Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Clin Psychol Sci. 2014 Nov 26;3(5):663–674. doi: 10.1177/2167702614551766

Pretraumatic Stress Reactions in Soldiers Deployed to Afghanistan

Dorthe Berntsen 1, David C Rubin 2
PMCID: PMC4564108  NIHMSID: NIHMS624168  PMID: 26366328

Abstract

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder is a diagnosis related to the past. Pre-traumatic stress reactions, as measured by intrusive involuntary images of possible future stressful events and their associated avoidance and increased arousal, have been overlooked in the PTSD literature. Here we introduce a scale that measures pre-traumatic stress reactions providing a clear future-oriented parallel to the posttraumatic stress reactions described in the diagnostic criteria for PTSD. We apply this pre-traumatic stress reactions checklist (PreCL) to Danish soldiers before, during, and after deployment to Afghanistan. The PreCL has good internal consistency and is highly correlated with a standard measure of PTSD symptoms. The PreCL as answered before the soldiers’ deployment significantly predicted level of PTSD symptoms during and after their deployment, while controlling for baseline PTSD symptoms and combat exposure measured during and after deployment. The findings have implications for the conceptualization of PTSD, screening, and treatment.


Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is a diagnosis related to the past, as indicated by the prefix ‘post’ as well as by the diagnostic requirements (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Curing PTSD is healing the wounds of the past. Some years ago, however, an entertainment newspaper, the Onion, turned the notion of PTSD on its head by featuring a satirical article stating that an increasing number of U.S. soldiers suffered from pre-traumatic stress (The Onion, 2006). This alleged syndrome involved vivid intrusive images and ‘flashforwards’ of stressful events that the soldiers might potentially experience or witness during their future deployment. The soldiers were suffering from possible future events, events that had not yet taken place. They were wounded, not by their past, but by their own projections of the future. Consistent with the ironic approach, the Onion article stated that a risk factor for this disorder was watching more than three hours of television news per day.

But are pre-traumatic stress reactions fiction? From its introduction in 1980 until 2013, PTSD was classified as an anxiety disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 2000, 2013) and so concern for future negative events is to be expected (Ehlers & Clark, 2000). Indeed, apprehension of catastrophic or socially embarrassing future events is often observed in anxiety disorders (e.g., Clark, 1999; Ottaviani & Beck, 1987). Here we show that pre-traumatic stress reactions, in terms of intrusive images and dreams about negative future events, accompanied by attempts at avoidance and increased levels of arousal, are a real aspect of the phenomenology of PTSD, which hitherto has been overlooked.

The term pre-traumatic stress reactions here designates disturbing future-oriented cognitions and imaginations as measured in terms of a direct temporal reversal of the conceptualizations of past-directed cognitions in the PTSD diagnosis. For example, the concept of ‘repeated, disturbing memories, thoughts, or images of a stressful experience from the past’ is reformulated to the future-directed version of having ‘repeated, disturbing imaginings, thoughts, or pictures concerning a possible stressful experience in the future.’ Having pre-traumatic stress reactions, as defined here, does not imply that the person has had no prior traumatic events or that the person has no symptoms of PTSD in relation to such prior events. On the contrary, we propose that pre-traumatic stress reactions are a central (but largely ignored) aspect of the phenomenology of PTSD.

We present data from Danish soldiers whose levels of post- and pre-traumatic stress were measured before, during, and after deployment to Afghanistan. Because our data were collected before the introduction of the DSM-5, they are based on the PTSD diagnosis as described in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). We demonstrate that pre-traumatic stress reactions exist, and that pre-traumatic stress reactions as measured before deployment are a reliable predictor of PTSD symptoms during and after deployment, even when controlling for baseline PTSD symptoms and combat exposure. Our findings have important implications for the conceptualization of PTSD, as well as for screening and treatment.

Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future

PTSD is conceived of as a disorder caused by past events. A person suffering from PTSD has repeated intrusive memories and dreams associated with the past traumatic event(s), persistent avoidance of potential reminders of the past traumatic event(s), negative alterations in cognition and mood associated with the past event(s), as well as alterations in arousal and reactivity that are also associated with the past traumatic event(s). All these symptoms are to have begun after the occurrence of the traumatic event(s) in question, according to the diagnostic criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Consistent with this emphasis on the past, hundreds of studies have examined how PTSD impacts memory (for reviews and discussions, see Brewin, 2007; Brewin & Holmes, 2003; Dalgleish, 2004; Kihlstrom, 2006; Klein, Caspi & Gil, 2003; McNally, 2003a, 2003b; Rubin, Berntsen, & Bohni, 2006). At the same time, little is known as to how PTSD affects constructing possible events in the future. This oversight is surprising. First, anxiety disorders generally involve negative future-oriented cognition in terms of worries and spontaneously occurring images of threatening situations (see Clark, 1999, for a review). Thus, given the fact that PTSD traditionally has been viewed as an anxiety disorder, and, as such, should be concerned with the appraisal of imminent threat (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), we would expect future-directed cognitions to be common also in PTSD.

Second, a rapidly growing body of neurocognitive research has demonstrated that factors affecting memory for past events tend to exert similar effects on the construction of possible events in the future (see D’Argembeau, 2012; Szpunar, 2010, for reviews). Such studies have shown that the two processes recruit largely the same areas in the brain (e.g., Addis, Wong & Schacter, 2007; Botzung, Denkova & Manning, 2008; Okuda et al., 2003; Szpunar, Watson & McDermott, 2007) and are similarly affected by a range of behavioral manipulations (e.g., D’Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2004; Spreng & Levine, 2006; Szpunar & McDermott, 2008; but see Berntsen & Bohn, 2010, and Rubin, 2013, for some deviations from this pattern). The ability to remember the personal past and imagine possible events in the personal future also seems to develop around the same time in the preschool years (e.g., Suddendorf, 2010), and show similar effects of aging (e.g., Addis, Wong & Schacter, 2008; Addis, Musicaro, Pan et al., 2010) as well as similar deficits in mental disorders, such as depression and schizophrenia (e.g., D’Argembeau, Raffard & van der Linden, 2008; Williams et al., 1996). Extrapolating these findings to PTSD gives us reasons to assume that not just memories of the past, but also simulations of future events would be affected by this disorder. We have been able to identify one study directly examining this possibility. Brown et al. (2013) asked 12 combat veterans diagnosed with PTSD and 16 veterans without PTSD to generate memories and simulate future events in response to neutral cue words. The veterans with PTSD reported more overgeneral events for both temporal directions. They also reported more events with a combat-related content, but this effect was strongest for the past.

Here we take the idea of a parallelism one important step further by asking, as did the Onion (2006) article, whether symptoms of PTSD related to the past -- that is, the intrusive memories, the attempts at avoiding reminders and the associated increased levels of arousal -- are mirrored by similar symptoms related to the future. In other words, would people suffering from posttraumatic stress reactions experience intrusive involuntary future projections of traumatic and stressful events to a similar extent as they experience intrusive memories? Would they try to avoid cues to these images of future events to the same extent as they avoid cues to the memories, and would they show increased arousal in association with these reactions? On the basis of the parallels between remembering past events and imagining future events just reviewed, we predict that this indeed will be the case. Central to these predictions is also research on involuntary future projections, to which we now turn, showing that not only past events, but also possible future events often come to mind involuntarily (unbidden).

Involuntary Future Projections

Involuntary autobiographical memories are memories of past events that come to mind spontaneously, that is with no preceding attempts at retrieval (Berntsen, 1996). Unlike intrusive trauma memories in PTSD, involuntary memories in daily life typically are about mundane and predominantly positive events. Still, they share a number of characteristics with intrusive memories in PTSD by being spontaneously (non-strategically) retrieved, uncontrollable, typically cued by concrete perceptual features of the environment, dominated by visual imagery, and by often having identifiable emotional impact at the time of retrieval (see Berntsen & Watson, 2013, for a recent review) As a future parallel, Berntsen and Jacobsen (2008) introduced the notion of involuntary future event representations -- defined as representations of possible personal future events that come to mind involuntarily -- that is, with no preceding attempts at generating the imagined event. In a diary study involving a student sample, Berntsen and Jacobsen found that involuntary future events were facilitated by situational cues in much the same ways as involuntary memories, in that the cues were most frequently overlapping situational features in terms of objects, people, themes, or places. They also found that involuntary future projections differed from strategically generated future projections in ways that paralleled the differences observed between involuntary and voluntary memories, in that involuntary future projections were more specific, more vivid, less emotionally positive, and involved more negative mood impact as compared to their voluntary counterparts. These findings were replicated in samples of high versus low worriers (Finnbogadótir & Berntsen, 2011). Finnbogadótir and Berntsen (2013) showed that involuntary future projections occurred as frequently as involuntary memories in daily life, with an average of about 20 involuntary future projections (or memories) per day.

Involuntary future projections are also found in clinical populations. They have been observed in both panic disorders and social anxiety disorders (see Clark, 1999, for a review). For example, Ottaviani and Beck (1987) described several cases with automatic and catastrophic imagery preceding or accompanying the panic attack, such as an attorney imaging himself fainting in the courtroom. More recently, involuntary future projections have been studied systematically in relation to depression and bipolar disorder (Deeprose & Holmes, 2010; Deeprose, Malik & Holmes, 2011; Homes, Crane, Fennell & Williams, 2007). Holmes et al. (2007) examined images of suicide in fifteen depressed and previously suicidal patients. These images were vivid and persistent and could be conceived of as flash-forwards to the suicide. Deeprose and Holmes (2010) introduced the Impact of Future Events Scale (IFES) by adapting the Impact of Event Scale (Horowitz, Wilner & Alvarez, 1979) to be directed towards future instead of past events. Higher scores on the IFES were associated with higher levels of dysphoria (Deeprose & Holmes, 2010) and bipolar disorder (Deeprose et al., 2011).

However, by asking broadly about images of the future (e.g., “Pictures about the future popped into my mind”) the IFES does not differentiate between stressful intrusive involuntary future projections and mundane future projections, which are often positive. In fact,Deeprose et al. (2011) found that the emotional content of the future events was positive in two thirds of the cases, which is equivalent to the frequencies of positive versus negative future events found in diary studies of involuntary memories and future projections with non-clinical populations (e.g., Berntsen & Jacobsen, 2008) and in studies of voluntary autobiographical memory more generally (Walker, Skowronski, & Thompson, 2003). Thus, most of the future projections measured by the IFES may be adaptive simulations of mundane future events, even in clinical populations. Previous research therefore leaves a need for a measure of pre-traumatic stress reactions that specifically captures emotionally negative and intrusive future projections, and their potentially associated reactions of avoidance and increased arousal.

The Present Study

The present study has three central aims. The first is to introduce a scale that measures pre-traumatic stress reactions to future imagined events in a way that provides a clear future-oriented parallel to the emotionally negative stress reactions described in the diagnostic criteria for PTSD. The second aim is to examine its correlations with PTSD symptoms (i.e., symptoms related to stressful past events) within a population of Danish soldiers deployed to Afghanistan. The third aim is to use pre-traumatic stress reactions measured before the soldiers’ deployment to predict level of PTSD symptoms during and after their deployment, while controlling for PTSD symptoms (assessed before deployment) and combat exposure (measured during and after deployment).

Our measure of pre-traumatic stress reactions is developed from the PTSD Checklist (PCL; Weathers, Litz, Huska, & Keane, 1994), which measures the 17 official symptoms of PTSD in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The first eight items concern re-experiencing and avoiding reminders of traumatic events in the past. We reformulated these eight items to address pre-experiencing and avoiding reminders of possible traumatic events in the future, while leaving the remaining nine items unchanged. We designate our pre-traumatic stress reaction measure the Pre-traumatic Stress Reactions Checklist (PreCL).

We predict that pre-traumatic stress reactions, as measured by the PreCL, form an independent aspect of the phenomenology of PTSD that shows high internal consistency and that is highly correlated with, but not identical to, the posttraumatic stress reactions measured by the standard PCL. Therefore we also expect the two scales to correlate at similar levels with known risk factors for the development of PTSD, including earlier traumas, depression and neuroticism. Because they are temporally reversed, we expect the first eight items of the PreCL to have lower correlations with the corresponding eight items in the PCL than do the remaining nine items, which are identical in both the PCL and PreCL. We nonetheless predict high correlations between the two sets of items within each scale, thereby demonstrating high internal consistency for both the PreCL and the PCL. This prediction is based on our expectation that respondents will answer the last nine unchanged items differently in the two scales in the context of the differently worded instructions and the differently worded preceding eight items. We expect the PreCL obtained before deployment to predict PTSD symptoms during and after deployment, when controlling for baseline PTSD symptoms (measured before deployment) and combat exposure (measured during and after deployment). This is consistent with our assumption that pre-traumatic stress reactions form an independent aspect of the phenomenology of PTSD.

Method

The present study was part of a larger data collection, which involved a number of psychometric measures related to PTSD symptoms, mental health, and well-being in a team of Danish soldiers before, during, and after a 6-month deployment to Afghanistan in 2009. A detailed analysis of trajectories for the development of PTSD symptoms across time, their risk factors, and attrition rates are available elsewhere (Berntsen et al., 2012). Here we focus on those parts of the data collection that are of theoretical relevance for the present work.

Analysis sample

The participants in the present study belonged to the Danish Contingent of the International Security Assistance Force 7 (ISAF 7). These soldiers were deployed for six months to the forward-operating bases in the Helmand Province of southern Afghanistan, where the Danish soldiers took part in the British-led Task Force Helmand. Their deployment involved substantial combat exposure and attacks by the enemy (Berntsen et al., 2012). At least 80% of the team reported that they had (a) experienced shootings near or against their camp, (b) passed through areas with combat, and (c) passed through areas with danger of roadside bombs. Around half of the soldiers were combat soldiers; about 70% of whom reported having killed an enemy. The analysis sample was 218 soldiers (15 females; mean age = 25.58 years, range 18-51 years) who provided answers to our measures of posttraumatic stress reactions for the four assessments included in the present study: before deployment (Before), during deployment (During), 1-3 weeks after return from deployment (Return), and 2-4 months after return (Return +3 months).

Our analysis sample formed a subsample of the 610 soldiers who completed the questionnaire before deployment. In order to identify possible attrition biases, we compared the analysis sample (n = 218) with the remaining subsample (n = 392) on relevant background variables, including measures from before deployment of age, gender, time spent in the military, earlier traumas, depression, a range of psychological problems, personality traits, positive and negative affect, PTSD symptoms, pretraumatic stress reactions; and measures from during deployment of combat exposure, PTSD symptoms, pretraumatic stress reactions; and measures at return from deployment of combat exposure, PTSD symptoms; and measures obtained 2 to 4 months after return of combat exposure, PTSD symptoms, and pretraumatic stress reactions. The analysis sample had fewer earlier traumatic events, t(594) = 2.37, p <. 05, scored slightly higher on a neuroticism, t(594) = 2.30, p <. 05, and scored lower on extraversion, t(594) = 3.41, p <. 001. No other significant differences were found.

Materials

The data collections included many different psychometric measures of which the following were relevant to the present analyses (the PCL and the PreCL were never answered consecutively but always separated by several other questionnaires in the questionnaire package, which varied slightly across the assessment times):

PTSD Checklist (PCL)

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist, Civilian version (Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley, & Forneris, 2006; Weathers et al., 1994) is a standardized checklist for PTSD symptoms, which has been used in previous studies of deployment-related PTSD. The scale consists of 17 items (equivalent to the 17 PTSD symptom categories in the DSM-IV) with a possible sum score ranging from 17 to 85. The internal consistencies (Cronbach α) of the PCL in the present population of soldiers are 0.85 (Berntsen et al., 2012). The PCL was answered at all four assessment times.

Pretraumatic Stress Reactions Checklist (PreCL)

This is a checklist for pretraumatic stress reactions developed for the purpose of the present study. The scale consists of 17 items (equivalent to the 17 PTSD symptom categories in the DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) with a possible sum score ranging from 17 to 85. The first eight items of the scale are a temporal reversal of the first eight items of the PCL, directed towards the future instead of the past. The remaining 9 items are formulated identically to the items in the PCL, but are answered with regard to possible negative future events, instead of negative past events. The instructions for the PreCL are a temporal reversal of the instructions for the PCL (see Table 1, which also includes the temporally reversed items needed for a version of the PreCL based on the DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The PreCL was answered three times: before, during, and three months after return from deployment (internal consistencies are reported in the results section).

Table 1.

The Pre-traumatic Stress Reactions Checklist (PreCL), Temporally Reversed Items

DSM-IV version
Instructions Below is a list of problems and complaints that people sometimes have before possible stressful life experiences in the future. Please read each one carefully, then circle one of the numbers to the right to indicate how much you have been bothered by that problem in the past month.
Item #
Temporally reversed formulation
1. Repeated, disturbing imagined scenes, thoughts, or images concerning a possible stressful experience in the future?
2. Repeated, disturbing dreams concerning a possible stressful experience in the future?
3. Suddenly acting or feeling as if a possible stressful experience in the future already were happening (as if you were pre-living it)?
4. Feeling very upset when something reminded you of a possible stressful experience in the future?
5. Having physical reactions (e.g., heart pounding, trouble breathing, sweating) when something reminded you of a possible stressful experience in the future?
6. Avoiding thinking about or talking about a possible stressful experience in the future or avoiding having feelings related to it?
7. Avoiding activities or situations because they reminded you of a possible stressful experience in the future?
8. Trouble imagining important parts of a possible stressful experience in the future?

DSM-5version
Instructions Below is a list of problems and complaints that people sometimes have before a possible, very stressful experience in the future. Please read each one carefully, then circle one of the numbers to the right to indicate how much you have been bothered by that problem in the past month.
Item #
Temporally reversed formulation
1. Repeated, disturbing and unwanted images of a possible future stressful experience?
2. Repeated, disturbing dreams of a possible future stressful experience?
3. Suddenly acting or feeling as if a possible future stressful experience already were happening (as if you were pre-living it)?
4. Feeling very upset when something reminded you of a possible future stressful experience?
5. Having strong physical reactions (e.g., heart pounding, trouble breathing, sweating) when something reminded you of a possible future stressful experience?
6. Avoiding imaginings, thoughts or feelings related to a possible future stressful experience?
7. Avoiding external remindings of a possible future stressful experience (for example people, places, conversations, activities, objects or situations)?
8. Trouble imagining important parts of a possible future stressful experience?
10. Blaming yourself or someone else for a possible future stressful experience or what has led up to it?

Note: The DSM-IV version is rated on a scale from 1 to 5; the DSM-V is rated on a scale from 0 to 4; anchor points for both: ‘not at all’, ‘a little bit’, ‘moderately’, ‘quite a bit’, ‘extremely’. Both are based on the relevant PCL (Weathers et al., 1994, 2013). Both should be administered with the temporally non-reversed items included. The full scales may be obtained from the authors1.

Combat Exposure Scale

The Combat Exposure Scale (Keane et al., 1989) is a standard measure of exposure to combat, such as episodes with hostile fire, firing at the enemy, or fellow soldiers being injured or killed during combat. The scale consists of 7 items. The internal consistencies (Cronbach α) of the Combat Exposure Scale in the present population of soldiers are 0.85 (Berntsen et al., 2012). The Combat Exposure Scale was answered three times: during, at return, and three months after return from deployment.

BDI-II

Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996) measures level of depression. The inventory consists of 21 items with a possible sum score ranging from 0 to 63. The BDI-II was answered at all four measurement times. The internal consistencies (Cronbach α) of the BDI-II in the present population of soldiers are 0.85 (Berntsen et al., 2012). Measures obtained before deployment are relevant for the present analyses.

Neuroticism

The scale for Neuroticism is one of the five personality factors measured by the NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1989). The neuroticism factor consists of 12 items with a possible sum score ranging from 12 to 60. The scale was answered before and three months after deployment. The internal consistencies (Cronbach α) of the Neuroticism scale in the present population of soldiers are 0.77 (Berntsen et al., 2012). Measures obtained before deployment are relevant for the present analyses.

TLEQ

Trauma Life Experience Questionnaire (TLEQ; Kubana et al., 2000) was here accommodated to a Danish population of soldiers. Here it consists of 20 questions addressing different types of traumas. The questionnaire measures how often (from never to more than 5 times) different traumas have been experienced. The internal consistencies (Cronbach α) of the TLEQ in the present population of soldiers are 0.70 (Berntsen et al., 2012). This scale was answered before deployment.

Procedure

Data for the assessment before deployment were collected by military psychologists five to six weeks before deployment during group sessions at a military camp in Denmark. The soldiers were informed about the study including the fact that their responses were anonymous, would not be accessed by their leaders in the military, and would be used for research purposes only. Data for the assessment during deployment were collected in Afghanistan. All respondents had been deployed for at least two and less than five months, when they answered this set of questionnaires. The questionnaires were handed out by military personnel at Camp Bastion, Kabul International Airport, or Kandahar Airfield before the soldier went home on leave. The soldiers submitted their answers in closed envelopes in secured (locked) mail boxes. The locked mail boxes were transported to the research team in Denmark. The data for the Return assessment were collected at a standard homecoming meeting organized by military psychologists and physicians. This data collection took place one to three weeks after the soldiers had returned from deployment. The data for the Return + 3 months assessment were collected at different military camps in Denmark, or through mail for those who had returned to civilian life, roughly two to four months after return from deployment (for more details on the study, see Berntsen et al., 2012).

Results

We first examine the internal consistency of the PreCL in comparison with the PCL, their means, and their correlations. We next examine the extent to which the PreCL, answered before deployment, predicts PTSD symptoms during and after deployment, when controlling for baseline PCL and combat exposure.

Internal Consistency, Means and Correlations

The internal consistency of the PreCL and PCL were similar. For the three measurement times at which both were completed, Cronbach α was 0.85, 0.86 and 0.93, for the PreCL, and 0.89, 0.87, and 0.92 for the PCL (ns=213-218).

The means and standard deviations for the two scales over the same three assessment times and results from repeated measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) are presented in Table 2. Both the PreCl and the PCL varied across the three assessments. The PreCL and the PCL were significantly different only at Return + 3 months (t(214) = 3.91, p < .001), causing a significant interaction between assessment time and type of scale (F(2, 420)=3.81, p < .05; ηp2 =.02).

Table 2.

The PreCL and PCL scores Before, During and Three Months after Return from Deployment (Total and Items 1 to 8 only)

Before
During
Return +3 mo.
Effect of Time
Scales M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F ηP2
Total items
     PreCL (N=211) 22.85 (6.81) 20.80 (5.79) 21.00 (7.39) 11.93*** .05
     PCL (N=218) 22.82 (7.68) 21.19 (6.20) 21.96 (7.62)   6.07** .03
Items 1 to 8 only
     PreCL (N=212) 10.44 (3.33) 9.23 (2.61) 9.26 (3.44) 16.79*** .07
     PCL (N=218) 9.99 (3.64) 9.30 (2.80) 9.62 (3.28)   3.97* .02

Note: PreCL: Pre-traumatic Stress Reactions Checklist; PCL: PTSD Checklist; Items 1 to 8 are temporally reversed in the PreCL;

***

p < .0001;

**

p < .001;

*

p < .05

Examining just the first eight items that are temporally reversed in the PreCL yielded a similar pattern of findings (see Table 2). The sum scores for items 1 to 8 of both scales varied across assessment times. Again, they differed significantly only at Return + 3 months (t(215) = 2.35, p < .001) causing a significant interaction between assessment time and type of scale (F(2, 422)=5.23, p < .01 ηp2=.03).

Table 3 shows the correlations between the two scales at the three assessment times, when both scales were answered. As predicted, these correlations are high and less so for the temporally reversed items in the PreCL (items 1 to 8) than for the items for which only the temporal context of the instructions differed between the two scales (items 9 to 17).

Table 3.

Correlations Between the PreCL and PCL, Between Subsets of Items Within each Scale, and with Measures of Earlier Traumatic Events, Depression and Neuroticism

Measurement Times Before, During and after Deployment
Before
During
Return +3 mo.
Correlations r r r
Between PreCL and PCL
     Sums of items 1 to 8 .56 .60 .77
     Sums of items 9 to 17 .85 .82 .90
     Sums of all items .79 .79 .89
Between sums for items 1 to 8 and 9 to 17
     Within PreCL .62 .57 .70
     Within PCL .60 .59 .72
PreCL and TLEQ, BDI-II, and Neuroticism
     TLEQ (Before) .36 .35 .29
     BDI-II (Before) .74 .40 .33
     Neuroticism (Before) .45 .34 .26
PCL and TLEQ, BDI-II, and Neuroticism
     TLEQ (Before) .43 .39 .30
     BDI-II (Before) .76 .51 .36
     Neuroticism (Before) .49 .40 .30

Note: PreCL: Pre-traumatic Stress Reactions Checklist; PCL: PTSD Checklist; TLEQ: Traumatic Life Event Questionnaire; BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory; ns = 203–218; for the correlations: all ps<.0005; Items 1 to 8 are temporally reversed in the PreCL

The correlations between sums of items 1 to 8 and 9 to 17 shows that within each scale, the two sets of items are equally highly correlated. In other words, temporally reversing items 1 to 8 did not reduce the internal consistency of the PreCL relative to the PCL, lending further support to the observation that the internal consistency of the PreCL is as high as that of the PCL.

Finally, the two scales correlate similarly with measures of previous traumatic events, depression and neuroticism (see Table 3). As expected, both scales are moderately to highly correlated with these well-known risk factors for anxiety disorders. The correlations are highest before deployment where the measures are obtained concurrently. The PreCL does not correlate more highly with these measures than the standard PTSD scale in any of the reported cases in Table 3. Rather, in cases where numerical differences are seen, the correlation coefficients are higher for the PCL. This eliminates the possibility that the PreCL simply is a proxy for such well-known vulnerability factors.

PreCL as a Predictor for PTSD Symptoms

Table 4 shows the results from hierarchical multiple regression analyses predicting level of PTSD symptoms during deployment, at return, and at three months after return, as three separate analyses. The top panel of Table 4 illustrates the outcomes of these analyses when the PCL score from before deployment and the concurrent measure of combat exposure were entered in the first step and the PreCL was entered in the second step. In all three analyses, the second step significantly increased the level of variance explained.

Table 4.

Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting PTSD Symptoms During Deployment, at Return and Three Months After Return

Measurement Times During and after Deployment
During Return Return +3 mo.

Entire scales ΔR2 β ΔR2 β ΔR2 β
Step 1 .50*** .28*** .21***
   PCL Before .66*** .46*** .45***
   Combat exposure1 .18** .22** .06
Step 2 .02* .07*** .05***
   PCL Before .50*** .11 .15
   Combat exposure .16** .20** .01
   PreCL Before .20* .44*** .38***
Total R 2 .52*** .35*** .26***
N 193 197 203

Sums for items 1 to 8 ΔR2 β ΔR2 β ΔR2 β

Step 1 .34*** .14*** .18***
   PCL Before .52*** .26*** .41***
   Combat exposure .22*** .24** .07
Step 2 .04*** .08*** .10***
   PCL Before .38*** .07 .20**
   Combat exposure .19** .22** .01
   PreCL Before .25*** .34*** .39***
Total R 2 .38*** .22*** .28***
N 193 197 203

Note:

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

***

p<.0001;

1

Combat exposure is always measured concurrently with the dependent variable. Items 1 to 8 are temporally reversed in PreCL.

As also illustrated by Table 4, the PCL was not a significant predictor in two of the three analyses, after the PreCL had entered the regression equation. Accordingly, if the PreCL was entered in the first step and the PCL in the second step of these analyses, then the second step added significantly to the variance explained in only one of the three analyses -- the one predicting PTSD symptoms during deployment (ΔR2 = .09). Taken together these findings suggest that PreCL completed before deployment predicts level of PTSD symptoms during and after deployment over and above the PCL completed before deployment and combat exposure as assessed concurrently with the dependent variable.

This pattern of results did not change if the sum scores for the TLEQ, BDI-II and neuroticism were entered in the first step of the regression analyses, together with the PCL and the measure of combat exposure. The PreCL as entered in Step 2 still added significantly to the amount of variance explained in all three analyses (with ΔR2= 0.02, 0.05, and 0.03, respectively, all ps < .05). In short, the PreCL answered before deployment explained independent variance in the level of PTSD symptoms at all three assessment times.

The PCL and PreCL as measured before deployment were highly correlated (Table 3), which might suggest a risk of multicollinearity affecting the results of the regression analyses. We therefore conducted the same analyses as the ones described in the top panel of Table 4, predicting the whole PCL, but this time we included as predictors only the sum scores for items 1 to 8 of the PreCL and PCL, respectively. Thus, as predictor variables, we limited both scales to the sums of the items that were temporally reversed in the PreCL because they correlated only moderately (r = 0.56) before deployment (see Table 3). These analyses (presented in the bottom panel of Table 4) largely replicated the analyses based on the entire scales: When the sum score for the PreCL (items 1 to 8) was entered in the second step it significantly increased the amount of variance explained for all three analyses, with ΔR2= 0.04, 0.08, and 0.10, respectively. After the PreCL had entered, the sum score for the PCL (items 1 to 8) remained significant in two of the three analyses, but not in the one predicting PTSD symptoms as measured at Return (see Table 4). Accordingly, if the PreCL (items 1 to 8) was entered in the first step and the PCL (items 1 to 8) in the second step of these analyses, then the second step added significantly to the variance explained in only two of the three analyses: the ones predicting PTSD symptoms during deployment (ΔR2 = .10) and three months after return (ΔR2 = .03) but not at return (ΔR2 = .003). In sum, the PreCL (as answered before deployment) predicted PTSD symptoms during, at return, and 3 months after return over and above the PCL (answered before deployment) and combat exposure (measured concurrently with the dependent variable in the analyses). Taken together, these findings support the idea that pre-traumatic stress reactions as measured by the PreCL form an independent aspect of the phenomenology of PTSD.

Discussion

We have introduced a scale for pre-traumatic stress reactions—that is, disturbing future oriented cognitions and images measured in terms of a temporal reversal of the standard conceptualization of past directed cognition in PTSD. We have administered this scale in a group of soldiers before, during and after deployment to Afghanistan. The study has two major findings. First, we have demonstrated that pre-traumatic stress reactions are an important aspect of the phenomenology of PTSD: Involuntary intrusive images and thoughts of possible future events, their associated attempts at avoidance, and increased levels of arousal were experienced at the same level as posttraumatic stress reactions to past events before and during deployment. The two measures were highly correlated at all assessment times, even when the analyses were limited to the temporally reversed items (Table 3). They also correlated similarly with well-known predisposing factors for the development of PTSD. These findings suggest that pretraumatic- and posttraumatic stress reactions are two subjectively different manifestations of the same underlying phenomenon. Second, we have demonstrated that pre-traumatic stress reactions are a risk factor for the development of PTSD symptoms in that the PreCL completed before deployment predicted subsequent PTSD symptoms over and above baseline PTSD symptoms and combat exposure. These findings have several theoretical and practical implications:

First, the findings challenge the current understanding of PTSD as a disorder primarily related to the past. Instead, they are consistent with a new line of clinical research emphasizing the importance of mental imagery across a range of clinical disorders (for a review see Pearson, Deeprose, Wallace-Hadrill, Heyes, & Holmes, 2013) as well as with a rapidly growing literature demonstrating that episodic remembering and future simulations draw upon overlapping neural and cognitive structures (e.g., D’Argembeau, 2012; Rubin, 2013; Schacter & Addis, 2007; Szpunar, 2010). Following this evidence and given the fact that PTSD involves a well-documented bias towards threatening information (e.g., Bryant & Harvey, 1995) and most likely a subjective sense of continuous threat (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), we would expect not just memories for past events, but also simulations of future events, to be affected by the disorder. This is consistent with the present findings.

Second, the present findings hold potential implications for treatment. Currently, the most commonly used treatments for PTSD focus on the past traumatic events as they are represented in memory and appraised by the person. One common strategy is exposure therapy, including imaginal exposure, where the event is re-experienced in memory, and in vivo exposure, where the person is exposed to situations that involve reminders of the trauma and therefore are associated with avoidance. Although there is evidence for the beneficial effects of exposure therapies in their current memory-directed forms (Institute of Medicine, 2008), the present findings suggest that such therapies could be further advanced by including imaginal exposure to frequently intruding representations of stressful future events, maybe in combination with other forms of therapy, such as imagery rescripting (e.g., Arntz, 2012) or eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (Engelhard, van den Hout, Janssen, & van der Beek, 2010) directed towards such event representations. Also, recent evidence that mindfulness-based exercises normalize cortisol levels and reduce PTSD symptoms among nurses (Kim, Schneider, Kravitz, et al. 2013) can be seen as consistent with the present findings, in that mindfulness therapies are not directed towards specific types of mental contents, but are likely to apply equally well to intrusive images of future and past events.

Third, our measure of pre-traumatic stress reactions might serve as a useful instrument for screening for soldiers at risk for developing PTSD symptoms during deployment given the finding that pre-traumatic stress reactions measured before deployment significantly predicted PTSD symptoms during and after deployment, even when controlling for baseline PTSD symptoms measured before deployment, combat exposure measured during and after deployment, as well as earlier traumas, depression, and neuroticisms measured before deployment. Future research should examine whether it also may be used as a screening instrument in relation to non-military traumatic events as well as other subjectively stressful events, such as exams, medical procedures, or childbirth.

More broadly, our findings suggest that intrusive images and dreams of future events and associated avoidance and increased arousal are experienced to the same extent as re-experiencing, avoidance, and increased arousal associated with past events. Therefore, assessing only the latter in diagnostic interviews and self-report questionnaires entails a one-sided focus on the past aspect of a phenomenology that includes both a future and a past temporal dimension.

The present study should be evaluated with a number of limitations in mind. Our sample included almost no females and very few participants above the age of 40. In addition, the soldiers generally presented with low levels of PTSD symptoms. Future research should examine whether the present findings can be generalized to more gender- and age balanced populations, as well as populations with higher PTSD levels. Also, the present study did not examine the content or qualities of the soldiers’ intrusive memories and future projections. It therefore leaves unanswered whether the future projections were vivid, whether the content of the future projections was thematically related to the content of the memories, and whether one of the two types of mental contents was experienced as being more emotionally intense and self-relevant than the other. Following previous work involving healthy populations, we would expect intrusive images related to the future to be seen as more self-relevant (e.g., Berntsen & Jacobsen, 2008) and experienced as more emotionally extreme and stressful (Rubin, 2013) than intrusions of traumatic events in the past. Whether this indeed is the case in populations with reactions to severe traumatic events is a question for future research.

In summary, the present work opens an important new line of research on PTSD by showing that pre-traumatic stress reactions are a genuine aspect of the phenomenology of this disorder. This discovery challenges the current understanding of the symptomology of PTSD and calls for a reconsideration of procedures in relation to screening and treatment.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Danish National Research Foundation grant DNRF93, Danish Council for Independent Research: Humanities, Royal Danish Defense College, and NIH grant R01MH066079 for support. We thank Yvonne Duval Thomsen, Kim Berg Johannessen, Robert Jonasen, Per Bach, Christoffer Holmgård Pedersen, and Mette Bertelsen for their assistance, and Christin Ogle and Kaitlyn Batt for comments.

Footnotes

1

The copyright for the scales is held by the authors (© 2014, Berntsen & Rubin). Permission is given to use the scales for research purposes.

Authorship statement

The data collection for the present study was part of a more extensive data collection, which the first author designed and conducted in collaboration with the Danish military and members of her lab. The first author is also responsible for the formulation of the PreCL and its inclusion in the data collection. The first and second author together designed the specific analyses for the present paper and together interpreted and discussed the results. Both authors were actively involved in the writing process, for which the first author had a leading role.

Contributor Information

Dorthe Berntsen, Aarhus University.

David C. Rubin, Duke University, and Aarhus University

References

  1. Addis DR, Musicaro R, Pan L, Schacter DL. Episodic simulation of past and future events in older adults: Evidence from an experimental recombination task. Psychology and Aging. 2010;25:369–376. doi: 10.1037/a0017280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL. Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1363–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL. Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science. 2008;19:33–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. text rev.) [Google Scholar]
  6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  7. Arntz A. Imagery rescripting as a therapeutic technique: Review of clinical trials, basic studies, and research agenda. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 2012;3:121–126. [Google Scholar]
  8. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory Manual. 2nd. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  9. Berntsen D. Involuntary autobiographical memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 1996;10:435–454. [Google Scholar]
  10. Berntsen D, Bohn A. Remembering and forecasting. The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Memory & Cognition. 2010;38:265–278. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.3.265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Berntsen D, Jacobsen AS. Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. Consciousness & Cognition. 2008;17:1093–1104. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Berntsen D, Johannessen KB, Thomsen YD, Bertelsen M, Hoyle RH, Rubin DC. Peace and war: Trajectories of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms before, during and after military deployment in Afghanistan. Psychological Science. 2012;23:1557–1565. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Berntsen D, Watson AL. In: Involuntary autobiographical memories in daily life and in clinical disorders. Perfect T, Lindsay S, editors. The SAGE handbook of Applied Memory, SAGE; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  14. Botzung A, Denkova E, Manning L. Experiencing past and future personal events: Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel. Brain & Cognition. 2008;66:202–212. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blanchard EB, Jones-Alexander J, Buckley TC, Forneris CA. Psychometric properties of the PTSD checklist (PCL) Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1996;34:669–673. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00033-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brewin CR. Autobiographical memory for trauma: Update on four controversies. Memory. 2007;15:227–248. doi: 10.1080/09658210701256423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brewin CR, Holmes EA. Psychological theories of posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review. 2003;23:339–376. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(03)00033-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown AD, Root JC, Romano TA, Chang LJ, Bryant RA, Hirst W. Overgeneralized autobiographical memory and future thinking in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry. 2013;44:129–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bryant RA, Harvey AG. Processing threatening information in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:537–541. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.3.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Costa PT, McCrae RR. The NEO-PI/NEO-FFI Manual supplement. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  21. Clark DM. Anxiety disorders: why they persist and how to treat them. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1999;37:S5–S27. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00048-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. D’Argembeau A. Autobiographical memory and future thinking. In: Berntsen D, Rubin DC, editors. Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches. England: Cambridge; 2012. pp. 311–330. [Google Scholar]
  23. D'Argembeau A, Raffard S, Van der Linden M. Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:247–251. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. D'Argembeau A, Van der Linden M. Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness and Cognition. 2004;13:844–858. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dalgleish T. Cognitive approaches to posttraumatic stress disorder: The evolution of multirepresentational theorizing. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130:228–260. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Deeprose C, Holmes EA. An exploration of prospective imagery: The Impact of Future Events Scale. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2010;38:201–209. doi: 10.1017/S1352465809990671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Deeprose C, Malik A, Holmes EA. Measuring intrusive prospective imagery using the Impact of Future Events Scale: Psychometric properties and relation to risk for bipolar disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy. 2011;4:187–196. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2011.4.2.187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ehlers A, Clark DM. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2000;38:319–345. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00123-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Engelhard IM, van den Hout MA, Janssen WC, van der Beek J. Eye movements reduce vividness and emotionality of "flashforwards". Behavior Research and Therapy. 2010;48:442–447. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Finnbogadottir H, Berntsen D. Involuntary mental time travel in high and low worriers. Memory. 2011;19:625–640. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.595722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Finnbogadótir H, Berntsen D. Involuntary future projections are as frequent as involuntary memories, but more positive. Consciousness and Cognition. 2013;22:272–280. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Holmes EA, Crane C, Fennell MJV, Williams JMG. Imagery about suicide in depression -“Flash-forwards”? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2007;38:423–434. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Horowitz MJ, Wilner N, Alvarez W. Impact of event scale: A measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1979;41:209–218. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197905000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Institute of Medicine. Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. An assessment of the evidence. Washington: National Academies Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  35. Keane T, Fairbank J, Caddell J, Zimering R, Taylor K, Mora C. Clinical evaluation of a measure to assess combat exposure. Psychological Assessment. 1989;1:53–55. [Google Scholar]
  36. Kihlstrom JF. Trauma and memory revisited. In: Uttl B, Ohta N, Siegenthaler AL, editors. Memory and emotion: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Malden, MA: Blachwell; 2006. pp. 259–291. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kim SH, Schneider SM, Bevans M, Kravitz L, Mermier C, Qualls C, Burke MR. Symptom Reduction with Mindfulness-Based Stretching and Deep Breathing Exercise: Randomized Controlled Clinical Trial and Efficacy. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2013;98:2984–2992. doi: 10.1210/jc.2012-3742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Klein E, Caspi Y, Gil S. The relation between memory for the traumatic event and PTSD: Evidence from studies of traumatic brain injury. 2003;48:28–33. doi: 10.1177/070674370304800106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kubany ES, Haynes SN, Leisen MB, Owens JA, Kaplan AS, Watson SB, Burns K. Development and preliminary validation of a brief broad-spectrum measure of trauma exposure: The Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment. 2000;12:210–224. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.12.2.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McNally RJ. Progress and controversy in the study of posttraumatic stress disorder. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003a;54:229–252. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. McNally RJ. Remembering trauma. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2003b. [Google Scholar]
  42. Okuda J, Fujii T, Ohtake H, Tsukiura T, Tanji K, Suzuki K, et al. Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1369–1380. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00179-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ottaviani R, Beck AT. Cognitive aspects of panic disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 1987;1:15–28. [Google Scholar]
  44. Pearson DG, Deeprose C, Wallace-Hadrill SMA, Heyes SH, Holmes EA. Assessing mental imagery in clinical psychology: A review of imagery measures and a guiding framework. Clinical Psychological Review. 2013;33:1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rubin DC. Schema-driven construction of future autobiographical traumatic events: The future is more troubling than the past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2013 doi: 10.1037/a0032638. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rubin DC, Berntsen D, Bohni MK. A memory-based model of posttraumatic stress disorder: Evaluating basic assumptions underlying the PTSD diagnosis. Psychological Review. 2008;115:985–1011. doi: 10.1037/a0013397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schacter DL, Addis DR. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2007;362:773–786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8:657–661. doi: 10.1038/nrn2213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Spreng RN, Levine B. The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan. Memory & Cognition. 2006;34:1644–1651. doi: 10.3758/bf03195927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Suddendorf T. Linking yesterday and tomorrow: Preschoolers' ability to report temporally displaced events. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2010;28:491–498. doi: 10.1348/026151009x479169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Szpunar KK. Episodic future thought: An emerging concept. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2010;5:142–162. doi: 10.1177/1745691610362350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Szpunar KK, McDermott KB. Episodic future thought and its relation to remembering: Evidence from ratings of subjective experience. Consciousness and Cognition. 2008;17:330–334. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Szpunar KK, Watson JM, McDermott KB. Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104:642–647. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610082104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. The Onion. Report: More U.S. soldiers suffering from pre-traumatic stress disorder. The Onion. 2006 Nov 15; Retrieved from: http://www.theonion.com/articles/report-more-us-soldiers-suffering-from-pretraumati,2088/
  55. Tulving E. Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Walker RW, Skowronski JJ, Thompson CP. Life is pleasant – and memory helps to keep it that way. Review of General Psychology. 2003;7:203–210. [Google Scholar]
  57. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Huska JA, Keane TM. The PTSD checklist (PCL) Unpublished scale available from the National Center for PTSD. 1994 [Google Scholar]
  58. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Keane TM, Palmieri PA, Marx BP, Schnurr PP. The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5) 2013 Scale available from the National Center for PTSD at www.ptsd.va.gov.
  59. Williams JMG, Ellis NC, Tyers C, Healy H, Rose G, MacLeod AK. The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Memory & Cognition. 1996;24:116–125. doi: 10.3758/bf03197278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES