Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2015 Mar 23;28(1):15–26. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000255

Susceptibility effects of GABA receptor subunit alpha-2 (GABRA2) variants and parental monitoring on externalizing behavior trajectories: Risk and protection conveyed by the minor allele

Elisa M Trucco 1, Sandra Villafuerte 1, Mary M Heitzeg 1, Margit Burmeister 1, Robert A Zucker 1
PMCID: PMC4580498  NIHMSID: NIHMS675091  PMID: 25797587

Abstract

Understanding factors increasing susceptibility to social contexts and predicting psychopathology can help identify targets for prevention. Persistently high externalizing behavior in adolescence is predictive of psychopathology in adulthood. Parental monitoring predicts low externalizing behavior, yet youth likely vary in the degree to which they are affected by parents. Genetic variants of GABA receptor subunit alpha-2 (GABRA2) may increase susceptibility to parental monitoring, thus impacting externalizing trajectories. We had several objectives: (a) to determine whether GABRA2 (rs279827, rs279826, rs279858) moderates the relationship between a component of parental monitoring, parental knowledge, and externalizing trajectories; (b) to test the form of this interaction to assess whether GABRA2 variants reflect risk (diathesis-stress) or susceptibility (differential susceptibility) factors; and (c) to clarify GABRA2 associations on the development of problem behavior. This prospective study (N = 504) identified three externalizing trajectory classes (i.e., low, decreasing, and high) across adolescence. A GABRA2 × Parental Monitoring effect on class membership was observed, such that A-carriers were largely unaffected by parental monitoring, whereas class membership for those with the GG genotype was affected by parental monitoring. Findings support differential susceptibility in GABRA2.


Studies examining Gene × Environment (G × E) interactions in psychological research have proliferated in the past decade. This is largely due to the understanding that most multifaceted behaviors, including mental disorders, are products of both genetic and environmental factors (Rutter, Moffitt, & Caspi, 2006). Social sciences research has tended to focus on the identification of novel G × E relationships across various candidate genes, social environments, and outcomes, rather than on which associations withstand the test of replication (Risch et al., 2009). The interplay of genes and the environment is complex. Therefore, before we can think further about translating new knowledge into clinical practice, it is critical that findings are consistent across samples. Given preliminary evidence for the role of GABA receptor subunit alpha-2 (GABRA2) variants as both risk (Dick et al., 2009) as well as plasticity factors (i.e., the differential susceptibility hypothesis; Brody, Chen, & Beach, 2013; Simons & Lei, 2013), this study tests whether parental monitoring interactions with GABRA2 conform to a diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility model. Understanding G × E interactions outside of risk conceptualizations may have utility in identifying not only individuals who are most at risk for developing psychopathology but also those who are most likely to benefit from adaptive social contexts including interventions.

Externalizing Behavior

The development of externalizing behavior has been of longstanding interest given the difficulty in treating delinquent youth coupled with its direct cost to the larger society. Adolescent externalizing behavior has been associated with a variety of negative sequelae in adulthood, including criminal activity, antisocial personality disorder, and alcohol and drug dependence (Odgers et al., 2008; Shaw, Hyde, & Brennan, 2012). Researchers have come to understand engagement in externalizing behavior as being characterized by heterogeneous pathways, whereby different trajectories are associated with specific etiological pathways and outcomes (Fairchild, van Goozen, Calder, & Goodyer, 2013; Lacourse, Nagin, Tremblay, Vitaro, & Claes, 2003; Moffitt, 1993; Odgers et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2012; Weisner & Windle, 2004). The work of Moffitt (1993) is perhaps the most widely recognized theoretical model of adolescent externalizing taxonomies. She posits that for some individuals problem behavior develops early and is stable (i.e., life-course persistent), whereas for others it develops later and is limited to adolescence (i.e., adolescent limited). Externalizing behavior within the adolescent-limited trajectory typically desists in adulthood. In contrast, those continuing to engage in more persistent externalizing behavior are more likely to develop psychopathology in adulthood, such as alcohol dependence (Moffitt, 1993).

Recent refinements have been made to Moffitt's (1993) model to expand this typology to include five clusters: normative experimentation, childhood limited, childhood-onset persistent, adolescent limited, and adolescent-onset persistent (Fairchild et al., 2013). Based on a comprehensive literature review, the authors found that a majority of individuals with high rates of externalizing behavior in childhood tend to reduce these behaviors in early adolescence. In addition, a majority of individuals who begin engaging in externalizing behavior in adolescence tend not to remit in adulthood; rather, they usually extend these behaviors into their mid-20s (Fairchild et al., 2013). Empirical studies largely support these theoretical models. That is, usually three to five groups are identified, with the typical pattern reflecting stable low and stable high groups, a moderate group, a declining group, and a late-starting high group or increasing group (Odgers et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2012; Weisner & Windle, 2004). Understanding predictors differentiating these trajectories is of practical importance as it may identify adolescents most at risk for developing psychopathology.

Parental Monitoring and Knowledge

Parenting, a primary social context for adolescents, has garnered increasing support for its role in impacting adolescent adjustment. In particular, parental monitoring has been consistently supported as a robust predictor of externalizing behavior (e.g., Barnes, Hoffman, Welte, Farrell, & Dintcheff, 2006). Earlier work conceptualized parental monitoring as “parenting behaviors involving attention to and tracking of child whereabouts, activities, and associations” (Dishion & McMahon, 1998, p. 61). However, in their seminal work, Stattin and Kerr (2000) demonstrate that there is a significant gap in how parental monitoring is conceptualized and how it is typically measured. That is, most measures of parental monitoring reflect knowledge rather than direct tracking and surveillance. In more recent work, researchers have determined that this knowledge is acquired primarily in the context of an open and trusting parent–child relationship through frequent child disclosure and of the parent's ability to actively monitor the child (Kerr, Stattin, & Burk, 2010). Consistent with these findings, longitudinal studies demonstrate that a key component of parental monitoring, parental knowledge, predicts youth delinquent behavior as well as unique trajectories of delinquent behavior across adolescence (Laird, Pettit, Bates, & Dodge, 2003; Shaw et al., 2012). Given strong support for the role of parental knowledge on externalizing behavior, the current study focuses on this important component of monitoring.

Ecological perspectives (e.g., Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998) suggest that the degree to which social contexts exert an influence on adolescents' behaviors varies greatly. Therefore, research focusing on interactions between environmental variation and a child's individual characteristics may provide a more accurate description of the complexity of child development and its processes (Nigg, 2006). Conditional models encompassing parental monitoring and biology may be important for understanding unique etiological factors predicting different externalizing pathways.

G × E Effects

One of the most prominent examples of child characteristics moderating social environments has come from research on G × E interactions, which demonstrates that adolescents differ in vulnerability to parenting based on genotype (Dick et al., 2009; Kochanska, Kim, Barry, & Philibert, 2011). One example is the GABRA2 gene, located on chromosome 4, which codes for the alpha-2 subunit of the receptor of the neurotransmitter GABA-A. Dick and colleagues (2009) found evidence for a GABRA2 × Parental Monitoring interaction predicting externalizing trajectories, suggesting that those with the GABRA2 minor allele were more likely to demonstrate persistently elevated externalizing behaviors. This effect was strong among adolescents reporting low parental monitoring, consistent with diathesis–stress models whereby some individuals may be more vulnerable to environmental stressors due to their genes and at greatest risk for developing psychopathology (Zuckerman, 1999). However, in a later erratum (Dick et al., 2011), the authors state that it was those with the major allele who were most at risk for developing externalizing problems. These associations warrant clarification in two specific areas given the potential impact that these findings have on understanding the role of GABRA2 and parental monitoring on the development of externalizing behavior.

First, the reported direction of GABRA2 associations are mixed. The single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in the GABRA2 gene that are reported in most studies are in linkage disequilibrium with two common genetic forms or haplotypes, the major haplotype (∼50.4%) and the minor (∼44.0%) haplotype in Whites, with more than 10 SNPs that consistently differ between these two forms (International HapMap Consortium, 2003). Although a majority of studies report that the minor allele is associated with alcohol dependence and impulsivity (Bauer et al., 2007; Covault, Gelernter, Hesselbrock, Nellissery, & Kranzler, 2004; Edenberg et al., 2004; Enoch, Schwartz, Albaugh, Virkkunen, & Goldman, 2006; Pierucci-Lagha et al., 2005; Villafuerte, Strumba, Stoltenberg, Zucker, & Burmeister, 2013), some studies report that the major allele increases risk for alcohol dependence, conduct disorder, and externalizing behavior (Agrawal et al., 2006; Dick et al., 2006, 2009). This illustrates recent critiques that question the burgeoning interest in G × E research because of inconsistencies across studies (Risch, et al., 2009).

Second, recent work, including a Special Issue in Development and Psychopathology in 2011, suggests that traditional diathesis-stress models may be limited by their negative focus on contextual adversity and therefore fail to fully capture all processes relevant to environmental influences on behavior (Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007; Belsky & Pluess, 2009). The differential susceptibility hypothesis suggests that individuals with genotypes traditionally conferring risk may also be more affected by adverse as well as adaptive environments (Belsky et al., 2007). Research has largely supported differential susceptibility in the serotonergic and dopaminergic systems (e.g., Belsky & Pluess, 2009; Kochanska et al., 2011). There is preliminary evidence suggesting that GABRA2 variants may also reflect susceptibility factors. For example, Brody and colleagues (2013) examined differential effects of prevention trials on subsequent alcohol use based on genetic factors. Findings demonstrated that youth with genetic variants, including those in GABRA2, reported more alcohol use in the control condition but also reaped the most benefit from the prevention program, resulting in a greater reduction in alcohol use over time consistent with the differential susceptibility hypothesis (Brody et al., 2013).

Alpha-2 GABA-A receptors are expressed primarily in the amygdala and areas receiving innervation from the striatum, such as the substantia nigra (Brody et al., 2013; Schwarzer et al., 2001). GABRA2 variants are associated with increased activity in these brain areas, thereby likely increasing emotional responsiveness and sensitivity to social contexts (Brody et al., 2013; Simons & Lei, 2013). GABRA2 may reflect a susceptibility factor, with heightened sensitivity to a variety of social contexts, not just risk. This is a notable distinction from risk conceptualizations, as individuals with variants traditionally conferring risk may actually be more affected by adverse and adaptive environments alike (Belsky & Pluess, 2009). The current study examines whether there is evidence for GABRA2 as a susceptibility factor consistent with the differential susceptibility hypothesis. This is the first study to our knowledge to empirically test the differential susceptibility hypothesis in GABRA2.

Current Study

The primary aim of this study was to test whether the differential susceptibility hypothesis is supported in GABRA2 × Parental Monitoring interactions in the prediction of externalizing trajectories. This study is based on a sample of adolescents from the Michigan Longitudinal Study (MLS), which is an ongoing multiwave prospective study (Zucker, Ellis, Fitzgerald, Bingham, & Sanford, 1996; Zucker et al., 2000). The MLS is community sample enriched with high-risk families including fathers convicted of drunk driving, meeting criteria for alcohol use disorder (AUD). Given prior work (e.g., Fairchild et al., 2013; Lacourse et al., 2003; Odgers et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2012), we hypothesized that three to five trajectory classes would be identified. We also hypothesized weak effects of GABRA2 on externalizing behavior given small effect sizes of SNPs (Ioannidis, Trikalinos, & Khoury, 2006), but direct effects of parental knowledge, a component of parental monitoring, on externalizing behavior consistent with prior work (Barnes et al., 2006; Laird et al., 2003; Shaw et al., 2012). We expected that a comparative analysis of diathesis–stress versus differential-susceptibility models following recommended steps (Belsky et al., 2007) would help decipher if the minor allele is best characterized as a risk or susceptibility factor. Consistent with the majority of the literature reporting that the minor allele is associated with genetic risk (Agrawal et al., 2006; Bauer et al., 2007; Covault et al., 2004; Edenberg et al., 2004), we expected a significant GABRA2 × Parental Monitoring interaction, whereby monitoring would have a greater effect on externalizing trajectories among those homozygous for the minor allele (GG genotype) compared to A-carriers (AA or AG genotypes) across SNPs (rs279827, rs279826 and rs279858). Genotype data was dichotomized (A-carriers = 0, GG genotype = 1) given that those with one or two major alleles (A) had comparable levels of externalizing behavior.

Method

Participants

The MLS is an ongoing prospective study that utilizes population-based recruitment procedures to access a nonclinical sample of alcoholic families as well as ecologically comparable non-substance-abusing control families, resulting in three different risk categories (Zucker et al., 1996, 2000). The first represents a high-risk family including fathers convicted of drunk driving, meeting criteria for AUD. Families from the same neighborhoods were also recruited to represent two other risk categories: fathers with no history of AUD (low risk) or fathers identified as having AUD (moderate risk). Mothers' AUD was free to vary in the high and moderate risk category but was an exclusion criterion for the low-risk category. Families were eligible for the study if they had a son between the ages of 3 and 5 at the time of recruitment. This study included 504 adolescents from the following AUD risk categories: low risk (39.3%, n = 198), moderate risk (28.6%, n = 144), and high risk (32.1%, n = 162). Full biological siblings were also included. A majority of adolescents are male (71.2%, n = 359) and primarily White (96.8%, n = 488) because female siblings and non-White families were included after the early waves of the study were initiated. For a full description of MLS methods and demographic characteristics see Zucker et al. (1996, 2000).

Procedure

Parents and children completed extensive assessments in their homes following initial recruitment (Wave 1, ages 3 to 5) with subsequent assessments occurring every 3 years (e.g., Wave 2, ages 6 to 8, Wave 3 ages 9 to 11, up through ages 15 to 17). Children were assessed annually when the target child turned 11 years old. Given that externalizing behavior is dynamic in adolescence and likely to change in short spans of time compared to other developmental periods (Weisner & Windle, 2004), available annual data were used instead of wave data to capture important nuances.

Adolescents completed self-report measures reflecting their own behavior (e.g., externalizing behavior) and their parent's behavior (e.g., monitoring). In addition, a subset of families (both parent and child) provided blood or saliva for genotyping. Given potential evocative effects of GABRA2 on parental monitoring, gene–environment correlation (rGE; Belsky & Beaver, 2011) tests between adolescents' and parents' genotype and parental monitoring were conducted because rGE may confound G × E effects (Belsky & Beaver, 2011). This refers to a nonrandom distribution of environments across different genotypes (see Kendler, 2011, for a review of rGE). Otherwise, data for the present study are based on adolescent reports for those with at least two annual assessments. This study examines 504 children from 253 families. Eighty-one (32.0%) families had 1 child, 107 (42.3%) had 2 children, 52 (20.6%) had 3 children, and 13 (5.1%) had 4 or 5 children participate in the study.

Measures

Externalizing behavior

Adolescents tend to report more problem behaviors compared to reports from fathers, mothers, and teachers (Stanger & Lewis, 1993). As such, problem behavior was assessed using the Youth Self-Report (YSR) from the Achenbach System of Empirical Behavioral Assessment (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Items from the aggressive and delinquency subscales were used to calculate raw scores of externalizing behavior. Items on the YSR are rated on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = not true, 2 = very true or often true). For this study, externalizing behavior specific to the adolescent's age (i.e., externalizing at ages 12 to 17) was calculated based on the adolescent's date of birth and date of assessment. The YSR has been used extensively and has demonstrated strong reliability and validity (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Internal consistency across assessments in this study was good (Cronbach α = 0.88).

Parental monitoring

Consistent with the larger literature, parental monitoring reflects a dual process involving “parents' knowledge of the child's whereabouts, activities, and associations” (Kerr & Stattin, 2000, p. 368), as well as child-driven processes such as disclosure of information to parents. Thus, parental monitoring is viewed as ongoing bidirectional Person × Environment interactions that reflect a proxy for the family environment (DiClemente et al., 2001; Kerr & Stattin, 2003). Research suggests that parenting practices as reported by the adolescent have a stronger impact on future psychosocial development and may be less biased than parent-report (Kuppens, Grietens, Onghena, & Michiels, 2009). Therefore, the Parental Monitoring—Youth Form (Chilcoat & Anthony, 1996) was used to assess parental knowledge and disclosure processes, as reported by the adolescent. This seven-item measure reflects adolescent perceptions of parents' knowledge about their peer group, their whereabouts, and expectations about time spent away from the home. A maximum value reflecting parental monitoring across ages 11 and 12 was created. Internal consistency was adequate (Cronbach α = 0.71).

Background variables

A strong relationship has been demonstrated between family adversity characteristics (e.g., income, marital status) and race on adolescent externalizing behavior (Chung, Hill, Hawkins, Gilchrist, & Nagin, 2002). Growth factors were regressed on the following demographic characteristics: adolescent's race and parent's marital status, average years of education, and family income. Two additional covariates, biological sex and family risk group status (i.e., low vs. moderate or high risk), were included to control for potential differences in class trajectories.

Genotyping

Three GABRA2 SNPs were selected for this study (rs279827, rs279826, and rs279858) to correspond to previous work (e.g., Dick et al., 2009). For simplicity and clarity, findings focus on SNP rs279827 given previous haplotype analyses conducted on this sample demonstrates high linkage disequilibrium across these SNPs (r2 = .80–.92; Villafuerte et al., 2013). As expected when SNPs are highly correlated, findings were largely consistent across SNPs. SNP rs279827 was chosen for its potential as a functional SNP. It is located next to an acceptor splice site (Tian, Chen, Cross, & Edenberg, 2005). SNP rs279827 was included in the Illumina Addiction biology SNP array designed by Hodgkinson and colleagues (2008), a panel genotyped in the MLS sample using the Illumina GoldenGate platform (Illumina Inc., San Diego, CA). SNPs rs279826 (intron 4) and rs279858 (exon 5, K132K) were genotyped by Taqman (Villafuerte et al., 2012). We included duplicates (78 for the array and 12 for the Taqman assay) and no discrepancies were observed. All SNPs were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium.

Data analytic plan

Trajectory analyses proceeded in two separate steps according to recommended guidelines (e.g., Jung & Wickrama, 2008). First, growth mixture modeling (GMM) in MPlus, version 7.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012), was used to identify relatively homogenous subgroups of adolescents based on shared growth trajectories of self-reported externalizing problems across adolescence (i.e., ages 12 to 17). These GMM models assume an underlying continuous growth process conceptualized as latent growth factors (i.e., intercept and slope coefficients). The intercept represents the mean elevation at the origin of the time scale (i.e., age 12 in this study). The linear slope represents the rate of change per unit of time (i.e., per year in this study). These growth factors represent average trajectories in the sample, while the variance around these means represent heterogeneity (i.e., individual differences) in growth. Growth mixture modeling is more flexible compared to traditional growth curve models since it allows for identification of unique growth factors and individual variability around the mean intercepts, slopes, and rates of change across two or more subgroups (Muthén & Muthén, 2000). Given that the identification of within-group differences was not a primary research question in this study, variances for each growth factor were constrained to be equal across trajectory classes to avoid nonconvergence in the estimation of model parameters. According to Muthén (2004), exclusion of covariates from GMM may lead to model misspecification, resulting in distorted model results. Therefore, demographic characteristics (race, parents' marital status and education, and family income) were included as covariates and regressed on growth factors.

Growth mixture models were run with an increasing number of latent classes. Standard indices were used to determine the optimal number of latent classes. One criterion, entropy ranges from 0.00 to 1.00 based on individual class probabilities with higher values reflecting clear classification (Jung & Wickrama, 2008). The Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) were also examined. Smaller AIC and BIC values reflect better model fit. Research suggests that there may be limitations to the AIC and BIC (i.e., they may be sensitive to sample size and BIC may favor low parsimony; Jung & Wickrama, 2008). The bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT) was also estimated, given evidence that it may be a better indicator for determining number of classes (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007). The BLRT provides a p value comparing the k and k − 1 class model (i.e., comparing the current model to the model with one fewer class). A p value of less than .05 indicates that the model tested fits the data better when compared to a model with one fewer class. The number of classes was determined by a combination of factors, including fit indices, parsimony, theoretical justification, and interpretability (Jung & Wickrama, 2008).

A majority (74.4%; n = 375) of adolescents included in these trajectory models had at least half of the repeated assessments available and did not differ significantly from those adolescents who were not assessed annually or did not have at least two annual assessments on externalizing behaviors in the year immediately prior to the present study's initial assessment (i.e., age 11), F (1) = 0.38, p = .54, or parental monitoring, F (1) = 0.94, p = .33.

Each adolescent was assigned to the class with the highest posterior probability. These class labels were regressed using multinomial logistic regression via GMM on genes and parenting for adolescents with genetic data (38.6%, n = 195). Standardized values of child's biological sex and family AUD risk (low versus moderate or high risk) were included as covariates. Analyses were extended to include interactions between GABRA2 (0 = A-carriers, 1 = GG) and parental monitoring, which was standardized around the sample grand mean, before forming the cross-product interaction terms to eliminate nonessential multicollinearity (Aiken & West, 1991). Three two-way interactions (e.g., Rs279827 × Parental Monitoring) were tested to examine the role of GABRA2 as a moderator in the association between parental monitoring and externalizing trajectories. Using the fitted models, conditional odds ratios were calculated and reflected pairwise comparisons of class trajectory status. Adolescents with available genetic data did not differ significantly from those who did not have available genetic data on either trajectory class membership, F (1) = 1.19, p = .28, or parental monitoring, F (1) = 0.34, p = .56. Missing genotype was assumed to be missing at random and likely had minimal impact on the results. Furthermore, in order to avoid deleting cases, analyses utilized maximum-likelihood estimation with all available data.

Sensitivity analysis

Given that our sample included siblings, we performed a sensitivity analysis. The goal of this sensitivity analysis was to assess the impact of other sources of within-family dependence, specifically shared genes unlinked to GABRA2 and shared environment, that were not explicitly included in the analysis model. To do this, we constructed a data-generating model incorporating various forms of within-family dependence. We then ran the multinomial logistic regression on data simulated from this model. We varied the degree of within-family dependence from no dependence to complete dependence and considered how the empirical standard errors of the parameter estimates increased as the degree of within-family dependence increased.

Within family dependence can be viewed as arising from three sources: genetic similarity between siblings, similarity in parental behavior toward their different children, and the net effect of all other forms of common environment within a family. Since the GABRA2 genotype and the parental monitoring level are measured directly and are included in the analysis, these two sources of dependence are already accounted for. The goal of this sensitivity analysis was therefore to assess the impact of other sources of within-family dependence, specifically shared genes unlinked to GABRA2 and shared environment, that were not explicitly included in the analysis model.

For data simulation, we first specified a log-linear random effects model that matched the observed features of the data as captured through our modeling. Sibling genotypes were simulated using Mendelian inheritance, based on genetically independent parents, with genotype frequencies matching those found in our data. Parental monitoring for siblings within a family was simulated to have a .80 intraclass correlation. Additional correlation between siblings (reflecting unmeasured common environment and genetics) was simulated using a Gaussian copula to produce positively dependent exchangeable Bernoulli trials. Standard errors were calculated using 1,000 replications.

Results

Overall, findings indicate an increase in externalizing behaviors but a relatively stable level of variability across age. Externalizing behavior was significantly correlated across all ages. High levels of externalizing behaviors were associated with low levels of parental monitoring at three different ages (12, 13, and 15). See Table 1 for the means, standard deviations, and Pearson's correlations for age-specific externalizing behavior and parental monitoring scores. It is important to note that, although our sample was enriched for families with alcoholism, externalizing scores were largely comparable to previous work examining these relationships in community samples (Dick et al., 2009), as well as those reported by Achenbach and Rescorla (2001) for their nonreferred sample. That is, average T scores for 11- to 18-year-old males and females are approximately 54 for rule-breaking and aggressive behavior scales (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The T scores in the current sample ranged from 51 to 55.

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations between parental monitoring and externalizing across ages 12 to 17.

Correlations

Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Externalizing age 12 9.06 6.18
2. Externalizing age 13 9.09 5.91 .64**
3. Externalizing age 14 9.72 6.69 .46** .62**
4. Externalizing age 15 10.26 7.42 .45** .53* .68**
5. Externalizing age 16 9.90 6.30 .43** .51** .56** .71**
6. Externalizing age 17 10.43 6.42 .34** .46** .56** .66** .75**
7. Parental monitoring 1.90 0.66 −.19* −.18* −.07 −.17* −.11 −.08
*

p < .01.

**

p < .001.

Prior to determining the number of classes to extract, linear and quadratic growth factors were compared in an unconditional traditional growth model to assess whether subsequent trajectory models should include a nonlinear growth factor. Model fit indices determined whether a linear or nonlinear model fit the data better. Higher values on the comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and lower values on the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) represent the best fitting model. The model accounting for nonlinear growth (CFI = 0.987, TLI = 0.981, RMSEA = 0.04) was more representative of the data than a linear model (CFI = 0.957, TLI = 0.960, RMSEA = 0.07). A formal likelihood ratio test, χ2Δ (4) = 27.67, p < .0001, also supports the quadratic model. As such, subsequent models include a quadratic growth factor. Given that additional growth factors add computational burden and possible convergence problems, the variance of the linear term was fixed to zero.1

Identification of externalizing behavior trajectories

Exploratory conditional growth mixture models were estimated to determine the most probable class formation by sequentially increasing the number of estimated latent classes and evaluating standard indices when accounting for demographic covariates. In all, one-class, two-class, three-class, and four-class models were tested. When comparing the one- and two-class solutions, there was a decrease in AIC (11091.09 vs. 11010.60) and BIC (11192.42 vs. 11128.83). The BLRT favored the two-class solution (p < .001). The three-class solution resulted in a decrease in AIC (10969.57) and BIC (11104.69) but a decrement in entropy (from 0.81 to 0.75). The BLRT favored the three-class solution (p < .001). Although the BLRT suggested an improvement in the four-class solution, AIC and BIC did not improve and entropy decreased (0.65). Furthermore, the four-class solution resulted in convergence issues due to low counts (∼2% of the sample in one class). Upon consideration of the model fit information, prior research, and interpretability, the three-class solution was selected as the best fitting model.

Figure 1 shows the three trajectory groups. A majority of adolescents (76.6%, n = 386) exhibited a low trajectory. A small subset (9.8%, n = 50) demonstrated elevated levels of initial externalizing behavior (intercept = 20.48, p < .001), peaking at or before age 12 and displaying a steady nonlinear decline (linear = −5.43, p < .001; quadratic = 0.65, p < .05), with comparable levels of externalizing at age 16 and 17 as the low-externalizing class. The third group (13.5%, n = 68) demonstrated a moderate level of initial externalizing behavior at age 12 (intercept = 11.73, p < .001) that increased in a nonlinear fashion (linear slope = 5.19, p < .001; quadratic slope = −0.62, p < .05). We designated these groups low, decreasing, and high, respectively (see Figure 1). Of the covariates, only family income was related to growth factors (−0.66, p < .05), suggesting that income is related to lower levels of initial externalizing behavior. Table 2 presents genotype frequencies across classes.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Externalizing trajectories for the total sample.

Table 2. Genotype frequencies across single nucleotide polymorphisms per trajectory classes.

Class Trajectories

Low Decreasing High
rs279826
 A-carriers (n = 165, 74.3%) 130 12 23
  Wthin A-carriers 78.8% 7.3% 13.9%
  Within class 74.3% 75.0% 74.2%
 GG (n = 57, 25.7%) 45 4 8
  Within GG 79.0% 7.0% 14.0%
  Within class 25.7% 25.0% 25.8%
rs279827
 A-carriers (n = 148, 75.9%) 108 14 26
  Within A-carriers 73.0% 9.4% 17.6%
  Within class 75.5% 82.4% 74.3%
 GG (n = 47, 24.1%) 35 3 9
  Within GG 74.5% 6.4% 19.1%
  Within class 24.5% 17.6% 25.7%
rs279858
 A-carriers (n = 173, 80.1%) 133 15 25
  Within A-carriers 76.9% 8.7% 14.4%
  Within class 78.7% 100.0% 78.1%
 GG (n = 43, 19.9%) 36 0 7
  Within GG 83.7% 0.0% 16.3%
  Within class 21.3% 0.0% 21.9%

Parental monitoring and GABRA2

As expected, multinomial logistic regressions estimated using conditional GMM2 supported a main effect of parental monitoring. The odds of being in the decreasing versus the low class almost double when adolescents reported lower parental monitoring (odds ratio [OR] = 2.10, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.00–4.45). These effects were marginal when comparing the high to the low-externalizing class (OR = 1.96, 95% CI = 0.93–4.15). The odds of being in the high versus the low class almost double if adolescents came from a high AUD risk family (OR = 1.71, 95%CI= 1.00–2.93). Biological sex and GABRA2 were not significant predictors.

The GABRA2 × Parental Monitoring interaction was significant, providing evidence for moderation when comparing the decreasing (OR = 4.28, 95% CI = 1.24–14.76) and low classes (OR = 5.73, 95% CI = 1.17–38.47) to the high class. Figure 2 presents class trajectory membership as a function of GABRA2 by levels of parental monitoring. For illustrative purposes, a median split of parental monitoring was created, although it was modeled as a continuous variable. As depicted in the figure, among those reporting high parental monitoring, those with the GG genotype had (a) a greater proportion in the decreasing class, (b) a lesser proportion in the high class, and (c) an equal proportion in the low class compared to A-carriers. At low monitoring, those with the GG genotype had (a) a greater proportion in the high class and (b) a lesser proportion in both the decreasing and low classes compared to A-carriers. These results indicate that those with the GG genotype showed heightened susceptibility to parental monitoring.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Externalizing trajectory class by GABRA2 genotype (rs279827) and parental monitoring.

Figure 3 offers another depiction of the GABRA2 × Parental Monitoring interaction when comparing the decreasing and the high class at different values of parental monitoring (1 SD above and below the mean). When comparing the decreasing and high classes (Fig. 3a) at low levels of parental monitoring, those with the GG genotype had a lower probability of being in the decreasing compared to the high-externalizing class. At high levels of parental monitoring, they had a higher probability of being in the decreasing compared to the high-externalizing class. The simple slope of parental monitoring was statistically significant (1.11, p < .05) for the GG genotype but not significant for A-carriers (−0.35, p = .31). When comparing the low and high classes (Fig. 3b), at low levels of parental monitoring those with the GG genotype had a lower probability of being in the low compared to the high-externalizing class. At high levels of parental monitoring they had a higher probability of being in the low compared to the high-externalizing class. The simple slope of parental monitoring was statistically significant (2.12, p < .05) for the GG genotype but not significant for A-carriers (−0.37, p = .22). This indicates that those with the GG genotype may have heightened susceptibility to parental monitoring in a “for-better-and-for-worse” manner (Belsky & Pluess, 2009). In adverse environments (low monitoring) those with the GG genotype were more likely to belong to high-risk externalizing trajectories and less likely to belong in low-risk trajectories; in adaptive environments (high monitoring) the opposite was true.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The GABRA2 genotype (SNP rs279827) and predicted probabilities of externalizing trajectories by levels of parental monitoring. *p < .05.

rGE

Correlation analyses demonstrated no significant bivariate relationships (rs = −.09 to −.05 across SNPs) between adolescent and parent genotypes and parental monitoring. An absence of rGE effects indicates that identified G × E interactions do not simply reflect an evocative effect of GABRA2 genes on parenting (Belsky & Beaver, 2011).

Sensitivity analyses

As expected, the standard errors increase as the dependence in data increases; the standard error for zero dependence is almost identical to the standard error found in our analysis (e.g., OR = 4.3, 95% CI = 1.2–14.8, corresponding to SE = 0.62 for the interaction term). That is, this odds ratio will remain significantly different from 1 as long as the standard error remains below 0.73. We thus can have an excess sib/sib concordance as high as 0.9. This indicates that within-family dependence had a minimal impact on results.

Discussion

Externalizing behavior is a significant component of childhood maladjustment. Some degree of externalizing behavior is expected and not always predictive of later psychopathology (Moffitt, 1993). However, some adolescents continue engaging in externalizing behavior as they move into adulthood, and at that point what was dismissed or forgiven earlier becomes behavior with significant negative consequences. Therefore, developmental models distinguishing between normative and persistent problem behavior have significant utility. Parental monitoring and genetic make-up are likely to impact developmental patterns of externalizing behavior. This study systematically tests the form of GABRA2 by parental monitoring interactions on externalizing trajectories given preliminary evidence for differential susceptibility in this genetic system (Brody et al., 2013; Simons & Lei, 2013). Moreover, this study clarifies the association between these relationships given mixed findings in the GABRA2 literature on problem behavior. There was evidence for a significant GABRA2 × Parental Monitoring effect: Adolescents with the minor allele are greatly susceptible to both adverse and adaptive parenting consistent with the differential susceptibility hypothesis.

In line with the problem behavior literature (Lacourse et al., 2003; Odgers et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2012), three empirically differentiated externalizing classes were estimated. We identified a low-externalizing class that is consistent with a persistently low (e.g., Lacourse et al., 2003; Odgers et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2012) or a normative experimentation class (Fairchild et al., 2013). We also identified a group that demonstrated moderate levels of initial externalizing behavior that increased in severity across middle to late adolescence. This most closely maps on to a persistently high (e.g., Shaw et al., 2012) or an adolescence-onset persistent class (Fairchild et al., 2013; Lacourse et al., 2003; Odgers et al., 2008). Finally, we identified a decreasing class that demonstrated elevated levels of externalizing behavior peaking at or before early adolescence and gradually decreasing to low levels of externalizing behavior in late adolescence. This is consistent with a high-decreasing class (Shaw et al., 2012), or a childhood-limited class (Fairchild et al., 2013; Lacourse et al., 2003; Odgers et al., 2008). Given that some degree of externalizing behavior is normative in adolescence, identifying factors that discriminate between adolescents who will mature out of these behaviors and successfully transition to healthy adulthood and those experiencing sustained problems leading to psychopathology are critical.

Consistent with previous research, there was evidence for a main effect of parental monitoring on externalizing trajectories (Barnes et al., 2006; Laird et al., 2003; Shaw et al., 2012). That is, parents who are informed about key aspects of their child's behavior and environment in real-time have adolescents who are less likely to exhibit high-risk externalizing trajectories. Moreover, our findings indicate that adolescents' genotype did not have a main effect on externalizing trajectories; rather, GABRA2 moderated the effects of parental monitoring on externalizing trajectories, uniquely differentiating the lower risk classes (low and decreasing) and the high-risk class in a cross-over interaction pattern. Typically, main effects of genotype are not detected in cross-over patterns, perhaps due to vulnerability and protective effects within the same genotype balancing each other out (Uher & McGuffin, 2008). This highlights the critical need to move beyond bivariate associations and consider the role of both context and genetic information to gain a more precise understanding in the development of youth problem behavior.

In our study, A-carriers were largely unaffected by parental monitoring, whereas at high parental monitoring GG genotype adolescents were more likely to belong to lower risk externalizing classes but less likely to belong to lower risk externalizing classes at low parental monitoring. Although previous work demonstrated that adolescents with the major allele were more likely to develop high-risk externalizing trajectories, especially in the context of low parental monitoring (Dick et al., 2009), our findings are consistent with the larger literature on GABRA2, which frames the minor allele as being a potential susceptibility factor (Agrawal et al., 2006; Bauer et al., 2007; Covault et al., 2004; Edenberg et al., 2004; Villafuerte et al., 2012). Differences across studies may be attributable in part to our extension of prior work to include quadratic effects as well as assessing trajectories and conditional effects in the same model. Including a quadratic growth factor likely provided a more nuanced conceptualization of externalizing trajectories not captured by previous work. It is possible that prior work (Dick et al., 2009) collapsed across two putatively distinct groups (low and adolescent-limited classes), perhaps because of lower variance in externalizing behavior in a community sample compared to our sample enriched for alcoholism. Factors discriminating between adolescents who will successfully transition to adulthood and those who will experience sustained problems leading to psychopathology is critical.

When examining the form of the interaction (i.e., whether it is consistent with the diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility) we employed a series of proposed empirical steps for establishing genetic susceptibility to environmental influence (Belsky et al., 2007). There is evidence for (a) a crossover interaction between GABRA2 and parental monitoring, (b) independence of genotype and parental monitoring, (c) no association between genotype and externalizing trajectories, and (d) a significant slope for the susceptibility group (i.e., GG genotype) compared to a nonsignificant slope for the nonsusceptible group (A-carriers). Unfortunately, we were unable to examine regions of significance for these slopes (Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006) because this approach is currently unavailable for multinomial outcomes. Nevertheless, findings are consistent with other studies demonstrating that, in some contexts, genetic effects may convey greater susceptibility rather than risk (Kochanska et al., 2011). Moreover, findings are consistent with preliminary studies suggesting that GABRA2 variants may reflect susceptibility rather than risk (Brody et al., 2013; Simons & Lei, 2013).

Limitations and future directions

While this study provided an important advancement to the literature on the interaction between genes and parenting, there are a number of limitations. First, the heterotypic nature of externalizing behavior poses methodological challenges when examining developmental change (Leve, Kim, & Pears, 2005). One must weigh the pros and cons of assessing behaviors across a broad age range while keeping measurement constant. When multiple measures are used, it is difficult to discern whether change reflects differences in methodology. In an effort to assess true change, we chose to include only one measure (the YSR), limiting our findings to adolescence and one reporter. Although previous work (Dick et al., 2009) examined a broader age range, it is unclear whether different assessments added measurement variance to trajectories. In addition, although adolescent report of parenting practices and externalizing behavior may be more direct and accurate (Stanger & Lewis, 1993), this may have inflated shared-method variance. Future research encompassing other informants as well as a broader range of ages is necessary.

Though our sample size is comparable to other studies (Dick et al., 2009; Kochanska et al., 2011), a larger sample would allow for an examination of each genotype. Given the nature of large longitudinal datasets, data collected through the MLS goes through an extensive quality assurance process including screening and crosschecking. For this reason, some of the collected data was not yet available for this study. Although our rate of adolescents characterized by the high-externalizing class (13.5%) was comparable to rates reported in previous work (rates = 3%–16%; Walters, 2011), the use of growth mixture modeling led to several small cell sizes when examining conditional effects. Until findings are replicated with a larger sample, caution is warranted when drawing inferences.

It was also necessary to make an analytical tradeoff between conducting growth mixture modeling and estimating regions of significance, since testing regions of significance was not possible given the multinomial structure of the outcome variable. At the same time, the ability to examine heterogeneous trajectories of externalizing behavior consistent with developmental theory is a significant strength that outweighs the potential limitation of not being able to test regions of significance. This is especially true given other statistical procedures (e.g., probing simple slopes; Aiken & West, 1991). However, it will be important for future work to examine regions of significance and test other confirmatory models, such as reparameterized regression models (Widaman et al., 2012), in order to further substantiate the form of this interaction.

Our findings may not generalize to samples with different demographics. Our sample was largely White, and parenting practices may operate differently across race (Smith & Krohn, 1995). Our sample was also enriched with alcoholic fathers, potentially limiting its generalizability to nonproblem populations. However, the T scores were similar to community and nonreferred samples (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001; Dick et al., 2009).

Despite these limitations, the current study demonstrates that those with the GABRA2 minor allele (GG) are most susceptible to parental monitoring, which is consistent with the larger GABRA2 literature (Bauer et al., 2007; Covault et al., 2004; Edenberg et al., 2004; Enoch et al., 2006; Pierucci-Lagha et al., 2005). This is the first study to systematically examine the role of GABRA2 variants as susceptibility factors. Sole focus on diathesis–stress models likely increase the risk of misunderstanding the true nature of adolescent growth and the role that genes play in shaping behavior. Traditional conceptualizations of GABRA2 variants as risk factors may be misguided, as individuals with the GG genotype may also be more susceptible to adaptive environments. That is, adolescents traditionally categorized as at risk for later psychopathology may also be particularly sensitive to the degree to which parents are invested in creating a nurturing environment fostering trust, openness, and communication.

Acknowledgments

We thank the families participating in the Michigan Longitudinal Study and Dr. Kerby Shedden from the Center for Statistical Consultation and Research at the University of Michigan for his statistical support. This research was supported by Grants R01 DA027261 (R.A.Z., M.M.H., J.K.Z.), R01 AA007065 (R.A.Z., M.M.H.), and T32 AA007477 (R.A.Z., K.B.). Funding was also received from a pilot grant from the University of Michigan MICHR CTSA program supported in part by the National Center for Research Resources (Grant UL1RR024986) and is now at the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (Grant 2UL1TR000433).

Footnotes

1

Different combinations of freely estimating versus constraining the variance of the three growth factors led to similar trajectory patterns, and the results were largely consistent.

2

Given a largely White male sample, analyses were also conducted on White participants only and males only. The findings were comparable and frequency genotypes did not differ by race, χ2 (1) = 0.06, p = .81. Accordingly, the reported results are for the full sample.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Youth Self-Report for ages 11-18. Burlington, VT: ASEBA; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  2. Agrawal A, Edenberg H, Foroud T, Bierut LJ, Dunne G, Hinrichs AL, et al. Association of GABRA2 with drug dependence in the collaborative study of the genetics of alcoholism sample. Behavior Genetics. 2006;36:640–650. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9069-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE; 1991. p. 212. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barnes GM, Hoffman JH, Welte JW, Farrell MP, Dintcheff BA. Effects of parental monitoring and peer deviance on substance use and delinquency. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2006;68:1084–1104. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00315.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bauer LO, Covault J, Harel O, Das S, Gelernter J, Anton R, et al. Variation in GABRA2 predicts drinking behavior in Project MATCH subjects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2007;31:1780–1787. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00517.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:300–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Belsky J, Beaver KM. Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting and adolescent self-regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:619–626. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02327.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brody GH, Chen Y, Beach SR. Differential susceptibility to prevention: GABAergic, dopaminergic, and multilocus effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2013;54:863–871. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris P. The ecology of developmental processes. In: Lerner RM, editor. Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development. 5th. Vol. 1. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 993–1028. [Google Scholar]
  11. Chilcoat HD, Anthony JC. Impact of parent monitoring on initiation of drug use through late childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:91–100. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199601000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chung IJ, Hill KG, Hawkins J, Gilchrist LD, Nagin DS. Childhood predictors of offense trajectories. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2002;39:60–90. doi: 10.1177/002242780203900103. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Covault J, Gelernter J, Hesselbrock V, Nellissery M, Kranzler HR. Allelic and haplotypic association of GABRA2 with alcohol dependence. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2004;129B:104–109. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dick DM, Bierut L, Hinrichs A, Fox L, Bucholz KK, Kramer J, et al. The role of GABRA2 in risk for conduct disorder and alcohol and drug dependence across developmental stages. Behavior Genetics. 2006;36:577–590. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-9041-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dick DM, Latendresse SJ, Lansford JE, Budde JP, Goate A, Dodge KA, et al. Role of GABRA2 in trajectories of externalizing behavior across development and evidence of moderation by parental monitoring. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:649–657. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dick DM, Latendresse SJ, Lansford JE, Budde JP, Goate A, Dodge KA, et al. Error in table and results in: Role of GA-BRA2 in trajectories of externalizing behavior across development and evidence of moderation by parental monitoring. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011;68:980. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. DiClemente RJ, Wingood GM, Crosby R, Sionen C, Cobb BK, Harrington K, et al. Parental monitoring: Association with adolescents' risk behaviors. Pediatrics. 2001;1076:1363–1368. 1363. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dishion TJ, McMahon RJ. Parental monitoring and the prevention of child and adolescent problem behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 1998;1:61–75. doi: 10.1023/a:1021800432380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Edenberg HJ, Dick DM, Xuei X, Tian H, Almasy L, Bauer LO, et al. Variations in GABRA2, encoding the α2 subunit of the GABAA receptor, are associated with alcohol dependence and with brain oscillations. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2004;74:705–714. doi: 10.1086/383283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Enoch MA, Schwartz L, Albaugh B, Virkkunen M, Goldman D. Dimensional anxiety mediates linkage of GABRA2 haplotypes with alcoholism. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2006;141B:599–607. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fairchild G, van Goozen SHM, Calder AJ, Goodyer IM. Research review: Evaluating and reformulating the developmental taxonomic theory of antisocial behaviour. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2013;54:924–940. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hodgkinson CA, Yuan Q, Xu K, Shen PH, Heinz E, Lobos EA, et al. Addictions biology: Haplotype-based analysis for 130 candidate genes on a single array. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2008;43:505–515. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. International HapMap Consortium. The International HapMap Project. Nature. 2003;426:789–796. doi: 10.1038/nature02168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ioannidis JPA, Trikalinos TA, Khoury MJ. Implications of small effect sizes of individual genetic variants on the design and interpretation of genetic association studies of complex diseases. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2006;164:609–614. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jung T, Wickrama K. An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2008;2:302–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00054.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kendler KS. A conceptual overview of gene–environment interaction and correlation in a developmental context. In: Kendler KS, Jaffee S, Romer D, editors. The Dynamic Genome and Mental Health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kerr M, Stattin H. What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:366–380. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.3.366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kerr M, Stattin H. Straw men, untested assumptions, and bi-directional models: A response to Capaldi and Brody. In: Crouter AC, Booth A, editors. Children's influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 181–188. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kerr M, Stattin H, Burk WJ. A reinterpretation of parental monitoring in longitudinal perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2010;20:39–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00623.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kochanska G, Kim S, Barry RA, Philibert RA. Children's genotypes interact with maternal responsive care in predicting children's competence: Diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility? Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:605–616. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kuppens S, Grietens H, Onghena P, Michiels D. Associations between parental control and children's overt and relational aggression. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2009;27:607–623. doi: 10.1348/026151008X345591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lacourse E, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, Vitaro F, Claes M. Developmental trajectories of boys' delinquent group membership and facilitation of violent behaviors during adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;14:909–924. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Laird RD, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA. Parents' monitoring-relevant knowledge and adolescent's delinquent behavior: Evidence of correlated developmental changes and reciprocal influences. Child Development. 2003;74:752–768. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Leve LD, Kim HK, Pears KC. Childhood temperament and family environment as predictors of internalizing and externalzing trajectories from age 5 to age 17. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33:505–520. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-6734-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100:674–701. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Muthén B. Latent variable analysis: Growth mixture modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. In: Kaplan D, editor. The Sage handbook of quantitative methodology for the social sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2004. pp. 345–368. [Google Scholar]
  37. Muthén B, Muthén LK. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24:882–891. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. MPlus user's guide. 7th. Los Angeles: Author; 1998–2012. [Google Scholar]
  39. Nigg JT. Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:395–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling. 2007;14:535–569. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, et al. Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:673–716. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pierucci-Lagha A, Covault J, Feinn R, Nellissery M, Hernandez-Avila C, Oncken C, et al. GABRA2 alleles moderate the subjective effects of alcohol, which are attenuated by finasteride. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1193–1203. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Preacher KJ, Curran PJ, Bauer DJ. Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 2006;31:437–448. doi: 10.3102/10769986031004437. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang KY, Eaves L, Hoh J, et al. Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301:2462–2471. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.878. doi:10/1001/jama.2009.878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2006;47:226–261. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schwarzer C, Berresheim U, Pirker S, Wieselthaler A, Fuchs K, Sieghart W, et al. Distribution of the major gamma-aminobutyric acid(A) receptor subunits in the basal ganglia and associated limbic brain areas of the adult rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2001;433:526–549. doi: 10.1002/cne.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Shaw DS, Hyde LW, Brennan LM. Early predictors of boys' antisocial trajectories. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24:871–888. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Simons RL, Lei MK. Enhanced susceptibility to context: A promising perspective on the interplay of genes and the social environment. In: Gibson CL, Krohn MD, editors. Handbook of life-course criminology: Emerging trends and directions for future research. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 57–67. [Google Scholar]
  49. Smith C, Krohn MD. Delinquency and family life among male adolescents: The role of ethnicity. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1995;24:69–93. doi: 10.1007/BF01537561. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Stanger C, Lewis M. Agreement among parents, teachers, and children on internalizing and externalizing behavior problems. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1993;22:107–115. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2201_11. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Stattin H, Kerr M. Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development. 2000;71:1072–1085. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tian H, Chen HJ, Cross TH, Edenberg H. Alternative splicing and promoter use in the human GABRA2 gene. Molecular Brain Research. 2005;137:174–183. doi: 10.1016/j.molbrainres.2005.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Uher R, McGuffin P. The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: Review and methodological analysis. Molecular Psychiatry. 2008;13:131–146. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Villafuerte S, Heitzeg MM, Foley S, Yau W, Majczenko K, Zubieta JK, et al. Impulsiveness and insula activation during reward anticipation are associated with genetic variants in GABRA2 in a family sample enriched for alcoholism. Molecular Psychiatry. 2012;17:511–519. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Villafuerte S, Strumba V, Stoltenberg S, Zucker RA, Burmeister M. Impulsiveness mediates the association between GABRA2 SNPs and lifetime alcohol problems. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2013;12:525–531. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Walters GD. The latent structure of life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: Is Moffitt's developmental taxonomy a true taxonomy? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2011;79:96–105. doi: 10.1037/a0021519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Weisner M, Windle M. Assessing covariates of adolescent delinquency trajectories: A latent growth mixture modeling approach. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2004;33:431–442. doi: 10.1023/B:JOYO.0000037635.0693713. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Widaman KF, Helm JL, Castro-Schilo L, Pluess M, Stallings MC, Belsky J. Distinguishing ordinal and disordinal interactions. Psychological Methods. 2012;17:615–622. doi: 10.1037/a0030003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Zucker RA, Ellis DA, Fitzgerald HE, Bingham CR, Sanford K. Other evidence for at least two alcoholisms II: Life course variation in antisociality and heterogeneity of alcoholic outcome. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:831–848. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007458. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Zucker RA, Fitzgerald HE, Refior SK, Puttler LI, Pallas DM, Ellis DA. The clinical and social ecology of childhood for children of alcoholics: Description of a study and implications for a differentiated social policy. In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Zuckerman BS, editors. Children of addiction: Research, health and policy issues. New York: Routledge Falmer; 2000. pp. 109–141. [Google Scholar]
  61. Zuckerman M. Vulnerability to psychopathology: A biosocial model. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES