Abstract
Data were analyzed from a population-based, longitudinal sample of 8,650 U.S. children to (a) identify factors associated with or predictive of oral vocabulary size at 24 months of age and (b) evaluate whether oral vocabulary size is uniquely predictive of academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry. Children from higher socioeconomic status households, females, and those experiencing higher-quality parenting had larger oral vocabularies. Children born with very low birth weight or from households where the mother had health problems had smaller oral vocabularies. Even after extensive covariate adjustment, 24-month-old children with larger oral vocabularies displayed greater reading and mathematics achievement, increased behavioral self-regulation, and fewer externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors at kindergarten entry.
Keywords: Oral vocabulary, academic achievement, behavior, kindergarten, school readiness, early language
Children with greater academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry often experience better educational and societal opportunities as they age (Duncan et al., 2007; Sabol & Pianta, 2012). For example, children entering kindergarten with greater reading and mathematics achievement are more likely to attend college and enroll in higher-quality institutions. They are also more likely to own homes, have 401(k) savings, be married, and live in higher-income neighborhoods as adults (Chetty et al., 2011). Identifying factors contributing to greater academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry should help guide efforts to deliver early interventions to specific population subgroups at risk for lower school functioning (Hoff, 2013; Lesaux, 2012; Pagani, Fitzpatrick, & Parent, 2012), and so reduce later achievement gaps and increase post-secondary education, employment, productivity, and long-term wages (e.g., Heckman & Masterov, 2007).
Oral vocabulary is a malleable factor repeatedly theorized to contribute to increased academic and behavioral functioning, and so might be targeted in early interventions (e.g., Dickinson, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2010; Perfetti & Stafura 2014).1 Oral vocabulary refers to the words children use when speaking or recognize when listening. At-risk children’s oral vocabularies have been theorized to constitute a “first order” causal factor that, if increased, may enhance their developmental trajectories (Dickinson et al., 2010; Lesaux, 2012).
Oral Vocabulary’s Theorized Relation with Academic and Behavioral Functioning
Theoretically, at about 24 months of age, accelerating growth in children’s lexicons overtaxes their protosyllabary (i.e., meaningless speech-motor patterns) and their use of word gestures (Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999).2 To more effectively communicate, children are believed to rely increasingly on (a) phonemization, so that words begin to be represented by phonological segments, and (b) syntactization, in which lexical concepts begin to be grouped by categories and subcategories. Use of this dual coding process should allow young children to associate particular phonetic encodings and articulations with specific lexical representations, thereby facilitating acquisition and use of an oral vocabulary of increasingly greater size and complexity (Ouellette, 2006).
Acquiring a larger oral vocabulary should over time result in greater reading achievement (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014) by facilitating listening comprehension (Hoover & Gough, 1990) as well as decoding skills (Verhoeven, van Leeuwe, & Vermeer, 2011).3 Children with larger oral vocabularies should more efficiently store words in their lexicons as smaller segments, helping to increase their phonological sensitivity and, thus, their understanding of the alphabetic principle and decoding (Silven, Poskiparta, Niemi, & Voeten, 2007). A larger oral vocabulary should also result in more efficient word identification (Perfetti & Hart, 2001), allowing children to better identify and understand partially decoded, irregular or novel, or orthographically complex words, and to infer spelling-sound relations (Mitchell & Brady, 2013; Tunmer & Chapman, 2012).
A larger oral vocabulary is also theorized to result in greater mathematics achievement (LeFevre et al., 2010; Vukovic & Lesaux, 2013). Children’s initial understanding of the symbolic number system and its inter-relations (e.g., number sequence, basic arithmetic and subtraction) is thought to rely on language-based verbal associations (e.g., Spelke & Tsivkin, 2001). Having a larger oral vocabulary should result in children’s lexicons including more words and phrases representing abstract mathematical concepts, while also facilitating more complex understanding of these concepts. Children with larger oral vocabularies should more easily understand and solve mathematics problems presented in spoken or written stories, in part because of a better understanding of abstract terminology (Davidse, de Jong, & Bus, 2014; Fuchs et al., 2006). A relation between children’s oral vocabularies and mathematics achievement may emerge prior to school entry (Purpura, Hume, Sims, & Lonigan, 2011).
A larger oral vocabulary should also result in greater behavioral functioning (e.g., Qi & Kaiser, 2004). Developmentally, this should occur as children begin using words to monitor and modify their own behavior (Eisenberg, Sadovsky, & Spinrad, 2005). Young children should begin to adopt their caregiver’s regulatory speech, which increasingly emphasizes proactive or inhibitory rather than soothing behaviors (Winsler, Diaz, McCarthy, Atencio, & Chabay, 1999). Having a larger oral vocabulary should provide children both with greater symbolic representations of their internal states and better-articulated frameworks for understanding their experiences, resulting in greater ability to organize and guide actions, regulate emotions, and self-verbalize problem-solving strategies (Cole, Armstrong, & Pemberton, 2010). Observable indicators of greater behavioral self-regulation, or “approaches to learning,” include remaining attentive, persistent, flexible, engaged, and organized while completing classroom tasks (e.g., Li-Grining, Votruba-Drzal, Maldonado-Carreno, & Hass, 2010). Children’s language abilities and behavioral self-regulation positively correlate as early as 24 months of age (Vaughn, Kopp, & Krakow, 1984).
A larger oral vocabulary should also facilitate children’s understanding and communication with adults and peers (Cole et al., 2010). Conversely, children with smaller oral vocabularies should display more frequent externalizing or internalizing problem behaviors as they experience greater frustration and anger, lack of control of their environment, peer rejection, task avoidance, and withdrawal (Menting, van Lier, & Koot, 2010). Having a smaller oral vocabulary positively co-varies with greater avoidance and acting out behaviors in children as young as 24 months of age (Rescorla, Ross, & McClure, 2007).
Methodological and Substantive Limitations of Extant Research
Currently, however, the extant work’s methodological and substantive limitations constrain empirically derived conclusions as to oral vocabulary’s theorized importance for children’s academic and behavioral functioning generally and as a specific target of early intervention for at risk populations. For example, the National Early Literacy Panel (NELP; 2008) identified 11 factors considered predictive of conventional literacy and concluded that five were “moderately correlated with at least one measure of later literacy achievement but either did not maintain this predictive power when other important contextual variables were accounted for or have not yet been evaluated by researchers in this way” (p. viii). One of these five factors was oral language, including vocabulary.The NELP (2008) suggested that future research evaluate the contribution of these factors.
To date, “surprisingly little” research has been conducted with preschool-aged children, with most studies relying on very small clinical samples (Horwitz et al., 2003, p. 932). Hart and Risley’s (1995) seminal study analyzed data from a longitudinal sample of 42 families, including only 6 families representing the lowest SES class. Pan, Rowe, Singer, and Snow (2005) characterized data from these 6 families as constituting “nearly the entirety of what we know” about vocabulary gaps by very young children from low-income families (p. 764). Fernald, Marchman, and Weisleder’s (2013) recent study investigating vocabulary gaps at 18 and 24 months of age between children from high- and low-SES families was based on a sample of 48 children. Use of small samples limits generalizability to the larger, heterogeneous U.S. population. Knowledge is also limited about the age of onset and risk factors for vocabulary gaps for population subgroups that, in addition to children from low-SES families (Fernald et al., 2013) and racial or ethnic minorities (Farkas & Beron, 2004), may also be at risk. Associations with other modifiable factors (e.g., parenting, maternal mental health) have yet to be comprehensively examined. Early screening, monitoring, and intervention efforts would be better targeted if guided by findings from large-scale, epidemiological-type studies identifying the socio-demographic, gestational and birth, family risk and resilience factors most strongly associated with early and meaningful differences in children’s oral vocabularies. Many of these factors are known to be associated with or predictive of children’s later cognitive, academic, and behavioral functioning (Lynch, 2011; Reichman, 2005). Yet their specific associations with early vocabulary gaps is less clear.
Small convenience samples also limit the field’s understanding of whether other factors explain the relation between oral vocabulary size and children’s academic and behavioral functioning. For example, whether general cognitive functioning explains the relation between oral vocabulary and children’s academic achievement has yet to be firmly established (Benasich, Curtiss, & Tallall, 1993; Rescorla, Ross, & McClure, 2007). Although an association between oral vocabulary and reading achievement has been repeatedly observed (see Sénéchal, Ouellette, & Rodney, 2006, for a review), including comorbidity of language and reading disability (see Pennington & Bishop, 2009, for a review), evidence as to whether the relation is potentially causal is unclear due to the currently limited and mixed evidence (Oakhill & Cain, 2012; Ouellette, 2006; Schatschneider et al., 2004). To what extent toddler- and preschool-aged children’s oral vocabularies predict their later mathematics achievement or, separately, their behavioral functioning has yet to be comprehensively evaluated using population-based data (Justice, Bowles, Turnbull, & Skibbe, 2009). The extant studies almost entirely investigate single theorized pathways between oral vocabulary and (a) reading achievement, or (b) mathematics achievement, or (c) behavioral functioning instead of estimating multiple pathways simultaneously, particularly as children make the major transition to school.4
Potential Confounds of the Theorized Relation Between Oral Vocabulary and Academic and Behavioral Functioning
Rigorously evaluating the extent to which 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies uniquely predict their academic and behavioral functioning by kindergarten requires extensive statistical control for many potential confounds. Evidence of oral vocabulary’s predictive relations following this control would address identified limitations in the field’s knowledge base (NELP, 2008), provide stronger support for each relation’s potential causality (Hart & Risley, 1995), and empirically evaluate the merits of calls for greater emphasis on oral vocabulary and other language-based competencies in early intervention efforts (Dickinson et al., 2010; Hoff, 2013; Lesaux, 2012). To this end, we analyzed population-based data collected through a non-experimental panel design to evaluate evidence for potential causal relations between 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies and academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry.
We did so in two ways. First, we investigated whether 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies predicted their academic and behavioral functioning at 60 months of age, establishing temporal precedence. Second, we examined whether 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies continued to predict their academic and behavioral functioning at 60 months of age following extensive statistical control of many factors previously identified as potential confounds (e.g., general cognitive functioning, prior behavioral functioning), reducing the likelihood that any observed predictive relations between earlier oral vocabulary size and later academic and behavioral functioning were spurious (Finkel, 1995).
Potential confounding factors of a relation between 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies and their later academic and behavioral functioning may be grouped into several types of factors, each previously established as predictive of academic and/or behavioral functioning. The first group includes socio-demographic factors including SES, race or ethnicity, maternal age, and marital status. Low SES in particular may result in less cognitively stimulating and higher stress environments that constrain young children’s academic and behavioral growth (McLoyd, 1998). Low SES is strongly associated with non-White race or ethnicity, young maternal age, and being a single parent (DeNavas-Walt, Proctor, & Smith, 2013).
The second group of confounds are gestational and birth characteristics, including low or very low birth weight, multiple gestation, and medical risks and complications during pregnancy and at delivery (e.g., Conley & Bennett, 2000). For instance, being born at low or very low birth weight (for which twins and higher-order multiples have heightened risk) may result in neurodevelopmental impairments in behavioral self-regulation (Klebanov, Brooks-Gunn, & McCormick, 1994), general cognitive functioning (Hack, Klein, & Taylor, 1995), and academic achievement (Lynch, 2011). Medical risks during pregnancy (e.g., maternal substance use), and complications of pregnancy (e.g., gestational diabetes) and delivery (e.g., prolonged labor) are associated with later developmental delays (e.g., Anthopolos, Edwards, & Miranda, 2013).
Parenting and the quality of the home environment constitute a third group of potential confounds, with family stress and investment potentially explaining the effects of SES on children’s development (Guo & Harris, 2000; Yeung, Linver, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002). For instance, young children whose parents are warm and supportive, set consistent routines for their children, and provide more cognitively stimulating environments often display greater academic and behavioral functioning, even after accounting for SES and other socio-demographic characteristics (Iruka, LaForett, & Odom, 2012). Other potentially important features of the home and family include whether a family member has a mental illness, learning disability or special need, whether the mother has health problems, is depressed or socially isolated, how much time the child spends watching television, and whether or not the child attends daycare. Parental mental and physical health problems are associated with cognitive and other delays in young children (Breaux, Harvey, & Lugo-Candelas, 2013). Toddlers and preschool-aged children who frequently watch television are more likely to enter school with lower academic functioning, particularly in reading (Pagani, Fitzpatrick, & Barnett, 2013). This is possibly due to television viewing displacing storybook reading (Koolstra & Van der Voort, 1996) and limiting growth in children’s cognitive and attentional capacities (Christakis, Zimmerman, DiGiuseppe, & McCarty, 2004). Conversely, educational or subtitled programs may instead help increase young children’s achievement (Ennemoser & Schneider, 2007). Frequently attending childcare may increase children’s risk for behavior problems, including during the transition to kindergarten (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2003)
Young children’s own level of cognitive and behavioral functioning prior to or by kindergarten entry also constitute additional potential confounds. Cognitive functioning strongly predicts children’s later academic achievement and behavior (Duncan et al., 2007; Wang, Shen, & Byrnes, 2013), and is likely related to children’s oral vocabularies (e.g., Menting et al., 2010). Behavioral self-regulation (e.g., attentiveness, task persistence), externalizing behavior problems (e.g., aggressiveness), and internalizing behavior problems (e.g., anxiety, withdrawal) are also associated with children’s oral vocabularies (Menting et al., 2010), and should autoregressively predict later behavioral functioning (Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2009).
Study’s Purpose
We sought to identify socio-demographic, gestational and birth, cognitive and behavioral, and family functioning factors associated with or predictive of U.S. children’s oral vocabularies at 24 months of age. We also evaluated whether and to what extent the children’s oral vocabularies uniquely predicted their academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry. Our study investigated three specific research questions. First, which U.S. children display larger or smaller oral vocabularies at 24 months of age? Second, does having a larger oral vocabulary at 24 months of age uniquely predict greater academic functioning at kindergarten entry? Third, does having a larger oral vocabulary at 24 months of age uniquely predict greater behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry?
Method
Design and Analytical Samples
We analyzed data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), a population-based, longitudinal cohort assessed from birth to kindergarten entry (http://nces.ed.gov/ecls/birth.asp). This sample was selected from birth certificate records and includes oversamples of particular population subgroups (e.g., Native Americans and Alaska Natives, children born with very low birth weight), with sample weights provided to generate nationally representative estimates. ECLS-B field staff individually administered measures of children’s cognitive and academic functioning and conducted interviews with family members, when the children were 9, 24, 48, and 60 months of age. Kindergarten teachers rated the children’s behaviors at school entry.
We identified three analytical samples. For the 0-to-24-month analyses, children with missing data on the study’s 24-month oral vocabulary measure were excluded, as were children with congenital anomalies. We used multiple imputation (MI) procedures to account for missing data for predictor variables but not for the oral vocabulary variable in the remaining cases. Specifically, we used Imputation and Variance Estimation Software or IVEware (http://www.isr.umich.edu/src/smp/ive/) to repeatedly replace missing values with predictions based on random draws from the posterior distributions of observed sample parameters, which results in multiple complete data sets (Raghunathan, Solenberger, & van Hoewyk, 2002). We averaged results obtained across five different imputation samples to account for random variations in the datasets derived from MI (Raghunathan et al. 2002). This resulted in an analytical sample of 8,650 with data for analyses evaluating factors predictive of 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies. For the 24-to-60 month analyses of reading and mathematics achievement, we again multiply imputed the predictor variables but excluded cases with missing data on 60-month reading or mathematics achievement measures (n = 6,050). For the 24-to-60 month analyses of behavioral functioning, we multiply imputed the predictor variables but excluded cases with missing data on the behavioral measures (n = 4,350). (All reported sample sizes are rounded to the nearest 50 to comply with National Center for Education Statistics [NCES], U.S. Department of Education requirements for data reporting.) Table 1 displays weighted percentages or Ms or SDs for the variables. The Ms of the variables are quite similar across the three samples, suggesting that the samples are comparable despite varying in size due to missing dependent variable data.
Table 1.
Weighted Percentages or Means with Standard Deviations of the Study’s Three Analytical Samples
Variables | Sample 1 | Sample 2 | Sample 3 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total Word score | 24 months | 28.57 (11.83) | 28.84 (11.79) | 28.72 (12.00) |
Reading Test score | 60 months | 38.39 (14.83) | 38.35 (14.75) | 38.55 (14.86) |
Mathematics Test score | 60 months | 40.17 (10.56) | 40.24 (10.53) | 40.49 (10.38) |
Approaches to learning | 24 months | 13.92 (3.56) | 13.98 (3.53) | 13.97 (3.59) |
60 months | 18.53 (2.49) | 18.51 (2.50) | 18.50 (2.50) | |
Internalizing problems | 24 months | 4.41 (1.83) | 4.35 (1.80) | 4.40 (1.83) |
60 months | 6.31 (2.19) | 6.33 (2.20) | 6.32 (2.20) | |
Externalizing problems | 24 months | 4.87 (1.75) | 4.84 (1.73) | 4.86 (1.76) |
60 months | 6.96 (3.07) | 7.01 (3.10) | 7.02 (3.12) | |
Race | White | 54.2 % | 53.9 % | 54.4 % |
Black | 13.6 % | 14.0 % | 13.0 % | |
Hispanic | 24.8 % | 25.0 % | 25.5 % | |
Asian | 2.7 % | 2.6 % | 2.6 % | |
Native American | 0.5 % | 0.5 % | 0.5 % | |
Other race | 4.2 % | 4.1 % | 4.0 % | |
Age | 24 months | 24.16 (0.70) | 24.15 (0.69) | 24.15 (0.68) |
Gender | Male | 51.1 % | 50.6 % | 51.5 % |
SES quintiles | Lowest | 19.3 % | 19.1 % | 18.4 % |
Second lowest | 19.6 % | 20.0 % | 20.2 % | |
Middle | 20.0 % | 20.6 % | 21.0 % | |
Second highest | 20.7 % | 20.9 % | 20.8 % | |
Highest | 20.3 % | 19.4 % | 19.6 % | |
Non-singleton | 3.3 % | 3.2 % | 3.3 % | |
Mother’s age at child’s birth | Older than 35 | 14.0 % | 13.7 % | 13.5 % |
Younger or equal to 18 | 6.9 % | 7.3 % | 7.4 % | |
Marital status | Not married | 31.4 % | 31.2 % | 31.4 % |
Primary language | Non-English | 18.6 % | 18.0 % | 18.1 % |
Birth weight | Very low | 1.2 % | 1.2 % | 1.2 % |
Moderately low | 6.2 % | 6.2 % | 6.2 % | |
Labor complications | 35.5 % | 34.8 % | 35.0 % | |
Obstetric procedures | 58.5 % | 57.5 % | 56.7 % | |
Risks | Medical | 18.1 % | 17.7 % | 17.9 % |
Behavioral | 11.1 % | 10.8 % | 10.7 % | |
Parenting score | 24 months | 7.32 (1.15) | 7.33 (1.14) | 7.34 (1.14) |
Family member status | Mental illness | 10.7 % | 11.1 % | 10.7 % |
Learning disability | 15.3 % | 15.4 % | 15.7 % | |
Maternal status | Health problems | 7.3 % | 7.2 % | 7.0 % |
Household status | Special need | 7.4 % | 7.9 % | 7.8 % |
Mother depressed | 13.7 % | 13.9 % | 14.7 % | |
Stayed in day care center | > 10 hours/week | 14.0 % | 13.5 % | 13.8 % |
Child television usage | Middle third | 34.4 % | 33.9 % | 34.5 % |
Highest third | 32.6 % | 33.0 % | 32.5 % | |
Mother isolated | 0.16 (0.44) | 0.15 (0.44) | 0.15 (0.45) | |
Bayley Mental score | 24 months | 126.60 (10.49) | 126.86 (10.33) | 127.11 (10.67) |
Note. Sample size rounded to nearest 50 per ECLS-B confidentiality requirements. Sample 1 N = 8650, Sample 2 N = 6050, Sample 3 N = 4350. SES = socioeconomic status.
Measures, Criterion Variables
Oral vocabulary, 24 months
Oral vocabulary size at 24 months was assessed using a modified version of the MacArthur-Communicative Development Inventory (M-CDI; Fenson et al., 1993) developed specifically for the ECLS-B by the CDI Advisory Board. This measure is a parent survey of children’s expressive use of 50 words commonly known and spoken in the targeted age range. An equivalent list of Spanish words was provided for Spanish-speaking parents; non-Spanish-speaking language minority parents were not included in the analyses. Internal consistency for the original M-CDI vocabulary scales is high (α = .96). The M-CDI displays construct, concurrent, and predictive validity (Feldman et al., 2005; Fenson et al. 1994) and classifies children into language status groups with 97% accuracy (Skarakis-Doyle, Cambell, & Dempsey, 2009). The number of words spoken by children as identified by parents was summed to create the Total Word Score at 24 months. The M of this variable was about 29 words, with a SD of about 12. (Because this score was obtained using the shortened version of the M-CDI, it is not directly comparable to the 24-month M number of words spoken measured by the original M-CDI).
Reading and mathematics achievement, 60 months
The ECLS-B Reading Test consisted of 55 items designed to assess language development, emergent literacy, and basic reading. The Mathematics Test included 42 items designed to assess number sense, counting, operations, geometry, patterns, and measurement. Each measure consisted of a two-stage routing procedure and item response theory (IRT) scaling. All children were initially given the same 24-question test. Then, and depending on the number of correct responses on this test, they were administered one of three follow-up routing test forms on the basis of scoring in the low, middle, or high range of the initial test. The Reading and the Mathematics Tests display IRT theta reliability coefficients of .92, indicating high reliability of the assessment scores (Najarian, Snow, Lennon, Kinsey & Mulligan, 2010).
Behavioral self-regulation, externalizing problem behaviors, and internalizing problem behaviors, 60 months
Teachers rated children’s behavioral functioning using items from the Preschool Learning and Behavior Scales (2nd ed.), the Social Skills Rating System, and the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998–1999. Teachers rated the children’s frequency of behaviors using a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). We conducted an exploratory factor analysis, using a promax rotation, of 22-rated behaviors, retaining a four-factor solution after examining several potential factor solutions and considering a priori criteria. Items with factor loadings lower than .60 were removed. We identified three of the four factors as relevant to this study: (a) approaches to learning, (b) internalizing problem behaviors, and (c) externalizing problem behaviors. The Approaches to Learning scale (α = .91) contains five items measuring behavioral self-regulation (e.g., “pays attention well,” “keeps working until finished,” and [reverse scaled] “has difficulty concentrating”). The Internalizing Problem Behaviors scale (α = .64) contains three items related to anxious or withdrawn behaviors (i.e. “seems unhappy,” “worries about things,” and “acts shy”). The Externalizing Problem Behavior scale (α = .87) contains four items related to acting-out behaviors (e.g., “disrupts others,” “has temper tantrums,” “is physically aggressive”). Items were summed to obtain scale scores. Higher Approaches to Learning scale scores indicated more appropriate classroom-based behavioral self-regulation; higher Internalizing or Externalizing Problem Behaviors scale scores indicated more frequent problem behaviors.
Measures, Predictor Variables
Socio-demographic characteristics
Socio-demographic data were collected in parent interviews at each assessment, as well as from birth certificates. Race or ethnicity was defined as the race or ethnicity of the mother from birth certificates. Non-Hispanic White was the reference group; other groups included non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, Asian, Native American, and Other. Child age in months was included to account for variations in actual age at the time of assessment. Female was the reference gender category. ECLS-B staff calculated family SES using a composite of five parent-reported indicators that included the father/male guardian’s education and occupation, the mother/female guardian’s education and occupation, and household income. NCES estimated missing values, which averaged 2.5% across the five SES indicators, using hot deck imputation. The SES distribution was divided into quintiles and represented in regressions as a set of dummy variables. The first quintile represented the lowest SES. Children born individually were considered singletons; all others were coded as non-singletons. Dummy variables for whether the mother was over 35 years of age or less than or equal to 18 years of age were also included. Being married was used as the reference category for marital status. Children living in households where English was not the primary language spoken were identified using a dichotomous variable.
Birth characteristics from birth certificates
A count of medical conditions in pregnancy included incompetent cervix, acute or chronic lung disease, chronic hypertension, pregnancy-induced hypertension, eclampsia, diabetes, hemoglobinopathy, cardiac disease, anemia, renal disease, genital herpes, oligohydramnios, uterine bleeding, Rh sensitization, previous birth weighing 4,000 + g, or previous preterm birth. Behavioral risk factors during pregnancy included any maternal use of alcohol and/or tobacco during pregnancy (coded as 1 if present and summed to form a scale that ranged from 0 to 2). Obstetrical procedures were measured as a count of total procedures including induction of labor, stimulation of labor, tocolysis, amniocentesis, and cesarean section. Labor complications were measured as a count of the following: abruption placenta; anesthetic complications; dysfunctional labor; breech/malpresentation; cephalopelvic disproportion; cord prolapse; fetal distress; excessive bleeding; fever of > 100°F; moderate/heavy meconium; precipitous labor (< 3 h); prolonged labor (> 24 h); placental previa or seizures during labor. Two indicator variables were used to quantify very low (<1,500 grams) and moderately low (1,500–2,500 grams) birthweight. Normal (>2,500 grams) birthweight was the reference category.
Parenting quality
We averaged scores on two assessments conducted at 24 months to create a composite parenting score. The first assessment included items from the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) (Bradley & Caldwell, 1984), a widely used measure of the quality of the child’s parenting and the home environment (e.g., NICHD Early Childcare Research Network, 2005). NCES retained a subset of the original measure’s 21 items measuring (a) parental activities including reading to the child, telling stories, singing, and taking the child on errands or to public places; (b) having toys, records, books, and audiotapes available in the home; and (c) having a safe and supportive home environment. Because the modified HOME scale had relatively low internal consistency (α = .46), the ECLS-B manual advises researchers to use alternatives other than scaling the items. We therefore used a count of 14 HOME scores items to indicate the extent to which these positive activities were reported or observed.
The second parenting assessment consisted of ratings of the quality of a parent’s interactive support of their child, as coded from videotaped interactions during the Two Bags Task, a simplified version of the Three Bags Task used in the Early Head Start Research and Evaluation Project and the NICHD Study of Early Child Care (Nord, Edwards, Andreassen, Green, & Wallner-Allen, 2006). Interviewers read a script to the children’s parents, after which, over the next 10 minutes, parents were asked to play with their children using a wordless picture book (i.e., Goodnight Gorilla) and a set of toy dishes. A composite variable measuring parental support was created for the ECLS-B representing the mean of three characteristics of parental interactions with their children: (a) sensitivity; (b) stimulation of cognitive development; and (c) positive regard. Each item was scored on a 7-point scale, ranging from 1 = very low to 7 = very high. Mean inter-rater reliability for the parent rating scales was 97%, with mean reliabilities of 97%, 93%, and 94% for sensitivity, cognitive stimulation, and positive regard, respectively (see Andreassen, Fletcher, & Park, 2007 for additional measurement detail).
History of mental or physical illness or disabling conditions
Family member with mental illness. Mothers were asked at the 9-month survey whether they or a family member had “a serious mental illness, such as schizophrenia, a paranoid disorder, a bipolar disorder, or manic episodes?” “Yes” answers were coded as 1; “no” answers were coded as 0. Family member with learning disability. Mothers were also asked at 9 months, “Have you or any of your blood relatives ever had a learning disability?” “Yes” answers were coded as 1; “no” answers were coded as 0. Maternal health problems. During the 9-month wave, mothers rated their overall health on a scale of 1–5. The responses were transformed into a dichotomous variable, with responses of 4 (“fair”) or 5 (“poor”) coded as 1. All other responses (e.g., “excellent”) were coded as “0”. Special needs. Mothers responded during the 9-month survey whether they or any other household members had a special need, delay or disability. “Yes” responses were coded as 1; “no” responses were coded as 0. Maternal depression. A modified version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression (CES-D) Scale (Radloff, 1977) was used as part of the 9-month parent self-administered questionnaire. The modified CES-D Scale includes 12 items including having poor appetite, feeling lonely, and trouble keeping focus. Scores were dichotomized so that a total score greater than 9 was coded as 1 for presence of depressive symptoms, corresponding to the cutoff commonly used for the full CES-D of greater than 15 (Nord et al., 2006). Maternal social isolation. Five variables were used to construct a scale of maternal social isolation. Four of these variables asked the mother if she had people available to ask for help for various needs (1 = “no one,” for each variable). The other variable asked about being close to her own mother (1 = “not close”). The sum of these five variables was calculated and used as a measure of social isolation.
Hours per week in child care center
During the 24-month survey, parents were asked how many hours a week their child spends in a child care center. Responses to this question were coded dichotomously so that a response of 11 hours or more was coded as 1.
Television usage
Average weekly television usage was calculated based on a parent’s response at 24 months about the child’s average number of hours spent watching television or videos in the household during weekdays and weekends. Less than 9 hours was considered low television usage, between 9–17 hours was considered medium television usage, and more than 17 hours was considered high television usage.
Cognitive functioning
A standardized assessment was administered at the 24-month survey wave to measure children’s general cognitive functioning. Field staff individually administered the Bayley Short Form-Research Edition (BSF-R), a modified version of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd Edition (BSID-II; Bayley, 1993), which is designed for use with children from birth to 36 months of age. In both the BSID-II and the BSF-R, the mental score is based on the trained interviewer’s assessment of a child’s ability to perform tasks related to memory, habituation, preverbal communication, problem solving, and concept attainment. The IRT reliability coefficient of the BSF-R mental scale at 24 months was .88 (Andreassen et al., 2007). The reported R2 between BSF-R and BSID-II scores was .99, indicating that the BSF-R maintains the psychometric properties of the BSID-II and accurately measures children’s performance across the ability distribution (Andreassen et al., 2007).
Behavioral self-regulation, externalizing problem behaviors, and internalizing problem behaviors, 24 months
ECLS-B trained examiners used the Behavior Rating Scale-Research Edition (BRS-R) to rate children’s behaviors as they completed BSF-R tasks during the 24-month survey wave. The BRS-R was adapted from the Behavior Rating Scale (BRS; Bayley, 1993). Scores on the BRS correlate moderately-to-highly with scores on other measures of young children’s socio-emotional adjustment (Buck, 1997). The BRS-R included 11 interviewer-rated items from the full BRS measuring developmentally appropriate behaviors for 24-month-old children including their attention to task, persistence, cooperation with an examiner, interest in the testing materials, and frustration with testing tasks (Nord et al., 2006). A 5-point scale was used to rate the frequency of the observed behavior. A higher score indicated that the problem behavior occurred infrequently. The self-regulatory items on the BRS (e.g. attention to task, persistence) have an internal consistency of .92.
We used eight items from the BRS-R to control for prior behavioral functioning. Four items measured children’s prior behavioral self-regulation (e.g., “pays attention to tasks,” “is persistent in tasks”). Two items measured prior externalizing behaviors (i.e., “frustration in tasks,” “cooperation”). Two items measured prior internalizing problem behaviors (i.e., “fearlessness,” “social engagement”). Specific items were reverse coded as appropriate to be consistent with either appropriate (i.e., for behavioral self-regulation) or problematic (i.e., for externalizing or internalizing) behavioral functioning. Cronbach alphas were .90, .64, and .72 for the 24-month behavioral self-regulation, externalizing problem behaviors, and internalizing problem behaviors measures, respectively.
Analytical Methods
The score distribution of the measure of 24 month-old children’s parent-reported oral vocabulary generally resembled a normal curve, with a truncation point at 50 words (i.e., the maximum possible score). There are 280 cases out of about 9,500 (i.e., 2.9%) at this truncation point. To avoid bias due to truncation at the upper limit of the variable (ceiling effect), we used Tobit regression analysis (Breen, 1996) when predicting children’s scores on the oral vocabulary measure. Our first set of analyses estimated six Tobit regression models predicting 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies. We entered these sets of predictors sequentially. As shown in Table 2, Model 1 estimated to what extent children’s or families’ socio-demographic characteristics functioned as predictors. The socio-demographic variables included children’s race or ethnicity, age, gender, and SES status. Model 2 added additional socio-demographic variables to the regression equation. Model 3 added the children’s gestational and birth characteristics. Model 4 entered predictors related to family risk and resilience. Model 5 added children’s behavioral functioning; Model 6 added their cognitive functioning. Estimating these models sequentially allowed us to investigate whether there were significant direct effects of these groupings of variables on 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies, as well as whether these groupings of variables explained the direct effects of the previously entered variables.
Table 2.
Parameter Estimates (standardized) of Tobit Regression Equations Predicting Children’s Oral Vocabularies at 24 Months, ECLS-B Data (N = 8,650)
Variables | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | Model 5 | Model 6 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 0.92*** | 0.98*** | 0.96*** | 0.75*** | 0.69*** | 0.26*** | |
Race | Black | −0.02 | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.17*** | 0.16*** | 0.25*** |
Hispanic | −0.17*** | −0.12** | −0.12** | −0.06 | −0.06 | 0.05 | |
Asian | −0.13 | −0.05 | −0.05 | 0.12 | 0.13 | 0.11 | |
Native American | −0.15 | −0.16 | −0.16 | −0.07 | −0.04 | 0.16 | |
Other race | −0.06 | −0.08 | −0.06 | −0.0001 | 0.02 | 0.08 | |
Age | 24 months | 0.19*** | 0.18*** | 0.18*** | 0.18*** | 0.17*** | 0.08*** |
Gender | Male | −0.52*** | −0.51*** | −0.52*** | −0.49*** | −0.44*** | −0.26*** |
SES quintile | Lowest | −0.55*** | −0.57*** | −0.54*** | −0.22*** | −0.21*** | −0.08* |
Second lowest | −0.44*** | −0.47*** | −0.45*** | −0.2*** | −0.22*** | −0.06 | |
Middle | −0.32*** | −0.34*** | −0.33*** | −0.17*** | −0.18*** | −0.04 | |
Second highest | −0.21*** | −0.22*** | −0.22*** | −0.13** | −0.12** | −0.02 | |
Non-singleton | −0.47*** | −0.30*** | −0.28*** | −0.24*** | −0.13* | ||
Mother’s age at child’s birth | Older than 35 | −0.13*** | −0.13** | −0.14*** | −0.14*** | −0.11*** | |
Younger or equal to 18 | −0.05 | −0.04 | −0.03 | −0.01 | −0.05 | ||
Marital status | Not married | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.08* | 0.08** | |
Primary language | Non-English | −0.12* | −0.12** | −0.05 | −0.05 | 0.10* | |
Birth weight | Very low | −0.88*** | −0.87*** | −0.83*** | −0.39*** | ||
Moderately low | −0.24*** | −0.23*** | −0.21*** | −0.05 | |||
Labor complications | Labor complications | 0.01 | 0.002 | 0.01 | 0.001 | ||
Obstetric procedures | Obstetric procedures | 0.05** | 0.05* | 0.05* | 0.04** | ||
Risks | Medical | −0.04 | −0.03 | 0.01 | −0.03 | ||
Behavioral | −0.06 | −0.04 | −0.01 | 0.01 | |||
Parenting score at 24 months | 0.28*** | 0.24*** | 0.10*** | ||||
Family member status | Mental illness | −0.12** | −0.12** | −0.09 | |||
Learning disability | −0.03 | −0.02 | 0.03 | ||||
Maternal status | Health problems | −0.27*** | −0.24*** | −0.15*** | |||
Household status | Special need | −0.16*** | −0.14** | −0.05 | |||
Mother depressed | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.02 | ||||
Stayed in day care center | > 10 hours/week | 0.12** | 0.12** | 0.03 | |||
Child television usage | Middle third | 0.01 | 0.10 | −0.01 | |||
Highest third | −0.11*** | −0.09** | −0.04 | ||||
Mother isolated | −0.03* | −0.03* | −0.02 | ||||
Approaches to learning | 24 months | 0.62*** | 0.20*** | ||||
Internalizing problems | 24 months | −0.27*** | −0.08** | ||||
Externalizing problems | 24 months | −0.30*** | −0.07 | ||||
General cognitive functioning | 24 months | 0.61*** |
Note. Sample size rounded to nearest 50 per ECLS-B confidentiality requirements. SES = socioeconomic status.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001.
Our second and third set of analyses investigated whether and to what extent, and before and after extensive statistical control, having a larger oral vocabulary at 24 months of age predicted children’s academic and behavioral functioning at 60 months of age. We conducted separate ordinary least squares regression analyses predicting each of the five criterion variables. Predictor variables in these regression analyses included oral vocabulary, socio-demographic and birth characteristics, family risk and resilience characteristics, and children’s cognitive and behavioral functioning at 24 months of age. We used SAS 9.3 to perform the analyses. We incorporated sampling weights and design effects to account for oversampling of some population subgroups and for the stratified cluster design of the ECLS-B. We used an alpha of p<.05 in establishing statistical significance including for our primary analyses (Keppel, 1991; Moyé).5
Results
Which U.S. Children Display Larger or Smaller Oral Vocabularies at 24 Months Of Age?
Table 2 displays results from Tobit regressions predicting children’s oral vocabularies at 24 months of age. All continuous variables (including the dependent variable) were standardized. Thus, all coefficients represent directly comparable standardized coefficients (i.e., effect sizes). The first column (Model 1) shows the very strong and significant effects of gender and SES. Boys averaged .52 SD fewer words spoken than girls. This gap is similar in magnitude to the vocabulary gap by U.S. children in the lowest and highest SES quintiles. (Thus, a boy toddler from the lowest SES quintile, on average, experienced a double decrement summing to somewhat more than 1 SD.) The association between SES quintiles and words spoken was approximately linear, with a difference of about .10 SD for each successive quintile. In contrast, there was only an inconsistent association between children’s race or ethnicity and oral vocabularies.
The second column shows that, among additional socio-demographics, the largest association with 24 month-old children’s oral vocabularies involved the .47 SD decrement experienced by non-singletons. Although this association was reduced in size in subsequent models, it remained significant. A modest (.13 SD) but significant decrement was observed for the children of older mothers. A decrement of .12 was observed for children from homes where English was not the primary spoken language. Controlling for this language variable also partially explained the negative relation between children’s Hispanic status and their oral vocabulary size.
The third column adds gestational and birth characteristics as predictors. There was a very strong negative predictive relation (−.88 SD) between very low birth weight and 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies. The relation with moderately low birth weight was smaller (−.24 SD) but also statistically significant. The effects of labor complications, obstetric procedures, and the mother’s medical and behavioral risks during pregnancy were small. Including these covariates in the regression equation reduced the predicted effect of being a non-singleton by about one-third. Thus, a portion of this relation appeared to be due to the relatively lower birth weights of non-singletons. The fourth column displays the results of adding family risk and resilience factors to the regression equation. Two resilience factors—quality parenting and day care attendance—were both significantly associated with 24-month-old children having larger oral vocabularies, with predicted effect sizes of .28 and .12 SD, respectively. Among the risk factors, several were also significantly and negatively associated with oral vocabulary. These included a family member with a mental illness (−.12 SD), maternal health problems (−.27 SD), a household member with special needs (−.16 SD), and the child being in the highest third of television usage (−.11 SD). These factors helped explain the predicted effect of SES. Following their statistical control, the association between each SES quintile and children’s oral vocabularies decreased by about 50% but remained significant.
The fifth and sixth columns of Table 2 show the results of adding the 24-month behavioral and general cognitive functioning measures into the regression equation. The three behavioral measures were associated in expected directions with oral vocabulary, so that 24-month-old children who displayed more attentive, task persistent, and other types of learning-related behaviors also displayed larger oral vocabularies (.62 SD), while those children with stronger internalizing or externalizing problem behaviors displayed smaller oral vocabularies (−.27 and −.30 SD, respectively).
Because the measures of general cognitive functioning oral vocabulary were both language dependent, we expected to find a strong association between the two variables. As shown in Model 6, the relation was indeed very strong and significant (.61 SD). However, even with these behavioral and general cognitive functioning controls added to the regression equation, most of the associations remained statistically significant despite decreasing in magnitude. The exception was family SES, suggesting that the relation between SES and 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies was partially accounted for by other risk factors. These included being a raised by a single mother, being born with low birth weight, experiencing less warm or cognitively stimulating parenting, being raised in a household with a socially isolated mother or one who has health problems, and, mostly strongly, by children’s own level of general cognitive and behavioral functioning. However, being in the lowest SES quintile remained significantly negatively related to children’s oral vocabulary size despite extensive statistical control. Oral vocabulary at 24 months functioned somewhat independently of general cognitive functioning, which is a conclusion that is also supported by results in the study’s second set of analyses.
Does Having a Larger Oral Vocabulary at 24 Months of Age Uniquely Predict Greater Academic Functioning at Kindergarten Entry?
Table 3 displays the results of using 24-month-old children’s oral vocabulary, as well as the full set of additional risk and resilience factors just presented, to predict their academic functioning at kindergarten entry. The first model of Table 3 displays the unadjusted estimate between oral vocabulary and reading or mathematics achievement. Then the second model adds control variables to the regression equation. The third column adds general cognitive functioning as a control, allowing for a highly conservative test of the hypothesized relation. Oral vocabulary size initially predicted 24-month-old children’s reading and mathematics achievement at kindergarten entry. These predicted effect sizes were .22 and .27 SDs, respectively.
Table 3.
Parameter Estimates (standardized) of OLS Regressions Predicting Children’s Academic Achievement at 60 months, ECLS-B Data (N=6,050)
Reading achievement | Mathematics achievement | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|||||||
Variables | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |
Intercept | −0.07** | 0.37*** | 0.33*** | −0.05* | 0.38*** | 0.31*** | |
Oral vocabulary | 24 months | 0.22*** | 0.12*** | 0.08*** | 0.27*** | 0.17*** | 0.10*** |
Race | Black | −0.02 | 0.01 | −0.20*** | −0.17** | ||
Hispanic | −0.04 | −0.02 | −0.14* | −0.11 | |||
Asian | 0.46*** | 0.46*** | 0.30*** | 0.31*** | |||
Native American | −0.19* | −0.16 | −0.36*** | −0.30** | |||
Other race | −0.08 | −0.06 | −0.14 | −0.12 | |||
Child age | 24 months | 0.14*** | 0.13*** | 0.12*** | 0.11*** | ||
Gender | Male | −0.10** | −0.08* | 0.01 | 0.03 | ||
SES quintile | Lowest | −0.78*** | −0.77*** | −0.77*** | −0.75*** | ||
Second lowest | −0.59*** | −0.57*** | −0.59*** | −0.57*** | |||
Middle | −0.39*** | −0.38*** | −0.40*** | −0.38*** | |||
Second highest | −0.24*** | −0.23*** | −0.23*** | −0.22*** | |||
Non-singleton | −0.08* | −0.08* | −0.10* | −0.09* | |||
Mother’s age at child’s birth | Older than 35 | −0.01 | −0.02 | 0.03 | 0.02 | ||
Younger or equal to 18 | −0.10 | −0.11 | −0.11* | −0.12* | |||
Marital status | Not married | −0.07 | −0.07 | −0.04 | −0.04 | ||
Primary language | Non-English | −0.03 | −0.01 | −0.002 | 0.03 | ||
Birth weight | Very low | −0.10* | −0.05 | −0.28*** | −0.21*** | ||
Moderately low | −0.03 | −0.01 | −0.07 | −0.04 | |||
Labor complications | −0.06 | −0.05 | −0.002 | −0.003 | |||
Obstetric procedures | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.02 | |||
Risks | Medical | −0.07* | −0.07* | −0.004 | −0.01 | ||
Behavioral | −0.06 | −0.05 | −0.06 | −0.04 | |||
Parenting score | 24 months | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.05* | 0.02 | ||
Family member status | Mental illness | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.01 | ||
Learning disability | −0.07 | −0.07 | −0.10* | −0.09* | |||
Maternal status | Health problems | −0.03 | −0.02 | −0.08 | −0.07 | ||
Household status | Special need | 0.02 | 0.02 | −0.02 | −0.01 | ||
Mother depressed | −0.02 | −0.03 | −0.04 | −0.05 | |||
Stayed in day care center | > 10 hours/week | 0.04 | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.04 | ||
Child television usage | Middle third | 0.10* | 0.09* | 0.11** | 0.10** | ||
Highest third | 0.10* | 0.11** | 0.11** | 0.12** | |||
Mother isolated | −0.02 | −0.02 | −0.05*** | −0.05** | |||
Approaches to learning | 24 months | 0.18** | 0.12* | 0.14** | 0.06 | ||
Internalizing problems | 24 months | 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 0.05 | ||
Externalizing problems | 24 months | −0.05 | −0.01 | −0.06 | −0.02 | ||
General cognitive functioning | 24 months | 0.12*** | 0.16*** |
Note. Sample size rounded to nearest 50 per ECLS-B confidentiality requirements. SES = socioeconomic status.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001.
Model 2 shows that despite statistical control for a wide range of covariates, having a larger oral vocabulary at 24 months of age remained positively and significantly predictive of greater academic functioning at kindergarten entry. Model 3’s results indicate that even after controlling for 24-month-old children’s general cognitive functioning, having a larger oral vocabulary positively and significantly predicted children’s later reading and mathematics achievement. The adjusted effect sizes were .08 and .10 SD for reading and mathematics achievement, respectively.
Table 3 identifies additional factors consistently predictive of greater academic functioning. These included being of White or Asian race/ethnicity, being from a high SES family, being born as a singleton, watching television more frequently, and displaying higher levels of general cognitive functioning. However, 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies remained uniquely predictive of their academic functioning at kindergarten entry despite the large number of controls. We also investigated (results not shown) the possibility that SES moderated the predicted effect of oral vocabulary on academic functioning (as well as behavioral functioning). We did so by adding interaction terms between the four SES dummy variables and oral vocabulary to the regression equations. These interaction terms were not statistically significant.
Does Having a Larger Oral Vocabulary At 24 Months of Age Uniquely Predict Greater Behavioral Functioning at Kindergarten Entry?
Table 4 displays regressions predicting teacher ratings of kindergarten children’s behavioral self-regulation, as well as their internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors. Having a larger oral vocabulary significantly predicted all three indicators of behavioral functioning in Model 1. Specifically, 24-month-old children with larger oral vocabularies displayed greater behavioral self-regulation (.22 SD) and fewer internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors at kindergarten entry (−.11 and −.14 SD, respectively). Adding Model 2 and 3’s controls to the regression equation reduced oral vocabulary’s predictive relations with the three indicators of children’s behavioral functioning but the relations remained statistically significant. Having a larger oral vocabulary predicted greater behavioral self-regulation (.10 SD), as well as fewer internalizing (−.09 SD) and externalizing problem behaviors (−.06 SD) even following statistical control for autoregressive behavioral functioning. Other less consistent predictors of children’s behavioral functioning included being a boy, being raised by a single mother, and frequently attending childcare.
Table 4.
Parameter Estimates (standardized) of OLS Regressions Predicting Children’s Behavioral Functioning at 60 months, ECLS-B Data (N = 4,350).
Approaches to learning | Internalizing problems | Externalizing problems | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||||||||
Variables | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |
Intercept | 0.02 | 0.34*** | 0.30*** | −0.03 | −0.17* | −0.15 | 0.03 | −0.42*** | −0.40*** | |
Oral vocabulary | 24 months | 0.22*** | 0.15*** | 0.10*** | −0.11*** | −0.10*** | −0.09** | −0.14*** | −0.07*** | −0.06* |
Race | Black | 0.05 | 0.08 | −0.15* | −0.16* | 0.04 | 0.03 | |||
Hispanic | 0.04 | 0.06 | −0.04 | −0.05 | −0.13* | −0.14* | ||||
Asian | −0.06 | −0.05 | 0.03 | 0.02 | −0.16* | −0.16* | ||||
Native American | −0.02 | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.32 | 0.31 | ||||
Other race | −0.002 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.03 | −0.03 | −0.03 | ||||
Child age | 24 month | −0.03 | −0.03 | 0.05* | 0.06* | 0.003 | 0.01 | |||
Gender | Male | −0.22*** | −0.21*** | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.42*** | 0.42*** | |||
SES quintile | Lowest | −0.16 | −0.15 | 0.18* | 0.19* | 0.11 | 0.11 | |||
Second lowest | −0.20* | −0.18* | 0.22** | 0.22** | 0.14 | 0.14 | ||||
Middle | −0.12* | −0.11 | 0.26*** | 0.25*** | 0.06 | 0.05 | ||||
Second highest | −0.05 | −0.04 | 0.10 | 0.10 | 0.04 | 0.03 | ||||
Non-singleton | 0.05 | 0.06 | −0.04 | −0.04 | −0.05 | −0.05 | ||||
Mother’s age at child’s birth | Older than 35 | −0.02 | −0.03 | 0.01 | 0.01 | −0.01 | −0.001 | |||
Younger than or equal to 18 | −0.08 | −0.09 | −0.03 | −0.03 | 0.04 | 0.04 | ||||
Marital status | Not married | −0.21** | −0.21** | −0.05 | −0.05 | 0.23*** | 0.23** | |||
Primary language | Non-English | 0.02 | 0.04 | −0.14 | −0.15 | −0.11 | −0.12 | |||
Birth weight | Very low | −0.24*** | −0.19** | −0.01 | −0.03 | −0.06 | −0.07 | |||
Moderately low | −0.06 | −0.04 | 0.001 | −0.005 | −0.03 | −0.04 | ||||
Labor complications | −0.01 | −0.01 | 0.11** | 0.11** | 0.02 | 0.02 | ||||
Obstetric procedures | −0.04 | −0.04 | 0.001 | −0.0001 | −0.02 | −0.02 | ||||
Risks | Medical | −0.08 | −0.08 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.13* | 0.12* | |||
Behavioral | −0.13 | −0.12 | −0.06 | −0.07 | 0.17 | 0.17 | ||||
Parenting score | 24 months | 0.08** | 0.06* | −0.01 | −0.005 | −0.04 | −0.03 | |||
Family member status | Mental illness | −0.15 | −0.14 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.13 | 0.13 | |||
Learning disability | −0.02 | −0.01 | 0.11 | 0.11 | 0.11 | 0.11 | ||||
Maternal health problems | −0.13 | −0.13 | 0.22* | 0.22* | −0.03 | −0.03 | ||||
Household status | Special need | −0.06 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.06 | |||
Mother depressed | −0.11 | −0.11 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.15* | 0.15* | ||||
Stayed in day care center | > 10 hours/week | −0.14* | −0.15* | −0.06 | −0.06 | 0.30*** | 0.31*** | |||
Child television usage | Middle third | 0.10* | 0.10* | −0.03 | −0.03 | −0.004 | −0.003 | |||
Highest third | 0.09 | 0.10* | −0.10 | −0.11 | −0.05 | −0.05 | ||||
Mother isolated | −0.04* | −0.04* | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.04 | ||||
Approaches to learning | 24 months | 0.12* | 0.06 | |||||||
Internalizing problems | 24 months | 0.15* | 0.14 | |||||||
Externalizing problems | 24 months | 0.27** | 0.25** | |||||||
General cognitive functioning | 24 months | 0.11** | −0.03 | −0.04 |
Note. Sample size rounded to nearest 50 per ECLS-B confidentiality requirements.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p< .001.
Discussion
Our analyses of a population-based dataset identify which groups of 24-month-old children in the U.S. are at risk of having smaller oral vocabularies. Our results are consistent with prior research indicating that children being raised in low SES families are likely to have smaller oral vocabularies (e.g., Fernald et al., 2013; Hart & Risley 1995). This specific relation is partially explained by a more general set of socio-demographic, gestational and birth, family risk and resilience, and individual characteristics, particularly children’s own levels of cognitive functioning. Other studies report that family stress and investment characteristics mediate the relation between lower SES and academic achievement, but the extent to which this occurs for oral vocabulary has been unclear (Farkas & Beron, 2004), in part due to a range of measures, sampling, and analytical methods used when examining SES (Hoff, Laursen, & Bridges, 2012). Our results indicate that family stress and investment characteristics partially explain the relation between family SES and children’s oral vocabulary size.
The results further indicate that 24-month-old children with smaller oral vocabularies are at risk of entering kindergarten with lower academic and behavioral functioning. Consistent with previously theorized mechanisms, oral vocabulary has both a general and unique relation with children’s development. The relation is evident across multiple indicators of both academic and behavioral functioning during early childhood. Twenty-four-month children’s oral vocabularies remain predictive of their academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry despite extensive statistical control for many factors previously identified as potential confounds.
Limitations
This study has at least five limitations. First, our measure of children’s oral vocabulary size is brief and relies on parental recall. We also used children’s scores on this measure to predict their performance on independently administered measures of academic and behavioral functioning three years later. The resulting measurement error, as well as multi-year time interval, should bias our estimates downward, possibly making them overly conservative. This type of limitation extends to the measure of general cognitive functioning. Because the general cognitive functioning measure was also administered orally and at times required verbal responses, this confound should be highly correlated with 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies. Consequently, use of a brief survey of parental report of 50 words spoken by their 24-month-old children, as well as statistical control for correlated verbal intelligence, may have resulted in very conservative estimates of oral vocabulary’s “true” relation with later academic and behavioral functioning. Second, and again due to the ECLS-B’s design and measurement limitations, we were unable to directly contrast oral vocabulary’s predictive utility against a variety of other early language or numeracy competencies (e.g., phonological awareness, number sense, working memory) that may also be predictive of later academic and behavioral functioning (e.g., Fitzpatrick & Pagani, 2012; Hulme, Bowyer-Crane, Carroll, Duff, & Snowling, 2012). However, many of these competencies should be correlated with other controls included in our study (e.g., SES, prior behavioral and general cognitive functioning) and so indirectly accounted for (Aarnoudse-Moen, Weisglas-Kuperus, Duivenvoorden, van Goudoever, & Oosterlan, 2013, see Appendix table). Third, a measure of expressive but not receptive vocabulary was available in the ECLS-B’s 24-month survey wave. Therefore, we were unable to directly contrast the relative contribution of expressive versus receptive vocabulary knowledge to children’s academic or behavioral functioning at school entry. However, strong positive correlations have been found between receptive and expressive vocabulary (e.g., r = .66; Sideridis & Simos, 2010), and Tomblin and Zhang’s (2006) large-scale investigation of young children’s language abilities yielded no evidence for a receptive-expressive vocabulary dissociation. Measuring children’s ability to speak words should also better index the relative size of their vocabularies than measuring their ability to comprehend spoken words (Sénéchal, et al., 2006). Fourth, although other studies indicate that academic and behavioral functioning by school entry contribute to long-term educational and societal opportunities (e.g., Chetty et al., 2011), we were unable to directly evaluate whether oral vocabulary continued to contribute to children’s academic and behavioral functioning as they progressed through elementary school. This is because the ECLS-B’s data collection ended at kindergarten entry. Fifth, the ECLS-B’s data collection did not allow us to report on the quality and quantity of children’s overall language exposure in the home. Language input in the home may also explain the relation between family SES and children’s oral vocabulary (Fernald et al., 2013; Hart & Risley, 1995).
Study’s Contributions and Implications
Oral vocabulary is theorized to contribute to many indicators of children’s academic and behavioral functioning, including reading and mathematics achievement, behavioral self-regulation, and frequency of externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors (e.g., LeFevre et al., 2010; Perfetti, 2014; Qi & Kaiser, 2004). Meaningful differences in children’s oral vocabularies have been reported to occur by 24 months of age (e.g., Fernald et al., 2013). These differences have been hypothesized to strongly contribute to later achievement gaps (Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff, 2013). Yet methodological and substantive limitations in existing work have been identified (NELP, 2008), and oral vocabulary has sometimes been reported to fail to uniquely predict children’s achievement (Schatschneider et al., 2004). The result is substantial ambiguity as to oral vocabulary’s theoretical and practical importance as a contributor to children’s academic and behavioral functioning, as well as whether early interventions—as is increasingly suggested (e.g., Dickinson et al., 2010; Lesaux, 2012)—should emphasize preventing or remediating early vocabulary gaps in order to accelerate at-risk children’s developmental trajectories.
Our study therefore has several theoretical and practical implications. We identify by the 24 month time period a wide range of socio-demographic, gestational and birth, family risk and resilience, and child-level factors associated with larger or smaller oral vocabularies that themselves may be included in early screening, monitoring, and intervention efforts. Consistent with prior research (e.g., Fernald et al., 2013; Hart & Risley, 1995), we initially find that family SES initially has a strong, consistent association with 24-month-old children’s oral vocabularies. However, and which has been not previously reported, we subsequently find that this association is partially explained by children’s own level of behavioral and general cognitive functioning in addition to other factors. Low SES in particular is increasingly recognized as being associated with fewer oral vocabulary building opportunities (Mani, Mullainathan, Shafir, & Zhao, 2013) and child-directed speech (Wesileder & Fernald, 2013). Our study’s findings of low SES’s continuing negative relation with oral vocabulary, as well as the uniquely positive relation observed for parenting behaviors that are cognitively stimulating, child-centered, and positive, are consistent with these theoretical accounts, and suggest that interventions may need to be specifically targeted to 24-month-old children being raised in disadvantaged home environments (Hart & Risley, 1995). Our findings are also consistent with other research indicating that being born with very low birth weight (Stolt, Haataja, Lapinleium, & Lehtonen, 2008) and being raised in a household with a mentally ill family member (Zajicek-Farber, 2010) may contribute to delays in oral vocabulary acquisition. The negative association with being a non-singleton is consistent with other work (Hillemeier et al., 2009), and may result from parents speaking fewer words to each child when they are raising two or more children of the same age. Our findings indicate that the negative association with mental illness may not be unique to maternal depression, which itself is explained by children’s own lower behavioral and general cognitive functioning, but may instead be related to a more general set of health and well-being indictors including a family member having a mental illness, being raised by a mother experiencing health problems, and being raised by a mother who feels socially isolated.
Our findings are consistent with prior work hypothesizing that a larger oral vocabulary helps children experience greater achievement and behavior (e.g., Perfetti, 2014), thereby increasing the children’s educational and societal opportunities as they age (e.g., Hoff, 2013; Lesaux, 2012). However, and across the study’s five indicators of academic and behavioral functioning, other factors were also predictive. Consistent with this other work, additional risk factors including SES, low birth weight, watching television frequently, frequently being in childcare (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2003), and children’s own prior level of general cognitive functioning. Our study’s results extend prior work by suggesting that attending childcare may also positively impact children’s oral vocabularies. Our findings are consistent with prior work suggesting that parents who are stressed, over-burdened, less engaged, and experiencing less social support may less frequently talk, read, or otherwise interact with their children (e.g., Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1995; Paulson, Keefe, & Leiferman, 2009), resulting in these children having smaller oral vocabularies.
Prior empirical studies have largely been limited to investigations of single developmental pathways between vocabulary and (a) reading achievement, (b) mathematics achievement, or (c) behavioral functioning. Our study extends the field’s knowledge base by rigorously establishing that oral vocabulary uniquely predicts multiple developmental pathways simultaneously, and that this is evident as children are making the major transition to formal schooling. Our finding that oral vocabulary displays both temporal precedence and continued predictive utility despite extensive covariate adjustment across such a diverse number of indicators of children’s development functions as a type of internal replication, providing empirical support to oral vocabulary’s theorized importance as a first order factor potentially causally related to major indicators of children’s development. Our study also provides additional empirical support for claims that preventing or reducing later academic achievement gaps and their sequela may necessitate special emphasis on increasing at-risk children’s oral vocabularies. Such efforts may need to occur before the children are 24 months old. This is because oral vocabulary gaps are evident even by this very early time period, and in turn consistently predict children’s academic and behavioral functioning as they begin kindergarten in the U.S.
Acknowledgments
Funding for this study was provided by the National Center for Special Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education (R324A120046). Infrastructure support was provided by the Pennsylvania State University’s Population Research Institute through funding from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health (R24HD041025-11). No official endorsement should thereby be inferred.
We thank Bruce Tomblin, Philip Dale, and Betsy Crais for their thoughtful suggestions on earlier versions of this manuscript.
Footnotes
Oral vocabulary is often described as consisting of two distinct types: (a) expressive vocabulary (i.e., words and phrases that children speak) and (b) receptive vocabulary (i.e., words and phrases that children understand). Yet theoretical and empirical work indicates that this dichotomy may be spurious (Leonard, 2009; Sideridis & Simos, 2010; Tomblin & Zhang, 2006). In keeping with this prior work, we avoid the possibility of artificially dichotomizing oral vocabulary as either expressive or receptive and instead use the general term “oral vocabulary”
This process is not necessarily contingent on the “vocabulary spurt,” which itself may not occur for most children (Ganger & Brent, 2004) and can be explained by non-specialized cognitive processes (McMurray, 2007).
The relation between oral vocabulary and decoding eventually becomes bidirectional, as independent reading results in new vocabulary acquisition, especially from fourth grade on.
This limitation also extends to vocabulary interventions evaluated through experimental or quasi-experimental designs. For example, the NLEP identified only three studies using dependent measures of readiness or reading (Table 7.1, p. 212). Instead, most intervention studies have only evaluated the impact of vocabulary interventions on the more proximal indicator of word learning (see Marulis & Neuman, 2010; 2013). Those few interventions evaluated on the basis of their impact on more distal indicators (e.g. reading comprehension) yield fairly small effect sizes (d = .10), particularly when evaluated using standardized measures (Elleman, Lindo, Morphy, & Compton, 2009).
As an additional robustness check we applied the Holm-Bonferroni procedure to adjust for family-wise error when establishing statistical significance of the predictors of the oral vocabulary Table 2, and the secondary factors (i.e., the control covariates) also predicting academic and behavioral readiness in Tables 3 and 4. Use of Holm-Bonferroni did not change the overall pattern of results although, not surprisingly, it reduced the statistical significance of some coefficients due to the highly conservative alpha levels. The results discussed in the text retained statistical significance even after the Holm-Bonferroni adjustment.
Contributor Information
Paul L. Morgan, The Pennsylvania State University
George Farkas, University of California, Irvine.
Marianne M. Hillemeier, The Pennsylvania State University
Carol Scheffner Hammer, Temple University.
Steve Maczuga, The Pennsylvania State University.
References
- Aarnoudse-Moen CSH, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Duivenvoorden HJ, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlan J. Executive function and IQ predict mathematical and attention problems in very preterm children. PLOS One. 2013;8:e55994. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreassen C, Fletcher P, Park J. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B): Psychometric report for the 2-year data collection. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Anthopolos R, Edwards SE, Miranda ML. Effects of maternal prenatal smoking and birth outcomes extending into the normal range on academic performance in fourth grade in North Carolina, USA. Pediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. 2013;27:564–574. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Benasich AA, Curtiss S, Tallal P. Language, learning and behavioural disturbances in childhood: A longitudinal perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1993;32:585–594. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199305000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Caldwell BM. The HOME inventory and family demographics. Developmental Psychology. 1984;20:315–320. [Google Scholar]
- Breaux RP, Harvey EA, Lugo-Candelas CI. The role of parent psychopathology in the development of preschool children with behavior problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2013;43:777–790. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.836451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breen R. Regression Models: Censored, Sample-Selected, or Truncated Data. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Buck KR. Unpublished dissertation. Birmingham, AL: University of Alabama; 1997. A comparison of three measures of social/emotional development of infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. [Google Scholar]
- Bus AG, Van IJzendoorn MH. Mothers reading to their 3-year-olds: The role of mother-child attachment security in becoming literate. Reading Research Quarterly. 1995;30:998–1015. [Google Scholar]
- Chetty R, Friedman JN, Hilger N, Saez E, Whitmore Schanzenbach D, Yagan D. How does your kindergarten classroom affect your earnings? Evidence from Project STAR. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2011;126:1593–1660. doi: 10.1093/qje/qjr041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, DiGiuseppe DL, McCarty CA. Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics. 2004;113:708–713. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.4.708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole PM, Armstrong LM, Pemberton CK. The role of language in the development of emotion regulation. In: Calkins SD, Bell MA, editors. Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 59–77. [Google Scholar]
- Conley D, Bennett NG. Is biology destiny? Birth weight and life chances. American Sociological Review. 2000;65:458–67. [Google Scholar]
- Davidse NJ, Jong MT, Bus AG. Explaining common variance shared by early numeracy and literacy. Reading and Writing. 2014;27:631–648. [Google Scholar]
- DeNavas-Walt C, Proctor BD, Smith JC. Current Population Reports. Washington DC: US Government Printing; 2013. Income, poverty, and health insurance coverage in the United States: 2012; pp. 60–245. [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson DK, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K. Speaking out for language: Why language is central to reading development. Educational Researcher. 2010;39(4):305–310. [Google Scholar]
- Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, Klebanov P, Japel C. School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:1428–1446. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elleman AM, Lindo EJ, Morphy P, Compton DL. The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage-level comprehension of school-age children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness. 2009;2:1–44. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Sadovsky A, Spinrad TL. Associations of emotion-related regulation with language skills, emotion knowledge, and academic outcomes. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development. 2005;109:109–118. doi: 10.1002/cd.143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ennemoser M, Schneider W. Relations of television viewing and reading: Findings from a 4-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2007;99:349–368. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.349. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Farkas G, Beron K. The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social Science Research. 2004;33:464–497. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman HM, Dale PS, Campbell TF, Colborn DK, Kurs-Lasky M, Rockette HE, Paradise JL. Concurrent and predictive validity of parent reports of child language at ages 2 and 3 years. Child Development. 2005;76:856–868. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00882.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L, Dale P, Reznick J, Thal D, Bates E. The Macarthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L, Dale P, Reznick J, Bates E, Thal D, Pethick S. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994;59:1–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernald A, Marchman VA, Weisleder A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science. 2013;16:234–248. doi: 10.1111/desc.12019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkel SE. Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Fitzpatrick C, Pagani LS. Toddler working memory skills predict kindergarten school readiness. Intelligence. 2012;40:205–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2011.11.007. [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs LS, Fuchs D, Compton DL, Powell SR, Seethaler PM, Capizzi AM, Fletcher JM. The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in arithmetic, algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006;98:29–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.29. [Google Scholar]
- Ganger J, Brent MR. Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40:621–632. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo G, Harris KM. The mechanisms mediating the effects of poverty on children’s intellectual development. Demography. 2000;37:431–447. doi: 10.1353/dem.2000.0005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hack M, Klein NK, Taylor HG. Long-term developmental outcomes of low birth weight infants. Future Child. 1995;5:176–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young american children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Heckman JJ, Masterov DV. The productivity argument for investing in young children. Review of Agricultural Economics. 2007;29:446–493. [Google Scholar]
- Hillemeier M, Farkas G, Morgan PL, Martin M, Maczuga S. Disparities in the prevalence of cognitive delay: How early do they appear? Pediatric & Perinatal Epidemiology. 2009;23:186–198. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2008.01006.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoff E. Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:4–14. doi: 10.1037/a0027238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoff E, Laursen B, Bridges K. Measurement and model building in studying the influence of socioeconomic status on child development. In: Lewis M, Mayes L, editors. A developmental environmental measurement handbook. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2012. pp. 590–606. [Google Scholar]
- Hoover WA, Gough PB. The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. 1990;2:127–160. [Google Scholar]
- Horwitz SM, Irwin JR, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Bosson Heenan JM, Mendoza J, Carter AS. Language delay in a community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:932–940. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046889.27264.5E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hulme C, Bowyer-Crane C, Carroll JM, Duff FJ, Snowling MJ. The causal role of phoneme awareness and letter-sound knowledge in learning to read: Combining intervention studies with mediation analyses. Psychological Science. 2012;23:572–577. doi: 10.1177/0956797611435921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iruka IU, LaForett DR, Odom EC. Examining the validity of the family investment and stress models and relationship to children’s school readiness across five cultural groups. Journal of Family Psychology. 2012;26:359–370. doi: 10.1037/a0028290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Justice LM, Bowles RP, Turnbull KLP, Skibbe LE. School readiness among children with varying histories of language difficulties. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:460–476. doi: 10.1037/a0014324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keppel G. Design and analysis: A researcher’s handbook. 3. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Klebanov PK, Brooks-Gunn J, McCormick MC. Classroom behavior of very low birth weight elementary school children. Pediatrics. 1994;94:700–708. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koolstra C, Van der Voort T. Longitudinal effects of television on children’s leisure time reading: A test of three explanatory models. Human Communication Research. 1996;23:4–35. [Google Scholar]
- LeFevre J, Fast L, Skwarchuk S, Smith-Chant B, Bisanz J, Kamawar D, Penner-Wilger M. Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development. 2010;81(6):1753–1767. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard LB. Is expressive language disorder an accurate diagnostic category? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2009;18:115–123. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/08-0064). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesaux NK. Reading and reading instruction for children from low-income and non-English-speaking households. The Future of Children. 2012;22:73–88. doi: 10.1353/foc.2012.0010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levelt WJM, Roelofs A, Meyer AS. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1999;22:1–75. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99001776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li-Grining C, Votruba-Drzal E, Maldonado-Carreno C, Haas K. Children’s early approaches to learning and academic trajectories through fifth grade. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:1062–1077. doi: 10.1037/a0020066. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch JL. Infant health, race/ethnicity, and early educational outcomes using the ECLS-B. Sociological Inquiry. 2011;81:499–526. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-682X.2011.00390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marulis LM, Neuman SB. The effects of vocabulary intervention on young children’s word learning: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research. 2010;80:300–335. [Google Scholar]
- Marulis LM, Neuman SB. How vocabulary interventions affect young children at risk: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness. 2013;6:223–262. [Google Scholar]
- Mani A, Mullainathan S, Shafir E, Zhao J. Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science. 2013;341:976–980. doi: 10.1126/science.1238041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development, American Psychologist. 1998;53:185–204. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMurray B. Defusing the vocabulary explosion. Science. 2007;317:631. doi: 10.1126/science.1144073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menting B, Van Lier PAC, Koot HM. Language skills, peer rejection, and the development of externalizing behavior from kindergarten to fourth grade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2010;52:72–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02279.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell AM, Brady SA. The effect of vocabulary knowledge on novel word identificaiton. Annals of Dyslexia. 2013;63:201–216. doi: 10.1007/s11881-013-0080-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan PL, Farkas G, Wu Q. Five-year growth trajectories of kindergarten children with learning difficulties in mathematics. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2009;42:306–321. doi: 10.1177/0022219408331037. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219408331037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moyé LE. The multiple comparison issue in health care research. In: Rao CR, Miller JP, Rao DC, editors. Handbook of statistics: Epidemiology and medical statistics. Vol. 27. New York, NY: Elsevier; 2008. pp. 616–655. [Google Scholar]
- Najarian M, Snow K, Lennon J, Kinsey S. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), preschool–kindergarten 2007 psychometric report. National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics; 2010. NCES 2010-009. [Google Scholar]
- National Early Literacy Panel. Developing Early Literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Jeesup, MD: National Institute for Literacy; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Does amount of time spent in childcare predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten? Child Development. 2003;74:976–1005. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41:428–442. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nord C, Edwards B, Andreassen C, Green J, Wallner-Allen K. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), User’s manual for the ECLS-B longitudinal 9-Month-2-year data file and electronic codebook (NCES 2006-046) Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Oakhill JV, Cain K. The precursors of reading ability in in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2012;16:91–121. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2010.529219. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ouellette GP. What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006;98:554–566. [Google Scholar]
- Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Barnett TA. Early childhood television viewing and kindergarten entry readiness. Pediatric Research. 2013;74:350–355. doi: 10.1038/pr.2013.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Parent S. Relating kindergarten attention to subsequent developmental pathways of classroom engagement in elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40:715–25. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9605-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9605-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pan BA, Rowe ML, Singer JD, Snow CE. Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development. 2005;76:763–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00876.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulson JF, Keefe HA, Leiferman JA. Early parental depression and child language development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:254–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01973.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01973.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington BF, Bishop DVM. Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:283–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perfetti CA, Hart L. The lexical basis of comprehension skill. On the consequences of meaning selection: Perspectives on resolving lexical ambiguity. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association; 2001. pp. 67–86. [Google Scholar]
- Perfetti C, Stafura J. Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2014;18:22–37. [Google Scholar]
- Purpura DJ, Hume LE, Sims DM, Lonigan CJ. Early literacy and early numeracy: The value of including early literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;110:647–658. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qi CH, Kaiser AP. Problem behaviors of low-income children with language delays: An observation study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47:595–610. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/046). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Raghunathan TE, Solenberger PW, Van Hoewyk J. IVEware: Imputation and variance estimation software. Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Methodology Program, Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Reichman NE. Low birth weight and school readiness. Future of Children. 2005;15:91–116. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Ross GS, McClure S. Language delay and behavioral/emotional problems in toddlers: Findings from two developmental clinics. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007;50:1063–1078. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/074). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabol TJ, Pianta RC. Patterns of school readiness forecast achievement and socioemotional development at the end of elementary school. Child Development. 2012;83:282–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01678.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schatschneider C, Fletcher J, Francis D, Carlson C, Foorman B. Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96:265–282. [Google Scholar]
- Sénéchal M, Ouellette G, Rodney D. The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. In: Neuman SB, Dickinson D, editors. Handbook of Early Literacy Research. Vol. 2. New York, NY: Guilford; 2006. pp. 173–182. [Google Scholar]
- Sideridis GD, Simos P. What is the actual correlation between expressive and receptive measures of vocabulary? Approximating the sampling distribution of the correlation coefficient using the bootstrapping method. The International Journal of Educational and Psychological Assessment. 2010;5:117–133. [Google Scholar]
- Silven M, Poskiparta E, Niemi P, Voeten M. Precursors of reading skill from infancy to first grade in Finnish: Continuity and change in a highly inflected language. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2007;99:516–531. [Google Scholar]
- Skarakis-Doyle E, Campbell W, Dempsey L. Identification of children with language impairment: Investigating the classification accuracy of the MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories, level III. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2009;18:277–288. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0035). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spelke ES, Tsivkin S. Language and number: A bilingual training study. Cognition. 2001;78:45–88. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00108-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stolt S, Haataja L, Lapinleimu H, Lehtonen L. The early lexical development and its predictive value to language skills at 2 years in very-low-birth-weight children. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2008;42:107–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin BJ, Zhang X. The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:1193–1208. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/086). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tunmer WE, Chapman JW. The simple view of reading redux: Vocabulary knowledge and the independent components hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2012;45:453–466. doi: 10.1177/0022219411432685. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219411432685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaughn BE, Kopp CB, Krakow JB. The emergence and consolidation of self-control from eighteen to thirty months of age: Normative trends and individual differences. Child Development. 1984;55:990–1004. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verhoeven L, van Leeuwe J, Vermeer A. Vocabulary growth and reading development across the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2011;15:8–25. [Google Scholar]
- Vukovic RK, Lesaux NK. The relationship between linguistic skills and arithmetic knowledge. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013;23:87–91. [Google Scholar]
- Wang AH, Shen F, Byrnes JP. Does the Opportunity-Propensity Framework predict the early mathematics skills of low-income pre-kindergarten children? Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2013;38:259–270. [Google Scholar]
- Weisleder A, Fernald A. Talking to children matters early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science. 2013;24:2143–2152. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winsler A, Diaz RM, McCarthy EM, Atencio DJ, Chabay LA. Mother-child interaction, private speech, and task performance in preschool children with behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1999;40:891–904. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeung WJ, Linver MR, Brooks-Gunn J. How money matters for young children’s development: Parental investment and family processes. Child Development. 2002;73:1861–1879. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zajicek-Farber ML. The contributions of parenting and postnatal depression on emergent language of children in low-income families. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2010;19:257–269. doi: 10.1007/s10826-009-9293-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]