salamandra, Pleurodeles waltl, Ambystoma opacum, and Ambystoma mexicanum. Brain size ranges from 6.5 to 21.1 mm3.

Thus, in salamanders too, the degree of morphological complexity of the tectum is significantly negatively correlated with genome size in the sense that salamanders with smaller cells have more complex tecta (Fig. 4B).

Torus Semicircularis

In seven anuran species, the relative dimensions of the torus as well as the cerebellum were determined by calculating the ratio of the brain length (measured from the olfactory bulb to the sulcus between tecta and cerebellum) to the maximum rostrocaudal extension of the torus and the maximum dorsoventral extension of the cerebellum, respectively (Fig. 2; cf. Table 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Sagittal sections through the dorsal midbrain/cerebellar area of four different species. (A) Bombina bombina (11.38 pg DNA/n), (B) Discoglossus pictus (5.29 pg DNA/n), (C) Hyla chrysoscelis (4.67 pg DNA/n), and (D) Rana temporaria (4.26 pg DNA/n). Calibration bar fits to all subfigures. TO, tectum opticum; TS, torus semicircularis; Cer, cerebellum.

Table 2.

Relative dimensions of torus and cerebellum

Family Species n Genome size Brain length (µm) Torus (µm) Cerebellum (µm) Brain/torus Brain/cerebellum
Bombinatoridae B. bombina 2 11.38 4676 503 515 9.29 9.08
B. orientalis 2 8.18 4736 469 566 10.10 8.37
Discoglossidae D. pictus, 1 5.29 4909 581 649 8.45 7.56
Alytes obstetricans 2 9.17 4840 467 534 10.37 9.06
Ranidae R. temporaria 1 4.35 6675 871 1173 7.67 5.69
Hyperoliidae Afrixalus fornasinii 4 5.57 4028 496 700 8.12 5.76
Hylidae H. chrysoscelis 1 4.67 5461 652 841 8.38 6.49

In seven species, the relative dimensions of the torus as well as the cerebellum were determined by calculating the ratio of the brain length (measured from the olfactory bulb to the sulcus between tecta and cerebellum) to the maximum rostrocaudal extension of the torus and the maximum dorsoventral extension of the cerebellum.

In the anuran species studied, the length of the torus is negatively correlated with genome size, which means that species with larger genome sizes have a shorter torus than those with smaller genome sizes. This correlation is significant (Fig. 5A). In anurans, an anatomically distinct, migrated torus semicircularis is present and consists of five subnuclei with extensive cell migration and lamination (Potter 1965; Feng 1983). Three of the nuclei are involved in auditory processing, but receive other sensory input and descending input from diencephalic and telencephalic sources as well (Potter 1965; Endepols and Walkowiak 2000; Wilczynski and Endepols 2007). The principal nucleus with clusters of cells forms the central core of the torus. The laminar nucleus surrounds the principal nucleus rostrally and laterally in an onionskin-like manner. The magnocellular nucleus occupies a caudal or caudolateral portion of the torus.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Correlation between anuran genome size. (A) Ratio of brain length to torus extension (r = 0.776, p < 0.05). (B) Ratio of brain length to cerebellum extension (r = 0.891, p < 0.01). (C) Ratio of papilla amphibiorum region C to B (r = −0.443, p < 0.05).

Neurons of the torus are differentiated by their arrangement as well as by their dimensions, cytoarchitecture, and projection patterns (Feng 1983; Luksch and Walkowiak 1998). The bulk of auditory afferents terminates in the principal nucleus with projections that mainly terminate at the mesencephalic level. In contrast, magnocellular neurons and especially laminar nucleus neurons do not only project to other auditory centers, but also to premotor and motor centers in the brainstem and medulla oblongata, thus forming an audiomotor interface (Walkowiak and Luksch 1994; Luksch and Walkowiak 1998; Wilczynski and Ryan 2010). Single-unit (Hermes et al. 1982), as well as multiunit studies (Mohneke 1983; Schneichel and Schneider 1988) revealed a topic representation of tones in the torus. Low frequencies are represented most caudally; mid- and higher frequencies are represented along a caudorostral axis. The midbrain tonotopic representation appears to be correlated with the dimensions of the amphibian papilla and cytoarchitecture of the torus (W Walkowiak, unpubl.).

In urodeles, the region in the caudal mesencephalon corresponding to the torus semicircularis of anurans is morphologically indistinct. It consists of densely packed periventricular neurons without any lamination (Dicke and Roth 2007). This corresponds well with the fact that audition plays no role in the communicative behavior of salamanders.

Cerebellum

Compared with other tetrapods, the cerebellum of amphibians is small to very small. In most anurans, it consists of a plate-like central part, the corpus cerebelli, and two lateral auricular lobes (ten Donkelaar 1998a,b). Histologically, three layers can be distinguished in the amphibian corpus cerebelli. The molecular layer is the most rostral one and consists mainly of fibers and only few neurons. The most caudal layer (i.e., the granular layer), is densely packed with small cells. In between, a layer of large cells, the Purkinje cells, is located.

The cerebellum of salamanders is small and inconspicuous (Dicke and Roth 2007). It consists of lateral auricles and a simply organized medial corpus cerebelli. No cortical structure is detectable. In urodeles with very large genome sizes (e.g., N. maculosus, Amphiuma means) as well as in small-sized species, the corpus cerebelli is still a paired structure constituting the auricles. The least conspicuous cerebellum among amphibians is found in bolitoglossine salamanders, in which it is very small and has partly retreated under the tectum. In larger urodeles with relatively small genome sizes (e.g., A. mexicanum with 34.75 pg) as well as in all anuran species studied, the bilateral halves are fused forming a massive corpus cerebelli.

In anurans, the rostrocaudal and dorsoventral extensions of the corpus cerebelli related to the length of the brain (see above) are significantly negatively correlated with genome size (Fig. 5B) (i.e., in species with large genome size, the corpus cerebelli is smaller, whereas in those with small genome size, it is larger).

Amphibian Papilla

To evaluate the correlation between genome size and the number of hair cells in the amphibian papilla, cell counts were taken from the literature (Lewis et al. 1992). To avoid the confounding effects of body size differences among species, the ratio of the caudal extension of the amphibian papilla (region C in Fig. 3), which shows the greatest species differences to region B, was calculated.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Schematic outline of an amphibian papilla from the left ear of a neobatrachian species. Schematic outline comprising three patches: A, B, and C. A line from the tectorial corner (TC) to the caudomedial end of region A divides region B from C. This line corresponds to the thin diaphragm that spans the papillar chamber. (Modified after Lewis et al. 1992.)

Anuran species possess four sensory epithelia, which are sensitive to distinct frequency ranges of acoustic stimuli. These are the amphibian papilla (sensitive to low to mid-frequencies), basilar papilla (sensitive to high frequencies), the sacculus that covers frequencies below those of the amphibian papilla, and the lagena, which is partly an acoustic and partly an equilibrium sensor.

The amphibian papilla can be separated into three distinct regions, according to the shape and arrangement of hair cells denoted by regions A, B, and C in Figure 3. A line that corresponds to the position of the diaphragm subdivides regions B in the middle and C in the caudal extension. Regions A and B are more or less conserved among anuran species except in the most primitive living frogs Ascaphus truei (Ascaphidae) and Leiopelma hochstetteri (Leiopelmatidae) (Lewis 1984).

In urodeles too, only region A is found in the amphibian papilla (Lewis and Narins 1999). The length of the caudal extension and the number of hair cells, respectively, are extremely variable from species to species. Region C may even be lacking in some archaeobatrachian anurans (e.g., B. orientalis) (Lewis et al. 1992). In regions B and C, sound frequencies are tonotopically organized. It is plausible to assume that auditory range and/or quality of the spectral analysis of an acoustic signal are correlated with the dimension of the caudal extension.

Using the ratio of regions C to B, the number of hair cells of the amphibian papillae of 26 species is significantly negatively correlated with genome size (Fig. 5C; Table 3). Members of the permanent aquatic family Pipidae have not been included. The papilla amphibiorum is also reduced in size in salamanders in general and in plethodontid salamanders in particular (Lombard 1977).

Table 3.

Averaged genome sizes and amphibian papilla hair cell numbers of different anuran species

Family Species Genome size Hair cells (region A Hair cells (region B) Hair cells (region C) C/(A + B)
Bombinatoridae B. bombina 11.38 127 128 33 0.26
B. orientalis 8.18 183 173 0 0.00
Scaphiopodidae Scaphiopus hammondii 102 120 85 0.38
Ranidae Rana sylvatica 5.93 142 139 262 0.93
R. palustris 6.31 210 273 421 0.87
R. pipiens 6.76 174 214 419 1.08
R. sphenocephala 6.8 236 277 476 0.93
R. clamitans 6.85 175 266 390 0.88
R. catesbeiana 7.59 300 377 582 0.86
R. cascadae 8.01 167 170 316 0.94
Hylidae Acris crepitans 4.12 128 108 132 0.56
Hyla versicolor 4.88 128 74 110 0.54
H. cinerea 6.69 133 175 212 0.69
Bufonidae Bufo valliceps 4.60 159 145 182 0.60
B. marinus 4.79 244 173 220 0.53
B. americanus 5.17 154 94 118 0.48
B. cognatus 5.60 98 90 87 0.46
Bufo alvarius 5.65 197 209 169 0.42
B. viridis 5.12 179 160 114 0.34
Leptodactylidae Eleutherodactylus richmondi 3.93 196 158 549 1.55
E. antillensis 4.19 134 150 379 1.33
Microhylidae G. carolinensis 4.65 200 148 208 0.60

Cell counts are taken from Lewis et al. (1992). Averaged genome size data are taken from Gregory (2005b).

DISCUSSION

The data presented here confirm that nerve cell size is highly significantly correlated with genome size in anurans and urodeles. Furthermore, in both groups of amphibians, the morphological complexity of the tectum is highly significantly negatively correlated with genome size. Frogs with very small genomes and cells have the most complex tecta (e.g., E. coqui and Bufo bufo) (Fig. 1A,B), whereas the least complex morphologies of the tectum and other parts of the brain are found in A. rotunda and B. orientalis—two unrelated species that have the largest and second largest genomes and cell sizes among the frogs studied. In general, tecta of frogs are morphologically much more complex than those of all salamanders, which corresponds with the fact that, on average, frogs have much smaller genome and cell sizes than salamanders. In salamanders too, species with relatively small genomes and cells have the most complex brains, whereas species with relatively large cells and very large genomes have relatively simple brain morphologies. Although in anurans there is no correlation between genome–cell size and body–brain size, in salamanders we find a complex interaction between these parameters in the sense that smaller species with smaller brains tend to have larger nerve cells and genomes, and larger species with larger brains tend to have smaller genomes and nerve cells. A confounding factor may be that very probably a secondary reduction of genome size happened in miniaturized salamanders (Roth et al. 1990).

The described difference in complexity holds for the torus semicircularis and the cerebellum as well. In frogs, the torus is larger and consists of morphologically distinct nuclei and lamination, whereas in urodeles it is small, completely unlaminated, and shows no nucleization. In anurans, the length of the torus is negatively correlated with genome and cell size (i.e., frogs with larger genome and nerve cell sizes have relatively smaller tori).

The increase in nerve cell size in the torus and cerebellum almost certainly occurs because development is much slower in animals with large cell and genome sizes (for review, see Gregory 2005a). During brain development, the caudal part of the rhombencephalic roof and the most rostral parts of the rhombencephalic alar plates differentiate rather late; consequently, the primordia of the torus semicircularis and the cerebellum consist of thin undifferentiated cell layers around stage 29 (Fig. 6) (stages according to Gosner 1960). Because morphological differentiation starts rostralaterally and proceeds caudomedially (see cell migration and lamination in Fig. 6), both the torus and cerebellum are more affected by the late development than is the tectum. We show this by comparing the maximal rostrocaudal extension of the torus semicircularis and dorsoventral dimension of the cerebellum, respectively, with that of the brain in adult specimens, as measured from the olfactory bulb to the sulcus between optic tectum and cerebellum (Fig. 5A).

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Sagittal sections through a developing Bombina orientalis brain at stage 29. Distances from midline: (A) 315 µm, (B) 210 µm, and (C) 105 µm. TO, tectum opticum; TS, torus semicircularis; Cer, cerebellum.

Most urodeles with generally large genomes and cells compared with frogs do not communicate via acoustic signals, and anuran species with a relatively small torus and large toral cells either do not “call” or produce only simple (i.e., temporally unstructured) calls (Weber and Schumacher 1975, 1976). Large cells have a lower input resistance and different capacitance compared with smaller neurons. The specific membrane capacitance of a neuron influences synaptic efficacy and determines the speed with which electrical signals propagate along dendrites and the soma; consequently, there is strong impact on neuronal excitability (Kandel et al. 1991; Mendell 2005). Taking this into account, the behavioral preference of females for different pulse repetition rates in di-, tri-, and tetraploid specimen of the Hyla chrysoscelis complex might be a consequence of different genome sizes, which affect cell dimensions (Keller and Gerhardt 2001; Tucker and Gerhardt 2012). Moreover, electrophysiological recordings in the torus of B. bombina (11.38 pg) with large neurons and R. temporaria (4.26 pg) with smaller neurons indicate that neurons in B. bombina do not synchronize to high stimulus pulse repetition rates as do neurons in R. temporaria (Walkowiak 1980).

The inverse relationship between genome and cells size and the complexity of the anuran auditory system also holds for the auditory periphery, because the number of hair cells in the amphibian papilla is lower in species with larger genomes and cells in comparison to species with smaller genomes and cells.

The situation found in the third order of amphibians, the caecilians, corresponds with that found in urodeles (for details, cf. Schmidt and Wake 1997). Further evidence of the correlation of genome and cell size with brain complexity comes from the dipnoans, all of which have large genomes and cells and relatively simple brain differentiation (Northcutt 1987; Nieuwenhuys et al. 1998). In particular, the lepidosirenids, which have the largest genomes among vertebrates, have the least differentiated brains, with tecta whose simplicity resembles that of salamanders with very large genomes and cells.

Evolutionary Considerations

In amphibians, an inverse correlation is found between genome and nerve cell size and the size and morphological complexity of important parts of their brains like the tectum mesencephali, the torus semicircularis, and the cerebellum. This correlation does not directly explain causation (i.e., whether an increase in genome and cell size) by means of developmental retardation, induced the observed morphological and—at least in the auditory system—functional simplification, or whether natural selection favoring secondary simplification led to an increase in genome and cell size.

Several investigators have speculated about an adaptive value of the observed increase in genome and cell size in amphibians and lungfishes (Szarski 1970; Chipman et al. 2001; Chipman 2002; Vinogradov 2004). For example, Szarski (1970, 1983) suggested that these changes can be viewed as an adaptation to life in cold climates (cf. Szarski 1970, 1983). His reasoning is that large cell size is significantly correlated with a reduction in metabolic rates enabling the animals to survive in colder climates. Here, we favor an alternative view that the increase in genome size was nonadaptive in the first place owing to changes in intragenomic control mechanisms, which then induced an increase in cell size, and, consequently, in the other mentioned physiological and developmental parameters, leading, among others, to the observed state of secondary simplification of brain morphology. At least Szarski’s idea of a “frugal” metabolic strategy via increase in cell size is contradicted by the fact that the great majority of salamanders, and particularly those with large genomes and cells, do not live in “harsh,” but in tropical and subtropical climates, as do all caecilians. In lungfish, the enormous increase in genome size is likewise assumed to be secondary, because all other anamniote vertebrates except amphibians (assumed to be closely related to lungfish) have small to very small genome sizes (Roth et al. 1993; Gregory 2005a). Once this increase occurred, it enabled these animals to survive in African, South American, and Australian environments, in which their habitats dried out seasonally.

In addition, there are many other signs of simplification and retardation within the brains of these amphibians with large genome and cell sizes. Among these are the lack of cell migration in the motor columns of the hindbrain and spinal cord (Roth et al. 1988; Wake et al. 1988) as well as a reduction of the number of ganglion cells and of optic nerve fibers (Linke and Roth 1989, 1990) in salamanders with large genomes and cells. In anurans, there is a reduction of the peripheral auditory system in B. orientalis, an anuran with a large genome and cell size (Stadtmüller 1931).

Mueller (2015) discusses the possible mechanisms underlying the apparent coupling between genome size and cells size and possible adaptive and nonadaptive hypotheses of the enormous increase in genome size that has happened in many taxa, including lungfish and salamanders. According to Mueller, germ cells possess a genome-defense system against the increase of transposable elements (TE), which is mediated by the “piRNA” pathway. If this genome defense system limiting TE activity is relaxed, this could have happened either by natural selection, because this leads to an increase in organismal fitness, or by nonadaptive amino acid substitution patterns, in which, via increase in cell size, decrease of metabolic rates, as well as cell division and differentiation rate, constitutes a “burden” for the organism. Mueller proposes the outlines of a procedure for testing the two mentioned hypotheses.

We hypothesize that the increase in genome and cell size in amphibians (although to different degrees in frogs on the one hand and salamanders and caecilians on the other) and in lungfish is a nonselective constraint resulting from intragenomic processes that canalize and slow down the ontogenetic development and, consequently, the evolution of brains, sense organs, and the entire body (Wake and Roth 1989). An extensive phylogenetic comparative study of paedomorphosis in amphibians by Roth et al. (1993) showed that the observed morphological simplicity found in amphibians in general and in salamanders and caecilians in particular is a “derived” and not an ancestral feature, thus indicating that the common ancestors of amphibians had more complex neural and nonneural features than the extant forms.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank David B. Wake (University of California, Berkeley) and H. Carl Gerhardt (University of Missouri) for helpful criticism.

Footnotes

Editors: Rebecca Heald, Iswar K. Hariharan, and David B. Wake

Additional Perspectives on Size Control in Biology: From Organelles to Organisms available at www.cshperspectives.org

REFERENCES

*Reference is also in this collection.

  1. Alberch P, Gould SJ, Oster GF, Wake DB. 1979. Size and shape in ontogeny and phylogeny. Paleobiology 5: 296–317. [Google Scholar]
  2. Chipman AD. 2002. Variation, plasticity and modularity in anuran development. Zoology 105: 97–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chipman AD, Khaner O, Haas A, Tchernov E. 2001. The evolution of genome size: What can be learned from anuran development? J Exp Zool 291: 365–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Dicke U, Roth G. 2007. Evolution of the amphibian nervous system. In Evolution of nervous systems (ed. Kaas JH), Vol. 2, pp. 61–124. Academic, Orlando, FL. [Google Scholar]
  5. Endepols H, Walkowiak W. 2000. Integration of ascending and descending inputs in the auditory midbrain of anurans. J Comp Physiol A 186: 1119–1133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Feng AS. 1983. Morphology of neurons in the torus semicircularis of the northern leopard frog, Rana pipiens pipiens. J Morphol 175: 253–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fritz B, Vences M, Glaw F. 1994. Comparative DNA content in Discoglossus (Amphibia, Anura, Discoglossidae). Zool Anz 233: 135–145. [Google Scholar]
  8. Gosner KL. 1960. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae. Herpetologica 16: 183–190. [Google Scholar]
  9. Gould SJ. 1977. Ontogeny and phylogeny. Belknap, Cambridge, MA. [Google Scholar]
  10. Gregory TR. 2005a. Genome size evolution. In The evolution of the genome (ed. Gregory TR), pp. 3–87. Academic, Orlando, FL. [Google Scholar]
  11. Gregory TR. 2005b. Animal Genome Size Database, www.genomesize.com. [Google Scholar]
  12. Hermes DJ, Eggermont JJ, Aertsen AM, Johannesma PI. 1982. Spectro-temporal characteristics of single units in the auditory midbrain of the lightly anaesthetised grass frog (Rana temporaria L.) investigated with tonal stimuli. Hear Res 6: 103–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Horner HA, MacGregor H. 1983. C-value and cell volume: Their significance in the evolution and development of amphibians. J Cell Sci 63: 135–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Jokusch EL. 1997. An evolutionary correlate of genome size change in plethodontid salamanders. Proc Biol Sci 264: 597–604. [Google Scholar]
  15. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM. 1991. Principles of neural science. Elsevier, New York. [Google Scholar]
  16. Keller MJ, Gerhardt HC. 2001. Polyploidy alters advertisement call structure in gray treefrogs. Proc Biol Sci R Soc 268: 341–345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kimura M. 1983. The neutral theory of molecular evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA. [Google Scholar]
  18. Lewis ER. 1984. On the frog amphibian papilla. Scan Electron Microsc 1984 (Pt 4): 1899–1913. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lewis ER, Narins PM. 1999. The acoustic periphery of amphibians: Anatomy and physiology. In Comparative hearing: Fish and amphibians (ed. Fay RR, Popper AN), pp. 101–154. Springer, New York. [Google Scholar]
  20. Lewis ER, Hecht EI, Narins PM. 1992. Diversity of form in the amphibian papilla of Puerto Rican frogs. J Comp Physiol A 171: 421–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Linke R, Roth G. 1989. Morphology of retinal ganglion cells in lungless salamanders (fam. Plethodontidae): An HRP and Golgi study. J Comp Neurol 289: 361–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Linke R, Roth G. 1990. Optic nerves in plethodontid salamanders (Amphibia, Urodela): Neuroglia, fiber spectrum and myelination. Anat Embryol 181: 37–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lombard RE. 1977. Comparative morphology of the inner ear in salamanders (Caudata: Amphibia). Cont Vert Evol 2: 1–140. [Google Scholar]
  24. Luksch H, Walkowiak W. 1998. Morphology and axonal projection patterns of auditory neurons in the midbrain of the painted frog, Discoglossus pictus. Hear Res 122: 1–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mahony M. 1986. “Cytogenetics of myobatrachid frogs.” PhD thesis, Macquarie University, Ryde, New South Wales, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  26. Mendell LM. 2005. The size principle: A rule describing the recruitment of motoneurons. J Neurophysiol 93: 3024–3026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mohneke R. 1983. Tonotopic organisation of the auditory midbrain nuclei of the midwife toad (Alytes obstetricans). Hear Res 9: 91–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Morescalchi A. 1990. Cytogenetics and the problem of lissamphibian relationships. In Cytogenetics of amphibians and reptiles (ed. Olmo E), pp. 1–19. Birkhaeuser, Basel, Switzerland. [Google Scholar]
  29. *.Mueller RL. 2015. Genome biology and the evolution of cell-size diversity. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 10.1101/cshperspect.a019125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Nieuwenhuys R, ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C. 1998. The central nervous system of vertebrates. Springer, New York. [Google Scholar]
  31. Northcutt RG. 1987. Lungfish neural characters and their bearing on sarcopterygian phylogeny. In The biology and evolution of lungfishes (ed. Bemis WE, et al. ), pp. 277–297. Alan R. Liss, New York. [Google Scholar]
  32. Ohno S. 1970. Evolution by gene duplication. Springer, New York. [Google Scholar]
  33. Olmo E. 1983. Nucleotype and cell size in vertebrates: A review. Bas Appl Histochem 27: 227–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Olmo E, Morescalchi A. 1975. Evolution of the genome and cell sizes in salamanders. Experientia 3: 804–806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Olmo E, Morescalchi A. 1978. Genome and cell size in frogs: A comparison with salamanders. Experientia 34: 44–46. [Google Scholar]
  36. Potter HD. 1965. Mesencephalic auditory region of the bullfrog. J Neurophysiol 28: 1132–1154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Roth G. 2013. The long evolution of brains and minds. Springer, Heidelberg, Germany. [Google Scholar]
  38. Roth G, Wake DB. 1989. Conservatism and innovation in the evolution of feeding in vertebrates. In Complex organismal functions: Integration and evolution in vertebrates (ed. Wake DB, Roth G), pp. 7–22. Wiley, New York. [Google Scholar]
  39. Roth G, Nishikawa KC, Dicke U, Wake DB. 1988. Topography and cytoarchitecture of the motor nuclei in the brainstem of salamanders. J Comp Neurol 278: 181–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Roth G, Rottluff B, Grunwald W, Hanken J, Linke R. 1990. Miniaturization in plethodontid salamanders (Caudata: Plethodontidae) and its consequences for the brain and visual system. Zool J Linn Soc 40: 165–190. [Google Scholar]
  41. Roth G, Nishikawa KC, Naujoks-Manteuffel C, Schmidt A, Wake DB. 1993. Paedomorphosis and simplification in the nervous system of salamanders. Brain Behav Evol 42: 137–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Roth G, Blanke J, Wake DB. 1994. Cell size predicts morphological complexity in the brains of frogs and salamanders. Proc Natl Acad Sci 91: 4796–4800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schmid M, Steinlein C, Friedl R, de Almeida CG, Haaf T, Hillis DM, Duellman WE. 1990. Chromosome-banding in Amphibia. XV: Two types of Y chromosomes and heterochromatin hypervariability in Gastrotheca pseustes (Anura, Hylidae). Chromosoma 99: 413–423. [Google Scholar]
  44. Schmidt A, Wake MH. 1997. Cellular migration and morphological complexity in the caecilian brain. J Morph 231: 11–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schneichel W, Schneider H. 1988. Hearing and calls of the banana frog, Afrixalus fornasinii (Bianconi) (Anura: Rhacophoridae). Amphibia-Reptilia 9: 251–264. [Google Scholar]
  46. Sessions SK, Larson A. 1987. Developmental correlates of genome size in plethodontid salamanders and their implications for genome evolution. Evolution 41: 1239–1251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Shen XX, Liang D, Feng YJ, Chen MY, Zhang P. 2013. A versatile and highly efficient toolkit including 102 nuclear markers for vertebrate phylogenomics, tested by resolving the higher level relationship of the Caudata. Mol Biol Evol 30: 2235–2248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Stadtmüller F. 1931. Varianten im Mittelohrgebiet bei Bombinator (Colummella auris, Hyalbogenspange, Tuba auditiva) [Variations of middle ear cavity in Bombinator]. Gegenbaurs Morphol Jahrb 66: 196–219. [Google Scholar]
  49. Szarski H. 1970. Changes in the amount of DNA in cell nuclei underlying vertebrate evolution. Nature 226: 651–652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Szarski H. 1983. Cell size and the concept of wasteful and frugal evolutionary strategies. J Theol Biol 105: 201–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. ten Donkelaar HJ. 1998a. Anurans. In The central nervous system of vertebrates (ed. Nieuwenhuys R, ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C), Vol. 2, pp. 1151–1314. Springer, New York. [Google Scholar]
  52. ten Donkelaar HJ. 1998b. Urodeles. In The central nervous system of vertebrates (ed, Nieuwenhuys R, ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C), Vol. 2, pp. 1045–1150. Springer, New York. [Google Scholar]
  53. Tucker MA, Gerhardt HC. 2012. Parallel changes in mate-attracting calls and female preferences in autotriploid tree frogs. Proc R Soc B 279: 1583–1587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Vinogradov AE. 2004. Evolution of genome size: Multilevel selection, mutation bias or dynamical chaos? Curr Opin Genet Dev 14: 620–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wake DB. 1966. Comparative osteology and evolution of the lungless salamanders, family Plethodontidae. Mem South California Acad Sci 4: 1–111. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wake DB, Roth G. 1989. The linkage between ontogeny and phylogeny in the evolution of complex systems. In Complex organismal functions: Integration and evolution in vertebrates (ed. Wake DB, Roth G), pp. 361–377. Wiley, New York. [Google Scholar]
  57. Wake DB, Nishikawa KC, Dicke U, Roth G. 1988. Organization of the motor nuclei in the cervical spinal cord of salamanders. J Com Neurol 278: 195–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Walkowiak W. 1980. The coding of auditory signals in the torus semicircularis of the fire-bellied toad and the grass frog: Responses to simple stimuli and to conspecific calls. J Comp Physiol A 138: 131–148. [Google Scholar]
  59. Walkowiak W, Luksch H. 1994. Sensory motor interfacing in acoustic behavior of anurans. Amer Zool 34: 685–695. [Google Scholar]
  60. Weber E, Schumacher R. 1975. Der Aufbau der Abwehrrufe des Kammolches (Triturus cristatus) und des Fadenmolches (Triturus helveticus) (Amphibia, Caudata, Salamandridae). Salamandra 11: 119–129. [Google Scholar]
  61. Weber E, Schumacher R. 1976. Der Aufbau der Abwehrrufe des Bergmolches Triturus alpestris Laurenti (Amphibia, Urodela). Zool Anz 196: 61–66. [Google Scholar]
  62. Wilczynski W, Endepols H. 2007. Central auditory pathways in anuran amphibians: The anatomical basis of hearing and sound commmunication. In Hearing and sound communication in amphibians (ed. Narins PM, Feng AS, Fay RR, Popper AN), pp. 221–249. Springer, New York. [Google Scholar]
  63. Wilczynski W, Ryan MJ. 2010. The behavioral neuroscience of anuran social signal processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20: 754–763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

RESOURCES