Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
. 2015 Sep 15;6:1381. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01381

There or not there? A multidisciplinary review and research agenda on the impact of transparent barriers on human perception, action, and social behavior

Gesine Marquardt 1, Emily S Cross 2,3, Alexandra A de Sousa 4, Eve Edelstein 5, Alessandro Farnè 6, Marcin Leszczynski 7, Miles Patterson 8, Susanne Quadflieg 9,10,*
PMCID: PMC4569749  PMID: 26441756

Abstract

Through advances in production and treatment technologies, transparent glass has become an increasingly versatile material and a global hallmark of modern architecture. In the shape of invisible barriers, it defines spaces while simultaneously shaping their lighting, noise, and climate conditions. Despite these unique architectural qualities, little is known regarding the human experience with glass barriers. Is a material that has been described as being simultaneously there and not there from an architectural perspective, actually there and/or not there from perceptual, behavioral, and social points of view? In this article, we review systematic observations and experimental studies that explore the impact of transparent barriers on human cognition and action. In doing so, the importance of empirical and multidisciplinary approaches to inform the use of glass in contemporary architecture is highlighted and key questions for future inquiry are identified.

Keywords: extra-personal space, evidence-based design, translucency, transparency, multisensory integration

Introduction

Everyday experience attests that transparent barriers dominate modern architecture. As surfaces, apertures, windows, or walls, transparent panes of various sizes characterize numerous settings of significance, including airports, offices, schools, hospitals, restaurants, shops, homes, and exhibitions – to name just a few. According to renowned architect Richards (2006), the human fascination with erecting such barriers comes from the fact that they can be there and not there at the same time. In other words, while such barriers spatially separate and confine places (including their noise and climate conditions), they simultaneously keep these spaces visually connected. Thus, transparent barriers transmit light in a manner that enables us to see what is beyond them without allowing us to directly approach (or be approached by) what we see. In spite of these unique architectural qualities, the impact of transparent barriers on human cognition and behavior has not yet attracted much scientific attention.

This oversight is surprising given that the optimal architectural use of transparent structures depends not only on esthetic concerns, technological progress, and cost-efficiency, but also on the materials potential to serve setting-specific human needs and goals (cf. Steiner and Veel, 2015). In addressing the latter two issues, this article examines what is currently known, but also what remains to be studied, about human functioning in the presence of transparent barriers. More specifically, this review explores the extent to which barriers that are simultaneously there and not there from an architectural perspective, are actually there and/or not there from perceptual, behavioral, and social points of view. The reader is first introduced to the role of transparent barriers as architectural elements throughout the centuries. Subsequently, systematic observations and experimental studies that have begun to quantify the impact of transparent barriers on human functioning are summarized and discussed. Finally, the practical importance of empirical investigations on transparent barrier use and their real-world consequences is elucidated1.

The Architectural Use of Transparent Barriers

The use of transparent barriers for architectural purposes was originally intertwined with the human ability to produce glass (cf. Miodownik, 2013). Though the first intentional production of glass by humans took place in the form of beads around 3000 BC (Oppenheim et al., 1970), it was the advancement of a new architecture for cathedrals and churches in the Middle Ages (5th–14th century) that stimulated the production of transparent glass panes in Europe (Elkadi, 2006). During that time, glass began to serve as a decorative material that filled wall openings between load-bearing structures such as pillars, allowing daylight to penetrate buildings in an unprecedented manner (Marks, 1993). Although early glass panes varied in color and translucency, they were generally of modest size. To portray large images of Biblical scenes, several small panes would occasionally be joined together by metal frames. The creation of bigger and fully transparent panels, however, required new methods of glass production. Developed in the Baroque era (15th–18th century), these new production methods came at a significant expense, confining the use of generous glass elements mainly to palaces of the nobility and religious buildings (i.e., cathedrals, churches, and cloisters). The architecture of common dwellings, by contrast, involved translucent filters, created from canvas or animal skins.

The modest style of everyday housing quickly changed in the increasingly wealthy European cities at the beginning of the 16th century (Staib, 2008). Especially in Dutch merchants’ houses, load-bearing structures of bricks were increasingly separated from walls that featured large glass openings, enabling passersby to glance at a buildings’ interior. This tendency toward publicizing the private life was further reinforced through the influence of the Calvinists in the 17th century who aimed to demonstrate their pious way of living to God as well as their neighbors (Vera, 1989). Similarly, in 18th century Britain, bow-windows became a popular feature of shops and houses, turning ‘looking in’ as well as ‘looking out’ into common pastimes (Lindsay, 2009). Despite these architectural advances, until the 19th century, glass was primarily used to cover wall openings. Though load-bearing building structures were continuously minimized, stone and clay remained the materials of choice to enclose the interior space. It was not until the development of cast and wrought iron that architects were able to construct magnificent buildings from steel and glass alone. At the end of the 1820s, the first large-scale use of sheet glass appeared in the Parisian Arcades (McQuire, 2003). In 1851, the English architect Joseph Paxton designed and built the Crystal Palace that hosted the Great Exhibition in London (McKean, 1994). The iconic, largely transparent building paved the way for a modern exploration of clear glass in architecture.

In 1914, the German architect and urban planner Bruno Taut constructed a structure from concrete and glass for the Cologne Werkbund Exhibition, known as the Glass Pavilion (Nielsen and Kumarasuriyar, 2014). The pavilion (for a detailed description see Haag Bletter, 1981) was financed by the German glass industry and aimed to illustrate the potential of different types of glass for architecture (Weston, 2004). During the same year, the German writer Paul Scheerbart dedicated an influential monograph to Taut, called Glass Architecture (Scheerbart, 1914). In this monograph, the author called for eliminating closed rooms by introducing large scale glass walls, rather than mere windows, in future buildings. In line with his suggestion, many leading European architects produced glass designs in the 1920s (cf. Korn, 1968; McQuire, 2003). Based on this progress, the founder of the Bauhaus, Walther Gropius, concluded that glass architecture had overcome its status as a poetic utopia and had turned into an unconstrained reality (Gropius, 1926). Rapid technological innovations, such as the discovery of crack-preventing laminations and coatings and the development of powerful adhesives that allowed connecting multiple glass panels almost seamlessly, further spurred the widespread use of transparent glass (Staib, 2008; Weller and Vogt, 2012). The architecture critics Johnson and Hitchcock (1932) described the cross-cultural dissemination of glass architecture as a signal of globalized minimalistic and functionalist architectural tendencies.

To date, countries around the world use transparent glass in large scale building projects to demonstrate their progress, modernity, and wealth (Kulterman, 1999; Dawson, 2005). This development is intriguing, considering that in many geographic regions (such as the Gulf States) the use of glass leads to tremendous solar gains in a building’s interior, a circumstance that must be countered with energy-consuming air conditioning (Aboulnaga, 2006; Schittich, 2011). Despite this challenge, architects have continued to design and construct glass buildings worldwide (cf. Wigginton, 1996; Krampen and Schempp, 1999; Bell and Kim, 2009). Supported by the emergence of Computer Aided Design (CAD) software in the very early 21st century, the latest developments in glass architecture defy traditional constructional boundaries. Modern glass buildings can form amorphous structures of curvilinear, topographic design that lack right angles or symmetry, commonly referred to as Blob architecture (Lynn, 2009). Pivotal examples of contemporary Blob architecture include the National Centre for the Performing Arts in Bejing2 or the Great Glass House in the National Botanic Garden of Wales3. Aside from its role in large-scale architectural structures, glass has also penetrated modern domestic architecture, as illustrated by Ludwig Mies van der Rohe’s Farnsworth House4 or Philip Johnson’s Glass House5.

Despite their global distribution, the use of transparent barriers in contemporary construction seems largely governed by esthetic, financial, legal, structural, and city planning concerns (e.g., Elkadi, 2006; Richards, 2006; Haldimann et al., 2008; Staib, 2008; Arbab and Finley, 2010). The human response to these structures, in contrast, suffers from a lack of consideration and systematic evaluation (cf. Sommer, 1974; Hamilton and Watkins, 2009). Yet, among rare advocates of a psychological approach, the indiscriminate endorsement of transparent architecture has caused skepticism (cf. Widrich, 2015). For instance, as early as in 1963, the architectural critic Colin Rowe and the artist Robert Slutzky distinguished between literal transparency (i.e., the “quality of a substance”, p. 46) and phenomenal transparency (i.e., “an intellectual imperative […] for that which should be easily detected”, p. 45) in order to emphasize that physical and psychological states of transparency could, but would not necessarily have to, co-occur in glass buildings. In further support of their argument, the architectural historian Haag Bletter (1981) directly challenged the notion that transparent structures could somehow promote personal or societal progress. The contemporary architects Vidler (1992) and Colomina (2009), finally, went so far as to argue that transparent barriers could even pose psychological hazards by producing an uncanny loss of privacy. Given these concerns and the assumption that architectural structures are generally meant to benefit people, it must be asked whether and how transparent barriers actually impact human functioning and well-being (cf. Stone and Irvine, 1993). To answer this question in a systematic manner and to ultimately optimize the use of transparent barriers in their manifold manifestations, a thorough, data-driven understanding of the human response to such barriers is needed.

Transparent Barriers: A Visual Challenge

Before being able to evaluate the consequences of transparent barriers on human behavior and wellbeing, it is of crucial importance to understand how people usually notice their presence.

Although transparent materials are frequently detected on the basis of accidental cracks, traces of adhesives, or dirt in everyday life, humans are surprisingly skilled at seeing transparent structures even in the absence of such opaque markers. However, the visual perception of transparent barriers seems largely guided by perceivers’ experience-based expectations of where to find them (e.g., embedded in window frames, Sayim and Cavanagh, 2011). In addition, a person’s situation-specific monitoring for such panes (maybe resulting from a previous collision) and the detection of visual cues that signal the presence of an invisible structure are of essential importance (Awh et al., 2012). The cues that “give rise to the perception of two surfaces, one of which is seen through the other” (Metelli, 1974a, p. 95) have puzzled artists (e.g., Albers, 1963) and vision scientists (e.g., Helmholtz, 1867) for decades. On the basis of their work, it is now assumed that transparency, unlike other visual features such as shape or color, is detected through a combination of informative cues (Wolfe et al., 2005). Phrased differently, the human visual system must integrate various pieces of information to decipher that something see-through (i.e., non-opaque) is present (Kersten et al., 1992).

Pivotal cues that facilitate the detection of transparent entities are related to their reflectance and transmittance properties (Brzezicki, 2013a). Whereas reflectance properties describe the manner in which light is reflected by a material, transmittance properties refer to the way that light passes through it. Interestingly, transparent materials tend to possess specific surface reflectance properties that can give away their presence (Beck and Prazdny, 1981; Blake and Bülthoff, 1990). In particular, glossiness and highlights (see Figure 1A) are known to draw perceivers’ attention to transparent structures (Motoyoshi, 2010; Sayim and Cavanagh, 2011). Equally effective at attracting people’s attention to transparency are failures of light transmittance. Although transparent barriers, per definition, transmit the light that falls upon them, the quality of this transmission can vary based on their thickness and unevenness. Specifically, processes of diffusion (i.e., the spreading of light) or refraction (i.e., the bending of light) can result in image distortions that perceivers readily use to infer the presence of a transparent structure (see Figure 1B; Fleming et al., 2011). In a closely related manner, the detection of transparent barriers can be aided by their coloring (i.e., due to glazing). Under such conditions, even transparent barriers can absorb some light, thereby producing systematic luminance differences for entities that are seen through them (i.e., affecting their lightness, brightness, and contrast appearance; Masin, 2006; Kingdom, 2011).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

A computer-generated image that conveys the impression of a smooth, pebble-shaped, transparent blob inside a textured box. Transparency is conveyed by (A) reflections of light (e.g., highlights) as well as by (B) refractions of light (e.g., image distortions such as changes in texture and misalignments of borders due to bending of light). Source: From Fleming et al. (2011). Copyright 2011 American Psychological Association; reprinted by permission.

The impact of luminance cues on transparency perception was originally explored by the Italian psychologist Metelli (1970, 1974a,b). Metelli demonstrated that specific luminance differences between neighboring parts of a surface would induce the impression that some parts were seen directly, whereas others were seen through a transparent layer (see Figure 2A). In addition to these so-called photometric cues, geometric cues play a pivotal role in transparency detection. The configuration of contour junctions, for instance, frequently signals the relation between two surfaces. Whereas T-junctions typically imply occlusion (Figure 3A; Rubin, 2001; Hillstrom et al., 2013), contours aligned in the way of an X-junction (see Figure 3B) indicate transparency (Metelli, 1974b; Watanabe and Cavanagh, 1993; Rubin, 2001; Hillstrom et al., 2013). In a similar way, edge assignment can affect transparency perception (Nakayama et al., 1989; Qiu and von der Heydt, 2007). As illustrated in Figure 4A, the ellipse marks a border that is considered part of the contour of a small light-gray square. In Figure 4B, in contrast, the same visual input (but in a different context) is perceived as the overlapping edge between a horizontal transparent bar lying on top and a darker vertical bar underneath. Yet, when the corners of the light and dark squares are rounded off (Figure 4C), the ellipse again appears to mark the border of the small light square. These examples illustrate that numerous visual cues can induce the impression of seeing two surfaces along the same line of sight but at different levels of depths (Beck et al., 1984; Beck and Ivry, 1988; Singh and Anderson, 2002). Phrased differently, these cues enable perceivers to segregate or ‘scission’ visual input into several components, those that constitute a transparent pane and those that constitute opaque entities located behind it (Koffka, 1935; Robilotto et al., 2002; Delogu et al., 2010).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Illustration of the role of photometric cues in transparency detection. (A) A set of luminance relations (depicted as light > dark) signaling that the lighter square is seen as transparent and in front of the darker square on the farther surface. (B) A set of luminance relations failing to signal which square is seen as transparent and in front of the other.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Illustration of the role of geometric cues in transparency detection. (A) Opaque square and circle, with T-junctions marked by dashed circles. (B) Transparent square and circle, with X-junctions marked by dashed circles. Source: From Hillstrom et al. (2013). Copyright 2013 American Psychological Association; reprinted by permission.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Illustration of the interplay between border ownership assignment and transparency detection. The same edge (identified by an identical luminance difference throughout) is perceived as (A) part of the contour of the small square, (B) part of a black vertical bar that lies underneath a transparent horizontal bar, (C) again part of the contour of the small square. Source: From Qiu and von der Heydt (2007). Copyright 2007 Nature Publishing Group; reprinted by permission.

Unfortunately, this segregation process does not always succeed. For example, only specific combinations of luminance relations create the perception of transparency in a unique depth order (Kitaoka, 2005; Koenderink et al., 2010). Alternatively (see Figure 2B), perceptions of transparency with an ambiguous depth order due to non-diagnostic luminance relations are possible (creating the impression of so-called bistable transparency; Adelson and Anandan, 1990; Anderson, 1997; Delogu et al., 2010; Fukiage et al., 2014). In addition, if the shape of a transparent pane coincides with the shape of the background, no transparency may be seen (Arnheim, 1971). Such correspondence is not uncommon in architectural settings in which large glass panels fill a person’s entire field of view. Under such conditions, visual information about the transparent structures’ borders and edges remain inaccessible, making their detection particularly difficult (Brzezicki, 2014). Unsurprisingly, undetected transparent barriers can pose a serious health and safety risk. Accidents due to collisions with glass doors or walls have been reported for both children and adults (e.g., Gur et al., 2001; Algaze et al., 2012). Their occurrence has elicited attempts to monitor and standardize the architectural use of glass barriers. In the UK, for instance, health and safety regulations for workplaces require the conspicuous marking of windows and glass doors with warning stickers (United Kingdom Health and Safety Executive, 1992, Regulation 14). In Australia, it has been dictated that glass doors must be made from laminated or toughened glass to reduce the likelihood of serious injuries upon collision (see standards AS 1288 & AS2208; see Kimia et al., 2009; AGGA, 2011). In addition, architects worldwide have been urged to remember that very clear, smooth, faultless, and frameless panes of glass are most likely to result in detection failures (Brzezicki, 2013b).

Although the use of transparent structures in architecture is nowadays expected to be accompanied by a careful assessment of their visibility from different viewpoints and under different lighting conditions (daytime, nighttime, backlighting; Brzezicki, 2014), several important questions regarding the topic remain unanswered. For instance, rather than reflecting an inborn talent, the ability to perceive transparency seems to be actively acquired during the first months of one’s life (Johnson and Aslin, 2000). Exactly how and when the visual system establishes expertise with transparent structures or certain informative cues signaling their presence, however, is an issue of ongoing debate (cf. Otsuka et al., 2006; Kavšek, 2009). Equally unclear is whether frequent exposure to transparent barriers may facilitate a perceiver’s ability to spontaneously detect relevant visual cues. It also remains to be studied whether and to which extent distraction interferes with a person’s capacity to perceive transparent materials, for instance, by impairing the integration of several diagnostic cues. Given that undetected transparent barriers pose the risk of unintentional collisions, finding answers to the above questions is of pivotal relevance for their architectural use.

Transparent Barriers: A Multi-Sensory Challenge

Not only our sense of sight but also our sense of touch can signal the presence of transparent barriers. Consequently, transparent barrier can confine an individual to a limited physical space (i.e., the person’s sense of touch identifies an impenetrable barrier), while simultaneously providing visual access to a larger space (i.e., the person’s sense of vision fails to identify a barrier). The experience of such divergent sensory information is noteworthy because humans usually create representations of the space closely surrounding their bodies by integrating both tactile and visual information into one multisensory representation (Làdavas and Farnè, 2004; Spence et al., 2004). Compared to mere unisensory analyses, multisensory representations usually result in a more accurate perception of one’s environment (Ernst and Bülthoff, 2004), fostering individuals’ rapid and adaptive responses to their surroundings (Calvert et al., 2004). But what happens when the two sensory systems convey conflicting information, as is commonly the case in the presence of transparent barriers?

Initial evidence indicates that when faced with conflicting information coming from tactile and visual sensors, humans rely more strongly on the latter, especially when the sensory contradictions arise within a person’s peripersonal space (cf. Làdavas et al., 2000). The term peripersonal space denotes the space closely surrounding a person’s body (see Figure 5; Previc, 1998; Caggiano et al., 2009; Brozzoli et al., 2014). Conflicting tactile and visual information occurring within this space can lead to intriguing effects. In many species, including humans, avoidance reactions such as head or hand withdrawals are easily triggered by quickly approaching objects that threaten to collide with individuals by entering their peripersonal space (Dunkeld and Bower, 1980; Makin et al., 2009; Serino et al., 2009). Importantly, so-called defensive withdrawal also occurs toward images of objects that rapidly grow in size, even if those images are merely projected on a screen. In other words, under conditions in which objects growing in size seem to approach an individual but actually have no chance of touching him or her, perceivers are still likely to show rapid startle and withdrawal responses toward these objects (King et al., 1992). These findings suggest that, given the appropriate visual input, awareness that one’s body is shielded from events occurring behind a transparent barrier may not suffice to suppress physical arousal and motor reflexes.

FIGURE 5.

FIGURE 5

The peripersonal space, mainly based on the integration of tactile and visual information coming from the body and the space directly surrounding the body, constitutes a privileged interface for the body to interact with nearby objects. This figure depicts the body schema, head and hand-centered space, and the peripersonal/arm-centered reaching space. Source: From Cardinali et al. (2010). Copyright 2010 Elsevier Publishing Group; reprinted by permission.

Indeed, the ineffectiveness of transparent barriers to interfere with basic reflexes is regularly taken advantage of in medical examinations. For instance, the so-called blink reflex (i.e., a rapid closure of the eyelids to a quickly approaching object such as a thrown ball) is generally tested by separating patients from approaching objects with a piece of plexiglass. Under these circumstances, the transparent barrier not only protects patients from the impact of an impending collision, but also enhances the test’s diagnostic value by shielding them from drafts caused by the motion of the object which may trigger the reflex in a non-visual manner (van Hof-van Duin and Mohn, 1986; Guzetta et al., 2001). Further evidence for a lack of interference of transparent barriers with basic reflexes comes from two behavioral studies exploring the occurrence of so-called visual–tactile interactions (Farnè et al., 2003; Kitagawa and Spence, 2005). The first of the two studies examined a group of tactile extinction patients (Farnè et al., 2003). Such patients tend to display an intriguing deficit: a visual sensation experienced in close proximity to one side of their body (e.g., a finger moving down close to their right hand, signaling the potential for touch) inhibits their ability to experience a simultaneously applied tactile sensation to their other side (e.g., a tap applied to the left hand). Crucially, this tactile extinction is less likely to occur the farther the wiggling finger is located from the patient’s hand (i.e., the farther it is located beyond a perceiver’s peripersonal space and the less likely it is to result in physical contact). This pattern of results signals an integrated visual–tactile system coding of peripersonal space in humans. But how is such coding affected by the introduction of a transparent barrier in close proximity to a person’s hand?

Under such conditions, the relevant finger simultaneously exists within a person’s peripersonal space (based on its visual properties), but also outside of it (due to being located behind the barrier). Results indicate that even when a wiggling finger is presented behind a transparent barrier, it triggers tactile extinction in these patients, to the same extent as when no barrier is interposed (Farnè et al., 2003). In other words, the patients’ automatic representation of their peripersonal space is unaffected by the presence of the barrier. Thus, the mere knowledge that actual touch is impossible does not modulate the observed extinction effect. A similar observation has been reported for healthy adults (Kitagawa and Spence, 2005). Adopting a so-called cross-modal congruency task, participants received tactile stimulation to either upper or lower portions of the left or right hand. They then had to indicate as quickly and accurately as possible the place of stimulation while simultaneously seeing one of two visual distractor lights near their hands (see Figure 6). Again, the presence of a transparent barrier placed between the tactile stimulation and the visual distractors did not impact participants’ performance, indicating that visual–tactile interactions in peripersonal space are unaffected by the presence of transparent barriers.

FIGURE 6.

FIGURE 6

Schematic illustration of the experimental set-up to study visuo–tactile interactions in healthy adults. In the transparent barrier condition, the participants’ hands were covered by a transparent Perspex occluder. Source: From Kitagawa and Spence (2005). Copyright 2005 Springer Publishing Group; reprinted by permission.

The above work suggests that people can be fully aware of being protected from collisions or touch by a transparent wall, yet simultaneously show rapid responses to approaching objects or people as if no wall was present. What remains to be studied is whether frequent exposure to and experience with transparent barriers may alter mechanisms of multisensory integration and/or people’s spontaneous responses (cf. Wesslein et al., 2015). The recurrent perception of objects or people behind a transparent barrier may lead to habituation, such that the strength and/or likelihood of reflexive responses toward entities on the other side of a transparent shield may decline over time. If such habituation can be observed, several related questions deserve scientific consideration: (a) How much exposure to transparent barriers does it generally take until habituation is noticeable (i.e., is it a rapid or time-consuming process)? (b) Do some people habituate more or less quickly when shielded by a transparent barrier than others and if so, why? (c) Can habituation effects be transferred across different settings (i.e., would a person that has worked behind a transparent barrier in one setting more quickly habituate when placed behind a transparent barrier in another setting)? Finally, (d) would the habituation of reflexes behind a transparent barrier ultimately compromise a person’s rapid responses in settings that lack such barriers?

Navigating Transparent Barriers

Further evidence that people are easily inclined to disregard the presence of transparent barriers comes from research on spatial perception and navigation. Navigational research with opaque barriers suggests that humans frequently rely on the presence of walls or fences to orient in space (Acredolo and Boulter, 1984; Herman et al., 1987; Han and Becker, 2013; Buckley et al., 2014). Although such a strategy benefits learning the location of meaningful objects and spatial layouts, it can also distort the representation of spatial information: Distances between entities in different subdivisions of space are judged larger than the same distances between objects located within the same subdivision (Allen, 1981; Maki, 1981, 1982; Newcombe and Liben, 1982). In the case of transparent barriers, however, such spatial overestimation effects seem to be absent (Montello, 2005). The first study on distance estimation, for instance, revealed that preschoolers exaggerated distances between objects separated by both opaque as well as transparent barriers, whereas adults only exaggerated distances across opaque barriers (Kosslyn et al., 1974). A subsequent study demonstrated that neither adults, nor school-aged children overestimated distances between objects separated by transparent barriers (Herman et al., 1987). The absence of a spatial overestimation effect for transparent barriers in viewers familiar with their occurrence indicates that these barriers are not spontaneously used as spatial delineators.

Alternatively, the space-delineating properties of transparent barriers may only arise under specific circumstances. For instance, for short, untraveled distances (i.e., when people merely look at a space, but do not walk around in it), transparent barriers seem to elicit larger (rather than smaller) distance overestimation effects than opaque barriers (Sherman et al., 1979). Moreover, transparent barriers may primarily affect observers’ spatial representations when the barriers are placed between them and a target. Initial research on egocentric distance-to-target judgments suggests that such judgments are more accurate when perceivers are exposed to a continuous, homogenous texture ground surface than to a surface that contains a gap or an opaque barrier (Sinai et al., 1998; He et al., 2004; see Figure 7). Whether transparent barriers actually result in accurate or biased judgments is undetermined. An additional issue for further research on transparent barriers is whether distance-to-target judgments tend to be more accurate when taken outdoors (i.e., on a lawn) rather than indoors (i.e., in a hallway or lobby; Lappin et al., 2006). The effect appears to be mediated by the amount and kind of space that is visible beyond an actual target (so-called vista space, Witt et al., 2007). Yet again, due to a scarcity of empirical investigations, the effect of transparent barriers located in vista space on target-to-distance estimates is unclear. In summary, the effects of transparent barriers on spatial representations remain poorly understood. This lack of empirical insight is particularly worrisome as it undermines the ability to predict how people orient themselves in glass environments, a limitation that poses far-reaching health and safety concerns (i.e., in emergency flight situations; Piller and Sebrechts, 2003; Abu-Safieh, 2011).

FIGURE 7.

FIGURE 7

Schematic illustration of the experimental set-up to study distance-to-target judgments in healthy adults.

These concerns are perpetuated by the observation that humans must actively learn to treat transparent structures as physical barriers. Such learning is most notable in young infants who show a prevalent tendency for reaching or crawling into impermeable transparent surfaces. Butterworth (1977), for instance, tested 9-month-olds search abilities by hiding attractive toys behind opaque or transparent covers. Along similar lines, Diamond and Gilbert (1989) examined 7-11-months-olds’ reach strategies by putting toys in opaque and transparent boxes (see Figure 8). In both cases, children younger than 10 months of age tried to directly reach for toys “through” transparent surfaces, even though they readily reached around opaque barriers. Similar observations have also been made regarding young infants’ detour abilities. Lockman (1984), for example, used opaque and transparent barriers to investigate 8-months-olds’ ability to select a path to a goal. This work revealed that, up to 10-months of age, infants displayed significant difficulty in detour ability when obstructed by a transparent barrier. Specifically, they hesitated or refused to select an alternative path to a goal when their current path was blocked ‘merely’ by something transparent (cf. Lockman and Adams, 2001; Noland, 2008). This response pattern does not seem to result from an inability to see the barrier, but rather from a flawed assumption that transparent barriers can be physically penetrated (Johnson and Aslin, 2000; Shinskey and Munakata, 2001). Thus, once this assumption is rectified (e.g., by allowing 9-month-olds to play with transparent covers before testing them) infants systematically remove opaque as well as see-through covers before they reach for a toy of interest (Yates and Bremner, 1988).

FIGURE 8.

FIGURE 8

Experimental set-up to study infants’ toy retrieval in the presence of a transparent barrier. The arrows signal the required reaching behavior based on whether the transparent barrier (A) blocks or (B) fails to block direct retrieval. Source: From Diamond and Gilbert (1989). Copyright 1989 Elsevier Publishing Group; reprinted by permission.

These data demonstrate that negotiating transparent materials is developmentally dependent on interacting with them. The term social ontologies has been introduced to describe how human capabilities – both in terms of physical and cognitive abilities – are frequently forged through interactions with the human-made material world (Gosden, 2008). Research on barrier crossing provides further evidence that navigating transparent obstacles is an acquired skill. In studies on the topic, young children were asked to step over barriers that varied in height. Subsequently, thresholds for successful barrier crossing (i.e., crossings that do not result in damaging the obstacle) were compared for opaque and transparent barriers. Both 12- and 18-month-olds were found to successfully cross opaque barriers at larger thresholds than transparent barriers, suggesting that a child’s ability to perform an adequate motor response suffers when faced with transparency (Schmuckler, 1996). Additional research is necessary to determine if the observed difference reflects a reduced ability to use cues of transparency for adequate motor planning or whether colliding with transparent instead of opaque barriers may seem less severe to a naïve perceiver (cf. Schmuckler, 1996). Regardless of the underlying mechanism(s), however, these data signal that humans must actively learn to resist the alleged penetrability of transparent barriers.

Seminal work on the visual cliff further demonstrates the idea that transparent surfaces initially appear immaterial. In this work, the psychologists Gibson and Walk (1960) tested infants’ response to perceived downward depth using a horizontal transparent barrier that covered a cloth with a checkerboard pattern. While the transparent barrier sat directly on the cloth on one side of the apparatus, the cloth was dropped about four feet on its other side. In doing so, the researchers created an apparent cliff covered by a transparent pane (see Figure 9). In this setting, infants capable of crawling typically hesitated to cross the surface, despite encouragement from a parent on the other side of the cliff. The effect prevailed even when children were allowed to establish through touch that the surface was rigid and when they witnessed that a hard ball was able to bounce off the surface (Gibson and Schmuckler, 1989). Though it seems obvious that humans improve their ability to think of transparent barriers as impenetrable entities as they grow older, how this improvement takes place and to what extent is less clear. What seems evident is that even adults occasionally question the rigidity and durability of transparent barriers as they explore their environment.

FIGURE 9.

FIGURE 9

A mother urging her child from across the deep side of the visual cliff. Despite a transparent surface covering the cliff, the child hesitates to move forward. Source: From Gibson and Walk (1960). Copyright 1960 Nature Publishing Group; reprinted by permission.

This phenomenon is nicely illustrated by widespread responses toward glass walkways as erected over natural cliffs (e.g., in the Grand Canyon6; in the Tianmen Mountain7) or on top of modern buildings (e.g., the Transparent Observatory at the Oriental Pearl Radio and TV tower8). When entering such walkways, many visitors hesitate to step forward, despite witnessing other people standing or moving around on the same structure9 (Morse, 2011). Crucially, this hesitation tends to linger throughout a person’s initial steps and despite a contact-experience that signals the surface’s impenetrability. This phenomenon further confirms that in response to conflicting tactile and visual experiences humans are inclined to rely more strongly on the latter. Importantly, overcoming this overreliance on visual information in the presence of transparent barriers does not only seem to require extensive practice, but also consistent awareness of the problem at hand. People suffering from cognitive decline, such as Alzheimer patients, have been found to cope poorly with transparent environments. Their struggle to open glass doors (rather than trying to directly pass through them) or to look for alternative paths around transparent barriers suggests that they have lost the ability to overcome the visual illusion of penetrability caused by such structures (Passini et al., 2000).

The Impact of Transparent Barriers on Social Behavior

Aside from investigating people’s visual, tactile, and spatial representations of transparent barriers in their environment, the barriers’ impact on people’s social functioning has attracted initial scientific attention (Procter, 1970). To understand the consequences of transparent barriers on social interactions more fully, the interplay between these structures and the types of social behavior they may foster and/or hinder must be considered (cf. Drew, 1971; Knapp et al., 2014; Patterson and Quadflieg, 2015). The current section focuses therefore on two types of social situations in which transparent barriers may impact social behavior: situations in which people are separated from each other by transparent barriers and situations in which people are surrounded by transparent barriers. Before addressing these two types of situations, the link between social interactions and their physical environments is considered more broadly.

Decades of social–psychological research suggest that people’s physical surroundings fundamentally shape social processes related to privacy, crowding, interpersonal involvement, and territoriality. Privacy is best understood as a dialectic process through which people strive for some momentary optimal level of contact with others (Altman, 1975). A person’s temporary need for privacy may be violated by crowding. Crowding refers to a negative affective response elicited by a high density of people in a specific location (Stokols, 1972). To regulate privacy under conditions of crowding, people may adjust their interpersonal involvement with others, that is, they may avoid close interpersonal distance, mutual gaze or touch, facial expressions of approachability, and so on (Patterson, 2011, 2013). Alternatively, people may try to own and/or control access to specific physical locations, thus showing territorial behavior (Altman, 1975; Bell et al., 2001; Brown, 2009; Scannell and Gifford, 2010). Social discomfort resulting from privacy violations, crowding, unwanted interpersonal involvement, and territorial intrusions is a common experience in everyday life. Its occurrence is particularly likely when strangers share a common presence, as is the case in many public settings, such as restaurants, waiting rooms, elevators, public transportation, open plan offices, or checkout/ATM lines (e.g., Camperio and Malaman, 2002; Manzo, 2005; Evans and Wener, 2007; Li and Li, 2007). In managing negative experiences around privacy, crowding, interpersonal involvement, and territoriality people not only adjust their own non-verbal behavior relative to others, but also use and manipulate elements in their physical environment.

Barriers, both permanent (e.g., walls, columns, partitions) and/or movable (e.g., furniture or plants), play a pivotal role in the regulation of social interactions between strangers (Levitt and Weber, 1989; Manzo, 2005; Robson, 2008). These barriers, collectively termed anchors (Robson, 2002), can limit spatial access to a person and provide temporary screening from the sight, sound, or proximity of others (Robson, 2008). What remains uncertain is whether people also treat transparent barriers as architectural elements with anchoring qualities. Transparent barriers form unique kinds of barriers, given that they separate space physically, but not visually. Toddlers as young as 14 months of age understand that, unlike an opaque barrier, a transparent barrier fails to block another person’s line of sight (Dunphy-Lelii and Wellman, 2004). Thus, both children and adults assume that a physical separation through a transparent barrier does not interfere with the transmission of visual information. Given that the exchange of visual information remains unimpaired, to which extent does a separation of strangers by a transparent barrier impact social behavior?

Though architects have long claimed that the visual and acoustic permeability of barriers affects social encounters (Zeisel, 1981), empirical investigations on this topic remain rare. In consequence, the impact of transparent barriers on social behavior is poorly understood. Consider, for instance, the observation that most people try to maintain a minimal interpersonal distance from unfamiliar others (Hall, 1959; Sommer, 1959). When a stranger initiates an inappropriately close approach, arousal, and discomfort result (Sommer, 1959; McBride et al., 1965; Middlemist et al., 1976; Patterson, 1976; Hayduk, 1983). Interpersonal distance violations can precipitate flight from a setting or, at least, compensatory non-verbal behavior, such as turning away or avoiding gaze to reduce the effect of a stranger’s close presence (Sommer, 1969; Patterson et al., 1971; Patterson, 1973; Konečni et al., 1975). It remains unclear, however, whether people’s need for controlling their involvement with others depends on the presence or absence of transparent barriers between them. On one hand, it could be argued that transparent barriers should not alter interpersonal distance preferences because visual information from others remains unobstructed. Alternatively, because transparent barriers interfere with tactile, auditory, and olfactory input (Crusco and Wetzel, 1984; Haans and IJsselsteijn, 2006), responses to the perceived proximity of others may change in their presence. By enabling the visual processing of others while preventing a physical interaction, transparent barriers may alter how we respond to the proximity of strangers (see Figure 10).

FIGURE 10.

FIGURE 10

Schematic depiction of two people waiting at a bus stop illustrating an uninvestigated question of social relevance: Would the close presence of a stranger be more comfortable when occurring (A) across a transparent barrier or (B) without such a barrier? [Images of humans were downloaded from www.shutterstock.com and are reproduced in this manuscript in adherence with the company’s standard license terms of service (http://www.shutterstock.com/licensing.mhtml)].

Similarly unresolved is the issue of how transparent barriers affect tacit norms of looking behavior. Establishing eye contact with another person frequently serves as a signal to initiate (verbal or non-verbal) communication (Kleinke, 1986). Thus, when such communication is not sought, people tend to respect others’ privacy by not looking at each other (Argyle and Dean, 1965; Laidlaw et al., 2011). But how are looking norms affected by the presence of transparent barriers? Are people inclined to stare longer at others through transparent barriers, knowing that under these conditions they can gather social information without the obligation to engage in any further exchange? Equally important is the questions of whether targets of prolonged looks may be more comfortable with attracting someone’s gaze through a transparent barrier than when no barrier is present. Because neither a spatial intrusion, nor a verbal approach is likely to follow under such conditions, there may be less discomfort resulting from being looked at in the presence of a transparent barrier compared to a no barrier arrangement.

Social interactions may also change their course depending on whether they are taking place in a space surrounded by transparent barriers. Experimental studies have demonstrated, for instance, that close spatial proximity to strangers produces less discomfort in open than in confined spaces (Cochran et al., 1984). Along similar lines, encounters with members of social outgroups (e.g., people perceived as having a different racial background than a perceiver) elicit associations related to a fight when they occur in a small booth, but to flight when they occur in an open field (Cesario et al., 2010). In other words, confined spaces defined by non-transparent barriers seem to encourage aggression, rather than withdrawal, during stressful social encounters. Whether people consider spaces largely defined by transparent barriers as confined or open, however, remains an issue of debate. Given that transparent barriers can be as impenetrable as their non-transparent counterparts, they can clearly be understood as physically confining. Their visual permeability, however, may reduce a person’s sense of confinement (Stamps, 2010). As a result, the same density level may reduce feelings of crowding in spaces largely defined by transparent barriers than in spaces defined by non-transparent barriers. The effect of perceived spaciousness, in turn, may translate into a decreased readiness to aggress and an enhanced willingness to consider withdrawal during perceived social threat. In contrast, feelings of crowding may occur even in low density spaces if there is a high-density environment on the other side of a transparent barrier. In other words, humans may not be affected by perceived spaciousness per se, but by the actual content of the views through their surrounding transparent barriers (Kaplan, 2001). Thus, a person’s readiness to aggress against social threats encountered indoors may get reduced or amplified, depending on whether transparent barriers afford stress-reducing or stress-inducing views (e.g., exposure to nature versus built environments; Kahn et al., 2008; Bratman et al., 2015). With increased globalization and migration, many contemporary societies are characterized by frequent encounters between strangers from different ethnic, cultural, and/or religious backgrounds. A large body of work indicates that such encounters often elicit mutual discomfort and anxiety (Stephan and Stephan, 1985; Smith and Mackie, 2010). If design features could enhance the safety and comfort of such interactions, architects should consider their regulatory impact when designing public spaces that welcome human diversity.

The lack of empirical data addressing the above issues is particularly unfortunate because many closely related questions of importance remain equally unaddressed. For example, can beneficial consequences of social proximity, such as the inhibition of stress hormones in the presence of social ingroup members, occur across transparent barriers (cf. Millidine et al., 2009; Beckes and Coan, 2011)? Are spaces bounded by transparent barriers sufficient to fulfill privacy needs? To what degree can transparent barriers define a territory that is recognized by others? Finally, can spaces defined by transparent barriers influence social interactions via changes in environmental features, such as lighting conditions and the level of visual stimulation? Initial evidence suggests, for instance, that in work settings people prefer rooms with sunlight and outdoor views (Wang and Boubekri, 2010; Aries et al., 2015). But can such preferences shape the course and quality of social interactions? Changes in light conditions over the course of a day can certainly entrain circadian rhythms and modulate physiological states, such as a person’s endocrine levels and heart rate (Edelstein et al., 2007). Further evidence suggests that lighting conditions around transparent barriers may even impact people’s social behavior. Brighter rooms, for instance, seem to facilitate social inhibition (Hirsh et al., 2011), an effect that can reduce anti-social behavior (such as aggression or dishonesty, see Page and Moss, 1976; Prentice-Dunn and Rogers, 1980; Zhong et al., 2010) but also prosocial behavior (such as the willingness to collaborate, see Steidle et al., 2013). Most importantly, these rare examples of experimental research show that the effects of transparent barriers (beyond corresponding changes in lighting conditions) on social exchange deserve empirical attention in order to understand these barriers’ impact on our everyday life.

Practical Implications

A pivotal methodology, so-called evidence-based design, promotes the integration of traditional, predominantly intuition-driven architectural design with evidence-based decision-making (Rosswurm and Larrabee, 1999; Brown and Ecoff, 2011). The approach involves systematically tracking, comparing, and evaluating the consequences of architectural decisions on human health and wellbeing, so that the obtained findings can be applied to the design of new buildings (Lohr, 2004). The idea of evidence-based design is particularly relevant when it comes to the use of transparent barriers. Not only are such barriers unique in their dual nature (i.e., they are simultaneously absent and present), but they also form a novel type of environmental structure from an evolutionary point of view (Brzezicki, 2013a). In other words, the requirement to respond to and navigate transparent barriers has emerged only recently in human phylogeny. So what goals do architects typically have when using transparent design features and what is the evidence that these goals are met?

As frequently discussed in architectural circles “the quality, or state of being transparent is both a material condition […and…] an intellectual imperative” (Rowe and Slutzky, 1963). This twofold meaning of transparency has turned glass architecture into a marketing tool used to symbolize accessibility (e.g., in financial or governmental institutions; Whiteley, 2003) and democratic information exchange (Barnstone, 2005). Indeed, initial observations suggest that openness and transparency in workplace settings can facilitate productivity and innovation by enhancing the exchange of knowledge and skills between individuals (e.g., Hascher et al., 2002). At the same time, however, employees’ preferences and demands for privacy and defensible territories may interfere with architectural ideals of transparency (Kim and de Dear, 2013). Anecdotal evidence reveals that in modern buildings plants, posters, and other non-transparent items are often strategically placed to cover facades and interior walls made of glass. The well-known artist Wassily Kandinski, for instance, was once observed to cover a transparent glass wall of a Bauhaus building in white paint in order to avoid being constantly looked at by passersby (cf. Whiteley, 2003). These user-driven changes to built environments frequently signal contrasting preferences between architects, who aim to dematerialize spatial boundaries by using transparent glass, and building occupants who strive to re-establish them.

Differential preferences for transparency between architects and users may contribute to lethargy and difficulties concentrating as discussed in the context of the so-called sick building syndrome (Apter et al., 1994; Sahlberg, 2012). Although the potential contribution of transparent boundaries to this syndrome has yet to be studied, their role deserves particular scientific attention due to the profound perceptual, behavioral, and social repercussions as described in this article. At the same time, the use of transparent materials also merits consideration in the context of healing architecture. In contrast to the sick building syndrome, the concept of healing architecture refers to design features that promote human health and wellbeing. The term is usually applied in the context of healthcare buildings, where it denotes the capacity of architectural design to promote healing processes in the people it accommodates. A seminal article in this field “View through a window may influence recovery from surgery” was published in Science magazine (Ulrich, 1984). The authors analyzed outcomes of patients with gall bladder surgery. After undergoing the procedure, patients were accommodated in rooms on the second and third floors of a three-story wing of a hospital building. Windows of the patient rooms on one side of the wing looked out on either a grove of trees or on a brown brick wall. The results showed faster, less painful recovery for surgical patients with a windowed view to a natural setting than for those with a view of a brick wall. The authors noted, however, that many physical attributes in addition to view itself, such as the quality of light may have influenced the obtained results. Despite its ambiguity, this landmark study inspired further studies to investigate the relationship between architectural design and positive health outcomes (for a review see Lawson, 2010). Though it is generally agreed upon that environmental modifications can hardly ensure recovery from injury or disease, healthcare professionals increasingly recognize that architectural design can act as therapeutic assets by affecting occupants’ mood and social interaction patterns (Sommer, 1974; Kahn et al., 2008; Sternberg, 2009).

Building on this work, architects to date must strive to understand in which kinds of environments transparent barriers are likely to act as stressors or healers. In addition, they should explore the versatile cognitive and behavioral responses that humans adopt upon encountering and inhabiting transparent environments. A particular focus should lie on identifying human responses that arise specifically from adapting toward these rather novel environments. To systematically study the richness of human responses toward transparent barriers, the concept of behavior settings as originally introduced in ecological psychology may prove helpful (Barker, 1968). A behavior setting is a bounded geographical area in which human and environmental components interact in a coordinated fashion to facilitate an ordered series of events over a period of time (Wicker, 1979). Examples of a behavior setting include an office meeting, lunch at a restaurant, a church service, or a university lecture. Importantly, transparent barriers may have strikingly different consequences on human functioning across different behavior settings. A lecture room separated from a busy corridor by a transparent barrier, for instance, might interfere with students’ attention to the lecturer. In contrast, a transparent wall separating two administrative assistants working on collaborative tasks, but needing acoustic screening, may facilitate their effectiveness. Considering these examples, the decision to erect transparent barriers, either in order to replace non-transparent ones or to subdivide previously open spaces, should always entail evaluating potential changes in the series of events that characterize a specific setting.

Healing, or at least beneficial, effects of transparent barriers may be particularly likely in settings that require individuals to simultaneously connect with others, but also to protect their privacy. In this regard, open plan offices (i.e., offices that are not fully enclosed by internal walls) provide an interesting starting point for effective transparent barrier use. Open plan designs generally aim to optimize communication and information flow across individuals. Yet, their practical implementation is frequently accompanied by complaints about privacy violations (cf. De Croon et al., 2005; Kim and de Dear, 2013; De Been and Beijer, 2014). These complaints partially arise from intrusions caused by noise pollution from neighboring work stations (Lee and Brand, 2005; Veitch et al., 2007; Kaarlela-Tuomaala et al., 2009; Jahncke et al., 2011). Erecting transparent barriers between work stations may therefore allow architects to keep spaces visually connected, yet acoustically separated. In addition, placing workstations in close proximity to a window with an outside view may help employees cope with privacy challenges in open plan designs (Yildirim et al., 2007; Aries et al., 2010; Lottrup et al., 2015).

Finally, people not only select settings, but settings also select people (Wicker, 1979). That is, humans are rarely in a particular environment by chance. For example, behavioral scientists are likely to be found in university classrooms and research labs, but less frequently in corporate boardrooms or machine shops. The combined selection by individuals and settings increases the likelihood that people in a particular setting are more similar to one another than are people randomly sampled from a range of different settings. The process of structural constraints acting in concert with self- and setting-selection processes, social norms, and shared goals to limit behavioral options and to increase coordination in a given setting has been termed synomorphy (Wicker, 1979). The extent to which synomorphic mechanisms arise from environments with transparent barriers is another matter of speculation. It seems worthy of investigation, for instance, whether people with chronically low privacy needs, reduced impression management concerns, low territoriality claims, and/or claustrophobic tendencies experience greater satisfaction in surroundings with transparent barriers than people with contrasting needs and motives (e.g., people with high privacy needs and/or chronic fears of social evaluation by others). In a related manner, the use of glass features in architecture might be most welcome by occupants from cultures that are willing to publicize their everyday lives (Kükelhaus, 1973; Vera, 1989; Abel, 1997; Rieger-Jandl, 2006).

Concluding Remarks

As interest in understanding and predicting how people respond to built environments begins to grow, the need for systematic research on the affective, cognitive, and behavioral responses to varied environments increases. The widespread, and occasionally undifferentiated, use of transparent barriers in modern construction poses a pivotal example of how architectural decisions could benefit from valid and reliable data in order to ensure a space’s functionality and user-friendliness. Through focusing on how transparency is experienced visually, haptically, and socially, the present paper integrated a number of seemingly disparate domains in a multidisciplinary manner. In doing so, it revealed the impact of transparent surfaces on various important aspects of human behavior. It was shown that seeing transparent barriers requires the detection and integration of several visual features, a circumstance posing a unique challenge to the visual system. Failure to detect transparent barriers, irrespective of their protective or restraining function, is common and can result in unintentional collisions. Further evidence suggests that such collisions are possible even upon the detection of a transparent barrier. That is, very young children and individuals suffering cognitive decline struggle with navigating transparent structures in their environments. These observations signal that the human mind has to actively construe the presence of a transparent entity, a process that requires experience with the material as well as the capacity to retrieve these experiences.

Furthermore, although plenty of evidence suggests that a cognitive understanding of transparent barriers as solid separators of space is acquired at an early developmental stage, even healthy adults occasionally treat transparent barriers as if they were not there. Both, tests of reflexive responding around such barriers as well as high-stake transactions with the material (e.g., requiring to pass a high cliff on a transparent walkway) reveal a human tendency to respond to their environments predominantly on the basis of incoming visual information. As a result, the unique property of transparent barriers – their visual penetrability – can override a person’s tactile experience and/or explicit knowledge that such barriers form solid separators of space. This tendency to consider transparent barriers as not there is also seen in studies on distance estimation. Several studies failed to detect spatial bias around glass barriers. This observed lack of bias may signal that transparent barriers are less likely to aid a perceiver’s ability to remember and navigate a space’s layout than opaque barriers, a possibility that requires further empirical examination. Similarly pressing is the question of how transparent barriers affect issues of privacy, crowding, territoriality, and interpersonal involvement. Most importantly, the existing work indicates that building transparent structures to serve complex human needs and goals requires a design process grounded in more than “intuition.” That is, the impact of transparent structures on human perception, cognition, and behavior needs to be systematically researched, combining the expertise of architects, designers, and behavioral scientists. The current review calls for such cross-disciplinary investigations as they form the stepping stones necessary for developing detailed recommendations and guidelines for the architectural use of transparent barriers.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant awarded to GM, ESC, AdS, ML, and SQ from the European Platform for Life Sciences, Mind Sciences, and the Humanities of the Volkswagen Foundation. All authors would like to thank the reviewers for their valuable comments on a previous version of the manuscript.

Footnotes

1

It should be noted that the scholarship on transparent barriers summarized in this article was identified by searching two prominent academic databases (i.e., Google Scholar and Scopus) for the terms “glass architecture,” “transparency perception,” “transparent barrier perception,” “transparent barrier detection,” “transparent barrier AND distance,” “transparent barrier AND privacy,” and “transparent barrier AND space.” Once relevant work was identified, its bibliography and citations were searched for further material of interest. Material was included in this review if its content (a) referred to human cognition and/or behavior, (b) elucidated architectural, perceptual, navigational, or social aspects of transparent barrier use, and (c) was representative of a major line of research on the topic (i.e., not every single paper on the role of reflections in transparency detection was included, but rather typical exemplars). Finally, reviewer suggestions were incorporated to optimize the manuscript.

References

  1. Abel C. (1997). Architecture and Identity: Towards a Global Eco-Culture. Oxford: Architectural Press. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aboulnaga M. M. (2006). Towards green buildings: glass as a building element—the use and misuse in the gulf region. Renew. Energy 31 631–653. 10.1016/j.renene.2005.08.017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Abu-Safieh S. F. (2011). “Virtual reality simulation of architectural clues’ effects on human behavior and decision making in fire emergency evaluation,” in Pedestrian and Evacuation Dynamics, eds Peacock R. D., Kuligowski E. D., Averill J. D. (New York, NY: Springer; ) 337–347. [Google Scholar]
  4. Acredolo L. P., Boulter L. T. (1984). Effects of hierarchical organization on children’s judgments of distance and direction. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 37 409–425. 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90068-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Adelson E. H., Anandan P. (1990). “Ordinal characteristics of transparency,” in Paper Presented at the AAAI–90 Workshop on Qualitative Vision, Boston, MA. [Google Scholar]
  6. AGGA (2011). Glass in Buildings. Technical Fact Sheet. Available at: agga.org.au [Google Scholar]
  7. Albers J. (1963). Interaction of Color. New Have, CT: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  8. Algaze I., Snyder A. J., Hodges N. L., Smith G. A. (2012). Children treated in United States emergency departments for door-related injuries, 1999–2008. Clin. Pediatr. 51 226–232. 10.1177/0009922811423308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Allen G. L. (1981). A developmental perspective on the effect of “subdividing” macrospatial experience. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 7 120–132. 10.1037/0278-7393.7.2.120 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Altman I. (1975). The Environment and Social Behavior. Privacy, Personal Space, Territory, Crowding. Monterey, CA: Brooks Cole. [Google Scholar]
  11. Anderson B. L. (1997). A theory of illusory lightness and transparency in monocular and binocular images: the role of contour junctions. Perception 26 419–453. 10.1068/p260419 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Apter A., Bracker A., Hodgson M., Sidman J., Leung W. Y. (1994). Epidemiology of the sick building syndrome. J. Allergy Clin. Immunol. 94 277–288. 10.1053/ai.1994.v94.a56006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Arbab M., Finley J. J. (2010). Glass in architecture. Int. J. Appl. Glass Sci. 1 118–129. 10.1111/j.2041-1294.2010.00004.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Argyle M., Dean J. (1965). Eye-contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry 28 289–304. 10.2307/2786027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Aries M. B., Aarts M. P. J., van Hoof J. (2015). Daylight and health: a review of evidence and consequences for the built environment. Light. Res. Technol. 47 6–27. 10.1177/1477153513509258 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Aries M. B., Veitch J. A., Newsham G. R. (2010). Windows, view, and office characteristics predict physical and psychological well-being. J. Environ. Psychol. 30 533–541. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.12.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Arnheim R. (1971). Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology Of The Creative Eye. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Awh E., Belopolsky A. V., Theeuwes J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 16 437–443. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Barker R. G. (1968). Ecological Psychology: Concepts and Methods for Studying the Environment of Human Behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  20. Barnstone D. A. (2005). The Transparent State. Architecture and Politics in Postwar Germany. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  21. Beck J., Ivry R. (1988). On the role of figural organization in perceptual transparency. Percept. Psychophys. 44 585–594. 10.3758/BF03207492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Beck J., Prazdny S. (1981). Highlights and the perception of glossiness. Percept. Psychophys. 30 407–410. 10.3758/BF03206160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Beck J., Prazdny S., Ivry R. (1984). The perception of transparency with achromatic colors. Percept. Psychophys. 35 407–422. 10.3758/BF03203917 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Beckes L., Coan J. A. (2011). Social baseline theory: the role of social proximity in emotion and economy of action. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 5 976–988. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00400.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Bell M., Kim J. (2009). Engineered Transparency: The Technical, Visual, and Spatial Effects of Glass. New York, NY: Princeton Architectural Press. [Google Scholar]
  26. Bell P. A., Greene T. C., Fisher J. D., Baum A. (2001). Environmental Psychology. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt College Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  27. Blake A., Bülthoff H. (1990). Does the brain know the physics of specular reflection? Nature 343 165–168. 10.1038/343165a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Bratman G. N., Hamilton J. P., Hahn K. S., Daily G. C., Gross J. J. (2015). Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112 8567–8572. 10.1073/pnas.1510459112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Brown C. E., Ecoff L. (2011). A systematic approach to the inclusion of evidence in healthcare design. Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 4 7–16. 10.1177/193758671100400202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Brown G. (2009). Claiming a corner at work: measuring employee territoriality in their workspaces. J. Environ. Psychol. 29 44–52. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.05.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Brozzoli C., Ehrsson H. H., Farnè A. (2014). Multisensory representation of the space near the hand: from perception to action and interindividual interactions. Neuroscientist 20 122–135. 10.1177/1073858413511153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Brzezicki M. (2013a). Understanding transparency perception in architecture: presentatino of the simplified perforated model. Perception 42 60–81. 10.1068/p7245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Brzezicki M. (2013b). The role of specular reflection in the perception of transparent surfaces – the influence on user safety. Lecture Notes Comput. Sci. 8019 189–196. 10.1007/978-3-642-39360-0_21 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Brzezicki M. (2014). Dynamic perception of transparency in architecture: mechanisms of the proper recognition of light permeable surfaces. Compr. Psychol. 3 7. [Google Scholar]
  35. Buckley G. B., Smith A. D., Haselgrove M. (2014). Shape shifting: local landmarks interfere with navigation by, and recognition of, global shape. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40 492–510. 10.1037/a0034901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Butterworth G. (1977). Object disappearance and error in Piaget’s stage IV task. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 23 391–401. 10.1016/0022-0965(77)90034-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Caggiano V., Fogassi L., Rizzolatti G., Their P., Casile A. (2009). Mirror neurons differentially encode the peri- and extrapersonal space of monkeys. Science 324 403–406. 10.1126/science.1166818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Calvert G. A., Spence C., Stein B. E. (2004). The Handbook of Multisensory Processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  39. Camperio C. A., Malaman M. (2002). Where to sit in a waiting room: density, age and gender effects on proxemic choices. Hum. Evol. 17 175–186. 10.1007/BF02436369 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Cardinali L., Brozzoli C., Farnè A. (2010). “Peripersonal space and body schema,” in Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience, eds Koob G. F., Le Moal M., Thompson R. F. (London: Elsevier; ) 40–46. [Google Scholar]
  41. Cesario J., Plaks J. E., Hagiwara N., Navarrete C. D., Higgins E. T. (2010). The ecology of automaticity: how situational contingencies shape action semantics and social behavior. Psychol. Sci. 21 1311–1317. 10.1177/0956797610378685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Cochran C. D., Hale W. D., Hissam C. P. (1984). Personal space requirements in indoor versus outdoor locations. J. Psychol. 117 121–123. 10.1080/00223980.1984.9923667 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Colomina B. (2009). “Unclear vision: architectures of surveillance,” in Engineered Transparency: The Technical, Visual, and Spatial Effects of Glass, eds Bell M., Kim J. (New York, NY: Princeton Architectural Press; ) 78–87. [Google Scholar]
  44. Crusco A. H., Wetzel C. G. (1984). The Midas Touch: the effects of interpersonal touch on restaurant tipping. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 10 512–517. 10.1177/0146167284104003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Dawson L. (2005). China’s New Dawn: An Architectural Transformation. Munich: Prestel. [Google Scholar]
  46. De Been I., Beijer M. (2014). The influence of office type on satisfaction and perceived productivity support. J. Facil. Manag. 12 142–157. 10.1108/JFM-02-2013-0011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. De Croon E., Sluiter J., Paul Kuijer P., Frings-Dresen M. (2005). The effect of office concepts con worker health and performance: a systematic review of the literature. Ergonomics 48 119–134. 10.1080/00140130512331319409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Delogu F., Fedorov G., Belardinelli M. O., van Leeuwen C. (2010). Perceptual preferences in depth stratification of transparent layers: photometric and non-photometric factors. J. Vis. 10 1–13. 10.1167/10.2.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Diamond A., Gilbert J. (1989). Development as progressive inhibitory control of action: retrieval of a contiguous object. Cogn. Dev. 4 223–249. 10.1016/0885-2014(89)90007-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Drew C. J. (1971). Research on the psychological-behavioral effects of the physical environment. Rev. Educ. Res. 41 447–465. 10.3102/00346543041005447 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Dunkeld J., Bower T. G. (1980). Infant response to impending optical collision. Perception 9 549–554. 10.1068/p090549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Dunphy-Lelii S., Wellman H. M. (2004). Infants’ understanding of occlusion of others’ line-of-sight: implications for an emerging theory of mind. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 1 49–66. 10.1080/17405620444000049 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Edelstein E. A., Robert J. E., Sollers I. I. I. J., Thayer J. F. (2007). “The effects of lighting on autonomic control of the heart,” in Proceedings of Society for Psychophysiological Research Proceedings, October 17–21, Savannah, GA. [Google Scholar]
  54. Elkadi H. (2006). Cultures of Glass Architecture. Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  55. Ernst M. O., Bülthoff H. H. (2004). Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8 162–169. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Evans G. W., Wener R. E. (2007). Crowding and personal space invasion on the train: please don’t make me sit in the middle. J. Environ. Psychol. 27 90–94. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.10.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Farnè A., Dematte M. L., Ladavas E. (2003). Beyond the window: multisensory representation of peripersonal space across a transparent barrier. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 50 51–61. 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00124-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Fleming R. W., Jäkel F., Maloney L. T. (2011). Visual perception of thick transparent materials. Psychol. Sci. 22 812–820. 10.1177/0956797611408734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Fukiage T., Oishi T., Ikeuchi K. (2014). A simple photometric factor in perceived depth order of bistable transparency patterns. J. Vis. 14 1–27. 10.1167/14.5.2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Gibson E. J., Schmuckler M. A. (1989). Going somewhere: an ecological and experimental approach to development of mobility. Ecol. Psychol. 1 3–25. 10.1207/s15326969eco0101_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gibson E. J., Walk R. D. (1960). Visual cliff. Sci. Am. 202 67–71. 10.1038/scientificamerican0460-64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Gosden C. (2008). Social ontologies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363 2003–2010. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Gropius W. (1926). Glasbau. Die Bauzeitung 23 159–162. [Google Scholar]
  64. Gur E., Barnea Y., Leshem D., Zaretski A., Amir A., Weiss J., et al. (2001). Walk-through injuries: glass door facial injuries. Ann. Plast. Surg. 46 613–616. 10.1097/00000637-200106000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Guzetta A., Cioni G., Cowan F., Mercuri E. (2001). Maturation of visual function in infants with neonatal brain lesions: correlation with neuroimaging. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 5 107–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Haag Bletter R. (1981). The interpretation of the glass dream-expressionist architecture and the history of the crystal metaphor. J. Soc. Archit. Hist. 40 20–43. 10.2307/989612 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Haans A., IJsselsteijn W. A. (2006). Mediated social touch: a review of current research and future directions. Virtual Real. 9 149–159. 10.1007/s10055-005-0014-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Haldimann M., Luible A., Overend M. (2008). Structural Use of Glass Vol. 10 Zuerich: IABSE. [Google Scholar]
  69. Hall E. T. (1959). The Silent Language. New York, NY: Doubleday. [Google Scholar]
  70. Hamilton D. K., Watkins D. H. (2009). Evidence-Based Design for Multiple Building Types. Hoboken: Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  71. Han X., Becker S. (2013). One spatial map or many? Spatial coding of connected environments. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40 511–531. 10.1037/a0035259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hascher R., Arnold T., Jeska S., Klauck B. (2002). Office Buildings: A Design Manual. Stuttgart: Birkhäuser. [Google Scholar]
  73. Hayduk L. A. (1983). Personal space: where we now stand. Psychol. Bull. 94 293–335. 10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.293 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. He Z. J., Wu B., Ooi T. L., Yarbrough G., Wu J. (2004). Judging egocentric distance on the ground: occlusion and surface integration. Perception 33 789–806. 10.1068/p5256a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Helmholtz H. V. (1867). Handbuch Der Physiologischen Optik. Leipzig: L. Voss. [Google Scholar]
  76. Herman J. F., Miller B. S., Heins J. A. (1987). Barriers and spatial representation: evidence from children and adults in a large environment. Merrill Palmer Q. 33 53–68. [Google Scholar]
  77. Hillstrom A. P., Wakefield H., Scholey H. (2013). The effect of transparency on recognition of overlapping objects. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 19 158–170. 10.1037/a0033367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Hirsh J. B., Galinsky A. D., Zhong C.-B. (2011). Drunk, powerful, and in the dark: how general processes of disinhibition produce both prosocial and antisocial behavior. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6 415–427. 10.1177/1745691611416992 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Jahncke H., Hygge S., Halin N., Green A. M., Dimberg K. (2011). Open-plan office noise: cognitive performance and restoration. J. Environ. Psychol. 31 373–382. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.07.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Johnson P. C., Hitchcock H. R. (1932). The International Style: Architecture Since 1922. New York, NY: Norton & Company. [Google Scholar]
  81. Johnson S. P., Aslin R. N. (2000). Infants’ perception of transparency. Dev. Psychol. 36 808–816. 10.1037/0012-1649.36.6.808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kaarlela-Tuomaala A., Helenius R., Keskinen E., Hongisto V. (2009). Effects of acoustic environment on work in private office rooms and open-plan offices – longitudinal study during relocation. Ergonomics 52 1423–1444. 10.1080/00140130903154579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kahn P. H., Jr., Friedman B., Gill B., Hagman J., Severson R. L., Freier N. G., et al. (2008). A plasma display window? The shifting baseline problem in a technologically mediated natural world. J. Environ. Psychol. 28 192–199. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.10.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kaplan R. (2001). The nature of the view from home: psychological benefits. Environ. Behav. 33 507–542. 10.1177/00139160121973115 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kavšek M. (2009). Infant perception of static two-dimensional transparency information. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 6 281–293. 10.1080/17405620601095300 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kersten D., Bülthoff H. H., Schwartz B., Kurtz K. (1992). Interaction between transparency and structure from motion. Neural Comput. 4 573–589. 10.1162/neco.1992.4.4.573 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kim J., de Dear R. (2013). Workspace satisfaction: the privacy-communication trade-off in open-plan offices. J. Environ. Psychol. 36 18–26. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.06.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Kimia A., Waltzman M., Shannon M., Mays D. L., Johnston P. R., Hummel D., et al. (2009). Glass-table related injuries in children. Pediatr. Emerg. Care 25 145–149. 10.1097/PEC.0b013e31819b41c8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. King S. M., Dykeman C., Redgrave P., Dean P. (1992). Use of distracting task to obtain defensive head movements to looming visual stimuli by human adults in a laboratory setting. Perception 21 245–259. 10.1068/p210245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Kingdom F. A. A. (2011). Lightness, brightness and transparency: a quarter century of new ideas, captivating demonstrations and unrelenting controversy. Vision Res. 51 652–673. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Kitagawa N., Spence C. (2005). Investigating the effect of a transparent barrier on the crossmodal congruency effect. Exp. Brain Res. 161 62–71. 10.1007/s00221-004-2046-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Kitaoka A. (2005). A new explanation of perceptual transparency connecting the X-junction contrast-polarity model with the luminance-based arithmetic model. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 47 175–187. 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2005.00286.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. Kleinke C. L. (1986). Gaze and eye contact: a research review. Psychol. Bull. 100 78–100. 10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Knapp M. L., Hall J. A., Horgan T. G. (2014). “The effects of the environment on human communication,” in Nonverbal Communication in Human Interaction 8th Edn (Boston, MA: Wadsworth; ) 89–122. [Google Scholar]
  95. Koenderink J. J., van Doorn A. J., Pont S. C., Wijntjes M. (2010). Phenomenal transparency at X-junctions. Perception 39 872–883. 10.1068/p6528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Koffka K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & World. [Google Scholar]
  97. Konečni V. J., Libuser L., Morton H., Ebbesen E. B. (1975). Effects of a violation of personal space on escape and helping responses. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 11 288–299. 10.1016/S0022-1031(75)80029-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Korn A. (1968). Glass in Modern Architecture of the Bauhaus Period (1926). New York, NY: George Brazilier. [Google Scholar]
  99. Kosslyn S. M., Pick H. L., Fariello G. R. (1974). Cognitive maps in children and men. Child Dev. 45 707–716. 10.2307/1127837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Krampen M., Schempp D. (1999). Glasarchitekten: Konzepte, Bauten, Perspektiven. Stuttgart: AV Edition. [Google Scholar]
  101. Kükelhaus H. (1973). Unmenschliche Architektur. Von der Tierfabrik zur Lernanstalt. Köln: Gaia. [Google Scholar]
  102. Kulterman U. (1999). Contemporary Architecture in the Arab States: Renaissance of a Region. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  103. Làdavas E., Farnè A. (2004). Visuo-tactile representation of near-the-body space. J. Physiol. Paris 98 161–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Làdavas E., Farnè A., Zeloni G., di Pellegrino G. (2000). Seeing or not seeing where your hands are. Exp. Brain Res. 131 458–467. 10.1007/s002219900264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Laidlaw K. E. W., Foulsham T., Kuhn G., Kingstone A. (2011). Potential social interactions are important to social attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108 5548–5553. 10.1073/pnas.1017022108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Lappin J. S., Shelton A. L., Rieser J. J. (2006). Environmental context influences visually perceived distance. Percept. Psychophys. 68 571–581. 10.3758/BF03208759 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Lawson B. (2010). Healing architecture. Arts Health 2 95–108. 10.1080/17533010903488517 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. Lee S. Y., Brand J. L. (2005). Effects of control over office workspace on perceptions of the work environment and work outcomes. J. Environ. Psychol. 25 323–333. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.08.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  109. Levitt M. J., Weber R. A. (1989). Social involvement with peers in 2-1/2-year old toddlers: environmental influences. Environ. Behav. 21 82–98. 10.1177/0013916589211005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  110. Li S., Li Y.-M. (2007). How far is far enough? A measure of information privacy in terms of interpersonal distance. Environ. Behav. 39 317–331. [Google Scholar]
  111. Lindsay L. (2009). Scotland’s bow-fronted shops. Archit. Herit. 20 75–92. 10.3366/E135075240900020X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  112. Lockman J. J. (1984). The development of detour ability during infancy. Child Dev. 55 482–491. 10.2307/1129959 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Lockman J. J., Adams C. D. (2001). Going around transparent and grid-like barriers: detour ability as a perception–action skill. Dev. Sci. 4 463–471. 10.1111/1467-7687.00188 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  114. Lohr K. N. (2004). Rating the strength of scientific evidence: relevance for quality improvement programs. Int. J. Qual. Health Care 16 9–18. 10.1093/intqhc/mzh005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Lottrup L., Stigsdotter U. K., Meilby H., Claudi A. G. (2015). The workplace window view: a determinant of office workers’ work ability and job satisfaction. Landsc. Res. 40 57–75. 10.1080/01426397.2013.829806 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  116. Lynn G. (2009). Blobwall – Greg Lynn FORM. Archit. Des. 79 96–99. 10.1002/ad.861 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  117. Maki R. (1981). Categorization and distance effects with spatial linear orders. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1 15–32. [Google Scholar]
  118. Maki R. (1982). Why do categorization effects occur in comparative judgment tasks? Mem. Cogn. 10 252–264. 10.3758/BF03197637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Makin T. R., Holmes N. P., Brozzoli C., Rossetti Y., Farnè A. (2009). Coding of visual space during motor preparation: approach objects rapidly modulate corticospinal excitability in hand-centered coordinates. J. Neurosci. 29 11841–11851. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2955-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Manzo J. (2005). Social control and the management of “personal” space in shopping malls. Space Cult. 8 83–97. 10.1177/1206331204265991 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  121. Marks R. (1993). Stained Glass in England during the Middle Ages. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  122. Masin S. C. (2006). Test of models of achromatic transparency. Perception 35 1611–1624. 10.1068/p5034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. McBride G., King M. G., James J. W. (1965). Social proximity effects on galvanic skin responses in adult humans. J. Psychol. 61 153–157. 10.1080/00223980.1965.10544805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. McKean J. (1994). Crystal Palace: Joseph Paxton and Charles Fox. London: Phaidon. [Google Scholar]
  125. McQuire S. (2003). From glass architecture to Big Brother. Cult. Stud. Rev. 9 103–123. 10.5130/csr.v9i1.3587 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  126. Metelli F. (1970). An algebraic development of the theory of perceptual transparency. Ergonomics 13 59–66. 10.1080/00140137008931118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Metelli F. (1974a). “Achromatic color conditions in the perception of transparency,” in Perception: Essays in honor of James J. Gibston, eds MacLeod1 R. B., Pick H. L. (London: Cornell University Press; ) 95–116. [Google Scholar]
  128. Metelli F. (1974b). The perception of transparency. Sci. Am. 230 90–98. 10.1038/scientificamerican0474-90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Middlemist R. D., Knowles E. S., Matter C. F. (1976). Personal space invasions in the lavatory: suggestive evidence for arousal. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 33 541–546. 10.1037/0022-3514.33.5.541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Millidine K. J., Metcalfe N. B., Armstrong D. (2009). Presence of a conspecific causes divergent changes in resting metabolism, depending on its relative size. Proc. R. Soc. B 276 3989–3993. 10.1098/rspb.2009.1219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Miodownik M. (2013). Stuff Matters. The Strange Stories of the Marvelous Materials that Shape Our Man-Made World. New York, NY: Viking Press. [Google Scholar]
  132. Montello D. R. (2005). The perception and cognition of environmental distance: direct sources of information. Lecture Notes Comput. Sci. 1329 297–311. 10.1007/3-540-63623-4_57 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  133. Morse F. (2011). Skywalk on Tianmen Mountain in China Terrifying for tourists. Huffington Post. Available at: www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/2011/11/11/chinese-skywalk-tianmen-mountain-tourism-china_n_1087897.html [Google Scholar]
  134. Motoyoshi I. (2010). Highlight-shading relationship as a cue for the perception of translucent and transparent materials. J. Vis. 10 1–11. 10.1167/10.9.6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Nakayama K., Shimojo S., Silverman G. H. (1989). Stereoscopic depth: its relation to image segmentation, grouping and the recognition of occluded objects. Perception 18 55–68. 10.1068/p180055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Newcombe N., Liben L. S. (1982). Barrier effects in the cognitive maps of children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 34 46–58. 10.1016/0022-0965(82)90030-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Nielsen D., Kumarasuriyar A. (2014). The lily, client and measure of Bruno Taut’s Glashaus. Archit. Res. Q. 18 257–266. 10.1017/S1359135514000608 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  138. Noland J. S. (2008). Executive functioning demands of the object retrieval task for 8-month-old infants. Child Neuropsychol. 14 504–509. 10.1080/09297040701770819 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Oppenheim A. L., Brill R. H., Barag D., Saldern A. V. (1970). Glass and Glassmaking in Ancient Mesopotamia. New York, NY: Corning Museum of Glass. [Google Scholar]
  140. Otsuka Y., Kanazawa S., Yamaguchi M. K. (2006). Perceptual transparency in 3- to 4-month-old infants. Perception 35 1625–1636. 10.1068/p5386 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Page R. A., Moss M. K. (1976). Environmental influences on aggression: the effects of darkness and proximity of victim. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 6 126–133. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1976.tb01318.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  142. Passini R., Pigot H., Rainville C., Tétreault M. H. (2000). Wayfinding in a nursing home for advanced dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Environ. Behav. 32 684–710. 10.1177/00139160021972748 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  143. Patterson M., Quadflieg S. (2015). “The physical environment and nonverbal communication,” in APA Handbook of Nonverbal Communication, eds Matsumoto D., Hwang H. C., Frank M. G. (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; ) 189–220. [Google Scholar]
  144. Patterson M. L. (1973). Compensation in nonverbal immediacy behavior: a review. Sociometry 36 237–252. 10.2307/2786569 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  145. Patterson M. L. (1976). An arousal model of interpersonal intimacy. Psychol. Rev. 83 235–245. 10.1037/0033-295X.83.3.235 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  146. Patterson M. L. (2011). More than Words: The Power of Nonverbal Communication. Spain: Aresta S. C. [Google Scholar]
  147. Patterson M. L. (2013). “Toward a systems approach to nonverbal interaction,” in Nonverbal Communication, eds Hall J. A., Knapp M. L. (Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton; ) 527–550. [Google Scholar]
  148. Patterson M. L., Mullens S., Romano J. (1971). Compensatory reactions to spatial intrusion. Sociometry 34 114–121. 10.2307/2786354 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  149. Piller M. J., Sebrechts M. M. (2003). “Spatial learning in transparent virtual environments,” in Proceedings of the 47th Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (Denver, CO: ) 2133–2136. [Google Scholar]
  150. Prentice-Dunn S., Rogers R. W. (1980). Effects of deindividuating situational cues and aggressive models on subjective deindividuation and aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39 104–113. 10.1037/0022-3514.39.1.104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Previc F. H. (1998). The neuropsychology of 3-D space. Psychol. Bull. 124 123–164. 10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Procter R. (1970). An Investigation of Mental Hospital Nursing Station Design on Aspects of Human Behavior. Topeka, KS: The Environmental Research Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  153. Qiu F. T., von der Heydt R. (2007). Neural representation of transparent overlay. Nat. Neurosci. 10 283–284. 10.1038/nn1853 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Richards B. (2006). New Glass Architecture. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  155. Rieger-Jandl A. (2006). Architecture and Culture: Building in the Tension of Increasing Global Interaction. Wien: Neuer Wissenschaftlicher Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  156. Robilotto R., Khang B.-G., Zaidi Q. (2002). Sensory and physical determinants of perceived achromatic transparency. J. Vis. 2 388–403. 10.1167/2.5.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Robson S. K. (2002). A review of psychological and cultural effects on seating behavior and their application to food service settings. J. Foodserv. Res. 5 89–107. 10.1300/J369v05n02_07 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  158. Robson S. (2008). Scenes from a restaurant: privacy regulation in stressful situations. J. Environ. Psychol. 28 373–378. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.03.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  159. Rosswurm M. A., Larrabee J. H. (1999). A model for change to evidence-based practice. J. Nurs. Scholarsh. 31 317–322. 10.1111/j.1547-5069.1999.tb00510.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Rowe C., Slutzky R. (1963). Transparency: literal and phenomenal. Perspecta 8 45–54. 10.2307/1566901 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  161. Rubin N. (2001). The role of junctions in surface completion and contour matching. Perception 30 339–366. 10.1068/p3173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Sahlberg B. (2012). Indoor Environment in Dwellings and Sick Building Syndrome (SBS): Longitudinal Studies. Ph.D. dissertation, Uppsala University, Uppsala. [Google Scholar]
  163. Sayim B., Cavanagh P. (2011). The art of transparency. Iperception 2 679–696. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Scannell L., Gifford R. (2010). Defining place attachment: a tripartite organizing framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 30 1–10. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  165. Scheerbart P. (1914). Glasarchitektur. Berlin: Der Sturm. [Google Scholar]
  166. Schittich C. (2011). Taking a second look: glass pavillion at Broadfield House in Kingswinford. Detail 2 118–121. [Google Scholar]
  167. Schmuckler M. A. (1996). Development of visually guided locomotion: barrier crossing by toddlers. Ecol. Psychol. 8 209–236. 10.1207/s15326969eco0803_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  168. Serino A., Annella L., Aventanti A. (2009). Motor properties of peripersonal space in humans. PLoS ONE 4:e6582 10.1371/journal.pone.0006582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Sherman R. C., Croxton J., Smith M. (1979). Movement and structure as determinants of spatial representations. J. Nonverbal Behav. 4 27–39. 10.1007/BF00986910 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  170. Shinskey J. L., Munakata Y. (2001). Detecting transparent barriers: clear evidence against the means-end deficit account of search failures. Infancy 2 395–404. 10.1207/S15327078IN0203_7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Sinai M. J., Ooi T. L., He Z. J. (1998). Terrain influences the accurate judgment of distance. Nature 395 497–500. 10.1038/26747 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Singh M., Anderson B. L. (2002). Toward a perceptual theory of transparency. Psychol. Rev. 109 492–519. 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Smith E. R., Mackie D. M. (2010). “Affective processes,” in The SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination, eds Dovidio J. F., Hewstone M., Glick P., Esses V. M. (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; ) 508–525. [Google Scholar]
  174. Sommer R. (1959). Studies in personal space. Sociometry 22 247–260. 10.2307/2785668 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  175. Sommer R. (1969). Personal Space. The Behavioral Basis of Design. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
  176. Sommer R. (1974). Tight Spaces: Hard Architecture and How to Humanize It. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
  177. Spence C., Pavani F., Driver J. (2004). Spatial constraints on visual-tactile crossmodal distractor congruency effects. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci 4 148–169. 10.3758/CABN.4.2.148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Staib G. (2008). “Von den Ursprüngen bis zur Klassischen Moderne,” in Glasbau Atlas, eds Schittich C., Staib G., Balkow D., Schuler M., Sobek W. (Basel: Birkhaeuser; ). [Google Scholar]
  179. Stamps A. E. (2010). Effects of permeability on perceived enclosure and spaciousness. Environ. Behav. 42 864–886. 10.1177/0013916509337287 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  180. Steidle A., Hanke E.-V., Werth L. (2013). In the dark we cooperate: the situated nature of procedural embodiment. Soc. Cogn. 31 275–300. 10.1521/soco.2013.31.2.275 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  181. Steiner H., Veel K. (2015). Invisibility Studies: Surveillance, Transparency and the Hidden in Contemporary Culture. Bern: Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
  182. Stephan W. G., Stephan C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. J. Soc. Issues 41 157–175. 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1985.tb01134.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  183. Sternberg E. M. (2009). Healing Spaces: The Science of Place and Well-Being. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  184. Stokols D. (1972). On the distinction between density and crowding: some implications for future research. Psychol. Rev. 79 275–277. 10.1037/h0032706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Stone N. J., Irvine J. M. (1993). Performance, mood, satisfaction, and task type in various work environments: a preliminary study. J. Gen. Psychol. 120 489–497. 10.1080/00221309.1993.9711162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Ulrich R. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery. Science 224 420–421. 10.1126/science.6143402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. United Kingdom Health and Safety Executive. (1992). Workplace (Health, Safety and Welfare) Regulations. Approved Code of Practice. Available at: www.hse.gov.uk/pubns/priced/l24.pdf [Google Scholar]
  188. van Hof-van Duin J., Mohn G. (1986). Visual field measurements, optokinetic nystagmus and the visual threatening response: normal and abnormal development. Doc. Ophtalmol. Proc. Ser. 45 305–315. [Google Scholar]
  189. Veitch J. A., Charles K. E., Farley K. M. J., Newsham G. R. (2007). A model of satisfaction with open-plan office conditions: COPE field findings. J. Environ. Psychol. 27 177–189. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.04.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  190. Vera H. (1989). On Dutch windows. Qual. Sociol. 12 215–234. 10.1007/BF00988998 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  191. Vidler A. (1992). The Architectural Uncanny. Essays in the Modern Unhomely. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  192. Wang N., Boubekri M. (2010). Investigation of declared seating preference and measured cognitive performance in a sunlit room. J. Environ. Psychol. 30 226–238. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  193. Watanabe T., Cavanagh P. (1993). Transparent surfaces defined by implicit X junctions. Vision Res. 33 2339–2346. 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90111-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Weller B., Vogt I. (2012). Adhesive joints in glass and solar engineering. J. ASTM Int. 9 124–151. 10.1520/JAI104076 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  195. Wesslein A.-K., Spence C., Frings C. (2015). You can’t ignore what you can’t separate: the effect of visually induced target-distractor separation on tactile selection. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22 728–736. 10.3758/s13423-014-0738-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Weston R. (2004). Plans, Sections and Elevations: Key Buildings of the Twentieth Century. London: Laurence King Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  197. Whiteley N. (2003). Intensity of scrutiny and a good eyeful. J. Archit. Educ. 56 8–16. 10.1162/104648803321672915 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  198. Wicker A. W. (1979). An Introduction to Ecological Psychology. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole Publisher. [Google Scholar]
  199. Widrich M. (2015). “I’ll be your mirror: transparency, voyeurism, and glass architecture,” in Invisibility Studies, eds Steiner H., Veel K. (Bern: Peter Lang; ) 43–59. [Google Scholar]
  200. Wigginton M. (1996). Glass in Architecture. London: Phaidon Press. [Google Scholar]
  201. Witt J. K., Stefanucci J. K., Riener C. R., Proffitt D. R. (2007). Seeing beyond the target: environmental context affects distance perception. Perception 36 1752–1768. 10.1068/p5617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Wolfe J. M., Birnkrant R. S., Kunar M. A., Horowitz T. S. (2005). Visual search for transparency and opacity: attentional guidance by cue combination. J. Vis. 5 257–274. 10.1167/5.3.9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Yates D. J., Bremner J. G. (1988). Conditions for Piagetian stage IV search errors in a task using transparent containers. Infant Behav. Dev. 11 411–417. 10.1016/0163-6383(88)90002-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  204. Yildirim K., Akalin-Baskaya A., Celebi M. (2007). The effects of window proximity, partition height, and gender on perceptions of open-plan offices. J. Environ. Psychol. 27 154–165. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.01.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  205. Zeisel J. (1981). Inquiry by Design. Tools for Environment-Behaviour Research. New York, NY: Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
  206. Zhong C.-B., Bohns V., Gino F. (2010). Good lamps are the best police: darkness increases dishonesty and self-interested behavior. Psychol. Sci. 21 311–314. 10.1177/0956797609360754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES