Abstract
Introductions
Working donkeys are at risk of developing multiple, acute and chronic health problems. The ability to recognise and assess pain in donkeys associated with these health problems is important for people responsible for their care and treatment, including owners and veterinary or animal health workers.
Aims and objectives
The aims of this study were firstly to quantify the prevalence of a range of clinical abnormalities within a sample of working donkeys; and secondly to find out whether these abnormalities were associated with potential behavioural indicators of pain.
Materials and methods
One hundred and thirty-three entire male adult working donkeys were observed for ten minutes before and after a one-hour rest period. Using an ethogram developed and refined in associated studies, posture and event behaviours were recorded by a single observer. The health of each donkey was then assessed by a veterinarian for specific clinical abnormalities.
Results
Working donkeys have a high prevalence of clinical abnormalities and a number of behaviours are associated with these. Significant associations were found between observed behaviours and systemic, ocular and limb-related clinical abnormalities. Cumulative clinical scores for limb-related problems were associated with a higher frequency of leg trembling, knuckling of the forelimb, leg-lifting and weight-shifting behaviours (all R≥0.4; P<0.001) and with a lower frequency of weight-bearing evenly on all four feet (R=-0.458; P<0.001).
Conclusions
The specific behaviour changes associated with clinical abnormalities identified in this study, together with general changes in demeanour identified in related studies, may be useful in assessing the presence and severity of pain in working donkeys and their response to medical and palliative interventions.
Keywords: Donkeys, Behaviour, Welfare, Clinical, Pain
Introduction
There are an estimated 43.5 million donkeys worldwide (FAOSTAT 2013), the majority of which are working animals which support people living in the world's poorest communities, by transporting a wide range of goods and materials by pack or cart. As a consequence of their work requirements, living conditions and handling, many donkeys develop multiple, acute and chronic health problems (Pritchard and others 2005). The ability to recognise and assess pain in donkeys associated with these health problems is important for people responsible for their care and treatment, including owners and veterinary or animal health workers.
Behavioural indicators of limb pain (Price and others 2003), foot pain (Wright 1993), eye pain (Hendrix 2005), back pain (Wolf 2002), abdominal pain (Muir and Robertson 1985) and dental pain (Lane 1994) have been recorded for the horse, and similar behaviours have been observed and quantified in donkeys working in developing countries (Ashley and others 2006, Regan and others 2014, 2015). However, published information describing the behaviour of domestic donkeys in clinical contexts is limited (Ashley and others 2005). Clinical research relating to pain and pain management in donkeys has been undertaken primarily using healthy animals to investigate pharmacokinetic profiles of drugs (Matthews and others 1994, 2001), responses to anaesthetic regimens (Matthews and others 1992, 2002) and behavioural considerations when dealing with mammoth asses in a veterinary environment (Taylor and Matthews 1998). A review of the scientific literature on equine pain (Ashley and others 2005) found that behaviours described in horses were not reported for the donkey. This may be for several reasons: donkeys may not demonstrate clear source-specific behavioural indicators of pain, but exhibit a non-specific general change in demeanour which cannot be easily linked to particular pathologies (Taylor and Matthews 1998), donkeys studied previously may not have been in sufficiently acute pain to demonstrate the more obvious pain behaviours; or the many anecdotal reports of their stoical behaviour may be hindering further study of clinical behaviour, in the belief that it is unnecessary.
The aims of this study were first to quantify the prevalence of a range of clinical abnormalities within a sample of working donkeys; and secondly to find out whether these abnormalities were associated with observable behaviours which may prove useful as indicators of pain.
Materials and methods
Ethics statement
The study was carried out in Lahore, Pakistan, under ethical approval from the University of Bristol Faculty of Medical and Veterinary Sciences Ethics Committee (Investigation number UB/05/017) and was compliant with Pakistan law regarding ethical use of animals in science.
Donkeys
The high prevalence of clinical conditions within this population, with donkeys typically exhibiting more than one health problem, meant that it was not possible to perform accurate sample size calculations or to recruit control groups of donkeys free from pain for each of the conditions explored in this study. To prevent sex differences influencing study findings only entire male donkeys were chosen as subjects (Regan and others 2014).
In total 133 entire male adult working donkeys were recruited outside a wayside clinic in Lahore, Pakistan run by The Brooke. All donkeys were used to transport goods by cart or pack and were suffering from health issues including poor body condition, wounds, ocular conditions, dental abnormalities and lameness. Exclusion criteria included acute abdominal or respiratory distress, severe medical conditions such as tetanus or shock, inability to walk with reasonable encouragement and stereotypical behaviour patterns. Donkeys entering or receiving treatment at the clinic were not recruited. Selection of subjects was not random given the opportunistic sampling strategy; whoever was passing the clinic at the appropriate time, had an eligible donkey and gave their consent to participate in the study was recruited rather than a targeted strategy being adopted.
On arrival each donkey was placed in a holding area and offered 1 kg chopped maize forage, in order to distinguish pain-related behaviour from hunger. Water was available to all donkeys ad libitum throughout the study.
Experimental protocol
From the holding area, each donkey was moved to a 3 m×3 m sand-floored observation pen placed 8 m away from a busy road, replicating the examination area normally used by the clinic's veterinary staff. In the observation pen the donkey was tied loosely to a ground ring for five minutes, then a single handler (JA) entered the pen, untied the lead rope, held it 1.5 m from the donkey's head and remained still for another five minutes to habituate the donkey to the handler's presence. The handler did not interact or make eye contact with the donkey, except to encourage it to stand up at the start of the study period if it was lying down; following this the donkey was allowed to stand, lie or roll as it chose. The observer (FHR) then observed the behaviour of the donkey for 10 minutes, following which the donkey was tied again and the handler left the pen. This procedure (five minutes habituation to handler; 10 minutes observation) was repeated after one hour to test for a potential effect of rest on the behaviours expressed. Another donkey was continuously present in an adjoining pen so that the donkey was not socially isolated between and during the two observation sessions.
Behavioural observation
A single observer (FHR), seated 3.5 metres away from the front of the pen, used a previously developed ethogram (Table 1; Regan and others 2014) to record behaviour. The behaviours recorded fell into two types, Postural and Event behaviours, and were recorded in different ways. Anatomical components of posture (ear orientation, eyelid position (open/closed), muzzle tension, head carriage, limb placement and tail position) were recorded each minute over the 10 minute observation period using instantaneous (scan) sampling, giving us 10 observations of each measure for each observation session. In contrast all occurrences of the predefined event behaviours (eg, rolling, walking, eating, rubbing; see Table 1 for full list) were counted over the same 10-minute period, giving us one record for each behaviour for each 10 minute observation session.
TABLE 1:
Category | Behaviour | Description |
---|---|---|
Pen position | Back of pen | Head and forelimbs in back half of pen (irrespective of orientation) |
Front of pen | Head and forelimbs in front half of pen (irrespective of orientation) | |
Postural behaviours | ||
Lying down | Lying sternally | Lying down on sternum, legs folded underneath body frame |
Lying laterally | Lying on side with legs outstretched, head and neck may be in contact with ground | |
Standing | Standing on four feet | Weight-bearing on all four limbs with no preferred loading |
Standing on three feet | Weight-bearing on three limbs with a hindlimb resting | |
Pointing | Placing a foot in a forwards position outside of the main body frame (minimum one hoof length) with reduced weight-bearing | |
Knuckled | Forwards bend of one or both forelimbs with the knee bent in front of the placed foot or fetlock joint | |
Ear position | Forwards | Both ears facing forwards with ear cups fully visible when facing the donkey head on |
Sideways | Both ears facing sideways with ear cup orientated approximately 90 degrees laterally from forwards-facing position | |
Backwards | Both ears facing backwards with ear cups visible when standing behind the donkey | |
Combinations | Each ear in a different orientation, for example, one facing forwards, one facing backwards | |
Ear level | Down | Tip of ear level with or below level of base of ear, drooping downwards in any orientation |
Up | Tip of ear above base of ear, in any orientation | |
Head carriage | High | Poll higher than top of withers |
Level | Poll level with top of withers | |
Low | Base of ears below top of withers | |
Very low | Nose contact with ground | |
Head direction | No turn | Straight head carriage, no turning of head towards self, object or stimuli |
Turn to belly | Head is turned towards or makes contact with either side of abdomen | |
Turn to flank | Head is turned towards or makes contact with either flank | |
Turn to limb/foot | Head is turned towards or makes contact with either forelimb/hindlimb/foot | |
Turn to look | Head is orientated away from a straight position with attention drawn towards environmental stimuli and not directed towards a body part/region | |
Tail position | Relaxed | Tail held in a relaxed position, hanging freely in a vertical line from its body base |
Lifted out | Tail held in a fixed position, sticking out more than 45 degrees from the vertical line | |
Tucked | Tail held tightly against the rump in a fixed position, with tip of tail tucked between hindlegs | |
Swishing | Tail moves swiftly from its base in a side-to-side flicking manner around the hindquarters | |
Event behaviours | ||
Body | Rolling | From lying down laterally or sternally, vigorous rolling and wriggling movement of whole body over onto back |
Transition up/down | From lying down laterally or sternally to standing, or vice versa | |
Feet | Walking | Forwards, backwards or sideways movement of limbs to a new position |
Pawing | Repetitive lifting and backwards dragging or scraping of pointed hoof | |
Leg lifting | Temporary lifting of any limb from the ground with hesitant replacement near to its original position, often repetitive | |
Weight shifting | Weight temporarily offloaded from a fore or hindlimb onto the remaining three limbs, accompanied by subtle body rock | |
Foot stamping | Sudden lifting and forceful replacement of any limb in its original position | |
Oral | Eating | Prehension of food into the mouth, with repetitive chewing and swallowing |
Drinking | Muzzle touches water source followed by at least one visible swallow | |
Sniffing | Movement of muzzle towards object, ground or body, followed by inhalation and movement of nostrils | |
Flehmen response | Upper lip curls back to expose gums with incisors meeting, head tips back and rapidly points muzzle upwards | |
Licking and chewing | Repetitive licking and chewing motion in the absence of any food in the mouth | |
Yawning | Mouth opens wide, eyes close, head rises, tips back and shakes, lower jaw grinds and closes | |
Biting | Grasps object, self or another donkey in open mouth and bites or chews with single or repeated jaw closures | |
Head | Head shaking | Vigorous rotational shake of head and neck resulting in ears flapping against sides |
Head tossing | Rapid up and down movements of neck with successive nods of head | |
Vocal | Snorting | Quick forced exhalation of air through nostrils making an audible noise |
Braying | Series of short duration, loud inhalations, followed by a prolonged noisy exhalation | |
Sighing | Prolonged deep inhalation with a noticeable rise of the body, followed by a short burst of expiration, then gradual release of air | |
Groaning | Similar to a sigh, but accompanied by a monotone prolonged vocalisation and often head is turned towards body part | |
Coughing | Distinctive short duration exhalation from the lungs, deep and often moist in sound, often accompanied by a body heave | |
Maintenance | Self grooming | Repeated grooming movement of mouth and incisors directed at own body parts |
Rubbing | Moving a body part against another (eg, eye rubbing on foreleg) or repeatedly moving a body part back and forth against any object | |
Stretching | Head tucks into neck, mouth opens slightly, eyes often close, whole body contracts and rises up noticeably with inhalation | |
Dozing | State of body stillness with eyes closed, head lowered and standing on all four feet, or standing on three feet and resting a leg | |
Eliminative | Defaecating | Elimination of faeces |
Urinating | Elimination of urine | |
Play | Playing | Lifting up and tipping/holding objects with mouth, rearing, broncing, bucking or attempted interaction |
Clinical examination
Immediately after the second behavioural observation period, each donkey was examined by a single veterinarian (JG) blinded to the previous behavioural observations. Clinical abnormalities were recorded as present or absent (1 or 0) with the exception of body condition score which was rated on a five-point scale and response to palpation of the back, tail and forelimbs which was recorded on a four-point scale as described by Broster and others 2009 (see Table 2). Heart rate, respiratory rate and rectal temperature were recorded as continuous variables. The clinical examination check sheet is available as online supplementary material.
TABLE 2:
Observation | % donkeys | Observation | % donkeys | Observation | % donkeys |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Demeanour* | Nostrils/muzzle/dental | Tail—response to palpation median (IQR)† | 1 (1–1) | ||
Restless | 3 | Slit nostrils | 13 | 0=no response to palpation | 11 |
Bright and alert | 46 | Flared or fixed nostrils | 12 | 1=responds to light touch using one finger | 68 |
Apathetic | 49 | Discharge (uni/bilateral) | 72 | 2=medium touch using middle three fingers | 13 |
Dull and depressed | 2 | Nostril asymmetry | 89 | 3=firm touch using palm or whole hand | 8 |
Lip lesions | 90 | Tail base—swellings | 67 | ||
Systemic | Hypersalivation | 1 | Tail—mutilation/amputation | 16 | |
Abnormal lung sounds | 97 | Incisor overbite | 80 | ||
Abnormal borborygmi | 88 | Damaged or broken incisors | 3 | Limbs | |
Capillary refill time >2 seconds | 14 | Sharp molar edges/hooks | 86 | Forelimb—response to palpation median (IQR)† | 1 (1–1) |
Abnormal mucous membrane colour | 17 | Pocketing feed in cheeks | 6 | 0=no response to palpation | 1 |
Heart rate: median (IQR) | 45 (42–50) | 1=responds to light touch using one finger | 76 | ||
Respiratory rate: median (IQR) | 20 (16–25) | Hooves | 2=medium touch using middle three fingers | 16 | |
Rectal temperature: median (IQR) | 38 (37–38) | Bounding digital pulse | 61 | 3=firm touch using palm or whole hand | 7 |
Coronary band swollen | 86 | Forelimb—restricted range of movement | 98 | ||
Wounds‡ | Split or damaged hoof wall | 90 | Swollen joint(s) —any limb(s) | 100 | |
Head | 45 | Sole defects | 100 | Swollen tendon(s) —any limb(s) | 97 |
Ear | 2 | Response to palpation of coronary band | 99 | Forelimb(s) affected more severely than hindlimb(s) | 31 |
Neck | 4 | Hindlimb(s) affected more severely than forelimb(s) | 69 | ||
Breast | 60 | Body Frame | |||
Withers | 26 | Neck—response to palpation median (IQR)† | 1 (1–2) | Lameness§ | |
Ribs | 18 | 0=no response to palpation | 4 | 0=no gait abnormality | 0 |
Belly | 74 | 1=responds to light touch using one finger | 52 | 1=uneven gait | 2 |
Hindquarters | 14 | 2=medium touch using middle three fingers | 25 | 2=observable lameness | 98 |
Tail | 23 | 3=firm touch using palm or whole hand | 19 | ||
Limb | 40 | Neck—swellings | 65 | Evidence of¶: | |
Neck—restricted range of movement | 93 | Firing | 1 | ||
Eyes | Hobbling | 87 | |||
Discharge (uni/bilateral) | 60 | Back—response to palpation median (IQR)† | 1 (1–2) | Tethering | 5 |
Wet eyelashes | 29 | 0=no response to palpation | 4 | Blistering | 1 |
Steep eye lash angle | 74 | 1=responds to light touch using one finger | 70 | ||
Red/swollen conjunctivae | 10 | 2=medium touch using middle three fingers | 17 | Cumulative scores**: | |
Corneal abnormality | 84 | 3=firm touch using palm or whole hand | 9 | Number of wounds: median (IQR) | 4 (2–5) |
Globe abnormality | 17 | Back—swellings | 92 | Number of problems affecting eyes: median (IQR) | 3 (2–4) |
Absent menace response | 12 | Back—restricted range of movement | 92 | Number of problems affecting limbs: median (IQR) | 10 (8–11) |
Ears | Hindquarters/pelvis asymmetrical | 60 | |||
Slit or broken | 4 | Hindquarters—muscle atrophy | 55 | ||
Ear mite damage | 4 |
*Demeanour scored based on Pritchard and others (2005)
‡Wounds scored according to Pritchard and others (2005), where severity scores (0–3) relate to the area and depth of tissue damage. Hair loss, wounds or scars resulting from blistering, firing and hobbling were recorded separately
†Response to palpation from Broster and others (2009)
§Evaluated over 20 strides at walk
**Cumulative scores calculated by totalling the scores (1 present, 0 absent) each donkey received for abnormalities that fell into each grouping (wounds, eyes, limbs)
IQR, interquartile range
Statistical analysis
The prevalence of each clinical problem was calculated (percentage of donkeys with each abnormality out of the 133 donkeys sampled). For each postural behaviour the number of times it was recorded in each observation session was calculated giving each postural behaviour a score from 0 to 10 for each of the two observation sessions. The total number of occurrences of each event behaviour was calculated for each session; this was a continuous measure with each event behaviour scoring ≥0. Data were not normally distributed, so non-parametric Wilcoxon's signed rank tests for matched pairs were used to investigate differences in behaviour between the first and second observation sessions for each donkey, to test for any effect of rest. As no significant differences were found the remainder of the analyses use the behaviour data obtained in the first observation session.
Relationships between the behaviours observed were explored using Spearman's rank correlations. Due to the high prevalence of clinical conditions within this population and collinearity of many of the behavioural measures (see results) the data were not suitable for multivariate analysis. Instead Mann-Whitney U tests for independent samples were used to explore behavioural differences between donkeys with and without each clinical condition where the prevalence of the condition in the 133 donkeys sampled was ≤80 per cent and ≥10 per cent (see Table 2 for the prevalence of each clinical abnormality). Cumulative scores were calculated for eye abnormalities and limb abnormalities by totalling the scores (1 present, 0 absent) that each donkey received for each grouping. The cumulative number of wounds was also calculated by totalling the wounds present in each body area to create a total number of wounds for each donkey.
Spearman's rank correlations were used to test for associations between the frequency of behaviours and cumulative number of wounds, problems affecting eyes or problems affecting limbs as well as heart rate, respiratory rate and rectal temperature.
As there was a high risk of Type I errors (finding false positives) through the multiple tests conducted, Holm's sequentially rejective multiple test procedure (Holm 1979) was used post hoc for all tests. The adjusted P value denoted by the procedure was P≤0.001 and only variables meeting this adjusted value and thereby retaining their significance were reported as such. SPSS V.21.0 (SPSS, Illinois, USA) was used for all analyses.
Results
The majority of donkeys recruited were 5–15 years of age (62 per cent), with 27 per cent <5 years old and 11 per cent aged 16 years or older. None of the donkeys had a body condition score greater than 3 (medium); 12 per cent were scored 1 (very thin), 72 per cent scored 2 (thin) and 16 per cent scored 3 (medium) on a scale of 1–5 (1=very thin, 5=very fat).
All of the donkeys recruited had multiple conditions of varying severity and at different stages of natural progression. Table 2 presents the prevalence of clinical abnormalities in the study population. All of the donkeys sampled had sole defects and swollen joints, while 98 per cent had observable lameness (score 2). A restricted range of movement was seen in the forelimbs of 98 per cent of the sample, swollen tendons in 97 per cent and abnormal lung sounds were detected in 97 per cent of the donkeys.
Effect of rest on behaviour
There were no significant differences in the behaviour observed in the first and second observation sessions. For example, lying down had a median of 0 in both observation sessions (Z=45.0, P=0.637), walking had a median of 1 in both sessions (Z=2048.0, P=0.707) and weight-shifting had a median of 4 in the first observation session and 5 in the second session, however, this difference was not statistically significant (Z=3671.5, P=0.096).
The remainder of the following analyses uses the behaviour data obtained in the first observation session.
Associations between the behaviours observed
In total 45 different postural and event behaviours were observed during the study period; associations between some of these behaviours are presented in Table 3. Fifty-nine correlations between behaviours were greater than or equal to R=0.4 indicating a moderate correlation. Walking was strongly positively correlated with sniffing (R=0.871). There was also a strong positive correlation between having a knuckled forelimb and trembling leg.
TABLE 3:
Positive correlations |
Negative correlations |
||
---|---|---|---|
Correlated behaviours | Correlation coefficient (R) | Correlated behaviours | Correlation coefficient (R) |
Weight-bearing evenly | Weight-bearing evenly | ||
Walking | 0.412 | Knuckled forelimb | −0.439 |
Sniffing | 0.454 | Pointed limb | −0.536 |
Knuckled forelimb—trembling leg | 0.629 | Trembling leg | −0.492 |
Both ears forward | Lifting leg | −0.416 | |
Head held alert (poll above withers) | 0.484 | ||
Walking | 0.455 | Shifting weight between limbs | −0.435 |
Sniffing | 0.514 | Both ears forward | |
Both ears sideways | −0.535 | ||
Turn to look at environmental stimuli | 0.458 | Eyes closed | −0.589 |
Both ears sideways | Both ears sideways | ||
Eyes closed | 0.434 | Both ears back | −0.675 |
Head held low | 0.443 | Head held alert (poll above withers) | −0.634 |
Head held alert (poll above withers) | Eyes closed | ||
Walking | 0.466 | Head held alert (poll above withers) | −0.578 |
Sniffing | 0.456 | Walking | −0.468 |
Licking/chewing | 0.442 | Sniffing | −0.477 |
Turn to look at environmental stimuli | 0.492 | Turning to look at environmental stimuli | −0.559 |
Head very low | Head held alert (poll above withers) | ||
Rolling | 0.424 | Head level with withers | −0.474 |
Walking | 0.577 | Head lower than withers | −0.614 |
Sniffing | 0.629 | Relaxed tail—swishing tail | −0.758 |
Turning to flank | 0.420 | Muzzle normal—muzzle droopy | −0.865 |
Snorting | 0.405 | ||
Tail out—walking | 0.416 | ||
Trembling leg | |||
Lifting leg | 0.417 | ||
Shifting weight between limbs | 0.460 | ||
Rolling—turning to flank | 0.451 | ||
Walking | |||
Sniffing | 0.871 | ||
Flehmen response | 0.400 | ||
Licking/chewing | 0.551 | ||
Turning to look at environmental stimuli | 0.543 | ||
Snorting | 0.463 | ||
Touching handler | 0.483 | ||
Sniffing | |||
Flehmen response | 0.405 | ||
Licking/chewing | 0.567 | ||
Turning to flank | 0.421 | ||
Turn to look at environmental stimuli | 0.596 | ||
Snorting | 0.484 | ||
Touching handler | 0.483 | ||
Flehmen response—snort | 0.535 | ||
Licking/chewing—turning to look at environmental stimuli | 0.450 | ||
Turning to limb—self grooming | 0.632 | ||
Self grooming—turning to belly | 0.434 | ||
Lifting leg—shifting weight between limbs | 0.431 | ||
Rubbing body—rubbing eye | 0.771 |
Associations between clinical and behavioural findings
Associations between systemic health problems and behaviour
Weak negative correlations were found between respiratory rate and both ears being held sideways (R=−0.295; P<0.001) and eyes being closed (R=−0.343; P<0.001), indicating that the higher the respiratory rate the fewer times ears held sideways and eyes closed were seen across the 10 observation points. A positive correlation was found between respiratory rate and swishing the tail (R=0.291; P=0.001). Rectal temperature was positively correlated with tail swishing (R=0.312; P<0.001) and negatively correlated with tucking the tail between the hindlegs (R=−0.341; P<0.001).
Abnormal mucous membrane colour was associated with a higher frequency of lying down (Mann-Whitney U 1475.0; abnormal mucous membrane colour present median=0 (interquartile range (IQR) 0–0, min=0, max=9), abnormal mucous membrane colour absent median=0 (IQR 0–0, min=0, max=2); P<0.001) and a lower frequency of holding both ears forward in an alert/vigilant position (Mann-Whitney U 630.0; abnormal mucous membrane colour present median=0 (IQR 0–0), abnormal mucous membrane colour absent median=2 (IQR 0–4); P<0.001).
No significant associations were identified between heart rate, capillary refill time >2 seconds, abnormal borborygmi, and any of the behaviours observed.
Associations between ocular problems, limb problems, hindquarter abnormalities and behaviour
Significant associations between ocular problems, limb problems, hindquarter abnormalities and behaviour are presented in Table 4. Hindquarter asymmetry had the greatest number of significantly associated behaviours, and these encompassed a range of different behaviours including ear position, head carriage, weight distribution and oral behaviours.
TABLE 4:
Clinical abnormality present | Clinical abnormality absent | Mann-Whitney U test statistic | Level of significance | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Clinical abnormality | Behavioural indicator | Median* (IQR) | Median* (IQR) | P value | |
Ocular problems | |||||
Eye discharge | Closed eyes | 8 (3–10) | 2 (2–8) | 2873.0 | <0.001 |
Steep eyelash angle | Head level with withers | 3 (1–7) | 0 (0–3.25) | 2360.0 | <0.001 |
Limb problems | |||||
Bounding digital pulse | Turn towards belly | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–1) | 1648.5 | <0.001 |
Lifting limb | 0 (0–1.5) | 0 (0–0) | 2404.0 | 0.001 | |
Forelimbs more severely affected than hindlimbs | Knuckled forelimb | 1 (0–4.5) | 0 (0–0) | 2555.0 | <0.001 |
Lifting limb | 0 (0–3) | 0 (0–0) | 2443.0 | <0.001 | |
Hindlimbs more severely affected than forelimbs | Knuckled forelimb | 0 (0–0) | 1 (0–4.5) | 1217.0 | <0.001 |
Lifting limb | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–3) | 1328.0 | <0.001 | |
Problems with the hindquarters | |||||
Hindquarter asymmetry | Ears held to the side | 5 (2–9) | 3 (1–5) | 2879.0 | <0.001 |
Weight shifting between limbs | 6.5 (3–12) | 3 (1–4.5) | 3104.0 | <0.001 | |
Ears held forwards | 1 (0–3) | 3 (1–5) | 1353.5 | <0.001 | |
Walking | 0 (0–2.75) | 5 (0–11) | 1235.0 | <0.001 | |
Sniffing | 0 (0–3) | 3 (0–5) | 1295.0 | <0.001 | |
Chewing/biting† | 0 (0–0) (min=0,max=0) | 0 (0–0) (min=0, max=8) | 1800.0 | <0.001 | |
Turn to look at environmental stimuli | 2 (0–4) | 5 (2–8.5) | 1146.0 | <0.001 | |
Lifting limb | 0 (0–1.75) | 0 (0–0) | 2624.0 | 0.001 | |
Limbs weight-bearing evenly | 2 (0–7) | 7 (1.5–10) | 1419.0 | 0.001 | |
Very low head | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–1) | 1530.0 | 0.001 | |
Licking/chewing | 2 (1–3) | 3 (2–5) | 1395.0 | 0.001 | |
Muscular atrophy of hindquarters | Chewing/biting† | 0 (0–0) (min=0, max=0) | 0 (0–0) (min=0, max=8) | 1898.0 | 0.001 |
*Median (IQR) portrays the median number of times the behaviour was observed out of a maximum of 10 times for postural behaviours and median number of times the behaviour was observed over the whole observation session for event behaviours
†Min and max provided to show direction of effect not seen in the IQR
IQR, interquartile range
Behaviours associated with cumulative clinical abnormalities
Table 5 presents the behaviours associated with cumulative scores for wounds and limb conditions. Weak negative correlations were found between cumulative wound scores and walking, sniffing and snorting. Moderate positive correlations were found between cumulative limb abnormalities score and trembling legs, weight shifting, knuckling of the forelimb and lifting the legs. Cumulative limb abnormalities score was moderately negatively correlated with weight-bearing evenly on all four limbs, that is, the higher the cumulative score of limb abnormalities, the less often the donkey was observed bearing it's weight evening on all four limbs. There were no significant associations between any behaviour and cumulative eye abnormalities score.
TABLE 5:
Accumulation of clinical abnormalities | Behaviours observed more frequently (positive R) or less frequently (negative R) in animals with higher cumulative clinical score | Correlation coefficient (R) | Significance (P) |
---|---|---|---|
Cumulative wound score | |||
Walking | −0.322 | <0.001 | |
Sniffing | −0.292 | 0.001 | |
Snorting | −0.286 | 0.001 | |
Cumulative limb score | |||
Trembling leg | 0.636 | <0.001 | |
Bear weight evenly on all four feet | −0.458 | <0.001 | |
Shifting weight between limbs | 0.457 | <0.001 | |
Knuckled forelimb | 0.419 | <0.001 | |
Lifting leg | 0.407 | <0.001 | |
Sniffing | −0.387 | <0.001 | |
Very low head carriage | −0.384 | <0.001 | |
Walking | −0.350 | <0.001 | |
Turn to look at environmental stimuli | −0.348 | <0.001 | |
Relaxed tail | 0.338 | <0.001 | |
Swishing tail | −0.311 | <0.001 | |
Licking/chewing | −0.291 | 0.001 | |
Both ears forward | −0.282 | 0.001 |
Discussion
The aims of this study were to quantify the prevalence of clinical abnormalities within a sample of entire male working donkeys and to find out whether these abnormalities were associated with potential behavioural indicators of pain. There was a high prevalence of clinical abnormalities within the working donkeys sampled and many of the observed behaviours were significantly correlated with each other, as well as being associated with specific clinical abnormalities, and with cumulative limb or wound scores.
No significant differences in behaviour were found between the first and second observation periods, indicating that an hour-long rest period does not affect the behavioural expression of working donkeys. This has important methodological implications for future studies on this population, as it suggests that there may be no benefit of waiting to observe behaviour until after a rest period and equally that any delay in observations will not significantly affect the behaviour observed. However, it should be noted that the behavioural observations used in this study were relatively broad in nature, and more detailed observations may have detected an effect of rest on subsequent behaviour.
Auscultation of the lungs and abdomen demonstrated a high prevalence of abnormal lung sounds, which may be attributable to the animals’ polluted working environment. This raises concerns for donkey health, work capacity and longevity. No other data on respiratory disorders in animals living or working in comparable environments were found. However, there may be parallels drawn with the health of the human population in this region, and greater understanding of the effects of environmental pollutants on animals may help to inform or support studies on human subjects (Reif 2011).
Although the findings of this study suggest that ocular abnormalities are prevalent within the working donkey population, research on ocular pain in animals is limited and almost non-existent for donkeys. Anecdotal comments suggest that domestic donkeys show a muted pain response compared with horses (Inglis 1997). Behavioural indicators of eye pain in horses include blepharospasm and frequent blinking (Hendrix 2005), while even a small downward change in eyelash angle may indicate ocular pain and discomfort (Plummer 2005). In this study, donkeys with eye discharge were more frequently observed with their eyes closed and those with a steep eyelash angle were more often seen with their head held with their poll level with the withers. Some donkeys did not display a menace response, although this abnormality had no consistent behavioural association. While this may indicate a defect in the visual nervous pathway and suggest partial or complete blindness in one or both eyes, it has also been stated that depressed or stoic animals may show a reduced menace response (Lunn and Mayhew 1989) and this may provide an alternative explanation for the lack of response seen in the donkeys sampled.
Abnormal borborygmi were found in 88 per cent of the donkeys sampled. Many factors could contribute to abdominal abnormalities in working donkeys including poor quality and irregular feed, little or no long-stem forage, poor hydration status, dental malfunction, systemic health problems and ingestion of foreign objects such as plastic bags. Classic behaviours associated with acute equine abdominal pain, such as kicking at the abdomen (Muir and Robertson 1985), rolling (Crane 2002), flank watching (Pritchett and others 2003) and stretching (Jochle 1989) were used as exclusion criteria for this study; donkeys showing these signs were sent directly for treatment. Although not part of the study, donkeys were seen entering the field clinic with visible indicators of acute abdominal pain, including rolling, groaning, stretching and sweating, despite dullness and depression being commonly reported as the only behaviours indicating abdominal pain in the domestic donkey (Whitehead and others 1991, Matthews and others 1997, Duffield and others 2002).
Working donkeys suffering from chronic and/or episodic acute pain as a result of musculoskeletal problems are likely to show reduced work performance, with consequences for their owners’ livelihoods. Feral asses rarely gallop (Moehlman 1998), yet working donkeys are frequently required to trot and gallop on uneven and hard ground (including roads) from a young age, while pulling carts or carrying pack-loads. This is likely to account for the high prevalence of limb problems and lameness observed in the present study donkeys (98 per cent), comparable with that of working donkeys assessed in previous surveys (Pritchard and others 2005, Reix and others 2014).
Some limb abnormalities were found in all donkeys so could not be analysed for behavioural differences between affected and unaffected animals. These included swollen joints, swollen tendons, a restricted range of movement in forelimbs and split or damaged hoof walls. There is an abundance of literature regarding limb and foot pain in the domestic horse, but comparable evidence for the donkey is lacking. Anecdotal descriptions suggest an absence of the postural changes seen in the horse: such as no obvious alteration in the stance of donkeys with laminitis (Trawford and Crane 1995); often the only behavioural changes were an increase in lying time and a reduction in movement. Stiffness and difficulty rising are seen in donkeys suffering from arthritis (Whitehead and others 1991). Source-specific behavioural indicators of limb and foot pain in equids, including weight-shifting, leg lifting and abnormal weight distribution (Ashley and others 2005), were observed in donkeys in the present study and these were associated with the presence of clinical abnormalities such as asymmetrical hindquarters/pelvis, a bounding digital pulse and donkeys whose forelimbs were more severely affected than their hindlimbs. These behavioural indicators of limb pain have immediate application to the improvement of pain recognition, assessment and management in the donkey. The frequency of limb-related behaviours also correlated positively with the cumulative number of clinical problems affecting the limbs, indicating their usefulness as part of a tool to monitor the severity of limb pain and response to treatment strategies.
This study benefited from being carried out entirely in the field, replicating clinicians’ opportunities for observing behavioural signs of pain and relating them to a clinical examination. As such, the findings of this study have significant practical application for future studies on working donkey populations. The study environment accurately represented the environment in which behaviours are normally performed and animals were observed for a realistic duration (10 minutes). The behavioural repertoire was not diminished by the busy environment, if anything the donkeys appeared more relaxed and expressive than in previous studies when they had been moved to quieter locations for observation (Regan and others 2014, 2015). Behaviours were selected to be directly relevant for assessing pain and response to treatment, as described in equine literature and investigated in previous two studies by the same authors (Regan and others 2014, 2015).
However, practical field studies bring their own limitations: it was not possible to recruit equal numbers of donkeys with each clinical abnormality, or control groups, due to the complex clinical picture presented by the majority of working donkeys. Similarly, due to the scarcity of information on the prevalence of clinical abnormalities in working donkeys, sample size calculations could not be performed. While the study sample was opportunistic by necessity, the study design and analysis were intended to reduce variability as much as possible by using only male animals, habituating them to the observation area and handler and accounting for any potential effect of rest on behaviour. Behaviours that are strongly influenced by time of day (Ashley and others 2006, Regan and others 2014) were also excluded. Although the presence of a handler lightly restraining the donkey will have some impact on behaviour, observations were made in the presence of a handler to replicate an authentic situation when an animal is presented to the clinic for examination. Despite these precautions it is not known how representative the findings of this study are of other populations of working donkeys and this should be given consideration when interpreting the results of this study.
The high prevalence of clinical abnormalities and the majority of donkeys being presented with multiple conditions also made the exploration of significant associations between behaviour and clinical conditions more complex. While post hoc tests were used to control for the multiple tests conducted, it should be understood that the findings of this study are not as clear-cut as those from studies exploring such relationships in subjects with only one condition and a control group. Some of the correlations identified were relatively weak (R<0.4) despite the high level of statistical significance required to retain significance after the post hoc test. These relationships would benefit from further exploration, ideally using donkeys affected by only the condition of interest and an appropriate control group. The interrelationships between the behaviours observed also warrant further investigation and may help to refine behavioural observations and analysis in future studies.
The assessment of the donkeys’ health was designed to document the presence or absence of abnormalities and not as a diagnostic clinical examination evaluating the severity of every abnormality found. The logical progression from this study would be to incorporate an evaluation of clinical severity into the assessment protocol, alongside an indication of the impact this degree of abnormality may have on the donkey in terms of pain and suffering. The findings of this study have immediate practical application however, and can be used to augment existing clinical practice at roadside treatment sites and equine field clinics by including more focused behavioural observations in the clinical examination process. These will inform treatment decisions and enable better assessment of response to analgesia and palliative management of pain in working donkeys.
Conclusion
Working donkeys with a range of clinical abnormalities show both general and specific behavioural indicators of pain. Some behaviours were expressed more frequently in association with cumulative clinical abnormalities. These behaviours may have the potential to be used as a tool for assessing pain severity and response to medical and palliative interventions. As such these behaviours potentially have great value in enabling owners to monitor the welfare of their donkeys, to determine when to solicit treatment and to evaluate the efficacy of any treatment undertaken.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank The Brooke for supporting this project, Brooke Pakistan staff in Lahore for their assistance, especially Dr Javid Gondal and Mr Jamil Ahmad, and all the owners who allowed their donkeys to be used in this study.
Footnotes
Funding: This study was funded by The Brooke.
Competing interests: None.
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Data sharing statement: The data are available upon request to the corresponding author (bec.whay@bristol.ac.uk).
References
- Ashley F. H., Waterman-Pearson A. E., Whay H. R. (2005) Behavioural assessment of pain in horses and donkeys: application to clinical practice and future studies. Equine Veterinary Journal 37, 565–575 doi:10.2746/042516405775314826 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashley F. H., Waterman-Pearson A. E., Whay H. R. (2006) Pain behaviour in working donkeys: Identifying consistent behaviours. Proceedings of the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour Winter Conference London, UK: Behaviour into Welfare, 20–21 [Google Scholar]
- Broster C. E., Burn C. C., Barr A. R. S., Whay H. R. (2009) The range and prevalence of pathological abnormalities associated with lameness in working horses from developing countries. Equine Veterinary Journal 41, 474–481 doi:10.2746/042516409X373907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crane M. (2002) Colic in the donkey. Proceedings of the BEVA 41st Congress Glasgow, UK, 33–34 [Google Scholar]
- Duffield H. F., Bell N., Henson F. M. D. (2002) Factors associated with impactive colic in the donkey. In: Proceedings of the 7th International Equine Colic Research Symposium, Manchester, UK, 14th–16th July 2002, pp 122 [Google Scholar]
- FAOSTAT (2013) FAO statistical database. Food and Agricultural Organisation of the United Nations; http://faostat.fao.org/ [Google Scholar]
- Hendrix D. (2005) Eye examination techniques in horses. Clinical Techniques in Equine Practice 4, 2–10 doi:10.1053/j.ctep.2005.03.010 [Google Scholar]
- Holm S. (1979) A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics 6, 65–70 [Google Scholar]
- Inglis R. (1997) Ocular disease in the donkey. In The Professional Handbook of the Donkey. 3rd edn. Ed Svendsen E. D., London: Whittet Books Ltd; 37–42 [Google Scholar]
- Jochle W. (1989) Field trial evaluation of detomidine as a sedative and analgesic in horses with colic. Equine Veterinary Journal Supplement 7, 117–120 doi:10.1111/j.2042-3306.1989.tb05669.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane J.G. (1994) A review of dental disorders of the horse, their treatment, and possible fresh approaches to management. Equine Veterinary Education 6, 13–21 doi:10.1111/j.2042-3292.1994.tb01074.x [Google Scholar]
- Lunn D. P., Mayhew I. G. (1989) The neurological evaluation of horses. Equine Veterinary Education 1, 94–101 doi:10.1111/j.2042-3292.1989.tb01355.x [Google Scholar]
- Matthews N. S., Peck K. E., Taylor T. S., Mealey K. L. (2001) Pharmacokinetics of phenylbutazone and its metabolite oxyphenbutazone in miniature donkeys. American Journal of Veterinary Research 62, 673–675 doi:10.2460/ajvr.2001.62.673 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews N. S., Taylor T. S., Hartsfield S. M. (1997) Anaesthesia of donkeys and mules. Equine Veterinary Education 9, 198–202 doi:10.1111/j.2042-3292.1997.tb01306.x [Google Scholar]
- Matthews N. S., Taylor T. S., Hartsfield S. M., Hayton W. L., Jones D. H. (1994) Pharmacokinetics of ketamine in mules and mammoth asses premedicated with xylazine. Equine Veterinary Journal 26, 241–243 doi:10.1111/j.2042-3306.1994.tb04377.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews N. S., Taylor T. S., Hartsfield S. M., Williams J. D. (1992) A comparison of injectable anaesthetic regimes in Mammoth asses. Equine Veterinary Journal Supplement 11, 37–40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews N. S., Taylor T. S., Sullivan J. A. (2002) A comparison of three injectable anaesthetics in miniature donkeys. Veterinary Anaesthesia and Analgesia 29, 36–42 doi:10.1046/j.1467-2987.2001.00066.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moehlman P. D. (1998) Behavioural patterns and communication in feral asses (Equus africanus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 60, 125–169 doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00162-2 [Google Scholar]
- Muir W. W., Robertson J. T. (1985) Visceral analgesia: effects of xylazine, butorphanol, meperidine and pentazocine in horses. American Journal of Veterinary Research 46, 2081–2084 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plummer C. (2005) Equine eyelid disease. Clinical Techniques in Equine Practice 4, 95–105 doi:10.1053/j.ctep.2005.03.004 [Google Scholar]
- Price J., Catriona S., Welsh E. M., Waran N. K. (2003) Preliminary evaluation of post-operative pain in horses following arthroscopic surgery. Veterinary Anaesthesia and Analgesia 30, 124–137 doi:10.1046/j.1467-2995.2003.00139.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pritchard J. C., Lindberg A. C., Main D. C. J., Whay H. R. (2005) Assessment of the welfare of working horses, mules and donkeys, using health and behaviour parameters. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 69, 265–283 doi:10.1016/j.prevetmed.2005.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pritchett L. C., Ulibarri C., Roberts M. C., Schneider R. K., Sellon D. C. (2003) Identification of potential physiological and behavioural indicators of post-operative pain in horses after exploratory celiotomy for colic. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 80, 31–43 doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00205-8 [Google Scholar]
- Regan F. H., Hockenhull J., Pritchard J. C., Waterman-Pearson A. E., Whay H. R. (2014) Behavioural repertoire of working donkeys and consistency of behaviour over time, as a preliminary step towards identifying pain-related behaviours. PLoS ONE 9, e101877 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0101877 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Regan F. H., Hockenhull J., Pritchard J. C., Waterman-Pearson A. E., Whay H. R. (2015) Identifying behavioural differences in working donkeys in response to analgesic administration. Equine Veterinary Journal doi:10.1111/evj.12356doi:10.1111/evj.12356. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reif J.S. (2011) Animal sentinels for environmental and public health. Public Health Reports 126, 50–57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reix (Nee Broster) C. E., Burn C. C., Barr A. R. S., Whay H. R. (2014) The range and prevalence of pathological abnormalities associated with lameness in working draught donkeys from Pakistan. Equine Veterinary Journal 46, 771–777 doi:10.1111/evj.12231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor T. S., Matthews N. S. (1998) Mammoth asses: selected behavioural considerations for the veterinarian. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 60, 283–289 doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00177-4 [Google Scholar]
- Trawford A. F., Crane M. A. (1995) Nursing care of the donkey. Equine Veterinary Education 7, 36–38 doi:10.1111/j.2042-3292.1995.tb01180.x [Google Scholar]
- Whitehead G., French J., Ikin P. (1991) Welfare and veterinary care of donkeys. In Practice (Supplement Veterinary Record) 13, 62–68 [Google Scholar]
- Wright I. M. (1993) A study of 118 cases of navicular disease: clinical features. Equine Veterinary Journal 25, 488–492 doi:10.1111/j.2042-3306.1993.tb02999.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf L. (2002) The role of complementary techniques in managing musculoskeletal pain in performance horses. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Equine Practice 18, 107–115 doi:10.1016/S0749-0739(01)00005-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]