Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2015 Sep 14;10(9):e0138160. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138160

Effects of Presentation Rate and Attention on Auditory Discrimination: A Comparison of Long-Latency Auditory Evoked Potentials in School-Aged Children and Adults

Naseem A Choudhury 1,2,*, Jessica A Parascando 2, April A Benasich 2
Editor: Frederic Dick3
PMCID: PMC4569142  PMID: 26368126

Abstract

Decoding human speech requires both perception and integration of brief, successive auditory stimuli that enter the central nervous system as well as the allocation of attention to language-relevant signals. This study assesses the role of attention on processing rapid transient stimuli in adults and children. Cortical responses (EEG/ERPs), specifically mismatch negativity (MMN) responses, to paired tones (standard 100–100Hz; deviant 100–300Hz) separated by a 300, 70 or 10ms silent gap (ISI) were recorded under Ignore and Attend conditions in 21 adults and 23 children (6–11 years old). In adults, an attention-related enhancement was found for all rate conditions and laterality effects (L>R) were observed. In children, 2 auditory discrimination-related peaks were identified from the difference wave (deviant-standard): an early peak (eMMN) at about 100–300ms indexing sensory processing, and a later peak (LDN), at about 400–600ms, thought to reflect reorientation to the deviant stimuli or “second-look” processing. Results revealed differing patterns of activation and attention modulation for the eMMN in children as compared to the MMN in adults: The eMMN had a more frontal topography as compared to adults and attention played a significantly greater role in childrens’ rate processing. The pattern of findings for the LDN was consistent with hypothesized mechanisms related to further processing of complex stimuli. The differences between eMMN and LDN observed here support the premise that separate cognitive processes and mechanisms underlie these ERP peaks. These findings are the first to show that the eMMN and LDN differ under different temporal and attentional conditions, and that a more complete understanding of children’s responses to rapid successive auditory stimulation requires an examination of both peaks.

Introduction: Mismatch Responses in School-Age Children and Adults

The auditory system is intricately involved in the temporal analysis of spoken language by facilitating the decomposition of an incoming sound stream into its constituent features [13]. Effective language acquisition is characterized largely by the ability of the auditory system to efficiently process the spatiotemporal properties of the acoustic signal. Studies have shown that the inability to accurately perceive, discriminate and decode the brief, rapidly successive spectrotemporal events (referred to here as rapid auditory processing, “RAP”) that constitute language is associated with developmental language disorders such as Specific Language Impairment (SLI) and dyslexia [46]; for reviews, see [1, 711]. It is also well established that auditory processing abilities in general, and RAP in particular, improve over the course of development [1218]. What remains relatively unclear, however, is the role of active attention in facilitating RAP abilities at different developmental periods. In the present study we assess the effect of attention on RAP processing abilities, in adults (18–42 years) and school-aged children (6–11 years).

Studies of auditory processing frequently use the electrocortical mismatch response to assess neural responses to changes in the acoustic signal in adults, children [19, 20], and also in infants [21]. The mismatch negativity (MMN) is an event-related potential (ERP) component elicited by a deviant or rarely occurring stimulus presented within a stream of standard stimuli [22, 23]). The MMN occurs approximately 150–300 ms after the onset of a deviant stimulus, can be elicited passively and is thought to precede conscious awareness [22, 24]. The MMN component peaks earlier than attention-based components (approx. 150 ms from deviance detection in adults) and reflects the early sensory stages of sound processing. The MMN is a modality-specific component, with neural generators within auditory cortices [24]. The component is highly sensitive to acoustic stimulus properties and can be elicited when the acoustic difference is near psychophysical thresholds [25] or when stimulus differences are consciously imperceptible [26]. These properties make it ideal for the study of mechanisms underlying auditory perception.

The MMN originates bilaterally in the auditory cortex, with contributions from auditory association areas, medial geniculate nucleus of the thalamus, and hippocampus [22, 2733]). The MMN and its characteristics have been extensively described in adults, however the literature describing the MMN’s developmental progression in childhood and the role of attention in its expression is not as extensive. Maturational changes in auditory ERPs include decreases in latency and amplitude as well as dynamic morphological changes [21, 3435]. These maturational changes are thought to result from increased synaptic density in auditory cortex, increases in myelination as well as differences in the location and orientation of the mismatch generators [3642]. Various studies have reported maturational changes in the latency and amplitude of the MM response. For example, using tone stimuli, the latency of an MM response has been shown to decrease between the ages of 3 to 44 months [43], 6 and 48 months [21], 4 to 10 years-of-age [35], and in 7 to 16-year-olds [44]. These findings are consistent with reports of latency differences in the MMN of adults to tones as compared to children [44]. In contrast, the MM response elicited by speech stimuli in school-age children and adults has been found to be similar in latency and morphology (i.e. in 5–10 yr olds, [45]: for just perceptibly different variants of /da/; [46]: for variants of /da/ and /ga/; [47]). Despite their apparent similarities, the adult and child MMN response may not be equivalent processes [48]. Indeed, differences in the amplitude of the mature and developing mismatch response have been documented with most studies reporting that the MMN is larger in children as compared to adults [44, 4951].

Additionally, a second later “discrimination” component, the late difference negativity, (LDN) also with frontocentral topography, has been identified in children (for a review, see [5256]). While the early MMN (eMMN) component occurs at about 150–400 ms after stimulus onset and is thought to reflect pre-attentive, automatic change detection processes associated with the adult MMN, the LDN occurs at about 400–600 ms [53, 5759] and is hypothesized to reflect further processing (i.e. a second look) of the deviant stimulus, particularly when the stimuli are complex or near threshold. These findings suggest that the LDN may be non-modality specific, reflect integration of information from earlier processing stages [60] and possibly be dependent on the concomitant development of attentional mechanisms ([52, 56, 61]; for review see [62]). An LDN-like component has also been described in adults with complex stimuli [44, 6365], but the amplitude of this component has been found to decrease rapidly with age [52], making it difficult to use in adult studies (but see findings from [63] on LDN and dyslexia, and Ortiz-Mantilla and colleagues, [66], on late and early bilinguals and LDN). In studies of children with language and reading difficulties, abnormal or absent responses have been reported for both the eMMN and LDN [61, 63, 6771], supporting the idea that these two components are sensitive measures of language-related auditory discrimination.

While the MMN is thought to reflect a pre-attentive process, attention can influence the formation of the MMN. This can take place either by mediating the formation of the cortical representation of the standard stimuli, or by impacting the accuracy and resolution of the deviant stimulus representation [72]. For example, in typically developing populations the MMN amplitude is greater when subjects actively attend to stimuli that are difficult to discriminate [64, 73] as compared to easily discriminable stimuli [64, 74]. Attention-related enhancements of the MMN have also been found to differ between adults and children. In a frequency discrimination task, Gomes and colleagues [75] compared MMN amplitudes of 8–12 year-old children and adults, and showed that in adults the MMN responses were not significantly altered by attention, while in children the MMN to difficult frequency contrasts were modulated by attention. Sussman and Steinschneider [76] examined stream segregation abilities in adults and 9- to 12-year-old children using passively and actively acquired ERPs (MMN and P3b), as well as behavioral measures of discrimination and found that, in children, stream segregation perception abilities assessed behaviorally were reflected in ERPs acquired during active, but not passive, listening. In adults, however, ERPs in both conditions were similar, and these electrophysiological measures mirrored behavioral perception of stream segregation. The authors suggest that in 9- to 12-year-old children, attending to relevant acoustic input may enhance and refine auditory representations established during passive exposure, resulting in greater automaticity of auditory processing. These studies suggest that attention is a critical factor in the development of efficient, automatic auditory processing and in the short term, may drive plasticity, so that the neural response to a sensory event that is initially voluntary eventually becomes automatic. A major limitation of the existing literature however, is that, to date, nearly all of the studies assess the role of attention to adult MMN and/or the child eMMN but not the LDN.

Here, two experiments examine auditory processing using an oddball paradigm with temporally modulated non-speech stimuli. Mismatch negativity responses were recorded from adults and 6 to 11 year-old children in conditions where the participants were either required to attend or ignore (passive condition) the auditory signal. The goal of these studies was to characterize automatic auditory discrimination of temporally-modulated stimuli of varying difficulty in adults and children, and to investigate the comparative effect of attention on auditory processing in these two populations. Experiment 1 addresses this research question in adults while experiment 2 examines the same aims in children. In keeping with prior research it was hypothesized that MMN in adults, and the eMMN in children, elicited by easily discriminable stimuli (i.e., slower-rate stimuli in this study) would be more robust than those elicited by difficult-to-differentiate stimuli (i.e., fast rate) [22, 77]. It is also hypothesized that MMN and the eMMN would be enhanced by attention allocation to the stimulus, and finally that in children the LDN may be more variable (in amplitude or latency), particularly for the more difficult stimuli (fast rate) suggesting that these stimuli might engage mechanisms involved in “further”, more up-stream, acoustic processing.

Methods: Effect of Attention and Rate on Auditory Processing in Adults

Participants

Twenty-one monolingual English-speaking adults (12 males) with no history of hearing, neurological, or language/learning problems (including dyslexia, language delay or impairment, dyscalculia) were recruited to participate in this study (mean age = 27 years, range 18–42 years). All participants had completed high school and 12 (57%) had completed college (mean years of schooling post 8th grade 8.11, SD 2.94). Rutgers University’s Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures and written informed consent was obtained from all participating adults.

Stimuli

Stimuli were complex tones (70ms in duration) with a fundamental frequency of 100 or 300Hz including 15 harmonics (6 dB roll-off per octave). All stimuli were presented at 75 dB SPL free-field via speakers to the left and right of the participant. Tones were presented in pairs in a typical oddball paradigm using a blocked design with different interstimulus intervals (ISIs: the time between the 2 tones in a pair) of 300, 70, or 10ms. A trial consisted of the presentation of one tone pair and each block consisted of 833 trials. The standard tone pair was 100 –100Hz (85% probability: 708 trials), and the deviant tone pair was 100–300Hz (15% probability: 125 trials). Deviant tone-pairs were presented in a pseudo-random order with at least 3 and no more than 10 standards between each deviant. The ISIs were chosen based on previous research in our lab strongly suggesting that tone pairs with an interpolated 300ms ISI were easily discriminated, 70 ms ISI was of moderate difficulty and a 10 ms ISI was the most challenging to parse [66, 7881]. Inter-trial intervals (ITI, onset to onset) were 700ms for the 300ms ISI block, and 705ms for the 70 and 10ms ISI blocks.

Procedure

Study Design

All participants received two attention conditions, Ignore and Attend, and two rate-of-presentation conditions, 70ms ISI and 300ms ISI or 70ms ISI and 10ms ISI. The Ignore condition always preceded the Attend condition. Every participant received four blocks in the following order: Ignore condition with 70ms ISI tones, Ignore condition with either 300 or 10ms ISI tones, Attend condition with 70ms ISI tones, Attend condition with either 300 or 10ms ISI tones. This design minimized data loss due to participant fatigue. Pilot studies revealed that if all 6 blocks (2 attention and all 3 rate-of-presentation, 300ms, 70ms and 10ms ISI) were presented individuals became restless and their performance on the Attend conditions suffered significantly (as assessed by significant decreases in correct responses) [82]. Thus to maximize data quality and participant retention, each participant received two of the three rate conditions and therefore 2 groups were created based on rate-of-presentation. Participants in Group I were assessed on 70 and 300ms, (n = 12), and participants in Group II were assessed on 70 and 10ms (n = 9). This design also ensured that all participants received the 70ms ISI rate condition so that the two groups could be compared on rate processing equivalencies. As previously reported, Thomas et al., [82, 83] demonstrated that the MMN elicited by the 70ms ISI stimuli in Groups I and II did not differ in morphology and topography. These findings verify homogeneity of the MMN with the 70ms ISI stimuli between groups of adults and allow us to compare the responses to the 300 and 10ms ISI across group. During the Ignore condition, adults viewed a silent video with subtitles (n = 10) or read text (n = 11) and were asked to ignore the sounds. In the Attend condition, all participants were instructed to press a button located on a “response pad” resting on their lap as quickly as possible when they heard the deviant (target) tone pair (100Hz-300Hz). There were 33 training trials during which participants were given feedback. There was no feedback during the test sessions.

ERP Recording

Participants were seated in an acoustically shielded room. Geodesic Sensor Nets (GSN; Electrical Geodesics, Inc.) with 64 Ag/AgCl electrodes were used to measure EEG at all 64 sites. The vertex served as an online reference and vertical and horizontal eye movements (EOG) were measured with electrodes placed above, below and on the outer canthi of each eye. Online the EEG was sampled at 250 Hz with filters set at 0.1 to 100 Hz. Impedances were maintained below 50 kΩ.

Off-line EEG/ERP Analyses

All EEG data were processed using BESA (Brain Electrical Source Analysis V 5.1.8.10, MEGIS Software GmbH, 2006). The data were re-referenced off-line to an average (whole head) reference, and were scanned and corrected for lateral and horizontal eye movements [8486]. The thresholds for EOG artifact correction were 150 μV for horizontal and 250 μV for vertical eye movements/eye blinks. Artifact rejection criteria were set to exclude activity exceeding 120 μV. A minimum of 50 artifact-free trials was averaged by stimulus type (deviant or pre-deviant standard), and baseline corrected (baseline = -100 ms). Data were then grand averaged for each participant and bandpass filtered at 1–15 Hz.

Individual grand-averaged difference waveforms (deviant minus pre-deviant standard) were visually inspected for the presence of MMN in the 100–150ms latency range following the onset of the deviant tone. This study focuses on MMN component in frontal, frontocentral and central channels in left and right hemispheres and hence MMN peak amplitude and latency values were extracted from the following channels: frontocentral (channel 17/Fc3, 54/Fc4), frontal (13/F3, 62/F4), central (21/C5, 53/C6,), temporal (24/T7, 52/T8), parietal (28/P5, 46/P6), and occipital (38/Oz) channels (see [87] for verification of Geodesic sensor net electrode positions and their 10–10 international equivalents). Temporal channels were used to detect for the expected inversion of polarity seen for the MMN. As expected, topographic maps showed maximal amplitudes in frontocentral and central channels for the deviant as well as the difference waveforms and a relative inversion of polarity at mastoid channels. All MMN peak latencies presented here are relative to the onset of the second tone in the pair thus allowing for statistical comparison of latencies among different ISI conditions.

Statistical Analyses

A 4-way Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (2 x 2 x 3 x 2, ANOVA) was run in order to assess the effects of rate (2: slow, fast), attention (2: Ignore, Attend), regions of interest (ROI) (3: frontal, frontocentral, and central) and laterality (2: left, right) on the MMN peak. Given the nature of the study design, 2 separate analyses were conducted for Group I (G1), comparing performance on 300 and 70ms ISIs, and Group II (G2), comparing performance on 70 and 10ms ISIs. The 300 and 10ms ISI rates were compared using independent samples t-tests after verifying that Group I and Group II did not differ in amplitude or latency in the 70ms ISI.

Results

Morphology of Adult Waveform

Visual inspection of the standard, deviant and difference grand-averaged waveforms revealed that adults displayed typical MMN morphology and topography. A MMN peak, maximal in frontocentral regions, between 110–160 ms was observed for all three rates (300, 70 and 10ms ISI) and in both the Ignore and Attend conditions (see Figs 1 and 2). Topography of the grand-averaged waveforms are shown in Figs 3 and 4

Fig 1. Adult Ignore Condition.

Fig 1

Grand average ERP’s of the adults for the Ignore condition at Fc3 (Ch.17) and Fc4 (Ch.54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Response to the standard stimuli (blue line), the deviant stimuli (red line) and the difference condition (standard- deviant; green line) are plotted. Vertical pink bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli. Negative voltages are plotted up, positive voltages are plotted down.

Fig 2. Adult Attend Condition.

Fig 2

Grand average ERP’s of the adults for the Attend condition at Fc3 (Ch 17) and Fc4 (Ch 54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Response to the standard stimuli (blue line), the deviant stimuli (red line) and the difference condition (standard- deviant; green line) are plotted. Vertical pink bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli. Negative voltages are plotted up, positive voltages are plotted down.

Fig 3. Adult Ignore grand averaged topographic maps.

Fig 3

Whole-head topographies for grand average waveforms for the Ignore condition at Fc3 (Ch 17) and Fc4 (Ch 54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Negativity up, positivity down. The standard wave is shown in blue, deviant in red, and difference (deviant-standard) in green. Vertical bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli.

Fig 4. Adult Attend grand averaged topographic maps.

Fig 4

Whole-head topographies for grand average waveforms for the Attend condition at Fc3 (Ch 17) and Fc4 (Ch 54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Negativity up, positivity down. The standard wave is shown in blue, deviant in red, and difference (deviant-standard) in green. (deviant-standard) in green. Vertical bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli.

Effect of Rate and Attention on Auditory Processing in Adults

Amplitude Effects

Results of the 4 way ANOVA revealed a Rate x Attention interaction (G1, F (1, 11) = 7.67, p<.05; G2, F(1, 8) = 6.82, p<.05). In both models, the more difficult-to discriminate rates (70ms and 10ms ISI) in the Attend condition elicited significantly larger MMN peaks compared to the Ignore condition, and in comparison to the 300 and 70 ms ISI’s (Table 1). A main effect for ROI and Attention was also found; Frontocentral channels had significantly larger peaks compared to Frontal and Central channels (G1, F (2, 10) = 11.36, p<.01; G2, F (2, 7) = 7.98, p<.01), and for both groups and all rates of presentation, attending to the stimuli lead to larger peak amplitudes.

Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of the Mismatch Negativity (MMN) Amplitude (mV) in the Ignore and Attend condition for Adults.
Group I: 300ms vs. 70ms (n = 12) Group II: 70ms vs. 10ms ISI (n = 9)
300ms ISI Mean SD 70ms ISI Mean SD 70ms ISI Mean SD 10ms ISI Mean SD
Location (ch) Ignore Condition
Left Frontal (13) -1.26 0.7 -1.40 0.6 -1.32 0.9 -1.27 0.8
Left Fronto-central (17) -1.30 1.1 -1.51 1.0 -1.59 1.0 -2.16 1.2
Left Central (21) -1.25 0.6 -1.25 0.7 -1.25 0.7 -1.18 0.5
Right Frontal (62) -1.28 0.6 -1.65 0.9 -1.75 0.7 -1.54 0.8
Right Fronto-central (54) -1.65 0.8 -1.88 0.9 -1.70 0.9 -1.99 0.9
Right Central (53) -0.91 0.5 -1.19 0.6 -1.01 0.6 -1.12 0.5
Attend Condition
Left Frontal (13) -1.56 0.7 -1.62 0.6 -1.74 0.7 -1.80 0.9
Left Fronto-central (17) -2.25 1.2 -2.80 1.1 -2.75 1.4 -2.83 1.6
Left Central (21) -1.39 0.8 -1.68 0.9 -1.51 0.6 -2.86 0.9
Right Frontal (62) -1.55 0.5 -1.85 1.1 -1.91 1.9 -2.65 1.1
Right Fronto-central (54) -2.24 0.8 -2.72 1.4 -2.79 1.6 -2.99 1.5
Right Central (53) -1.24 0.6 -1.84 0.6 -1.81 0.6 -2.20 1.0

Latency Effects

Examination of latency differences revealed a main effect for Attention, Ignore <Attend (G1, F (1, 11) = 14.82, p<.01; G2, F (1, 8) = 16.02, p<.01,) and a main effect for Hemisphere, Left <Right (G1, F (1, 11) = 4.96, p<.05); G2, F (1, 8) = 5.47, p<.05,) for both groups. In addition for G1 only (300 versus 70 ms ISI) there was a main effect of Rate, 300ms < 70ms ISI condition, (G1, F (1, 11) = 6.23, p<.05), and an independent sample t-tests comparing performance on 300 and 10ms ISI revealed significant latency differences for both the Attend and Ignore conditions on all frontal and frontocentral sites (300ms < 10ms ISI condition, t(20) = 2.2 to 3.6, p<.05). No significant Rate differences were observed between 70 and 10ms ISI conditions (Table 2).

Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations of the Mismatch Negativity (MMN) Latency (ms) in the Ignore and Attend condition for Adults.
Group I: 300ms vs. 70ms (n = 12) Group II: 70ms vs. 10ms ISI (n = 9)
300ms ISI Mean SD 70ms ISI Mean SD 70ms ISI Mean SD 10ms ISI Mean SD
Location (ch) Ignore Condition
Left Frontal (13) 100 22 120 9 119 9 126 29
Left Fronto-central (17) 112 21 121 6 124 6 119 21
Left Central (21) 111 21 128 20 127 18 117 24
Right Frontal (62) 100 33 117 4 116 6 115 25
Right Fronto-central (54) 112 19 117 7 117 7 116 23
Right Central (53) 115 22 127 20 135 25 127 31
Attend Condition
Left Frontal (13) 117 30 152 34 134 36 145 30
Left Fronto-central (17) 120 17 143 16 140 15 134 19
Left Central (21) 120 17 129 15 130 16 128 24
Right Frontal (62) 125 33 162 30 141 34 163 27
Right Fronto-central (54) 121 25 154 20 150 25 158 19
Right Central 135 27 144 24 152 27 155 20

Summary of results for experiment 1

Significant attention-related increases in MMN peak amplitude were observed in adults, and this was more prominent when processing a more difficult to discriminate stimuli. As expected, and consistent with previous studies, the maximal peak was observed in frontocentral regions. However attending to the stimuli also slowed processing speeds; for all three rates a slight delay in processing was observed when comparing the Attend and Ignore conditions. Adults showed a significant lateralization effect, stimuli were processed faster on the left as compared to the right.

Methods: Effects of Attention and Rate in Auditory Processing in Children

Participants

Twenty-three monolingual English-speaking children (9 males; mean-age 8 years, 7 months; range 6–11 years) with complete data participated in this study. An additional 8 children were recruited but did not complete the study and thus are not included in these analyses. All children were in their age-appropriate grade in school (mean 3rd grade, SD 1.34) and performed in the normal range on verbal and non-verbal cognitive tasks, had normal hearing, were born full-term and normal birthweight, and had no known neurological or developmental disorders. None reported any family history of language or learning impairments. Rutgers University’s Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures. Written informed consent was obtained from parents or legal guardians of all participating children, and children also provided either written consent or verbal assent for participation.

There were no statistically significant differences between the sample here and the 8 children with incomplete data on demographic or any of the outcome variables assessed [82].

Procedure

Experiment 2 followed the same protocol as Experiment 1 with the following exceptions: i) Stimulus probability. Based on pilot data that suggested that this age group had EEGs that were more likely to be contaminated with eye or muscle movement artifacts and to ensure an optimum number of useable trials, children received more deviant trials (166 trials) than adults (125 trials). ii) Off-line data processing and identifying ERP peaks. Artifact rejection criteria were set to exclude signals with fluctuations greater than 200 μV. The eMMN was observed between 100–300ms and the LDN was observed between 400–600ms. The above latencies were used as time windows to extract peak amplitude (maximum amplitude) and latency values for the maximum amplitude. Given these differences and the developmental nature of the data in Experiment 2, adult and child samples were not combined for analysis.

Statistical Analyses

Current analysis paralleled that of Experiment 1: A 4-way Repeated Measures ANOVA (2x2x3x2) was conducted in order to assess the effects of rate (2: slow, fast), attention (2: Ignore, Attend), regions of interest (ROI) (3: frontal, frontocentral, and central) and laterality (2: left, right) on the eMMN and LDN peaks. Separate analyses were run for Group I, comparing performance on 300 and 70ms ISIs (G1), and Group II, comparing performance on 70 and 10ms ISIs (G2). Performance on the 300 and 10ms ISIs were compared using independent samples t-tests after verifying that Group 1 (300/70 ms ISIs) and Group 2 (70/10 ms ISIs) did not differ in amplitude (range t = 0.38 to 1.38, n.s) or latency for the 70 ms ISI (range t = 0.02 to 2.07, n.s). The F and t statistics ranges presented below are for all 6 channels.

Results

Morphology of Children’s Waveform

The morphology of children’s ERPs was more widely distributed and had higher amplitude fluctuations as compared to adults. However, similar to adults, the highest amplitudes were observed in frontal and frontocentral channels. As expected, two components, eMMN and LDN, were identified in children’s ERP waveforms. Both eMMN and LDN were observed at all three rates of stimuli presentation (300, 70 and 10ms ISI) in both attention conditions (Ignore and Attend) in all children. The eMMN was broadly distributed but maximal at frontal and frontocentral sites, and associated with large positivities in temporo-mastoid areas. The LDN also had frontal and central topography. The grand-averaged waveforms for children are shown in Figs 5 and 6. Topography of the grand-averaged waveforms for children are presented in Figs 7 and 8.

Fig 5. Children Ignore Condition.

Fig 5

Grand average ERP’s of children for the Ignore condition at Fc3 (Ch 17) and Fc4 (Ch 54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Response to the standard stimuli (blue line), the deviant stimuli (red line) and the difference condition (standard- deviant; green line) are plotted. Vertical pink bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli. Negative voltages are plotted up, positive voltages are plotted down.

Fig 6. Children Attend Condition.

Fig 6

Grand average ERP’s of children for the Attend condition at Fc3 (Ch. 17) and Fc4 (Ch. 54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Response to the standard stimuli (blue line), the deviant stimuli (red line) and the difference condition (standard- deviant; green line) are plotted. Vertical pink bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli. Negative voltages are plotted up, positive voltages are plotted down.

Fig 7. Child Ignore grand averaged topographic maps.

Fig 7

Whole-head topographies for grand average waveforms for the Ignore condition at Fc3 (Ch 17) and Fc4 (Ch 54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Negativity up, positivity down. The standard wave is shown in blue, deviant in red, and difference (deviant-standard) in green. Vertical bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli.

Fig 8. Child Attend grand averaged topographic maps.

Fig 8

Whole-head topographies for grand average waveforms for the Attend condition at Fc3 (Ch 17) and Fc4 (Ch 54) for 300ms (top), 70ms (middle) and 10ms (bottom) ISI’s are shown. Negativity up, positivity down. The standard wave is shown in blue, deviant in red, and difference (deviant-standard) in green. Vertical bars on the baseline indicate the onset of the tones. Tone 1 and tone 2 are paired stimuli.

Effect of Rate and Attention on Auditory Processing in Children

Amplitude Effects for eMMN

Repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant 4-way interactions (Rate x Attention x Region x Hemisphere) for G1 (F (1, 4) = 7.59, p<0.01) and G2 (F (1, 4) = 4.92, p>0.05). Further examination revealed an Attention x Hemisphere effect in G1 (F (1, 4) = 12.81, p<0.05) and G2 (F (1, 4) = 6.40, p<0.05); the most robust responses were observed for the Attend condition in the Right hemisphere in both groups. A main effect for Attention was also observed for both groups; in general the Attend condition elicited larger peaks compared to the Ignore condition (G1, F (1, 6) = 51.19, p<0.05; G2, F (1, 9) = 17.13, p<0.05). Rate did not significantly alter the results of these analyses and an independent sample t-tests comparing eMMN peak response for 300 and 10 ms ISI stimuli confirmed that there were no significant rate-related amplitude differences (t(21) = 0.01–1.24, n.s.) (Table 3).

Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations of the early Mismatch Negativity (eMMN) Amplitude (mV) in the Ignore and Attend condition for Children.
Group I: 300ms vs. 70ms ISI (n = 11) Group II: 70ms vs. 10ms ISI (n = 12)
300ms ISI Mean SD 70ms ISI Mean SD 70ms ISI Mean SD 10ms ISI Mean SD
Location (ch) Ignore Condition
Left Frontal (13) -2.20 0.7 -1.56 1.3 -2.23 0.8 -1.02 0.7
Left Fronto-central (17) -1.89 0.9 -1.52 1.0 -1.52 0.8 -1.05 0.9
Left Central (21) -1.86 1.5 -0.48 0.9 -1.24 0.9 -0.98 0.8
Right Frontal (62) -2.05 1.3 -1.15 0.3 -1.61 0.8 -1.22 0.7
Right Fronto-central (54) -2.25 0.7 -1.71 1.2 -1.95 0.6 -1.02 1.1
Right Central (53) -1.10 0.7 -0.50 0.7 -1.07 0.9 -1.03 0.8
Attend Condition
Left Frontal (13) -2.68 0.4 -2.06 1.3 -2.67 0.8 -2.41 1.9
Left Fronto-central (17) -2.67 1.5 -2.39 0.2 -2.42 1.2 -1.49 2.0
Left Central (21) -2.54 1.9 -1.30 1.8 -1.22 1.2 -1.48 1.5
Right Frontal (62) -3.18 1.3 -2.30 0.9 -2.39 1.3 -2.38 1.9
Right Fronto-central (54) -3.16 1.1 -2.09 1.1 -2.07 1.5 -1.43 1.5
Right Central (53) -3.11 1.5 -1.67 1.1 -1.35 1.4 -1.71 1.1

Latency Effects for eMMN

A 4-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed a main effect for Rate in G1 only, 300 < 70ms ISI: (F (1, 4) = 31.4, p<.00). Independent sample t-test also revealed rate-related differences between 300ms and 10ms ISI peak latencies (300ms < 10ms ISI; t(21) = 2.9–4.6, p<.05). No differences were found between 70ms and 10ms ISI conditions (Table 4).

Table 4. Means and Standard Deviations of the Early Mismatch Negativity (eMMN) Latency (ms) in the Ignore and Attend condition for Children.
Group I: 300ms vs. 70ms ISI (n = 11) Group II: 70ms vs. 10ms ISI (n = 12)
300ms ISI Mean SD 70ms ISI Mean SD 70ms IS Mean SD 10ms ISI Mean SD
Location (ch) Ignore Condition
Left Frontal (13) 153 33 284 33 263 57 280 45
Left Fronto-central (17) 135 12 242 95 256 65 273 23
Left Central (21) 151 26 247 88 251 64 256 21
Right Frontal (62) 143 16 280 25 263 55 287 42
Right Fronto-central (54) 129 4 274 83 268 61 255 27
Right Central (53) 148 21 254 75 263 57 253 44
Attend Condition
Left Frontal (13) 143 23 268 33 263 66 286 52
Left Fronto-central (17) 136 13 241 92 272 91 287 19
Left Central (21) 154 23 233 23 256 80 286 15
Right Frontal (62) 146 23 239 70 272 52 288 31
Right Fronto-central (54) 131 9 211 77 274 92 281 27
Right Central (53) 142 31 252 32 236 40 279 26

Amplitude Effects for LDN

Findings from the LDN analyses differed from the eMMN results. Analyses revealed an Attention x Rate interaction for G1 (F (1,4) = 11.34, p<0.05) and G2 (F (1,4) = 8.54, p<0.05), which was decomposed to show that attending to the fast-rate stimulus (70/10ms ISI) lead to the larger peaks when compared to slower rate stimuli (Table 5).

Table 5. Means and Standard Deviations of the Late Difference Negativity (LDN) Amplitude (mV) in the Ignore and Attend condition for Children.
Group I: 300ms vs. 70ms ISI (n = 11) Group II: 70ms vs. 10ms ISI (n = 12)
300 ms ISI Mean SD 70 ms ISI Mean SD 70 ms ISI Mean SD 10 ms ISI Mean SD
Location (ch) Ignore Condition
Left Frontal (13) -1.47 0.9 -2.15 0.4 -.98 0.5 -1.35 0.5
Left Fronto-central (17) -1.67 1.2 -1.95 0.7 -1.82 0.4 -1.14 1.0
Left Central (21) -1.20 1.1 -1.07 0.9 -1.06 0.4 -1.11 0.8
Right Frontal (62) -2.06 0.8 -1.97 0.6 1.98 0.3 -1.69 0.7
Right Fronto-central (54) -2.10 1.1 -2.70 0.4 -2.26 0.6 -1.69 0.8
Right Central (53) -0.99 0.5 -1.67 0.6 -1.20 0.8 -1.45 0.7
Attend Condition
Left Frontal (13) -1.70 0.6 -1.64 1.6 -1.70 1.2 -2.60 1.8
Left Fronto-central (17) -2.10 0.8 -1.63 0.7 -1.72 0.5 -2.14 1.0
Left Central (21) -1.60 0.8 -1.50 0.7 -1.65 0.6 -1.70 1.0
Right Frontal (62) -1.70 1.1 -1.98 0.7 -1.75 1.3 -2.38 1.6
Right Fronto-central (54) -1.48 0.9 -1.51 0.7 -1.50 1.4 -1.30 1.6
Right Central (53) -1.45 0.5 -1.50 0.7 -1.47 0.9 -2.20 1.2

Latency Effects for LDN

Repeated measures ANOVA’s for the latency revealed Rate x Attention interactions for both groups (G1, F(1,4) = 10.81, p<.05; G2, F(1,4) = 7.84, p<.05) (Table 6). The latency of the LDN for the more difficult-to-discriminate stimuli (e.g., 70ms and 10ms ISI) in the Attend condition was significantly longer than those observed for Ignore conditions and for the easier to discriminate Attend condition (e.g., 70ms ISI Attend > 300ms ISI Attend, 300 and 70ms ISI Ignore). For G1 there was also a main effect for rate, 300<70ms ISI (F (1,4) = 114.6, p<.00), and for G2 there was a main effect for attention (F (1,4) = 54.9, p<.00), 70ms &10ms ISI Ignore < 70 & 10ms ISI Attend).

Table 6. Means and Standard Deviations of the Late Difference Negativity (LDN) Latency (ms) in the Ignore and Attend condition for Children.
Group I: 300ms vs. 70ms ISI (n = 11) Group II: 70ms vs. 10ms ISI (n = 12)
300 ms ISI Mean SD 70 ms ISI Mean SD 70 ms ISI Mean SD 10 ms ISI Mean SD
Location (ch) Ignore Condition
Left Frontal (13) 452 44 432 45 441 51 443 43
Left Fronto-central (17) 447 49 440 49 433 60 438 40
Left Central (21) 456 58 425 63 442 60 462 28
Right Frontal (62) 450 26 433 40 446 39 450 49
Right Fronto-central (54) 467 44 435 32 430 32 420 37
Right Central (53) 460 42 440 31 420 41 440 32
Attend Condition
Left Frontal (13) 466 100 553 66 560 88 573 83
Left Fronto-central (17) 481 51 519 36 537 83 572 67
Left Central (21) 510 96 515 31 528 32 578 43
Right Frontal (62) 478 100 561 46 548 36 580 82
Right Fronto-central (54) 473 58 526 87 521 62 575 24
Right Central (53) 487 88 521 22 540 73 567 42

Summary of results for experiment 2

When compared to the Ignore condition, the Attend condition elicited larger amplitude eMMN peaks for all three rates. For the LDN, attention-related amplitude differences were found for the faster-rate stimuli (70ms and 10ms ISI). This is consistent with prior studies suggesting that attending to a stimulus recruits resources at the sensory processing level, but not necessarily at the reorienting or ‘second-look’ phase, especially if the stimulus under consideration is relatively easy to detect and discriminate from others. The latency of the eMMN peak was only affected by rate of presentation, and not attention. For the LDN, however, there was a rate by attention interaction; as expected the more easy-to-discriminate stimuli elicited faster latencies compared to the more difficult to discriminate stimuli. Lateralization effects were observed for the eMMN amplitude only and, in contrast to adult MMN observations, children had larger eMMN’s on the right.

General Discussion

It is well established that maturational differences in the MMN may be modulated by the type of stimuli used (non-speech vs. speech; simple vs. complex), as well as the perceived difficulty of the discrimination. In both adults and children, the difficulty of the standard-deviant discrimination has been shown to affect the latency and amplitude of the MMN, such that it is characterized by shorter latencies and greater amplitude if the difference between standard and deviant stimuli is large (i.e., stimuli are easily discriminable) ([50, 8890], but see [31], i.e., for more complex stimuli this relation between latency/amplitude and stimulus difference magnitude may not be monotonic). Little however is known about the role of attention in facilitating this sensory discrimination process and even less is known about the more “upstream”, possibly non-modality specific processes involved in auditory discrimination.

In this study we found that selective attention in adults facilitated acoustic processing resulting in larger MMN peaks. This was particularly true for the more difficult-to-discriminate stimuli (i.e., beginning at the 70ms ISI range). These findings are in line with prior research that suggests that attending to acoustic stimuli initiates the recruitment of additional neural resources or induces greater neural synchrony leading to more robust, albeit slightly later appearing, peaks [73,75]. Children, however, exhibited two separate discrimination peaks (eMMN and LDN). The presence of these peaks were in the expected ranges (100–300, 400–600ms respectively), and, as hypothesized, the independence of these two peaks was demonstrated by their differing responses to changes in rate and attention. These findings are the first to show that the eMMN and LDN differ under different temporal and attentional conditions, and that a more complete understanding of children’s responses to auditory stimuli requires an examination of both peaks. Further, the differences observed here are clearly illustrative of separate cognitive processes and mechanisms underlying these ERPs peaks.

The eMMN in children is thought to be similar in nature to the adult MMN and possibly a precursor of the mature MMN [48, 53]. In these studies we expected, and observed, maturational differences; the eMMN appeared later in time and with larger amplitude when compared to the adult MMN. We also observed significant attention-related amplitude enhancements for all rates in children, and these enhancements were particularly pronounced in the right hemisphere. Adult MMN showed the same pattern of results; that is, there was a main effect for attention and an additional enhancement for the more difficult-to-discriminate stimuli. In addition, the adult data showed significant latency effects such that attending to the stimuli resulted in increased latencies of the MMN peak. In children, however, latency differences were only related to presentation rate of the stimuli and, specifically, were longer for the more difficult-to-discriminate rates (i.e. 70 and 10 ms ISI) as compared to the easier 300ms rate. Moreover these effects seem to suggest a threshold-type response as there were no latency differences between the two faster-rate stimuli for the MMN and eMMN. Finally, adults also showed a laterality effect for the MMN (Left > Right), but in children an opposite laterality effect (Right > Left) was observed for the eMMN, but not the LDN, in the attend conditions only.

It is well established that the process indexed by the MMN takes place in the auditory cortex with contributions from frontal regions [22, 33]. The present study supports the hypothesis that children’s processing of complex auditory stimuli may in fact be more reliant on frontal contributions, possibly from anterior cingulate cortex and our findings are in line with the underlying neurodevelopmental changes, specifically ongoing maturation of the frontal generators and the protracted maturational course of prefrontal cortex, which may influence the automaticity of MMN in children as compared to adults [91]. A similar explanation may be used to account for the laterality effect observed only in adults: that is, in a non-disordered adult, consistent and reliable experience with temporally-modulated stimuli may have increased the efficiency of the systems that process these types of stimuli. Given the left hemisphere’s role in resolving brief, rapidly-occurring and successive acoustic cues, such as those found in human speech, it is not surprising that in adults the left hemisphere would be better at generalizing to include these non-speech sounds, thereby demonstrating a hemispheric advantage. In the child this may still be an ongoing process, and more effort may be required to resolve these non-speech sounds that contain linguistic-like acoustic cues.

With regard to the LDN, longer latencies were observed in response to the difficult-to-discriminate rates under attentional control. Given the hypothesis that the LDN is “a second look”, and that the faster rates are in fact more difficult to process, this finding is unsurprising: it should be expected that a developing system takes longer to resolve difficult and more complex stimuli. Further, the LDN is also posited to be dependent on the development of attention mechanisms prior to automaticity [52, 56, 61], thus stimuli that require effortful processing and those that engage selective and focused attention systems, post sensory processing, should lead to longer processing times. Given this explanation, it is possible as well that the attention related latency effect on the LDN observed in children in this study may be influenced by the presence of a P3b-like component typically occurring between the eMMN and LDN. Closer examination of the data does, in fact, suggest that for some of the children there may be a P3b-like component present. This component is not within the scope of this paper and warrants a more complete examination. In relation to the LDN in attend conditions, however, we propose that the presence of the P3b-like peak supports our contention that children are more reliant on active attentional processes in resolving these discriminations as compared to the more automatic processes observed in adults. Interestingly there were no associations between the eMMN and the LDN, suggesting that separate mechanisms may underlie these peaks.

A number of hypotheses have been proposed to account for how attention may function to stimulate change in the auditory cortex. These include either the recruitment of more neural resources during the processing of a stimulus or event [9295] or an increase in synchrony among neural ensembles responding to the stimuli [96, 97], or both [75, 98]. It has been shown that focusing attention on salient information drives neural dynamics and that selective attention increases both gain and feature selectivity of the mature human auditory cortex [99, 100]. Thus it is likely that either one of these processes may then drive plasticity so that neural responses to sensory events that were initially voluntary become automatic.

An extremely interesting question that unfortunately is outside the scope of this study revolves around the idea that the attention system is key in establishing the memory trace of the standard stimulus (the fidelity of the standard stimuli) to which the deviant stimulus will then be compared [101, 102]. This implies that the MMN functions as a deviance detection device and that the quality of the MMN response will be very dependent upon the fidelity of the standard memory trace. Studies have shown that attention does, in fact, modulate the standard response and not necessarily the deviant one (see [72, 103, 104]). Thus attending to relevant acoustic input may enhance and refine auditory representations established during passive exposure, resulting in greater automaticity of auditory processing and in the short term, may drive plasticity, so that the neural response attains automaticity [105]. However, there is some evidence that top down factors may also modulate initial stimulus-driven auditory organization [102, 103]. It is clear that additional studies are necessary in order to further identify the specific attentional mechanisms and pathways that are engaged in development of acoustic representations.

In sum, the present set of experiments demonstrates that mismatch negativity in adults is influenced by selective attention directed to the auditory modality and modulated as a function of presentation rate. In 6- to 11-year-old children both rate and attention modulation of the two developmental mismatch components were observed, with the eMMN and LDN reflecting somewhat different aspects of auditory discrimination. Thus, in children the eMMN categorically varied as a function of rate, dependent upon whether the stimuli fell inside or outside the developing temporal window of integration, rather than on a “slow to fast” continuum. Moreover, the children’s mismatch components were shown to be differentially influenced by attention in a manner that is consistent with the purported mechanisms they represent. The eMMN reflects early, automatic discrimination and thus attention appears to facilitate processing, while the LDN, purported to index further processing of complex stimuli, is modulated by attention.

Taken together, the present findings contribute to a better understanding of RAP abilities, as indexed by ERP mismatch responses, in mature adults and young school-age children. The results from this set of studies are consistent with the idea that the ability to automatically process subtle auditory changes improves over time due to both maturation and experience-related changes in the brain, and that attention plays a critical part in developing and refining neural auditory processing mechanisms. Given the importance of attention in fine-tuning typically developing auditory processing abilities, one can also posit that atypically developing populations can benefit as well from processing these types of auditory stimuli, which explicitly focuses on engaging the attentional system, thereby normalizing the auditory system.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the families who participated, and the members of the Infancy Studies Laboratory for their practical and intellectual contributions.

Data Availability

Due to ethical restrictions imposed by the Rutgers University Institutional Review Board related to protecting patient confidentiality, all relevant data are available upon request to Naseem Choudhury (nchoudhu@ramapo.edu), Jessica Parascando (jparascando3@gmail.com) or April Benasich (abenasich@andromeda.rutgers.edu).

Funding Statement

Funding was provided by the Elizabeth H. Solomon Center for Neurodevelopmental Research. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Benasich AA, Choudhury N. Timing, information processing, and efficacy: early factors that impact childhood language trajectories In: Benasich AA, Fitch HR, editors. Developmental dyslexia. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc; 2012. pp. 99–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kuhl PK. Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron. 2010;67: 713–727. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Musacchia G, Choudhury NA, Ortiz-Mantilla S, Realpe-Bonilla T, Roesler CP, & Benasich AA. Oscillatory support for rapid frequency change processing in infants. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(13): 2812–2824. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Hämäläinen JA, Guttorm TK, Richardson U, Alku P, Lyytinen H, Leppänen PHT. Auditory event related potentials measured in kindergarten predict later reading problems at school age. Dev Neuropsychol. 2013;38(8): 550–566. 10.1080/87565641.2012.718817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Raschle NM, Stering PL, Meissner SN, & Gaab N. Altered neuronal response during rapid auditory processing and Its relation to phonological processing in prereading children at familial risk for dyslexia. Cereb Cortex. 2013. 10.1093/cercor/bht104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Vandermosten M, Boets B, Luts H, Poelmans H, Golestani N, Wouters J, et al. Adults with dyslexia are impaired in categorizing speech and nonspeech sounds on the basis of temporal cues. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107: 10389–10394. 10.1073/pnas.0912858107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hämäläinen JA, Salminen HK, & Leppanen PHT. Basic Auditory Processing Deficits in Dyslexia Systematic Review of the Behavioral and Event-Related Potential/Field Evidence. J Learn Disabil. 2013;46(5): 413–427. 10.1177/0022219411436213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Heim S, & Benasich AA. Disorders of language In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental psychopathology. 2nd ed. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 2006. pp. 268–316. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Fitch H, & Tallal P. Neural mechanisms of language based learning impairments: insights from human populations and animal models. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2003;2(3): 155–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Tallal P, & Benasich AA. Language learning disabilities. Dev Psychopathol. 2002; 14: 559–579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Leonard LB. Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Moore JK, & Guan YL. Cytoarchitectural and axonal maturation in human auditory cortex. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2001;2(4): 297–311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Trehub SE, Schneider BA, & Henderson JL. Gap detection in infants, children, and adults. J Acoust Soc Am. 1995;98: 2532–2541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Werner LA, Marean GC, Halpin CF, Spetner NB, & Gillenwater JM. Infant auditory temporal acuity: gap detection. Child Dev. 1992;63: 260–272. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Banai K, & Ahissar M. Auditory processing deficits in dyslexia: Task or stimulus related? Cereb Cortex. 2006;16(12): 1718–1728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Sutcliffe P, & Bishop D. Psychophysical design influences frequency discrimination performance in young children. J Exp Child Psychol. 2005;91: 249–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Werner LA, & Marean GC. Human auditory development. Boulder, CO.: Westview Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Moore JK. Maturation of human auditory cortex: Implications for speech perception. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2002;Suppl. 189: 7–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Sussman E, Steinschneider M, Gumenyuk V, Grushko J, & Lawson K. The maturation of human evoked brain potential to sound presented at different stimulus rates. Hear Res. 2008;236: 61–79. 10.1016/j.heares.2007.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Näätänen R, Kujala T, Escera C, Baldeweq T, Kreegipuu K, Carlson S, et al. The mismatch negativity (MMN)—a unique window to disturbed central auditory processing in ageing and different clinical conditions. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123(3): 424–458. 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Choudhury N & Benasich AA. Maturation of auditory evoked potentials from 6 to 48 months: prediction to 3 and 4-year language and cognitive abilities. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122(2): 320–338. 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, & Alho K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118(12): 2544–2590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Paavilainen P. The mismatch-negativity (MMN) component of the auditory event-related potential to violations of abstract regularities: a review. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;88(2): 109–123. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.03.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Alho K. Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes. Ear Hear. 1995;16(1): 38–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Näätänen R. Attention and brain function. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Allen J, Kraus N. and Bradlow AR. Neural representation of consciously imperceptible speech stimuli. Percept Psychophys. 2000;62: 1383–1393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Shafer VL, Schwartz RG, Morr ML, Kessler KL, Kurtzberg D, & Ruben RJ. Neurophysiological indices of language impairment in children. Acta Otolaryngol. 2001;121(2): 297–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM, Heit G, Marinkovic K, Devaux B, et al. Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. II. Medial, lateral and posterior temporal lobe. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1995;94: 229–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell T, King C, Littman T, & Nicol T. Discrimination of speech-like signals in auditory thalamus and cortex. J Acoust Soc Am. 1994a;96: 2758–2768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Kraus N, McGee T, Littman T, Nicol T, & King C. Encoding of acoustic change involves non-primary auditory thalamus. J Neurophysiol. 1994b;72: 1270–1277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Sams M & Hari R. Magnetoencephalography in the study of human auditory information processing. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1991;620(1): 102–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Scherg M, Vajsar J, & Picton TW. A source analysis of the late human auditory evoked potentials. J Cogn Neurosci. 1989;1: 336–355. 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.4.336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Giraud AL, Collet L, Chéry-Croze S, Magnan J, & Chays A. Evidence of a medial olivocochlear involvement in contralateral suppression of otoacoustic emissions in humans. Brain Res. 1995;705(1): 15–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Thomas DG, & Crow CD. Development of evoked electrical brain activity in infancy In: Dawson G, Fischer KW, editors. Human behavior and the developing brain. New York: The Guilford Press; 1994. pp. 207–231. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Shafer VL, Morr ML, Kreuzer JA, & Kurtzberg D. Maturation of mismatch negativity in school-age children. Ear Hear. 2000;21(3): 242–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Martin A, Shafer VL, Morr ML, Kreuzer JA, & Kurtzberg D. Maturation of mismatch negativity: a scalp current density analysis. Ear Hear. 2003;24: 463–471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Gomot M, Giard MH, Roux S, Barthelemy C, & Bruneau N. Maturation of frontal and temporal components of mismatch negativity (MMN) in children. NeuroReport. 2000;11: 3109–3112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Eggermont JJ. Physiology of the developing auditory system In: Trehub SE, Schneider B, editors. Auditory development in infancy. New York: Plenum; 1988a. pp. 21–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Eggermont JJ. On the maturation of sensory evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1988b;70: 293–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Eggermont JJ. Development of auditory evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;112: 197–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Eggermont JJ. Is there a neural code? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1998;22(2): 355–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Huttenlocher PR, De Courten C, Garey LG, & Van der Loos H. The synaptogenesis in human visual cortex-evidence for synapse elimination during normal development. Neurosci Lett. 1982;33: 247–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Morr ML, Shafer VL, Kreuzer JA, & Kurtzberg D. Maturation of mismatch negativity in typically developing infants and preschool children. Ear Hear. 2002;23: 118–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Bishop DV, Hardiman MJ, & Barry JG. Is auditory discrimination mature by middle childhood? A study using time-frequency analysis of mismatch responses from 7 years to adulthood. Dev Sci,. 2011;14(2): 402–416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Korpilahti P, Lang H. Auditory ERP components and mismatch negativity in dysphasic children. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;91: 256–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell T, Sharma A, & Nicol T. Mismatch negativity to speech stimuli in school-age children. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1995b;88(2): 123–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Kraus N, McGee T, Sharma A, Carrell T & Nicol T. Mismatch negativity event-related potential elicited by speech stimuli. Ear Hear. 1992;13: 158–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Cheour M, Leppänen PH, & Kraus N. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and auditory sensory memory in infants and children: a review. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;117: 4–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Csépe V, Pantev C, Hampson S, Ross B, & Hoke M. Mismatch field to minor pitch changes in single and paired tones: neuromagnetic source localization from derived fields. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1995;44: 204–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Kraus N, McGee T, Micco A, Sharma A, Carrell T, & Nicol T. Mismatch negativity in school-age children to speech stimuli are just perceptibly different. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993b;88: 123–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Oades RD, Dittmann-Balcar A, & Zerbin D. Development and topography of auditory event-related potentials (ERPs): Mismatch and processing negativity in individuals 8–22 years of age. Psychophysiol. 1997;34: 677–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Cheour M, Korpilahti P, Martynova O, & Lang AH. Mismatch negativity and late discriminative negativity in investigating speech perception and learning in children and infants. Audiol Neurootol. 2001;6(1): 2–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Korpilahti P, Krause CM, Holopainen I, & Lang HA. Early and late mismatch negativity elicited by words and speech-like stimuli in children. Brain Lang. 2001;76: 332–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Kraus N, McGee T, Ferre J, Hoeppner JA, Carrell T, Sharma A, & Nicol T. Mismatch negativity in the neurophysiologic/behavioral evaluation of auditory processing deficits: a case study. Ear Hear. 1993a;14(4): 223–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Ceponiené R, Lepistö T, Soininen M, Aronen E, Alki P, & Näätänen R. Event-related potentials associated with sound discrimination versus novelty detection in children. Psychophysiol. 2004;41(1): 130–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Ceponiené R, Yaguchi K, Shestakova A, Alku P, Suominen K, & Näätänen R. Sound complexity and ‘speechness’ effects on pre-attentive auditory discrimination in children. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;43(3): 199–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Ceponiené R, Cheour M, & Näätänen R. Interstimulus interval and auditory event-related potentials in children: evidence for multiple generators. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol: Evoked Potentials. 1998;108: 345–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Korpilahti P, Krause CM, Holopainen I, & Lang HA. The late MMN wave in normal and Language impaired children. Pathophysiol. 1998;5(1): 202. [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Bitz U, Gust K, Spitzer M, & Kiefer M. Phonological deficit in school children is reflected in the mismatch negativity. NeuroReport. 2007;18(9): 911–915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Picton TW. The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;9(4): 456–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Hämäläinen JA, Leppänen PHT, Guttorm TK & Lyytinen H. Event-related potentials to pitch and rise time change in children with reading disabilities and typically reading children. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(1): 100–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Wetzel N, & Schröger E. On the development of auditory distraction: A review. PsyCh J. 2014;3(1): 72–91. 10.1002/pchj.49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Hommet C, Vidal J, Roux S, Blanc R, Barthez MA, De Becque B, et al. Topography of syllable change-detection electrophysiological indices in children and adults with reading disabilities. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(3): 761–770. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Alho K, Woods DL, Algazi A, Näätänen R. Intermodal selective attention: II. Effects of attentional load on processing auditory and visual stimuli in central space. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;82: 356–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Trejo LJ, Ryan-Jones DL, & Kramer AF. Attentional modulation of the mismatch negativity elicited by frequency differences between binaurally presented tone bursts. Psychophysiol. 1995;32(4): 319–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Ortiz-Mantilla S, Choudhury N, Alvarez B, & Benasich AA. Involuntary switching of attention mediates differences in event-related responses to complex tones between early and late Spanish-English bilinguals. Brain Res. 2010;1362: 78–92. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Rinker T, Kohls G, Richter C, Maas V, Schulz E, & Schecker M. Abnormal frequency discrimination in children with SLI as indexed by mismatch negativity (MMN). Neurosci Lett. 2007;41(2): 99–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Alonso-Bua B, Diaz F, & Ferraces MJ. The contribution of AERPs (MMN and LDN) to studying temporal vs. linguistic processing deficits in children with reading difficulties. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;59: 159–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Uwer R, Albrecht R, & von Suchodoletz W. Automatic processing of tones and speech stimuli in children with specific language impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44: 527–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Shafer VL, Morr ML, Datta H, Kurtzberg D, & Schwartz RG. Neurophysiological indices of speech processing deficits in children with specific language impairment. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17: 1168–1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Kraus N, McGee TJ, Carrell TD, Zecker SG, Nicol TG, & Koch DB. Auditory neurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children with learning problems. Science. 1996;273: 971–973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Sussman E. A new view on the MMN and attention debate: Auditory context effects. J Psychophysiol. 2007;21(3): 164–175. [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Tiitinen H, Jiang D, & Alho K. Attention and memory. Psychophysiol. 1993a;30(5): 436–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Woods DL, Alho K, & Algazi A. Intermodal selective attention. I. Effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;82: 341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Gomes H, Molholm S, Christodoulou C, Ritter W, & Cowan N. The development of auditory attention in children. Front Biosci. 2000;5: 107–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Sussman E, & Steinschneider M. Attention effects on auditory scene analysis in children. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47: 771–785. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Sculthorpe LD, Stelmack RM, & Campbell KB. Mental ability and the effect of pattern violation discrimination on P300 and mismatch negativity. Intelligence. 2009;37(4): 405–411. [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Benasich AA, Thomas JJ, Choudhury N, & Leppänen PHT. The importance of Rapid auditory processing abilities to early language development: Evidence from converging methodologies. Dev Psychobiol. 2002;40(3): 278–292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Benasich AA, & Tallal P. Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136(1): 31–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Choudhury N & Benasich AA. A family aggregation study: The influence of family history and other risk factors on language development. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46: 261–272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Benasich AA, Choudhury N, Friedman JT, Realpe-Bonilla T, Chojnowska C, & Gou Z. Infants as a prelinguistic model for language learning impairments: Predicting from event-related potentials to behavior. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(3): 396–411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Friedman J. Development and plasticity of the mismatch negativity in typically developing children, children with language impairments, and adults. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Newark (NJ): Rutgers University. 2009.
  • 83. Thomas JJ, Leppänen PHT, Choudhury N, Abu Shaba J, & Benasich AA. Separation of afferent (N1) and change detection (MMN) responses demonstrated by varying gap durations within tone pairs. Poster presented at the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CA; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Berg P, & Scherg M. A multiple source approach to the correction of eye artifacts. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;90: 229–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Ille N, Berg P, & Scherg M. A spatial components method for continuous artifact correction in EEG and MEG. Biomed Eng. 1997;42(suppl. 1): 80–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Ille N, Berg P, & Scherg M. Artifact correction of the ongoing EEG using spatial filters based on artifact and brain signal topographies. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;19(2): 113–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Luu P, Ferree T. Determination of the Geodesic Sensor Nets’ average electrode positions and their 10–10 international equivalents Technical note. Electrical Geodesics Inc., Oregon, US; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Sams M, Hämäläinen M, Antervo A, Kaukoranta E, Reinikainen K, & Hari R. Cerebral neuromagnetic responses evoked by short auditory stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;61: 254–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Alho K, Reinikainen K, & Sams M. Do event-related potentials reveal the mechanism of the auditory sensory memory in the human brain? Neurosci Lett. 1989;98(2): 217–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Paavilainen P, Karlsson ML, Reinikainen K, & Näätänen R. Mismatch negativity to change in spatial location of an auditory stimulus. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1989;73(2): 129–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Sussman E, & Steinschneider M. Attention modifies sound level detection in young children. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2011;1(3): 351–360. 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Jancke L, Mirzazade S, & Shah NJ. Attention modulates activity in the primary and the secondary auditory cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects. Neurosci Lett. 1999;266(2): 125–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Grady CL, VanMeter JW, Maisog JM, Pietrini P, Krasuski J, & Rauschecker JP. Attention-related modulation of activity in primary and secondary auditory cortex. NeuroReport. 1997;8(11): 2511–2516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Woodruff PW, Benson RR, Bandettini PA, Kwong KK, Howard RJ, Talavage T, et al. Modulation of auditory and visual cortex by selective attention is modality-dependent. NeuroReport. 1996;7: 1909–1913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Woldorff MG, Gallen CC, Hampson SA, Hillyard SA, Pantev C, Sobel D, et al. Modulation of early sensory processing in human auditory cortex during auditory selective attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90 (18): 8722–8726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Niebur E, Hsiao SS, & Johnson KO. Synchrony: a neuronal mechanism for attentional selection? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12(2): 190–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Steinmetz PN, Roy A, Fitzgerald P, Hsiao SS, Johnson KO, & Niebur E. Attention modulates synchronized neuronal firing in primate somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2000;404: 187–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98. Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Tiitinen H, Jiang D, & Alho K. Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. NeuroReport. 1993b;4: 503–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99. Kauramäki J, Jääskeläinen IP, & Sams M. Selective attention increases both gain and feature selectivity of the human auditory cortex. PLoS ONE. 2007;2(9): e909 10.1371/journal.pone.0000909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100. Woods DL, Stecker GC, Rinne T, Herron TJ, Cate AD, Yund EW, et al. Functional maps of human auditory cortex: Effects of acoustic features and attention. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(4): e5183 10.1371/journal.pone.0005183 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101. Sussman E. Attention matters: pitch vs. pattern processing in adolescence. Front Dev Psychol. 2013;4(333): 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102. Muenssinger J, Stingl KT, Matuz T, Binder G, Ehehalt S, & Preissl H. Auditory habituation to simple tones: reduced evidence for habituation in children compared to adults. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103. Sussman E, Steinschneider M, Lee W, & Lawson K. Auditory scene analysis in school-aged children with developmental language disorders. Int J of Psychophysiol. 2014. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104. Sadia G, Ritter W, & Sussman E. Category effects: Is top-down control alone sufficient to elicit the mismatch negativity (MMN) component? Biol Psychol. 2013;92(2): 191–198. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105. Benasich AA, Choudhury N, Realpe-Bonilla T, & Roesler CP. Plasticity in developing brain: Active auditory exposure impacts prelinguistic acoustic mapping. J Neurosci. 2014;34(40): 13349–13363. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0972-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Due to ethical restrictions imposed by the Rutgers University Institutional Review Board related to protecting patient confidentiality, all relevant data are available upon request to Naseem Choudhury (nchoudhu@ramapo.edu), Jessica Parascando (jparascando3@gmail.com) or April Benasich (abenasich@andromeda.rutgers.edu).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES