Skip to main content
Neural Plasticity logoLink to Neural Plasticity
. 2015 Sep 3;2015:391349. doi: 10.1155/2015/391349

Audiomotor Integration in Minimally Conscious State: Proof of Concept!

Antonino Naro 1, Antonino Leo 1, Antonino Cannavò 1, Antonio Buda 1, Rocco Bruno 2, Carlo Salviera 2, Placido Bramanti 1, Rocco Salvatore Calabrò 1,*
PMCID: PMC4573993  PMID: 26425370

Abstract

Patients suffering from chronic disorders of consciousness (DOC) are characterized by profound unawareness and an impairment of large-scale cortical and subcortical connectivity. In this study, we applied an electrophysiological approach aimed at identifying the residual audiomotor connectivity patterns that are thought to be linked to awareness. We measured some markers of audiomotor integration (AMI) in 20 patients affected by DOC, before and after the application of a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation protocol (rTMS) delivered over the left primary motor area (M1), paired to a transauricular alternating current stimulation. Our protocol induced potentiating of the electrophysiological markers of AMI and M1 excitability, paired to a clinical improvement, in all of the patients with minimally conscious state (MCS) but in none of those suffering from unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS). Our protocol could be a promising approach to potentiate the functional connectivity within large-scale audiomotor networks, thus allowing clinicians to differentiate patients affected by MCS from UWS, besides the clinical assessment.

1. Introduction

Patients suffering from chronic disorders of consciousness (DOC) show dissociation between the two main components of consciousness, that is, awareness and wakefulness. Indeed, the unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS) patients do not show signs of awareness (with preservation of wakefulness) whereas the minimally conscious state (MCS) individuals show some purposeful behaviors [1, 2]. DOC differential diagnosis relies on awareness assessment through ad hoc behavioral scales, such as the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R) [3]. Behavioral impairment could be related to an extensive connectivity disruption within complex corticothalamocortical networks [46]. Nevertheless, some patients could be unable to properly react to stimuli for other reasons, such as poor cooperation or cognitive impairment [7]. Hence, specific paradigms aimed at objectifying a possible correlation between wide brain disconnectivity and motor output failure should be fostered. To this end, there is growing evidence regarding auditory-motor integration processes (AMI) in DOC patients, showing residual preservation of the auditory processing, also involving the associative areas [812].

In addition, it has been shown that some noninvasive neurostimulation protocol could unmask residual covert connectivity patterns in some DOC patients, including UWS [13]. Recently, paired associative stimulation (PAS) protocol has been employed in shaping the AMI in healthy individuals [14]. PAS is an electrophysiological technique that pairs conditioning stimuli (e.g., visual, sensory, and auditory stimuli, motor imagery, or movements) with transcranial magnetic stimuli (TMS) over the motor cortex [1517], thus inducing a long-lasting change in cortical excitability probably by means of Hebbian long-term potentiation or depression-like process (LTP, LTD) [18]. Concerning AMI, conditioning auditory stimuli affect the motor cortex excitability [14], whereas acoustic stimuli paired with TMS over the auditory cortex induce tonotopically specific and tone-unspecific auditory cortex plasticity [19]. In addition, speech perception can modulate the motor cortical excitability within hand, lips, and tongue area representation [2022].

Hence, aim of the current study was to investigate whether it was possible to induce plasticity within the motor system by applying an audiomotor PAS protocol in DOC patients. To this end, we paired a 5 Hz repetitive TMS (rTMS) over the left M1 with a transauricular repetitive electric stimulation (rES) of the right acoustic nerve in a DOC sample and in healthy individuals (HC). We hypothesized that such paired protocol could induce a M1 excitability increase through the recruitment of residual audiomotor pathways, thus allowing us to differentiate MCS (that should show residual connectivity properties) from UWS individuals (who should lack of such properties), besides the clinical assessment.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

Of the 47 chronic DOC subjects who attended over two years to the Neurorehabilitation Unit of the IRCCS Centro Neurolesi “Bonino-Pulejo” (Messina, Italy), we enrolled 20 patients who met the criteria for vegetative state and MCS diagnosis [2, 23, 24] and the following inclusion criteria: a DOC condition lasting more than 3 months after the brain injury; no other severe neurological or systemic diseases; no critical conditions (i.e., inability to breathe independently and hemodynamic instability); no cortical excitability-modifying drugs assumption beyond L-DOPA and baclofen; absence of epileptic history, pace-maker, aneurysms clips, neurostimulator, brain/subdural electrodes or other electromechanical devices; absence of electroencephalographic (EEG) burst-suppression pattern; presence of long-latency auditory evoked potentials (LLAEP); no lesion of eardrum or external meatus. In addition, we included 10 HC (6 females and 4 males, mean age: 45.3 ± 6.2 years) as control group in the study.

We resumed the clinical and demographic characteristics in Table 1. DOC etiology consisted of postanoxic or posttraumatic brain damage. The neurological examination mainly showed a pattern of spastic tetraparesis. Two neurologists, skilled in DOC diagnosis, independently evaluated the patients through the JFK CRS-R, which was daily administered for 30 days consecutively, at different times, in order to steadily establish the level of consciousness impairment. EEG examination evidenced continuous slowing in theta and/or delta frequency ranges.

Table 1.

The clinical and demographic characteristics of the whole sample. We reported the monthly individual and group CRS-R scores ± SD (the CRS-R was daily administered for 30 consecutive days before protocol enrollment), with the unpaired t-test values. The MCS patients who ameliorated at the auditory function (n. 3, 6, 7, 8, and 10) at T post following the real_protocol are marked in bold.

DOC Gender Etiology Age BI MRI CRS-R
Total A V M OM C Ar
MCS (n = 10) F A 72 6 WMH 19 ± 1.5 4 ± .6 4 ± 1.5 5 ± 1.3 2 ± .7 1 3 ± .7
M T 51 18 WMH, RBG_h 18 ± .9 3 ± .7 3 ± 1.1 7 ± .9 1 ± 1.4 1 3 ± .7
F A 66 9 WMH 11 ± .8 1 ± 1.1 3 ± .8 2 ± .9 1 ± 1.2 1 3 ± .8
F T 70 22 LFb_h 15 ± 1 3 ± .6 2 ± .7 5 ± 1.6 2 ± 1.6 1 2 ± .9
M T 33 8 multiple_h 14 ± 1 2 ± 1.4 2 ± 1 5 ± 1.6 2 ± .9 1 2 ± .7
F A 41 15 WMH 11 ± 1 1 ± .8 1 ± 1.3 3 ± 1.5 2 ± 1.6 1 3 ± 1
M T 35 16 WMH, R B G_h 11 ± .9 1 ± 1.2 1 ± 1.3 3 ± .7 2 ± 1.6 1 3 ± .7
F A 29 17 WMH 11 ± .9 1 ± 1.2 1 ± 1.7 3 ± .7 2 ± 1.6 1 3 ± .8
M T 23 18 WMH, LBG_h 12 ± 1 2 ± 1.3 1 ± 1.5 3 ± .9 2 ± 1.1 1 3 ± .9
F A 47 14 WMH 11.9 ± 1 11.4 3 ± 1.5 2 ± .9 2 ± 1.3 1 3 ± .9
mean ± SD 47 ± 18 14 ± 5 24 ± .8 4 ± 1.1 4 ± 1.1 5 ± 1.6 1.8 ± .4 1 3 ± .4

UWS (n = 10) M A 53 8 WMH 5 ± .4 1 ± .4 1 ± 1 1 ± 1.5 1 ± 1.2 0 1 ± 1.1
F T 26 3 DAI, SAH 4 ± .5 1 ± .5 1 ± 1.6 1 ± 1.8 0 ± 1 0 1 ± 1.1
F T 56 8 RFP_h 6 ± .9 0 ± .9 2 ± 1.4 2 ± 1.2 1 ± .7 0 1 ± .8
F A 62 11 WMH 6 ± .9 1 ± .9 1 ± 1.5 2 ± 1.5 0 ± 1.7 0 2 ± .8
M T 51 9 SAH 4 ± .5 1 ± .9 1 ± 1.6 1 ± 1.4 0 ± 1.2 0 1 ± .9
M A 69 11 WMH 7 ± .8 1 ± .5 1 ± 1.9 2 ± 1 1 ± .9 0 2 ± 1.2
F T 74 12 DAI, SAH 6 ± .9 1 ± .8 2 ± 1.7 1 ± 1 0 ± 1.9 0 2 ± 1.6
M A 69 13 WMH 7 ± 1 1 ± .9 2 ± 1.2 2 ± .9 0 ± 1.3 0 2 ± 1.6
F T 44 14 DAI, SAH 7 ± 1 1 ± .9 2 ± 1.7 2 ± 1.3 0 ± 1.8 0 2 ± .9
F T 52 15 RFT_h 7 ± 1 1 ± .9 2 ± .9 2 ± 1.3 0 ± 1 0 2 ± 1.6
mean ± SD 56 ± 14 10 ± 4 5.9 ± 1.1 0.9 ± .3 1.5 ± .5 1.6 ± .5 0.3 ± .4 0 1.6 ± .5

Unpaired t-test NS NS NS 0.03 NS <0.001 0.02 NS 0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001

Age in years; BI: brain injury onset in months; CRS-R: Coma Recovery Scale-Revised including auditory (A), visual (V), motor (M), oromotor (OM), communication domain (C), and arousal induction (Ar); etiology: A, postanoxic; T, posttraumatic brain injury; MRI: structural patterns including WMH (white matter hyperintensity), _h (hemorrhagic lesion), RFP (right frontopolar), RBG (basal ganglia), LFb (left frontobasal), SAH (subarachnoid hemorrhage), and DAI (diffuse axonal injury); SD: standard deviation.

Our Research Institute Ethics Committee approved the present study and either the HC or the legal guardian of each patient gave their written informed consent.

2.2. Experimental Design

HC were seated on a comfortable reclining chair, in a mild-lighted room during the entire experimental procedure, whereas the patients were lying in their bed. At baseline (T pre), we assessed the audiomotor domain score of the CRS-R (in DOC patients), the resting motor threshold (RMT), the motor evoked potential (MEP) peak-to-peak amplitude, the LLAEP latency and amplitude, and the strength of audiomotor interaction (AMI). Then, each participant underwent three different protocols, administered in a random scheme at one-day interval: (i) a real_protocol (rTMS paired to rES); (ii) a rTMS_alone (i.e., rTMS paired to a sham_rES); and (iii) a rES_alone (i.e., rES paired to a sham_rTMS). We repeated the aforementioned baseline measures immediately (T post) and 30 minutes after (T +30) the application of each conditioning protocol. The experimental design is summarized in Figure 1. The experimenters who analyzed the data were blinded on the scheme procedure.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Resuming the experimental design. We measured before (T pre) and after (T post and T +30) each conditioning protocol (real_protocol, rTMS_alone, and rES_alone) the motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitude, the audiomotor integration (AMI) strength, the long-latency auditory evoked potentials (LLAEP) latency and amplitude, and the CRS-R auditory function.

2.3. Clinical Assessment

The JFK CRS-R is a reliable and standardized scale that integrates neuropsychological and clinical assessment; it includes the current diagnostic criteria for coma, VS, and MCS and allows the clinician to assign a patient to the most appropriate diagnostic category. Hence, the CRS-R represents a good approach for characterizing the level of consciousness and for monitoring the neurobehavioral function recovery [24].

2.4. Motor Evoked Potentials

We positioned the coil over the optimum position (hot-spot) to elicit a stable MEP of 0.5 mV peak-to-peak amplitude in the right first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle at rest. The hot-spot was identified by moving the coil in 0.5 cm steps around the presumed hot-spot. The coil was held tangentially to the scalp, with the handle pointing backwards and laterally to 45° from the midline (approximately perpendicular to the line of the central sulcus). We thus estimated the RMT, which was defined as the minimum intensity able to evoke a peak-to-peak MEP amplitude of 50 μV in at least five-out-of-ten consecutive trials in the relaxed FDI muscle [25]. Therefore, fifteen MEPs were recorded from the right FDI muscle at rest (using a stimulation intensity of 120% of RMT) at baseline (T pre), immediately (T post), and 30 minutes after (T +30) the application of each conditioning protocol. The peak-to-peak amplitude of each MEP was measured offline, and the mean amplitude was calculated. MEP amplitude changes were calculated as percent of the baseline MEP (T pre).

We used a high-power Magstim 200 stimulator (Magstim, Whitland, Dyfed, UK) and a standard figure-of-eight coil, with external loop diameters of 9 cm. The magnetic stimuli had monophasic pulse configuration and a rise-time of ~100 μs, decaying back to zero over ~800 μs. The coil current during the rising phase of the magnetic field flowed toward the handle. Thus, the induced current in the cortex flowed in a posterior-to-anterior direction.

2.5. Long-Latency Auditory Evoked Potentials

Since a standard AEP assessment in DOC patients is extremely challenging owing to the low and inconsistent cooperation, we chose a rES approach [26] in order to elicit LLAEP. We used a battery-driven stimulator (Brain Stim, E.M.S., Bologna, Italy) with a couple of silver electrodes. The stimulation electrode (a silver ball) was placed in the right external auditory meatus near the eardrum (after having flushed the external auditory meatus with physiologic saline solution) and the reference electrode (a silver disk) on the skin of the patient's neck (near the right mastoid). We delivered two consecutive trains of 200 electric stimuli (500 Hz sine tones at an intensity of 500 μA, at 5 Hz). The intertrain interval was 30 sec. The stimulation procedure induced a hearing sensation of intermediate loudness in the HC. Each participant wore an earplug in the left ear. During the stimulation, we recorded the EEG from electrode Cz referring to the right mastoid using Ag/AgCl electrodes. An electrode at the centre of forehead served as ground. Two additional channels were employed for the electrooculogram (active electrode on the left supraorbital position and the reference electrode on the left infraorbital position). Impedance was ≤10 kΩ. Signals were digitized (A/D = 1000 Hz), amplified (1000 times), and filtered (0.15–100 Hz, 50 Hz-notched) through a 1401 plus AD laboratory interface (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) and a Digitimer D360 (Digitimer Ltd., Welwyn Garden City, UK) and stored on a personal computer for offline analysis (Signal software, Cambridge Electronic Design, UK). Then, data were processed by artifact rejecting (±100 μV and by subtracting ocular artifacts), epoch from −100 to 500 ms, filtered (1–30 Hz, 12 dB/octave) and averaged. Hence, we registered a cortical triphasic positive-negative-positive potential (P1-N1-P2), starting at around 50 ms in the HC, in analogy to previous LLAEP findings [27, 28]. We measured the component latencies and the baseline-peak amplitude of N1. Latencies were determined by using a modified box-plot method known as the median rule.

2.6. Audiomotor Integration

In analogy to a previous work [14], we applied pairs of stimuli consisting of a conditioning stimulus (500 Hz sine tone burst) followed by a magnetic test (90% of AMT), with an interstimulus interval of subject's N1 peak-latency +50 ms [20, 29, 30]. Although it has been reported that speech sounds topographically activate the motor cortex (e.g., [21]), others suggest that the motor cortex might be also nontopographically activated by nonspeech sounds [31]. We registered 15 MEP (test MEP) intermingled with 15 electric-magnetic pairs of stimuli interactions (conditioned MEP) in a single trial, delivered at a frequency of 0.2 Hz at baseline (T pre) and immediately (T post) and 30 minutes after (T +30) the application of each conditioning protocol. We measured the mean amplitude of the conditioned MEP as percentage of the amplitude of the unconditioned MEP (test MEP), which was taken as a measure of the strength of AMI.

2.7. rTMS and rES

rTMS was employed in either the real_protocol or the rTMS alone. We delivered 600 stimuli at a frequency of 5 Hz (3 blocks of 200 pulses in 40 seconds, intertrain interval of 10 seconds). The intensity of magnetic stimulation was set at 90% of RMT. For the sham_rTMS, we used the same abovementioned set-up, but with a sham coil. Each rTMS protocol was carried out in accordance with published safety recommendations [32].

Repetitive magnetic stimuli were delivered through a figure-of-eight coil connected to a Magstim Rapid stimulator (Magstim Company, Whitland, Dyfed, UK), with a biphasic waveform of the magnetic stimulus and a pulse width of ~300 μs. The coil was positioned over the hot-spot for the right FDI muscle. During the first phase of the biphasic stimulus, the current flowed in the coil toward the handle and induced a posterior-anterior current within the brain. EMG activity of the right FDI muscle was continuously monitored through loudspeakers throughout the entire rTMS session.

rES was employed in either the real_protocol or the rES alone. It consisted of 600 bursts of 500 Hz sine tone at 5 Hz (3 blocks of 200 pairs in 40 seconds, intertrain interval of 10 seconds) in the right ear, delivered through the aforementioned battery-driven stimulator. With regard to the sham_rES, the electric stimulator was switched off after 30 sec.

2.8. Conditioning Protocols

Each participant underwent three different conditioning protocols, administered in a random scheme (i, ii, and iii) and in different sessions, at one-day interval:

  1. The real_protocol, which consisted of rTMS paired to rES, thus delivering 600 pairs of electric-magnetic stimuli at a frequency of 5 Hz, with an interstimulus interval of subject's N1 latency +50 ms (as in AMI).

  2. The rTMS_alone (i.e., rTMS paired to a sham_rES), in which the electric stimulator was switched off after 30 sec (thus 600 pairs of sham electric stimuli and real TMS pulses).

  3. The rES_alone (i.e., rES paired to a sham_rTMS), in which we used a sham_rTMS coil (thus 600 pairs of real electric stimuli and sham TMS pulses).

2.9. Statistical Analysis

We compared the baseline clinical and electrophysiological parameters among HC, MCS patients, and UWS patients, through unpaired t-tests (calculated on the mean of the three T pre values). We thus evaluated the effects of the conditioning protocols on each electrophysiological variable (RMT%, MEP amplitude, AMI strength, and LLAEP latency and amplitude) through separated three-way repeated-measure analyses of variance (rmANOVA), implying time (three levels: T pre, T post, and T +30) and protocol (three levels: real_protocol, rTMS_alone, and rES_alone), as within-subject factors, and group (three levels: MCS patients, UWS patients, and HC) as between-subject factor. The effect of the conditioning protocols on audiomotor CRS-R was measured through a Wilcoxon test. The Greenhouse-Geisser method was used if necessary to correct for nonsphericity. Conditional on a significant F value, we performed post hoc t-tests (Bonferroni) to explore the strength of main effects and the patterns of interaction between the experimental factors. All statistical tests were applied two-tailed. A significant p value was <0.05. All data are given as means or percent changes ±se. We calculated a Spearman correlation test in order to assess an eventual correlation among clinical and electrophysiological parameters.

3. Results

We did not observe any side effect in both the patients and HC, either during or after the entire experimental procedure.

3.1. DOC/HC Clinical and Electrophysiological Differences at Baseline

We resumed the DOC sample demographic characteristics and the monthly CRS-R scores in Table 1. There were no significant MCS-UWS differences concerning the demographic characteristics, except for slightly longer disease duration in the MCS than the UWS patients. Instead, the monthly and daily CRS-R scores were significantly higher in the MCS than the UWS individuals (≤7). Daily CRS-R scores in each patient showed a relatively low variability during the 30-day observation period. The auditory CRS-R score at each T pre was superimposable to the monthly CRS-R score in each patient. Similarly, the baseline electrophysiological parameters were similar and stable during the three days of experimentation. We reported the raw values of the electrophysiological parameters at T pre (calculated as mean of the three T pre values) for each participant in Table 2. RMT and MEP amplitudes were similar in the three groups. The LLAEP amplitude was slightly reduced only in the UWS individuals, whereas LLAEP latency was significantly increased in the DOC participants (more in the UWS than the MCS patients). The stimulation set-up we used to elicit AMI induced clear inhibitory effects on MEP amplitude in the HC, but such effects were reduced in the MCS patients and nearly absent in the UWS patients.

Table 2.

The time course of the individual raw values of the electrophysiological parameters in the HC and MCS and UWS participants. At T pre, there were not significant differences among the three groups concerning RMT% and MEP amplitude. MCS displayed partially preserved audiomotor connectivity, although weakened (i.e., increased AMI). The UWS showed instead markedly weakened AMI and a global impairment of cortical excitability, connectivity, and LLAEP N1 latency and amplitude (increased and decreased, resp.). Data are summarized as mean ± se (in italic). The MCS patients who ameliorated at the auditory function (n. 3, 6, 7, 8, and 10) at T post following the real_protocol are marked in bold.

(a).
Parameter Protocol HC MCS patients UWS patients
T pre T post T +30 T pre T post T +30 T pre T post T +30
RMT (%) rES_alone 58 52 52 58 56 58 58 62 67
52 57 58 60 57 55 68 59 57
53 57 57 59 54 58 57 60 53
55 55 54 56 60 55 60 62 57
58 58 54 59 55 56 66 59 56
52 56 55 56 58 59 55 66 67
53 55 53 58 60 57 57 55 65
56 52 53 57 58 59 66 62 60
56 55 52 56 60 56 53 60 61
57 55 53 57 59 58 64 63 54
55 ± .7 55 ± .6 54 ± .6 58 ± .5 58 ± .7 57 ± .5 60 ± 1.7 61 ± .9 60 ± 1.6
rTMS_alone 54 54 56 57 54 60 68 53 65
53 56 53 57 55 58 63 63 65
58 52 57 58 59 57 67 55 53
55 58 53 59 59 55 66 67 60
53 55 58 54 55 57 64 61 58
52 53 53 55 55 59 60 65 56
55 57 55 56 56 57 62 55 56
56 53 55 54 58 60 63 61 59
53 56 58 58 54 57 56 66 58
56 54 52 59 56 57 53 54 59
55 ± .6 55 ± .6 55 ± .7 57 ± .6 56 ± .6 58 ± .5 62 ± 1.5 60 ± 1.7 59 ± 1.2
real_protocol 53 52 52 60 56 57 60 58 62
58 56 58 54 56 56 57 53 67
58 57 56 59 54 59 61 58 56
58 57 55 58 54 59 54 64 55
53 54 53 60 55 60 61 60 69
56 52 52 56 60 57 56 53 61
53 58 57 57 56 60 66 60 64
52 57 58 58 59 57 58 60 57
56 56 55 59 54 56 65 67 67
56 53 52 58 58 59 57 61 57
55 ± .8 55 ± .7 54 ± .8 57 ± .6 56 ± .7 58 ± .5 59 ± 1.2 59 ± 1.4 61 ± 1.6

MEP (mV6) rES_alone 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.5 0.5 0.7 0.6 0.6 0.5
0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.7 0.5 0.5 0.6
0.9 0.9 0.9 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.6 0.6 0.7 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.9 0.9 0.9 0.6 0.7 0.5 0.6 0.4 0.5
0.9 0.9 0.9 0.7 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.6 0.5 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.6
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.7 0.5 0.7 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.5
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.7 0.5 0.6 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.7 ± .1 0.7 ± .1 0.7 ± .1 0.6 ± 0.02 0.6 ± 0.02 0.6 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02
rTMS_alone 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.7 0.5 0.7 0.5 0.5 0.6
0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.9 0.9 0.9 0.7 0.5 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.9 0.9 0.9 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6
0.9 0.9 0.9 0.5 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.6 0.4
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.6 0.4
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.6 0.5 0.7 0.5 0.4 0.4
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.6 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.4
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.7 ± .1 0.7 ± .1 0.7 ± .1 0.6 ± .1 0.6 ± .1 0.6 ± .1 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02
real_protocol 0.3 0.7 0.3 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.5 0.6 0.5
0.6 0.8 0.6 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.9 1.2 0.9 0.6 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.6 0.4
0.8 1.0 0.8 0.8 0.9 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.4
0.9 1.2 0.9 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.9 1.2 0.9 0.9 1 0.9 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.8 1.1 0.8 0.8 1 0.8 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.8 1.1 0.8 0.3 0.9 0.3 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.8 1.2 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.5 0.5 0.4
0.8 1.1 0.8 0.9 1 0.9 0.5 0.5 0.6
0.7 ± .1 1.1 ± .1 0.7 ± .1 0.7 ± .1 0.9 ± .05 0.7 ± .1 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02

AMI (mV) rES_alone 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.3 0.5 0.4 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.4 0.5 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.5 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.5 0.5 0.3 0.4 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.6 0.5
0.5 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5
0.3 0.3 0.4 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.4 0.5 0.6
0.5 0.4 0.3 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4
0.3 0.4 0.3 0.5 0.6 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.4
0.5 0.4 0.3 0.5 0.4 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.4
0.4 ± 0.02 0.4 ± 0.02 0.4 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02
rTMS_alone 0.5 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5
0.5 0.4 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.6
0.4 0.3 0.3 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.5
0.3 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5
0.3 0.5 0.4 0.6 0.4 0.6 0.6 0.4 0.6
0.3 0.3 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.3 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4
0.4 0.3 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.4 0.3 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.4 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.5
0.4 ± 0.02 0.4 ± 0.01 0.4 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02
real_protocol 0.5 0.9 0.4 0.6 0.3 0.5 0.6 0.4 0.6
0.4 1 0.3 0.4 0.6 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.5
0.3 1 0.4 0.6 0.9 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.4
0.3 0.8 0.5 0.5 0.8 0.5 0.6 0.6 0.6
0.4 0.9 0.5 0.4 0.9 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.5
0.5 0.8 0.3 0.5 0.9 0.6 0.6 0.5 0.4
0.4 0.8 0.4 0.4 0.8 0.5 0.4 0.5 0.5
0.4 0.9 0.4 0.5 0.8 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.5 1 0.4 0.4 0.8 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.5
0.4 0.8 0.5 0.4 0.8 0.5 0.5 0.6 0.6
0.4 ± .03 0.9 ± .05 0.4 ± .02 0.5 ± 0.5 0.7 ± .1 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02 0.5 ± 0.02

LLAEP amplitude (μV) rES_alone 16 3 7 7 11 11 5 0 2
6 4 2 11 11 7 4 2 5
3 6 7 10 10 13 0 7 1
14 1 8 11 9 6 7 6 2
2 3 6 6 12 5 2 0 0
9 10 15 5 11 8 6 7 2
16 8 15 12 7 12 6 7 6
10 12 11 12 5 5 5 1 4
1 5 16 5 13 9 6 6 2
9 3 16 8 6 10 5 2 6
9 ± 1 6 ± 2 10 ± 2 9 ± 1 10 ± 1 9 ± 1 5 ± 1 4 ± 0.4 3 ± 1
rTMS_alone 16 0 2 12 8 7 2 1 7
7 10 7 13 13 6 3 1 4
7 6 8 12 11 8 7 7 6
16 15 10 5 11 5 2 7 4
14 1 0 8 9 11 4 7 6
16 8 4 12 8 7 4 6 2
5 10 11 6 11 13 5 7 4
5 16 13 5 13 10 6 1 5
15 9 5 6 9 10 3 4 0
1 2 2 7 13 13 2 1 4
10 ± 2 8 ± 2 6 ± .4 9 ± 1 11 ± 1 9 ± 1 4 ± 1 4 ± 1 4 ± 1
real_protocol 12 3 13 9 9 11 0 0 2
16 5 6 9 9 7 0 1 4
6 6 6 12 12 10 7 4 5
3 16 2 13 11 8 6 2 2
12 10 2 7 9 12 7 3 6
4 7 16 11 6 5 2 4 3
6 11 10 10 5 9 4 5 6
14 15 8 13 11 13 3 2 2
3 3 14 10 11 6 3 5 6
4 14 6 9 5 7 5 6 4
8 ± 2 9 ± 1 8 ± 1 10 ± .4 9 ± 1 9 ± 1 4 ± .4 3 ± 1 4 ± 1

LLAEP latency (ms) rES_alone 89 124 100 111 118 114 131 124 123
81 95 90 98 117 126 162 135 125
95 127 133 95 115 131 117 118 106
84 97 110 110 147 135 162 163 121
89 96 127 111 145 114 161 161 119
95 105 126 110 130 151 164 126 122
82 110 88 109 140 140 139 138 110
79 84 103 102 107 115 130 140 107
91 109 105 108 132 143 138 133 106
86 94 103 101 136 128 116 148 108
87 ± 2 104 ± 4 109 ± 5 106 ± 2 129 ± 4 130 ± 4 142 ± 3 138 ± 5 115 ± 6
rTMS_alone 89 109 97 113 111 133 126 123 124
81 86 82 103 98 111 171 125 157
95 130 106 103 95 105 135 106 146
84 107 89 148 110 126 132 121 153
89 95 117 122 111 111 166 119 129
95 119 104 110 110 115 142 122 147
82 95 107 132 109 117 146 110 134
79 104 84 131 102 114 131 107 127
91 113 105 138 108 128 145 106 126
86 109 100 135 101 108 113 108 110
87 ± 2 107 ± 4 99 ± 4 123 ± 2 106 ± 5 117 ± 3 141 ± 3 115 ± 6 135 ± 5
real_protocol 89 116 117 155 119 111 128 137 159
81 97 90 124 122 98 106 146 139
95 96 114 102 110 95 133 110 121
84 86 96 135 107 110 147 126 128
89 120 111 139 106 111 142 140 119
95 121 124 135 122 110 132 146 168
82 108 106 111 110 109 132 153 125
79 86 94 103 107 102 103 119 109
91 97 116 116 106 108 150 123 110
86 105 97 117 108 101 126 115 132
87 ± 2 103 ± 4 106 ± 4 124 ± 2 112 ± 5 106 ± 5 130 ± 2 132 ± 5 131 ± 6
(b).
Parameter p
HC/DOC
p
MCS/UWS
RMT (%) NS NS
MEP (mV) NS NS
AMI (%) 0.002 0.002

N1 latency (ms) <0.001 0.005
N1 amplitude (μV) NS 0.05

AMI: audiomotor integration; LLAEP: long-latency auditory potential; MEP: motor evoked potential; NS: nonsignificant; rES: repetitive electric stimulation; RMT: resting motor threshold; rTMS: repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.

3.2. Conditioning Protocol's Effects on Clinical Assessment

The Wilcoxon test showed a statistically significant increase of the audiomotor CRS-R score only in the MCS patients after the real_protocol at T post (p = 0.04). Indeed, five MCS patients (numbers 3, 6, 7, 8, and 10) upgraded from a T pre “auditory startle” response (1 point at the CRS-R auditory function scale) to a “localization to sound” (2 points) at T post (Table 1).

3.3. Conditioning Protocol Electrophysiological Effects

We resumed in Table 2 and in Figure 2 the time course of electrophysiological parameters following each protocol. We summarized the data statistical analysis in Table 3. The RMT and LLAEP latency and amplitude did not significantly vary after each conditioning protocol. MEP and AMI amplitude significantly increased only in the HC and MCS patients after the real_protocol at T post. Instead, the T +30 values were comparable to T pre (Figure 2). Notably, none of the UWS patients showed any protocol-induced effect (Figure 2). There were no significant differences concerning the protocol posteffects in relation to the clinical and demographic characteristics. Interestingly, we observed a correlation trend between audiomotor CRS-R amelioration and AMI modulation at T post (r = 0.576, p = 0.07).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

It shows the electrophysiological posteffects induced by the real_protocol. We observed a significant, although short lasting, increase of MEP and AMI amplitude in the HC and MCS patients, whereas the UWS patients did not show any significant effect. The latency and amplitude of LLAEP were affected in none of the groups. The values are reported as mean of the percent change in comparison to the baseline value. The error bars refer to the se. indicates a significant change (p < 0.05).

Table 3.

We observed significant real_protocol posteffects in the HC and MCS patients concerning MEP amplitude and AMI strength at T post, whereas the UWS patients did not show any significant posteffect. The rTMS_alone and the rES_alone did not induce any significant posteffect. The nonsignificant interactions, factors, and time intervals are not shown.

Time × group × protocol interaction F (8,216), p Time × protocol interaction F (4,36), p Time effect
F (2,18), p t (1,9), p
MEP amplitude 22, <0.001 HC 90, <0.001 90, <0.001 3.5, 0.001
MCS 53, <0.001 real_protocol 12, <0.001 T 0 3.6, 0.001
AMI % 13, <0.001 HC 78, <0.001 78, <0.001 3.5, 0.002
MCS 6.3, 0.006 9.4, 0.001 2.4, 0.02

4. Discussion

For the first time ever, we assessed the presence of residual audiomotor functional plasticity in a DOC sample by means of an audiomotor PAS. Only the real_protocol (rTMS + rES) induced strengthening of the M1 excitability (MEP amplitude increase) and a modification of audiomotor functional connectivity (weakening of inhibitory AMI) in the HC and MCS patients. Such posteffects were paralleled by a transient audiomotor CRS-R score improvement in some MCS patients (i.e., from “auditory startle” to “sound localization”). On the contrary, the UWS patients did not show any clear posteffect.

The clinical and electrophysiological ameliorations in HC and MCS patients mainly depended on the type of the conditioning protocol that was employed, as also previously shown in healthy individuals [14, 19]. In fact, neither the rTMS_alone nor the rES_alone induced any significant posteffect. Indeed, PAS has been suggested to induce associative LTP or LTD-like neuronal synapses via mechanisms of spike-timing dependent synaptic plasticity [18]. Therefore, in our patients, the real-protocol modulated the audiomotor connectivity probably through time-locked neural activity encompassing the primary auditory area and M1. It has been hypothesized that plasticity and connectivity recovery in individuals suffering from DOC might depend on the modulation of postischemic LTP, the production of specific neurotrophins, and the regulation of excitatory/inhibitory dynamics within corticothalamocortical circuits [3337]. Thereby, it is conceivable that one or more of these mechanisms may have been triggered by the real_protocol and could have favored the recruitment of silent or stunned residual corticothalamocortical projections, thus enhancing the behavioral output in some of our patients. To this end, we could hypothesize the enrolment of a wide audiomotor network including multiple and interconnected cortical areas (encompassing primary auditory cortex, motor areas, and prefrontal cortex) and probably other cortical and subcortical areas (maybe the cerebellum and the basal ganglia) [3840]. Such network could hierarchically organize different audiomotor processes, thus allowing a repertoire of audiomotor responses ranging from protective reflex motor activations to complex feedback and feedforward processes regarding purposeful motor responses [38, 4149].

We can therefore argue that the enhancement of the audiomotor clinical responses in the MCS patients could express a functional upgrading, although transient, of the residual brainstem-thalamocortical and corticocortical networks supporting AMI processes, so as to get a higher and more complex motor behavior. On the other hand, our data further confirm the connectivity impairment affecting UWS individuals within audiomotor integration pathways [1, 8]. Nevertheless, the presence of residual functional connectivity in some UWS patients has been evidenced within other sensory-motor modalities (e.g. [13]), thus allowing us to suppose a condition of functional locked-in syndrome [7, 50]. Hence, such issue needs to be further clarified in more detailed audiomotor integration studies.

Notably, we have to highlight other issues concerning the physiological effects of our combined real_protocol:

  1. Maladaptive plasticity phenomena could play an important role in limiting the range of our posteffects in all of the UWS and in some MCS patients [51].

  2. In the pioneering work of Sowman and coworkers [14], the authors applied speech sound stimuli paired to TMS, being therefore the posteffects potentially dependent on phonological motor resonance and tonotopic-topographic specificity [20, 29], as also suggested by a recent study employing 1–4 kHz tones paired to primary auditory area rTMS [19]. Instead, we triggered brain networks with different tonotopic specificity, whereas the topographic specificity should be more deeply investigated (e.g., by studying the muscle involved in articulation).

  3. Since RMT, LLAEP, and MEP amplitude were not substantially different at baseline between HC and DOC and RMT and LLAEP did not vary after the conditioning protocols, we can exclude the possibility that baseline cortical excitability or LLAEP differences could have influenced our posteffects.

  4. We may exclude differences in the attentive level in the HC participants in reason of their blinded condition concerning the different experimental sessions [52].

  5. The lack of rTMS_alone posteffects on MEP amplitude confirms the findings of a previous high-frequency PAS study in healthy individuals, in which 600 magnetic stimuli failed in producing a significant corticospinal excitability modulation [53]. Therefore, the heterologous sensory stimulation we employed (rTMS + rES) boosted up the cortical effects of rTMS, similarly to previous rapid PAS reports [53, 54].

The relatively small sample size and the consequent mixed etiology represent the main limiting factor in our study. Nonetheless, it is difficult to study a large sample of patients with DOC, since the negative outcome of such patients is still unfortunately high.

5. Conclusions

In our opinion, the present study shows a promising approach in an attempt to identify residual patterns of AMI in patients affected by severe DOC. Indeed, our data further support the importance of diagnostic approaches that are independent from patient's cooperation, aimed at assessing the brain connectivity patterns, whose impairment is proportionally related to the awareness impairment. In addition, the possibility to identify such partially preserved corticocortical and corticosubcortical networks in DOC may be useful in the selection of candidate patients for therapeutic and rehabilitative trials by means of noninvasive neurostimulation approaches.

Conflict of Interests

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests regarding the publication of this paper.

References

  • 1.Laureys S., Faymonville M.-E., Degueldre C., et al. Auditory processing in the vegetative state. Brain. 2000;123(8):1589–1601. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.8.1589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Giacino J. T., Ashwal S., Childs N., et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002;58(3):349–353. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.3.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kalmar K., Giacino J. T. The JFK coma recovery scale—revised. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2005;15(3-4):454–460. doi: 10.1080/09602010443000425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dehaene S., Changeux J.-P., Naccache L., Sackur J., Sergent C. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10(5):204–211. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Del Cul A., Baillet S., Dehaene S. Brain dynamics underlying the non-linear threshold for access to consciousness. PLoS Biology. 2007;5(10, article e260) doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vanhaudenhuyse A., Noirhomme Q., Tshibanda L. J.-F., et al. Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain. 2010;133(1):161–171. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bruno M.-A., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Thibaut A., Moonen G., Laureys S. From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness. Journal of Neurology. 2011;258(7):1373–1384. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6114-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Boly M., Faymonville M.-E., Peigneux P., et al. Auditory processing in severely brain injured patients: differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state. Archives of Neurology. 2004;61(2):233–238. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.2.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Boly M., Garrido M. I., Gosseries O., et al. Preserved feedforward but impaired top-down processes in the vegetative state. Science. 2011;332(6031):858–862. doi: 10.1126/science.1202043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Owen A. M., Coleman M. R., Menon D. K., et al. Residual auditory function in persistent vegetative state: a combined PET and fMRI study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2005;15(3-4):290–306. doi: 10.1080/09602010443000579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Perrin F., Schnakers C., Schabus M., et al. Brain response to one's own name in vegetative state, minimally conscious state, and locked-in syndrome. Archives of Neurology. 2006;63(4):562–569. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.4.562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cavinato M., Volpato C., Silvoni S., Sacchetto M., Merico A., Piccione F. Event-related brain potential modulation in patients with severe brain damage. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2011;122(4):719–724. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Naro A., Calabrò R. S., Russo M., et al. Can transcranial direct current stimulation be useful in differentiating unresponsive wakefulness syndrome from minimally conscious state patients? Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2015;33(2):159–176. doi: 10.3233/RNN-140448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sowman P. F., Dueholm S. S., Rasmussen J. H., Mrachacz-Kersting N. Induction of plasticity in the human motor cortex by pairing an auditory stimulus with TMS. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8, article 398 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Thabit M. N., Ueki Y., Koganemaru S., Fawi G., Fukuyama H., Mima T. Movement-related cortical stimulation can induce human motor plasticity. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(34):11529–11536. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1829-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mrachacz-Kersting N., Kristensen S. R., Niazi I. K., Farina D. Precise temporal association between cortical potentials evoked by motor imagination and afference induces cortical plasticity. The Journal of Physiology. 2012;590(7):1669–1682. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.222851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Suppa A., Biasiotta A., Belvisi D., et al. Heat-evoked experimental pain induces long-term potentiation-like plasticity in human primary motor cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23(8):1942–1951. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stefan K., Kunesch E., Benecke R., Cohen L. G., Classen J. Mechanisms of enhancement of human motor cortex excitability induced by interventional paired associative stimulation. The Journal of Physiology. 2002;543(2):699–708. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.023317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Schecklmann M., Volberg G., Frank G., et al. Paired associative stimulation of the auditory system: a proof-of-principle study. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(11) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027088.e27088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fadiga L., Craighero L., Buccino G., Rizzolatti G. Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;15(2):399–402. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01874.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Flöel A., Ellger T., Breitenstein C., Knecht S. Language perception activates the hand motor cortex: implications for motor theories of speech perception. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;18(3):704–708. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02774.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Watkins K. E., Strafella A. P., Paus T. Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(8):989–994. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00316-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1994;330(21):1499–1508. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199405263302107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state the multi-society task force on PVS. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1994;330(22):1572–1579. doi: 10.1056/nejm199406023302206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gerrard P., Zafonte R., Giacino J. T. Coma recovery scale-revised: evidentiary support for hierarchical grading of level of consciousness. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2014;95(12):2335–2341. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Alibiglou L., Mackinnon C. D. The early release of planned movement by acoustic startle can be delayed by transcranial magnetic stimulation over the motor cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 2012;590(4):919–936. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.219592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hofmann E., Preibisch C., Knaus C., Müller J., Kremser C., Teissl C. Noninvasive direct stimulation of the cochlear nerve for functional MR imaging of the auditory cortex. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 1999;20(10):1970–1972. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Key A. P. F., Dove G. O., Maguire M. J. Linking brainwaves to the brain: an ERP primer. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;27(2):183–215. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2702_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wassermann E. M. Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5–7, 1996. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section. 1998;108(1):1–16. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(97)00096-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ventura L. M. P., Alvarenga K. D. F., Costa Filho O. A. Protocol to collect late latency auditory evoked potentials. Brazilian Journal of Otorhinolaryngology. 2009;75(6):879–883. doi: 10.1016/S1808-8694(15)30553-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Roy A. C., Craighero L., Fabbri-Destro M., Fadiga L. Phonological and lexical motor facilitation during speech listening: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Physiology Paris. 2008;102(1–3):101–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rossini P. M., Burke D., Chen R., et al. Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1994;91:79–92. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Liotti Mario M., Ingham J. C., Takai O., Paskos D. K., Perez R., Ingham R. J. Spatiotemporal dynamics of speech sound perception in chronic developmental stuttering. Brain and Language. 2010;115(2):141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kokaia Z., Andsberg G., Yan Q., Lindvall O. Rapid alterations of BDNF protein levels in the rat brain after focal ischemia: evidence for increased synthesis and anterograde axonal transport. Experimental Neurology. 1998;154(2):289–301. doi: 10.1006/exnr.1998.6888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Crepel V., Hammond C., Chinestra P., Diabira D., Ben-Ari Y. A selective LTP of NMDA receptor-mediated currents induced by anoxia in CA1 hippocampal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1993;70(5):2045–2055. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.5.2045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Di Filippo M., Tozzi A., Costa C., et al. Plasticity and repair in the post-ischemic brain. Neuropharmacology. 2008;55(3):353–362. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ding M.-C., Wang Q., Lo E. H., Stanley G. B. Cortical excitation and inhibition following focal traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(40):14085–14094. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3572-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Medalla M., Barbas H. Specialized prefrontal ‘auditory fields’: organization of primate prefrontal-temporal pathways. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2014;8, article 77 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Demanez J.-P., Demanez L. Anatomo-physiology of the central auditory nervous system: basic concepts. Acta Oto-Rhino-Laryngologica Belgica. 2003;57(4):227–236. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ermolaeva V. Y., Borgest A. N. Intercortical connections of the auditory areas with the motor area. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology. 1980;10(3):210–215. doi: 10.1007/bf01182212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sharma A., Dorman M. F. Central auditory development in children with cochlear implants: clinical implications. Advances in Oto-Rhino-Laryngology. 2006;64:66–88. doi: 10.1159/000094646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sharma A., Gilley P. M., Dorman M. F., Baldwin R. Deprivation-induced cortical reorganization in children with cochlear implants. International Journal of Audiology. 2007;46(9):494–499. doi: 10.1080/14992020701524836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lee C. C. Thalamic and cortical pathways supporting auditory processing. Brain and Language. 2013;126(1):22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lee C. C., Sherman S. M. On the classification of pathways in the auditory midbrain, thalamus, and cortex. Hearing Research. 2011;276(1-2):79–87. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hackett T. A. Information flow in the auditory cortical network. Hearing Research. 2011;271(1-2):133–146. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tourville J. A., Reilly K. J., Guenther F. H. Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech. NeuroImage. 2008;39(3):1429–1443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Perkell J. S. Movement goals and feedback and feedforward control mechanisms in speech production. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2012;25(5):382–407. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lane H., Tranel B. The Lombard sign and the role of hearing in speech. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 1971;14(4):677–709. doi: 10.1044/jshr.1404.677. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Curio G., Neuloh G., Numminen J., Jousmäki V., Hari R. Speaking modifies voice-evoked activity in the human auditory cortex. Human Brain Mapping. 2000;9(4):183–191. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200004)9:4&#x00026;lt;183::AID-HBM1&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Formisano R., D'Ippolito M., Catani S. Functional locked-in syndrome as recovery phase of vegetative state. Brain Injury. 2013;27(11):p. 1332. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2013.809555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bagnato S., Boccagni C., Sant'Angelo A., Fingelkurts A. A., Fingelkurts A. A., Galardi G. Emerging from an unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: brain plasticity has to cross a threshold level. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37(10):2721–2736. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Stefan K., Wycislo M., Classen J. Modulation of associative human motor cortical plasticity by attention. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;92(1):66–72. doi: 10.1152/jn.00383.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Quartarone A., Rizzo V., Bagnato S., et al. Rapid-rate paired associative stimulation of the median nerve and motor cortex can produce long-lasting changes in motor cortical excitability in humans. The Journal of Physiology. 2006;575(2):657–670. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.114025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Naro A., Russo M., AbdelKader M., et al. A local signature of LTP-like plasticity induced by repetitive paired associative stimulation. Brain Topography. 2014;28(2):238–249. doi: 10.1007/s10548-014-0396-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neural Plasticity are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES