Summary
The human amygdala is critical for social cognition from faces, as borne out by impairments in recognizing facial emotion following amygdala lesions [1] and differential activation of the amygdala by faces [2–5]. Single-unit recordings in the primate amygdala have documented responses selective for faces, their identity, or emotional expression [6, 7], yet how the amygdala represents face information remains unknown. Does it encode specific features of faces that are particularly critical for recognizing emotions (such as the eyes), or does it encode the whole face, a level of representation that might be the proximal substrate for subsequent social cognition? We investigated this question by recording from over 200 single neurons in the amygdalae of seven neurosurgical patients with implanted depth electrodes [8]. We found that approximately half of all neurons responded to faces or parts of faces. Approximately 20% of all neurons responded selectively only to the whole face. Although responding most to whole faces, these neurons paradoxically responded more when only a small part of the face was shown compared to when almost the entire face was shown. We suggest that the human amygdala plays a predominant role in representing global information about faces, possibly achieved through inhibition between individual facial features.
Results
Behavioral Performance
We recorded single-neuron activity from microwires implanted in the human amygdala while neurosurgical patients performed an emotion categorization task. All patients (12 sessions from 10 patients, 1 female) were undergoing epilepsy monitoring and had normal basic ability to discriminate faces (see Table S1 available online). Patients were asked to judge for every trial whether stimuli showing a face or parts thereof were happy or fearful (Figure 1) by pushing one of two buttons as quickly and accurately as possible. Each individual face stimulus (as well as its mirror image) was shown with both happy and fearful expressions, thus requiring subjects to discriminate the emotions in order to perform the task (also see Figure S1D). Each stimulus was preceded by a baseline image of equal luminance and complexity (“scramble”). We showed the entire face (whole face, WF), single regions of interest (eye or mouth “cutouts,” also referred to as regions of interest [ROIs]), and randomly selected parts of the face (“bubbles”; Figure 1A). The randomly sampled bubbles were used to determine which regions of the face were utilized to perform the emotion classification task using a reverse correlation technique [9]. The proportion of the face revealed in the bubble stimuli was adaptively modified to achieve an asymptotic target performance of 80% correct (Figure 1B; see Supplemental Experimental Procedures); the number of bubbles required to achieve this criterion decreased, on average, over trials (Figure 1B). Average task performance across all trial categories was 87.8 ± 4.8% (n = 12 sessions, ±standard deviation [SD]; worst performer was 78% correct; see Figure S1A for details). The behavioral classification image derived from the accuracy and reaction time (RT) of the responses showed that patients utilized information revealed by both the eyes and the mouth region to make the emotion judgment (Figure 1C; Figure S1B). Overall, the behavioral performance-related metrics confirmed that patients were alert and attentive and had largely normal ability to discriminate emotion from faces (cf. Figure S1).
Face-Responsive Neurons
We isolated a total of 210 single units (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures for isolation criteria and electrode location within the amygdala) from nine recording sessions in seven patients (three patients contributed no well-isolated neurons in the amygdala). Of these, 185 units (102 in the right amygdala, 83 in the left) had an average firing rate of at least 0.2 Hz and were chosen for further analysis. Only correct trials were considered. To analyze neuronal responses, we first aligned all trials to the onset of the scramble or face epochs and compared the mean firing rate before and after. We found that 11.4% of all units showed a significant modulation of spike rate already at the onset of the scramble (Table S2; see Figure 2A for an example), indicating visual responsiveness [10, 11], whereas 51.4% responded to the onset of the face stimuli relative to the preceding baseline (Table S2). Thus, although only about a tenth of units responded to phase-scrambled faces relative to a blank screen, half responded to the facial stimuli relative to the scramble. Some units increased their firing rate, whereas others decreased their rate in response to stimulus onset (42% and 36% of the responsive units increased their rate for scramble and face stimuli onset, respectively; Table S2; Figure S2). The large proportion of inhibitory responses may be indicative of the dense inhibitory network within the amygdala [12].
Of these face-responsive neurons, 36.8% responded in bubble trials, 23.8% to whole faces, and 14.1% and 20.0% to eye and mouth cutouts, respectively (all relative to scramble baseline); some units responded to several or all categories (see Figure 2A for an example). To assess relative selectivity, we next compared responses among different categories of face stimuli (see Table S2 for comprehensive summary). We found that a substantial proportion of units (19.5%) responded selectively to whole faces, compared to cutouts (Figure 2; Figure S2). Only a small proportion of units distinguished between eye and mouth cutouts or between cutouts and bubbles (<10%). We found on the order of 10% of neurons whose responses differentiated between emotions, gender, or identity, similar to a prior report [6]. We thus conclude that (1) amygdala neurons responded notably more to face features than unidentifiable scrambled versions otherwise similar in low-level properties, (2) of the units responding to face stimuli, some responded regardless of which part of the face was shown, and (3) approximately 20% of all units, however, responded selectively only to whole faces and not to parts of faces, a striking selectivity to which we turn next.
Whole-Face-Selective Neurons
We next focused on the whole-face (WF)-selective units, defined in our study as those that responded differentially to WFs compared to the cutouts (n = 36). The majority of such units showed no correlation with RT of the patient’s behavioral response (only 3/36 showed a significant positive correlation, and 2/36 a significant negative correlation with RT in the bubble trials; 1/36 showed a significant positive correlation with RT in the WF trials), favoring a sensory over a motor-related representation. The majority of the units (32 of 36, 89%) increased their firing rate for WFs relative to bubble trials. Focusing on these units that increase their rate (see below for the others), the first temporal epoch showing significantly differential responses to WFs and bubble trials was 250–500 ms after stimulus onset (Figure 3A). Note that this is an independent confirmation of the response selectivity, because only cutouts rather than bubble trials were used to define the WF selectivity of the neurons to begin with (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures).
How representative are the WF-selective neurons of the entire population of amygdala neurons? To quantify the differential response across all neurons to WFs compared to bubble stimuli, we calculated a whole-face index (WFI; see Supplemental Experimental Procedures) as the baseline-normalized difference in response to whole faces compared to bubbles. The average WFI of the entire population (n = 185) was 11% ± 3% (significantly different from zero, p < 0.0005), showing a mean increase in response to WFs compared to bubbled faces. The absolute values of the WFI for the previously identified class of WF-selective units (n = 36) and all other units (n = 149) were significantly different (53% ± 7% and 18% ± 2%, respectively; p < 1e-7; Figures 3C and 3D). We conclude that a subpopulation of about 20% of amygdala neurons is particularly responsive to WFs.
Nonlinear Face Responses
We next systematically analyzed responses of WF-selective neurons as a function of the proportion of eye and mouth region that was revealed in each bubble trial (number of bubbles that overlap with the eye and mouth ROI) (Figure 4A). Because mean firing rates varied between 0.2 and 6 Hz (cf. Figure 3B), we assured equal weight from each unit by normalizing (for each unit) the number of spikes relative to the number of spikes evoked by the WF. The resulting normalized response as a function of the proportion of the ROI that was revealed across the bubble trials is shown for several representative single units in Figure 4B. We found several classes of responses: some did not depend on the proportion of the face revealed (Figure 4B3), some increased as a function of the proportion revealed (Figures 4B6 and 4B7), and some decreased (Figures 4B1, 4B2, 4B4, and 4B5). Statistically, most individual units had a response function whose slope did not achieve significance (28 of 36 units), and thus most units did not clearly increase or decrease their firing rate as a function of the proportion of the face revealed. However, in nearly all cases, there was a striking discrepancy between responses to bubbles compared to whole faces: responses to bubble trials were not at all predictive of responses to WFs, even when substantial portions of the face or its features were revealed. We next quantified this observation further.
The population average of all single-trial responses of all units that increase their rate for WFs (32 units) showed a highly significant negative relationship with the amount of the eye and mouth revealed in the bubble trials (Figure 4C). This negative relationship was statistically robust across all trials as well as units, as assessed by a bootstrap statistic (mean slope −0.18 ± 0.05; see Figure 4C for details). Although this result was based on normalized firing rates, an even more significant negative relationship was found when considering absolute firing rates (Figure S3D) or the proportion of the whole face revealed (Figure S3E). The slope of the curve became more negative as the partial face became more similar to the WF (Figure 4C). Moreover, the same pattern, but with opposite sign, was found for the population average of all units that decreased their spike rate to WFs (n = 4): these units increased their spike rate with greater proportion of the face or ROI revealed (Figure S3A). Thus, in both cases, the population average of neurons that were WF selective (as defined by the initial contrast between WF and cutouts) showed a strong and statistically significant relationship with the amount of the face that was shown, despite a complete failure to predict the response to whole faces. For neurons that were not WF selective to begin with, there was no systematic effect in response to the proportion of the face revealed—the slope was not significantly different from zero (Figure 4D). However, even for these non-WF-selective neurons, there was still a surprising difference between full-face and bubble trials, indicating that some of the non-WF units remain sensitive to WFs to some degree (also see Figure 3C). None of the above effects could be explained by mere differences in visibility or contrast (quantified by the contrast threshold) between the bubble and WF trials (Figure 4E; Figure S3C).
Might the above effect somehow result from the fact that the majority of bubble trials only revealed a small proportion of the face? We tested this possibility in one patient by disabling the dynamic change in bubbles and showing a relatively fixed and large number of bubbles (~100), revealing a large proportion of the face on all trials (Figure S3F shows examples; typically >90% of the eyes and mouth ROI was revealed). In this patient, we found 5 out of 21 units (24%) that were WF selective (see Figure S3H for an example), and these neurons showed a similar nonlinear response profile (Figure S3G). The average WFI for the WF units and the entire population was 130% ± 14% and 37% ± 8%, respectively. Once again, we found that neurons that selectively respond to WFs failed to respond to parts of the face, in this case even when almost all of the face was revealed.
Could a difference in eye movements contribute to the responses we observed? This issue is pertinent, given that the human amygdala is critical for eye movements directed toward salient features of faces: lesions of the human amygdala abolish the normal fixations onto the eye region of faces [13]. Although we did not record eye movements in the present study as a result of technical constraints, we measured eye movements in a separate sample of 30 healthy participants (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures) in the same task. The mean and variance of the fixation patterns along the x and y axes did not differ between whole and bubbled faces (p > 0.20, two-tailed paired sign test). Similarly, in a previous study we found that fixation times on eyes and mouth in WFs and bubbled faces did not differ [14].
Finally, to examine the possible effects of recording from neurons that were in seizure-related tissue, we recalculated all analyses excluding any neurons within regions that were later determined to be within the epileptic focus. After excluding all units from that hemisphere in which seizures originated (see Table S1), a total of 179 units remained. Of those, 157 had the minimal required firing rate of 0.2 Hz, and 32 of those units (20%) were WF-selective units. Using only those units, all results remained qualitatively the same. It is also worth noting that, with one exception, all the patients with a temporal origin of seizures had their seizure foci in the hippocampus rather than the amygdala (Table S1), further making it unlikely that the inclusion of neurons within seizure-related tissue might have biased our findings.
Discussion
Recording from single neurons in the amygdalae of seven neurosurgical patients, we found that over half of all neurons responded to face stimuli (compared to only 10% of neurons responding to phase-scrambled faces), and a substantial proportion of these showed responses selective for whole faces as compared to pieces of faces (WF-selective). Also, most neurons (31 of 36) did not show any association with reaction time, arguing that the majority of WF-selective neurons in the amygdala are driven by the sensory properties of whole faces rather than decisions or actions based on them. The earliest responses to WFs occurred within 250–500 ms after stimulus onset (Figure 3A). WF-selective neurons showed a highly nonlinear response, such that their response to WFs was inversely correlated with their response to variable amounts of the face or its features (eyes or mouth) that were revealed. Neurons that decreased their response as a function of the amount of the face revealed increased their response to WFs (Figure 4C). In contrast, neurons that increased their response as a function of the amount of the face revealed decreased their response to WFs (Figure S3A). In both cases, neurons showed the greatest difference in response between WFs and pieces of faces when facial features shown were actually the most similar between the two types of stimulus categories. Thus, the response to partially revealed faces was not predictive of how the unit would respond to WFs. These findings provide strong support for the conclusion that amygdala neurons encode holistic information about WFs, rather than about their constituent features.
We identified WF-selective neurons based on comparisons with the eye and mouth cutout trials. Because the remainder of the analysis was based on responses of these neurons in the bubble trials, the selection and subsequent analysis are statistically independent. This also allows later comparison of the response to the cutouts with the bubble trials (Figures 4C and 4D), which reveals that the cutout responses (unlike the WF responses) are consistent with what the bubble trials predict.
Our subjects performed an emotion categorization task, but amygdala responses to faces have been observed also in a variety of other tasks [2, 4, 6, 7, 11]. Also, classification images for face identification tasks are very similar to those we obtained using our emotion discrimination task [15] (cf. Figure 1C). This makes it plausible that WF-selective units would be observed regardless of the precise nature of the task requirements. We emphasize the distinction between responsive and selective neurons in our study—although about 50% of neurons responded to facial stimuli (compared to scrambles), this does not make them face selective because they might also respond to a variety of nonface stimuli (which were not shown in our study). Thus, the WF-selective units we found were selective for WFs compared to face parts, but their response to nonface stimuli remains unknown.
We analyzed the responses to the bubble trials by plotting neuronal responses as a function of the amount of the eye and mouth features revealed in these trials (Figure 4), as well as plotting the proportion of the entire face revealed (Figure S3E). The two measures (percentage of ROI revealed, percentage of entire face revealed) were positively correlated across trials (on average r = 0.46, p < 0.001; Figure S1C), because bubbles were independently and uniformly distributed over the entire image and the average number of bubbles (typically converging to around 20 during a session) was sufficiently high to make clustering of all bubbles on one ROI unlikely. As expected, the response as a function of the proportion of the entire face revealed (Figure S3E) thus shows a similar relationship at the population level. We used percentage of ROI for our primary analysis because it offered a metric with greater range, due to variability in the spatial location of the bubbles.
Facial Features Represented in the Amygdala
Building on theoretical models [16] as well as findings from responses to faces in temporal neocortex that provides input to the amygdala [17, 18], several studies have asked what aspects of faces might be represented in the amygdala. Various reports have demonstrated that the amygdala encodes information both about the identity of an individual’s face, as well as about the social meaning of the face, such as its emotional expression or perceived trustworthiness [2, 4, 6, 7, 11]. Patients with amygdala lesions exhibit facial processing deficits for a variety of different facial expressions, including both fearful and happy [1, 13], and we found that most WF-selective units do not distinguish between fearful and happy, suggesting that the amygdala is concerned with a more general or abstract aspect of face processing than an exclusive focus on expressions of fear. At which stage of information processing does the amygdala participate? Because the amygdala receives highly processed visual information from temporal neocortex [19], one view is that it contains viewpoint-invariant [20], holistic [21] representations of faces synthesized through its inputs. Such global face representations could then be associated with the valence and social meaning of the face [22, 23] in order to modulate emotional responses and social behavior. This possibility is supported by blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) activations within the amygdala to a broad range of face stimuli (e.g., [3, 5]). An alternative possibility is motivated by the finding that the amygdala, at least in humans, appears to be remarkably specialized for processing a single feature within faces: the region around the eyes. For instance, lesions of the amygdala selectively impair processing information from the eye region in order to judge facial emotion [13], and BOLD-fMRI studies reveal amygdala activation during attention to the eyes in faces [24] and to isolated presentation of the eye region [25, 26]. These opposing findings suggest two conflicting views of the role of the amygdala during face processing. Our results generally support the first possibility.
Face Responses in the Primate Amygdala
The amygdala receives most of its visual inputs from visually responsive temporal neocortex [19, 27], and there is direct evidence from electrical microstimulation of functional connections between face-selective patches of temporal cortex and the lateral amygdala in monkeys [28]. Although there is ongoing debate regarding a possible subcortical route of visual input to the amygdala that might bypass visual cortices [29], both the long response latencies and WF selectivity of the neurons we report suggest a predominant input via cortical processing. This then raises a core question: What is the transformation of face representations in the amygdala, relative to its cortical inputs?
The regions of temporal cortex that likely convey visual information about faces to the amygdala themselves show remarkable selectivity to faces [30–32] and to particular identities [33, 34] and emotions [17, 35] of faces. Regions providing likely input to the amygdala [28] are known to contain a high proportion (>80%) of face-selective cells and have highly viewpoint-invariant responses to specific face identities [20]. In humans, studies using BOLD-fMRI have demonstrated between 3 and 5 regions of cortex in the occipital, temporal, and frontal lobes that show selective activation to faces and that appear to range in encoding parts of faces, identities of faces, or changeable aspects of faces such as emotional expressions [18, 36]. Intracranial recordings in humans have observed electrophysiological responses selective for faces in the anterior temporal cortex [37, 38]. However, although there is thus overwhelming evidence for neurons that respond to faces rather than to other stimulus categories, many temporal regions also respond to specific parts or features of faces to some extent [32, 39, 40]. In contrast, the highly nonlinear face responses we observed in the amygdala have not been reported.
Single-unit responses in the monkey amygdala have described responses selective for faces [41], with cells showing selectivity for specific face identities and facial expressions of emotion [7, 42, 43] as well as head and gaze direction [44]. Interestingly, the proportion of face-responsive cells in the monkey amygdala has been reported to be approximately 50% [7, 42], similar to what we found in our patients. Similarly, cells recorded in the human anteromedial temporal lobe including the amygdala have been reported to exhibit highly specific and viewpoint-invariant responses to familiar faces [11, 45], as well as selectivity for both the identity and emotional expression of faces [6]. The present findings are consistent with the idea that there is a convergence of tuning to facial features toward more anterior sectors of the temporal lobe, culminating in neurons with responses highly selective to WFs as we found in the amygdala. The nonlinear face responses we describe here may indicate an architecture involving both summation and inhibition in order to synthesize highly selective face representations. The need to do so in the amygdala likely reflects this structure’s known role in social behavior and associative emotional memory: in order to track exactly which people are friend or foe, the associations between value and face identity must be extremely selective in order to avoid confusions between different people.
It remains an important question to understand how the face representations in the amygdala are used by other brain regions receiving amygdala input. It is possible that aspects of temporal cortical face responses depend on recurrent inputs from the amygdala, because the face selectivity of neurons in temporal regions that are functionally connected with the amygdala (such as the anterior medial face patch) evolves over time and peaks with a long latency of >300 ms [20], and because temporal cortex can signal information about emotional expression at later points in time than face categorization as such [35]. Similarly, visually responsive human amygdala neurons respond with a long latency of on average around 400 ms [46]. Such a role for amygdala modulation of temporal visual cortex is also supported by BOLD-fMRI studies in humans that have compared signals to faces in patients with lesions to the amygdala [47].
In conclusion, our findings demonstrate that the human amygdala contains a high proportion of face-responsive neurons. Most of those that show some kind of selectivity are selective for presentations of the entire face and show surprising sensitivity to the deletion of even small components of the face. Responses selective for whole faces are more prevalent than responses selective for face features, and responses to whole faces cannot be predicted from parametric variations in the features. Taken together, these observations argue that the face representations in the human amygdala encode socially relevant information, such as identity of a person based on the entire face, rather than information about specific features such as the eyes.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was supported in part by grants from the Gustavus and Louise Pfeiffer Foundation, the National Science Foundation, the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation (R.A.), and the Max Planck Society (U.R.). We thank the staff of the Huntington Memorial Hospital Epilepsy and Brain Mapping Program for excellent support with participant testing, Erin Schuman for providing some of the recording equipment, and Frederic Gosselin for advice on the bubbles method.
Footnotes
Supplemental Information includes three figures, two tables, and Supplemental Experimental Procedures and can be found with this article online at doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.035.
References
- 1.Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio A. Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature. 1994;372:669–672. doi: 10.1038/372669a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Winston JS, Strange BA, O’Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:277–283. doi: 10.1038/nn816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fitzgerald DA, Angstadt M, Jelsone LM, Nathan PJ, Phan KL. Beyond threat: amygdala reactivity across multiple expressions of facial affect. Neuroimage. 2006;30:1441–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hoffman KL, Gothard KM, Schmid MC, Logothetis NK. Facial-expression and gaze-selective responses in the monkey amygdala. Curr Biol. 2007;17:766–772. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Mattay VS, Fera F, Weinberger DR. The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroimage. 2002;17:317–323. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fried I, MacDonald KA, Wilson CL. Single neuron activity in human hippocampus and amygdala during recognition of faces and objects. Neuron. 1997;18:753–765. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80315-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gothard KM, Battaglia FP, Erickson CA, Spitler KM, Amaral DG. Neural responses to facial expression and face identity in the monkey amygdala. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:1671–1683. doi: 10.1152/jn.00714.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rutishauser U, Ross IB, Mamelak AN, Schuman EM. Human memory strength is predicted by theta-frequency phase-locking of single neurons. Nature. 2010;464:903–907. doi: 10.1038/nature08860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gosselin F, Schyns PG. Bubbles: a technique to reveal the use of information in recognition tasks. Vision Res. 2001;41:2261–2271. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00097-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Steinmetz PN, Cabrales E, Wilson MS, Baker CP, Thorp CK, Smith KA, Treiman DM. Neurons in the human hippocampus and amygdala respond to both low and high level image properties. J Neurophysiol. 2011;6:2874–2884. doi: 10.1152/jn.00977.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Category-specific visual responses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:946–953. doi: 10.1038/78868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mosher CP, Zimmerman PE, Gothard KM. Response characteristics of basolateral and centromedial neurons in the primate amygdala. J Neurosci. 2010;30:16197–16207. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3225-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR. A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature. 2005;433:68–72. doi: 10.1038/nature03086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Neumann D, Spezio ML, Piven J, Adolphs R. Looking you in the mouth: abnormal gaze in autism resulting from impaired top-down modulation of visual attention. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1:194–202. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schyns PG, Bonnar L, Gosselin F. Show me the features! Understanding recognition from the use of visual information. Psychol Sci. 2002;13:402–409. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bruce V, Young A. Understanding face recognition. Br J Psychol. 1986;77:305–327. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hasselmo ME, Rolls ET, Baylis GC. The role of expression and identity in the face-selective responses of neurons in the temporal visual cortex of the monkey. Behav Brain Res. 1989;32:203–218. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(89)80054-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed ) 2000;4:223–233. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01482-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Amaral DG, Price JL, Pitkanen A, Carmichael ST. Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: Aggleton JP, editor. The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss; 1992. pp. 1–66. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Freiwald WA, Tsao DY. Functional compartmentalization and viewpoint generalization within the macaque face-processing system. Science. 2010;330:845–851. doi: 10.1126/science.1194908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Andrews TJ, Davies-Thompson J, Kingstone A, Young AW. Internal and external features of the face are represented holistically in face-selective regions of visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30:3544–3552. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4863-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Adolphs R. What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1191:42–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Todorov A, Engell AD. The role of the amygdala in implicit evaluation of emotionally neutral faces. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3:303–312. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gamer M, Büchel C. Amygdala activation predicts gaze toward fearful eyes. J Neurosci. 2009;29:9123–9126. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1883-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Morris JS, deBonis M, Dolan RJ. Human amygdala responses to fearful eyes. Neuroimage. 2002;17:214–222. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Whalen PJ, Kagan J, Cook RG, Davis FC, Kim H, Polis S, McLaren DG, Somerville LH, McLean AA, Maxwell JS, Johnstone T. Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science. 2004;306:2061. doi: 10.1126/science.1103617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Iwai E, Yukie M. Amygdalofugal and amygdalopetal connections with modality-specific visual cortical areas in macaques (Macaca fuscata, M. mulatta, and M fascicularis) J Comp Neurol. 1987;261:362–387. doi: 10.1002/cne.902610304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Moeller S, Freiwald WA, Tsao DY. Patches with links: a unified system for processing faces in the macaque temporal lobe. Science. 2008;320:1355–1359. doi: 10.1126/science.1157436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pessoa L, Adolphs R. Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:773–783. doi: 10.1038/nrn2920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bruce C, Desimone R, Gross CG. Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. J Neurophysiol. 1981;46:369–384. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.2.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Perrett DI, Rolls ET, Caan W. Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp Brain Res. 1982;47:329–342. doi: 10.1007/BF00239352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tanaka K, Saito H, Fukada Y, Moriya M. Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66:170–189. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.1.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rolls ET. Functions of the primate temporal lobe cortical visual areas in invariant visual object and face recognition. Neuron. 2000;27:205–218. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00030-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Eifuku S, De Souza WC, Tamura R, Nishijo H, Ono T. Neuronal correlates of face identification in the monkey anterior temporal cortical areas. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:358–371. doi: 10.1152/jn.00198.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sugase Y, Yamane S, Ueno S, Kawano K. Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex. Nature. 1999;400:869–873. doi: 10.1038/23703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tsao DY, Moeller S, Freiwald WA. Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:19514–19519. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809662105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Allison T, McCarthy G, Nobre A, Puce A, Belger A. Human extrastriate visual cortex and the perception of faces, words, numbers, and colors. Cereb Cortex. 1994;4:544–554. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.5.544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Allison T, Puce A, Spencer DD, McCarthy G. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cereb Cortex. 1999;9:415–430. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.5.415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fujita I, Tanaka K, Ito M, Cheng K. Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature. 1992;360:343–346. doi: 10.1038/360343a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Freiwald WA, Tsao DY, Livingstone MS. A face feature space in the macaque temporal lobe. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1187–1196. doi: 10.1038/nn.2363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Leonard CM, Rolls ET, Wilson FA, Baylis GC. Neurons in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. Behav Brain Res. 1985;15:159–176. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90062-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Nakamura K, Mikami A, Kubota K. Activity of single neurons in the monkey amygdala during performance of a visual discrimination task. J Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1447–1463. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.6.1447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kuraoka K, Nakamura K. Impacts of facial identity and type of emotion on responses of amygdala neurons. Neuroreport. 2006;17:9–12. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000194383.02999.c5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Tazumi T, Hori E, Maior RS, Ono T, Nishijo H. Neural correlates to seen gaze-direction and head orientation in the macaque monkey amygdala. Neuroscience. 2010;169:287–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Quiroga RQ, Reddy L, Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature. 2005;435:1102–1107. doi: 10.1038/nature03687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mormann F, Kornblith S, Quiroga RQ, Kraskov A, Cerf M, Fried I, Koch C. Latency and selectivity of single neurons indicate hierarchical processing in the human medial temporal lobe. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8865–8872. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1640-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Vuilleumier P, Richardson MP, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:1271–1278. doi: 10.1038/nn1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.