Abstract
Background
Stress and conditioned drug cues have been implicated in the initiation, maintenance and relapse to substances of abuse. Although stress and drug cues are often encountered together, little research exists on whether stress potentiates the response to drug cues.
Method
Participants (N = 75) were 39 community recruited individuals with current prescription opioid (PO) dependence and 36 healthy controls. Participants stayed overnight in the hospital for one night and then completed laboratory testing the following morning. During laboratory testing, participants were randomly assigned to a stress task (Trier Social Stress Task; TSST) or a no-stress condition. Following the stress manipulation, all participants completed a PO cue paradigm. Immediately before and after the stress and cue tasks, the following were assessed: subjective (stress, craving, anger, sadness, happiness), physiological (heart rate, blood pressure, galvanic skin response), and neuroendocrine responses (cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone).
Results
Internal validity of the stress task was demonstrated, as evidenced by significantly higher subjective stress, as well as cortisol, heart rate and blood pressure in the TSST compared to the no-stress group. Individuals with PO dependence evidenced significantly greater reactivity to the stress task than controls. Craving increased significantly in response to the drug cue task among PO participants. No stress × cue interaction was observed.
Conclusions
In this study, heightened stress reactivity was observed among individuals with PO dependence. Exposure to acute stress, however, did not potentiate craving in response to conditioned drug cues.
Keywords: stress, Trier, cues, prescription opioids, opiates
1. INTRODUCTION
Stress and conditioned drug cues are key factors associated with the etiology and maintenance of substance use disorders (SUD; Brady and Sinha, 2005; Enoch, 2011; Hyman and Sinha 2009; Bruchas et al., 2010; Koob, 2009; Sinha, 2001; 2007; Stewart, 2003). Several models of addiction have attempted to explain the connection between stress and motivation to use alcohol or drugs (Breese et al., 2011; Sinha, 2001). These models relate that, for many individuals with SUD, substances are employed as a means of reducing negative affect; a pattern that is negatively reinforcing and may contribute to the subsequent development of SUD (Khantzian, 1985; Shiffman, 1982; Wills and Shiffman, 1985). However, not all substance use or relapses are precipitated by stress or negative affect. For example, Shiftman (1982) found that approximately one-third of relapses to nicotine were precipitated by smoking-specific stimuli (i.e., conditioned cues), in particular seeing someone else smoking.
The concept of craving also has a central role in theories concerning the development and maintenance of SUD (Breese et al., 2005; Childress et al., 1988; Drummond et al., 1990; Franken et al., 2000; Tiffany, 1990). One particularly salient feature that occurs during abstinence from drug use is the ability of drug-associated environmental cues to elicit craving, and consequently reinstate drug-seeking and drug-taking behaviors. The systematic investigation of craving has occurred largely through studies employing cue reactivity (Carter and Tiffany, 1999), which have given rise to a variety of theoretical models (Kosten et al., 2006; O'Brien et al., 1998; See, 2002; Siegel, 1999; Siegel and Ramos, 2002). Findings suggest that, through a process of associative learning, previously neutral stimuli (e.g., pill bottle, pharmacy) acquire incentive-motivational properties following repeated pairing with drug consumption. Conditioned stimuli thus play a critical role in ongoing drug-seeking behavior and relapse after periods of abstinence (Buffalari et al., 2014; Childress et al., 1988; O'Brien et al., 1998).
Stress and cues are frequently encountered together by patients, and the presence of stress may influence or change the rewarding value of a subsequently encountered conditioned stimuli (Bruchas et al., 2010). For example, if a person has an altercation with a coworker (stress) and then sees a billboard advertising their preferred alcoholic beverage (cue) on the commute home that day, does the stress experienced prior to seeing the cue modify the impact that the cue has on the person’s craving or drug seeking behaviors? If the person had not experienced a stressful situation that day, would he/she have noticed the billboard, and if so would the effect of the billboard have been as strong? The primary question of interest in the current study is whether exposure to a stressor potentiates reactivity (e.g., craving, physiological arousal) to a conditioned drug cue.
Given the notable contribution of stress and conditioned cues on addictive behaviors, an increasingly robust literature has investigated their ability to prompt drug-seeking behavior in preclinical and human laboratory studies. A number of different types of clinical laboratory-based stress induction tasks have been employed to study the influence of stress and cues on addictive responses. Sinha and colleagues were the first to demonstrate in a controlled human laboratory setting that acute stress increased craving in cocaine-dependent individuals (Sinha et al., 1999). Our group and others have also demonstrated that physical stressors (e.g., cold pressor task), psychological stressors (e.g., Trier Social Stress Task [TSST]; Kirschbaum et al., 1993) and pharmacological stressors (e.g., corticotropin releasing hormone) increase stress and craving among individuals with nicotine, cocaine, alcohol, or marijuana dependence (Back et al., 2005, 2010; Buchmann et al., 2010; Brady et al., 2006; Childs and de Wit, 2010).
Several clinical and preclinical studies have investigated the interactive effects of stress and cues, and the data are equivocal. Thomas and colleagues (2011) demonstrated that, among individuals with alcohol dependence, the TSST resulted in significantly increased subjective stress, as well as cortisol, ACTH, and blood pressure as compared to controls, and the alcohol cue paradigm resulted in significant craving. However, no interaction between the TSST and alcohol cues was revealed. McRae-Clark and colleagues (2011) demonstrated similar findings in that prior exposure to a laboratory-based stress task did not enhance craving response to drug cues in individuals with marijuana dependence. In contrast to these clinical studies, Liu and Weiss (2002) found that footshock stress and ethanol conditioned drug cues interacted to augment the resumption of ethanol seeking behavior following extinction in rats. Rats exposed to both footshock stress and ethanol cues, as compared to stress or cues alone, demonstrated twice as many lever presses and the response rate was sustained for a longer period of time. In addition, Buffalari and colleagues (2009) showed that while footshock stress and conditioned drug cues reinstate drug-seeking when presented in isolation, their interaction resulted in potentiated reinstatement.
The current study is focused on prescription opioid (PO) use disorders, which have been steadily increasing over the past decade and represent a significant public health concern (Back et al., 2010; Calcaterra et al., 2013; Garland et al., 2013; Hall et al., 2008; Kuehn, 2007; McHugh et al., 2015). To date, there have been no studies of the interaction of stress and cues in PO dependent individuals. In contrast to other types of SUD, individuals with PO use disorders may have different initiation histories (e.g., prescribed the drug by their physician, initially consumed by some for legitimate physical health reasons) and may present with different comorbidities, such as chronic pain, that could exert influences on stress reactivity. Using a human laboratory stress induction task (Back et al., 2014), the current study examined stress reactivity among individuals with and without PO dependence, as well as the interaction of stress and conditioned PO drug cues. Human laboratory paradigms offer a high degree of methodological precision and control, and are a reliable method of investigating the complexities of stress (Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010) and conditioned drug cues. We hypothesized that the PO group would demonstrate increased reactivity to the stress task, and that exposure to stress would potentiate craving in response to the drug cue task.
2. METHODS
2.1 Participants
Participants (N = 75) were individuals with current (past 6 months) PO dependence (n = 39; 57.9% female) or healthy controls (n = 36; 50.0% female). PO dependence was defined as meeting the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2002) criteria for substance dependence on opioid analgesics (e.g., oxycodone, hydrocodone). Newspaper and other media advertisements were the primary source of recruitment. Potential participants were initially screened by telephone using a brief form that was created for the purposes of this study and screened for current PO use and symptoms of SUD. Individuals meeting preliminary eligibility criteria came into the office for a clinical assessment and a history and physical examination. Exclusion criteria included: pregnancy or nursing; BMI ≥ 39; major medical problems (e.g., diabetes, HIV, Addison’s or Cushing’s disease) or comorbid psychiatric conditions that could affect the HPA axis (e.g., bipolar disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder); use of methadone in the past three months; use of antihypertensive medications, beta-blockers, synthetic glucocorticoid therapy, or treatment with other agents that may interfere with stress response in the past month. Individuals who met criteria for abuse of other substances had to identify POs as their primary drug of choice. Controls were excluded if they met DSM-IV criteria for current or history of substance dependence (except caffeine or nicotine); history of abuse was allowed. Participants were compensated $150 for completing the study.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1 Substance Use
The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; First et al., 2002) and the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al., 1998) were used to assess substance use disorders and other Axis I psychiatric disorders. Urine drug screens tests were performed using the On Track Test Cup®. Breathalyzer tests were administered to test for the presence of alcohol. Opioid withdrawal symptoms were assessed at the time of hospital admission using the Short Opioid Withdrawal Scale (Gossop, 1990). The Timeline Follow Back (TLFB; Sobell Sobell and Sobell, 1992) is a calendar-based assessment that was used to measure PO use during the one month prior to the laboratory test.
2.2.2 Subjective Reactivity
A visual analog scale derived from the Within Session Rating Scale (Childress et al., 1986) and anchored with adjective modifiers (from 0 = “not at all” to 10 = “extremely”) was used to assess subjective responses: craving, stress, anger, happiness, sadness, how hard it would be to resist using their opioid of choice, and the amount of money participants would be willing to spend on opioids (i.e., the “market value”). Participants responded to questions immediately before and after the TSST and at several time points after the drug cue paradigm (i.e., immediately, 15-, 30-, and 60-minutes post). The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, Form Y1; Spielberger, 1983) was completed immediately before and after the TSST, and then immediately, 15-, 30- and 60- minutes after the drug cue paradigm.
2.2.3 Neuroendocrine Assay
Unstimulated salivary samples were collected at baseline, immediately after the TSST, immediately after the drug cue paradigm, and at 15-, 30-, and 60-minutes post. Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) was assayed in duplicate using a salivary DHEA enzyme immunoassay system that has an intra-assay precision of 5.6% with a sensitivity of 5pg/mL. For cortisol, samples were assayed in duplicate using a high sensitivity salivary cortisol enzyme immunoassay system that has an intra-assay precision of 3.35% - 3.65% with a sensitivity of <0.003 ug/dL. Both DHEA and cortisol were analyzed using a PowerWave HT Microplate Spectrophotometer in conjunction with a Precision Series Automated Liquid Handling System.
2.2.4 Physiological Reactivity
Heart rate (HR) was collected via electrodes along the bottom of the participant’s ribcage and collarbone. Systolic (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) were measured using a GE Pro 400 Dinamap automated monitor. Mean arterial pressure (MAP) was calculated using the formula [(2 × DBP)+SBP/3]. Two measurements of HR, SBP and DBP were taken 15 minutes apart at baseline before testing began; the average of these two measurements was used as the baseline value. Physiological measures were then taken immediately after the TSST, immediately after the drug cue paradigm, and at 15-, 30-, and 60-minutes post drug cue paradigm.
2.3. Laboratory Procedures
All procedures were approved by the local institutional review board. Participants were informed about all study procedures, including the laboratory stress provocation. Written informed consent was obtained before any study procedures occurred. Eligible participants (both PO and controls) were scheduled for a one-night hospital stay and testing was completed. Three days of abstinence from alcohol and other substances (except caffeine and nicotine), as evidenced by self-report, breathalyzer and urine drug screen, as well as the absence of significant withdrawal symptoms were required prior to hospital admission for the overnight stay. Participants were admitted to the hospital at 2000h on the evening prior to testing to allow for the control of extraneous variables (e.g., sleep, caffeine intake) that could potentially affect stress sensitivity. Cigarette smokers were provided 24-hour nicotine replacement therapy throughout the hospital stay (≥ 20 cigarettes/day = 21 mg patch; 10–19 cigarettes/day = 14 mg patch; 5–9 cigarettes/day = 7 mg patch).
A detailed description of the test day procedures is included in Table 1. Participants were provided a standard breakfast at 0730h and then escorted for laboratory testing. Sedentary activities were allowed during a 60- minute acclimation period from 0830–0930h. Pre-assessments were conducted at 0930h and 0945h, and testing began at 0950h. Participants were randomly assigned, using urn randomization (Wei and Lachin, 1988), to either the TSST or a no-stress condition. In the no-stress condition, participants sat quietly and relaxed. The TSST is a standardized, 15-minute stress provocation (Kirschbaum et al., 1993) which consists of three components: (a) participants are given 5 minutes to think about and prepare to give a speech, (b) participants deliver a 5-minute speech to 3 confederates, and then (c) participants verbally complete serial subtractions for 5 minutes before the same audience. Following the TSST or no-stress condition, all participants completed a 15-minute PO drug cue paradigm (Back et al., 2014). The drug cue paradigm consists of three components: (a) a 5-minute audio induction script, which guides participants to relax and then think about the last time they used POs in detail; (b) 5 minutes viewing and handling drug paraphernalia; and (c) 5-minute watching a video depicting people using POs in a variety of ways. Following the drug cue paradigm, participants were assessed for 60-minutes post task and then debriefed, compensated, and discharged.
Table 1.
Laboratory test day procedures
Time | Procedure |
---|---|
7:30–8:30 am | Standard breakfast and then escorted to laboratory |
8:30 am | Electrodes attached |
8:30–9:30 am | Acclimation period (sedentary activities) |
9:30–9:49 am | Baseline Assessments:
|
9:50–10:05 am | Trier Social Stress Task (TSST) or no-stress |
10:06–10:07 am | Salivary sample + assessments #3 |
10:07–10:22 am | Prescription opioid drug cue paradigm |
10:23–10:24 am | Salivary sample + assessment #4 (immediate) |
10:39–10:40 am | Salivary sample + assessments #5 (15 min) |
10:55–10:56 am | Salivary sample + assessments #6 (30 min) |
11:11–11:12 am | Salivary sample + assessments #7 (60 min) |
11:15 am | Debriefing, compensation, discharge |
2.4. Statistical Analyses
Reactivity to the stress condition (TSST or no-stress) was measured as the change from baseline rating of a domain to the maximum response (i.e., peak) during post-task assessment. This previously established method results in a change score in which higher scores indicate greater reactivity (Back et al., 2008; Carpenter et al., 2007; Daughters et al., 2009; McRae-Clark et al., 2011). Distributions of change scores were checked to ensure that they conformed to the assumptions of a normal distribution. Distributions that violated normality were transformed according to the recommended guidelines (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2010). Drug cue reactivity was measured as the peak rating after exposure to the drug cue task. Differences in stress reactivity were assessed with a series of 2 (stress condition: TSST or no-stress) × 2 (drug status group: PO or healthy controls) ANCOVAs. Time since last use was included as a covariate in this analysis based on prior literature indicating pre-study substance use is highly correlated with post-study substance use (Paliwal et al., 2008). Post-hoc comparisons were used to identify differences in the event of a significant interaction.
3. RESULTS
Table 2 includes the sample characteristics. As can be seen, the average age was 34 years old. Approximately half of participates were male and 81% were Caucasian. Significantly more individuals in the PO group, as compared to the control group, endorsed nicotine use, as well as a history of treatment for addiction and chronic pain. Among the PO group, the most commonly used opioids included: OxyContin/ oxycodone (33.4%), Percocet (25.6%) and Lortab (15.4%). The average age of first PO use was 22.3 years old and the average age of onset of PO dependence was 27.6 years old. In the past 30 days prior to study entry, individuals in the PO group reported using POs approximately 18 out of 30 days. The most common routes of administration were oral (66.7%) and nasal (i.e., crush and snort, 28.2%).
Table 2.
Sample characteristics (N=75)
Variable | Control Group (n = 36) |
PO Group (n = 39) |
---|---|---|
Age, M (SD) | 33.58 (12.41) | 34.59 (12.55) |
Gender, % Male | 50.0 | 51.3 |
Education, % Some College | 97.2 | 69.2 |
Employment | ||
% Employed Full Time | 83.3 | 23.1 |
% Unemployed | 8.3 | 59.0 |
Race | ||
% Caucasian | 80.6 | 82.1 |
% African-American | 11.1 | 5.1 |
% Other | 8.3 | 12.8 |
Relationship Status | ||
% Married | 41.7 | 10.3 |
% Single, Never Married | 44.4 | 59.0 |
% Divorced, Separated | 13.9 | 20.5 |
Smoker (nicotine), % Yes | 11.1 | 82.1* |
History of chronic pain treatment | 0.0 | 33.3* |
History of addiction treatment | 0.0 | 41.0* |
Opioid use in the past month | ||
Total # days using | 0.2 t | 18.1* |
Average # pills per day | 0.007t | 2.79* |
Average # pills per using day | 0.07 t | 4.16* |
Comorbid psychiatric conditions╫ | ||
Alcohol use disorder | 0.0 | 10.3 |
Sedative use disorder | 0.0 | 5.1 |
Marijuana use disorder | 0.0 | 7.7 |
Cocaine use disorder | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Major depression | 0.0 | 5.1 |
Panic disorder | 0.0 | 15.4* |
PTSD | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Bipolar disorder | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Pain disorder | 0.0 | 7.7 |
Note. PO = prescription opioid. M(SD) = Mean(Standard Deviation).
p<.05 between-group differences.
Current conditions.
Two control patients reported medical use of opioids during the month prior to study entry. Both subjects used the medication as prescribed for acute medical procedures, had not used the medication in the past 5 days prior to laboratory testing and had negative urine drug screen tests.
3.1 Stress Task
Table 3 presents the pre and post stress task ratings, as well as the post cue task ratings. The interaction between the stress condition (TSST or no stress) and drug status group (PO or healthy controls) was significant for subjective ratings of stress (F (1, 58) = 0.003, p = 0.046, partial eta2 = 0.07), craving (F (1, 58) = 9.03, p = 0.004, partial eta2 = 0.14), and anger (F (1, 58) = 6.56, p = 0.013, partial eta2 = 0.10; see Table 3). Post-hoc comparisons showed that subjective stress significantly increased for both PO participants (F (1, 28) = 28.07, p < 0.001, partial eta2 = 0.50) and control participants (F (1, 30) = 25.08, p < 0.001, partial eta2 = 0.46) following the TSST. However, the magnitude of the change was greater for PO participants as compared to controls (mean change 4.2 vs. 2.4; see Figure 1A). Post-hoc comparisons showed that craving significantly increased for PO participants (F (1, 28) = 8.45, p = 0.007, partial eta2 = 0.23) and, as expected, there was no change for control participants (F (1, 30) = 0.00, p = 0.99, partial eta2 = 0.00). For anger, post-hoc comparisons indicated that anger significantly increased for both PO participants (F (1, 28) = 17.75, p < 0.001, partial eta2 = 0.39) and control participants (F (1, 30) = 4.69, p = 0.038, partial eta2 = 0.14), but the magnitude of change was greater for the PO group as compared to controls (mean change 3.4 vs. 1.2; see Figure 1B). Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the amount of money participants were willing to spend on opioids significantly increased for PO participants (F (1, 28) = 5.68, p = 0.024, partial eta2 = 0.17) and, as expected, there was no change among control participants (F (1, 30) = 0.00, p = 0.99, partial eta2 = 0.00). There were no significant main effects or interactions for subjective ratings of resistance, sadness, or happiness.
Table 3.
Descriptive information for pre and post stress task and drug cue task
PO Group | Control group | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stress | No Stress | Stress | No Stress | ||||||
Pre Stress Task | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Stress | 3.76 | 1.99 | 3.37 | 2.63 | 0.37 | 0.76 | 0.71 | 1.36 | |
Crave | 4.82 | 2.79 | 4.95 | 2.68 | 0.16 | 0.69 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Resist using | 7.65 | 3.16 | 7.11 | 3.26 | 0.16 | 0.69 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Anger | 0.94 | 1.48 | 1.42 | 2.04 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.41 | 1.23 | |
Sad | 1.88 | 1.80 | 1.95 | 2.59 | 0.05 | 0.23 | 0.41 | 1.06 | |
Happy | 3.53 | 2.18 | 4.21 | 2.20 | 5.63 | 1.64 | 5.88 | 2.83 | |
Market Value | 27.53 | 18.86 | 29.89 | 31.93 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
HR | 69.92 | 10.51 | 71.26 | 9.64 | 68.77 | 11.34 | 62.99 | 10.03 | |
GSR | 3.99 | 3.12 | 5.66 | 5.60 | 2.76 | 2.55 | 3.35 | 3.16 | |
Cortisol | 0.26 | 0.12 | 0.26 | 0.14 | 0.21 | 0.10 | 0.23 | 0.15 | |
DHEA | 151.60 | 110.49 | 187.17 | 116.07 | 134.96 | 199.94 | 146.24 | 112.48 | |
Blood Pressure | 86.39 | 10.10 | 83.48 | 9.74 | 81.39 | 8.51 | 77.89 | 9.88 | |
Post Stress Task | |||||||||
Stress | 8.00 | 1.97 | 2.84 | 2.67 | 2.79 | 2.49 | 0.35 | 0.79 | |
Crave | 6.94 | 3.23 | 4.89 | 2.89 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Resist using | 8.06 | 3.01 | 7.37 | 3.20 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Anger | 4.35 | 3.39 | 1.05 | 1.81 | 1.16 | 2.63 | 0.06 | 0.24 | |
Sad | 2.94 | 3.07 | 1.26 | 1.91 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.35 | 0.86 | |
Happy | 1.88 | 2.21 | 4.26 | 2.81 | 5.68 | 1.92 | 6.12 | 2.76 | |
Market Value | 45.31 | 33.24 | 30.53 | 34.19 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
HR | 85.88 | 17.30 | 80.98 | 9.27 | 81.89 | 12.91 | 68.08 | 18.92 | |
GSR | 6.19 | 3.97 | 6.30 | 6.81 | 7.24 | 5.14 | 10.82 | 25.87 | |
Cortisol | 0.39 | 0.32 | 0.24 | 0.15 | 0.26 | 0.15 | 0.19 | 0.13 | |
DHEA | 242.59 | 163.07 | 218.26 | 113.47 | 140.88 | 205.61 | 147.17 | 170.01 | |
BP | 92.16 | 12.28 | 84.53 | 9.16 | 87.89 | 11.65 | 77.27 | 11.30 | |
Post Cue Task | |||||||||
Stress | 6.00 | 3.25 | 5.89 | 3.27 | 0.84 | 1.80 | 0.24 | 0.44 | |
Crave | 8.19 | 1.97 | 7.83 | 2.77 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Resist | 9.13 | 1.20 | 8.00 | 2.57 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Anger | 2.69 | 2.50 | 3.39 | 2.85 | 0.37 | 1.21 | 0.12 | 0.49 | |
Sad | 2.94 | 3.21 | 2.44 | 3.05 | 0.05 | 0.23 | 0.47 | 1.07 | |
Happy | 3.88 | 2.53 | 4.94 | 3.17 | 6.74 | 1.76 | 7.00 | 2.42 | |
Market Value | 66.81 | 78.94 | 57.44 | 91.58 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
HR | 87.92 | 15.87 | 87.10 | 8.22 | 85.14 | 11.82 | 75.62 | 12.12 | |
GSR | 8.95 | 6.83 | 9.84 | 6.71 | 7.14 | 4.94 | 7.77 | 5.49 | |
Cortisol | 0.32 | 0.17 | 0.34 | 0.19 | 0.26 | 0.14 | 0.19 | 0.13 | |
DHEA | 255.43 | 183.14 | 277.43 | 149.37 | 197.64 | 148.17 | 203.07 | 196.13 | |
BP | 96.37 | 13.56 | 91.61 | 8.27 | 84.97 | 9.21 | 82.88 | 12.38 |
Note. HR = heart rate. GSR = galvanic skin response. DHEA = dehydroepiandrosterone. BP = blood pressure.
Figure 1.
Subjective and neuroendocrine reactivity among individuals with or without prescription opioid (PO) dependence
There was a significant main effect of stress condition for HR (F (1, 58) = 6.50, p = 0.014, partial eta2 = 0.14), cortisol (F (1, 58) = 9.60, p = 0.003, partial eta2 = 0.14; see Figure 1C) and blood pressure (F (1, 58) = 9.39, p = 0.003, partial eta2 = 0.14). Across these effects, levels in the TSST condition were significantly higher than the no-stress condition. There were no significant main effects for drug group and there were no stress × drug cue interactions for HR, blood pressure, cortisol or DHEA.
3.2 Drug Cue Task
As previously reported (Back et al., 2014), exposure to the drug cue task alone resulted in significant increases from pre to post in subjective (craving, stress, anger, difficulty resisting opioids), physiological (heart rate), and cortisol levels among PO participants, but not controls. Physiological and neuroendocrine responses to the stress task were predictive of these same responses to the drug cue paradigm. Specifically, GSR (β = 0.66, p = 0.038) and cortisol (β = 0.49, p = 0.007) sensitivity to the stress condition predicted GSR and cortisol response to the drug cue paradigm. Similarly, HR response to the stress condition predicted HR response to the drug cue paradigm (β = 0.88, p = 0.014). When probed at different conditions of the stress condition, HR response to the TSST was positively associated with HR response to the drug cue paradigm (b = 1.04, p < 0.001); this was not true for response in the no-stress condition (b = 0.07, p = 0.80). No other significant main effects or interactions were observed (Table 5).
4. DISCUSSION
The present study examined stress reactivity as well as potential stress × drug cue interactions on subjective ratings, physiological symptoms, and neuroendocrine levels in participants with and without PO dependence. All participants reacted to the stress condition in terms of subjective and physiological indices compared to the no stress condition, thus supporting the internal validity of the stress task. However, participants with PO dependence demonstrated a greater increase in subjective ratings of stress, anger and the amount of money they were willing to spend on opioids compared to control participants. PO dependent individuals also demonstrated increased craving in response to the stress task. This is the first study to our knowledge to show increased craving in response to a laboratory stressor among individuals with PO dependence. Examination of a potential stress × cue interaction revealed that the stress task and drug cue task alone both increased subjective and physiological reactivity, but did not interact (i.e., the stress task did not elevate response to the cue). These findings are consistent with previous clinical studies in individuals with alcohol and marijuana use disorders (McRae-Clark et al., 2011; Thomas et al., 2011). Together, these studies demonstrate a consistent pattern of responses across different types of stress induction procedures, study teams, and substances in human trials.
The present study is the first to investigate the interaction of stress and PO drug cues, a unique type of SUD with regard to initiation history (e.g., typically prescribed by a physician) and comorbidities (e.g., chronic pain; Back et al., 2010). Contrary to hypothesis, the stressor task did not potentiate reactivity to the drug cue. One potential reason for the unexpected finding may be the stressor and cue paradigms employed. A robust stress response was observed in reaction to the TSST, and a robust craving response was seen as a reaction to the drug cue task; thus, a ceiling effect may have limited the ability to detect differences. Two prior investigations examining stress × cue interactions among humans also failed to observe an interaction utilizing the TSST. In the future, it may be useful to examine other types of stressors (e.g., the cold pressor test; McRae et al., 2006) or drug cues (e.g., Sinha et al., 2009), which may produce a greater range of physiological and subjective responses. Additionally, other types of stress tasks may be more relevant to individuals with PO disorders, such as tasks employing physical stress (e.g., quantitative sensory testing), and it may be helpful to explore more naturalistic methods of examining stress. Several prior studies, however, have investigated the effects of negative mood on cue reactivity in patients with alcohol use disorders, and no interaction between negative mood and subsequent cue reactivity were observed (Cooney et al.1997; Litt et al., 1990; Mason et al. 2008). This suggests that the negative results of the current and previous studies regarding stress × cue interactions are likely not solely due to the stress task employed.
Although further research is clearly needed, the extant literature suggests that while stress may lead to increase craving and drug-seeking behaviors it does not appear to do so by enhancing the incentive value of drug cues. A study by Ray and colleagues (2013) examined the effects of varenicline on nicotine craving and found differential effects. Subjects (N=40) were first exposed to guided imagery (stress or neutral) and then all subjects were exposed to in vivo cigarette cues. Results indicated that varenicline resulted in significantly reduced craving to the cues among subjects in the neutral imagery condition, but not the stress condition. The authors speculate that the absence of stress-specific reductions in craving following varenicline may be interpreted in light of the dissociation between neural mechanisms of stress- and cue-induced craving (Schank et al., 2011). Furthermore, Snelleman and colleagues (2014) conducted a review of studies (N=12) to examine the evidence supporting the relationship between stress and cue sensitivity. The findings were mixed and although an association between stress and cues exists, the exact nature of that association is unclear. Snelleman and colleagues noted that the few studies that did observe increased cue sensitivity following exposure to a stressor (vs. neutral task) were focused on patients with PTSD and/or individuals who were heavy drinkers with a primary motive of ‘drinking to cope.’ Finally, it was noted that, in general, stress states induced by acute laboratory tasks, such as the TSST, have no link or association with past substance use and therefore do not enhance reactivity to cues (Snelleman et al., 2014).
From a clinical perspective, the current findings suggest that PO dependent individuals may present for treatment with heightened stress reactivity, which may confer increased risk for relapse, although not necessarily in a direct fashion, and should be addressed in treatment. Among individuals with heroin dependence, recent studies demonstrate that a single maintenance dose of heroin reduces subjective anxiety and craving, and normalizes hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity; this highlights the emotion regulation effects of heroin among heroin-dependent individuals which is likely a contributing factor to maintaining drug-seeking behaviors (Walter et al., 2013). More recently, Schmidt and colleagues (2015) used a cross-over, double-blind design to examine neural responses to fearful faces among heroin dependent individuals and healthy controls, after heroin and placebo administration. The findings showed hyperactive amygdala-related connectivity during fearful faces among the heroin dependent individuals as compared to controls, which was normalized after heroin administration. These studies point to neurobiological mechanisms that may underline the stress regulation effect of heroin in dependent patients. In addition, empirical evidence demonstrates dysfunctional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex at rest among heroin users as compared to controls, and shows that the level of dysfunction at rest is associated with greater craving in response to heroin cues (Liu et al., 2011). Future research examining these mechanisms and associations among patients with PO dependence would be useful and may inform the development of treatment interventions.
Physicians prescribing PO medications should consider assessing stress level and coping resources, in addition to standard pain assessments, to determine the appropriateness of prescribing POs as compared to alternate medications with lower misuse potential. One potential alternative may be duloxetine, which is a dual selective serotonin and norepinephrine inhibitor that has been shown to address pain, as well as stress and depressed mood (Lunn et al., 2009; Thor et al., 2007). Other alternatives include tricyclic antidepressants (e.g., amitriptyline), anticonvulsants (e.g., gabapentin), topical or transdermal agents (e.g., lidocaine patch), and alternative medicines (Lee and Raja, 2011; Rowbotham, 2015). In addition to pharmacologic treatments, psychosocial interventions targeting improved stress management may be helpful for PO dependent patients endorsing high stress and/or poor stressing coping skills. For example, behavioral activation psychotherapy has been shown to be quick and effective for reducing stress and improving mood, is easily administered, and can be adapted to primary care settings (Gros and Haren, 2011). Psychoeducation regarding adapative ways to cope with both stress- and cue-induced cravings, including stimulus control and mindfulness techqniues, may be helpful (Garland et al., 2010). The systematic exposure to drug cues (e.g., exposure and response prevention) coupled with pharmacologic agents used to augment extinction learning may also play a role in the future treatment of patients with PO dependence (Kamboj et al., 2012; Marissen et al., 2007; Rohsenow et al., 1995). The findings from this study iindicate that additional research is needed to more clearly identify the influence of stress and cues in PO disorders.
Several limitations of the present study warrant consideration. The salience of the TSST and the drug cue paradigm may have limited our ability to differentiate highly sensitive participants from those with lower levels of sensitivity. Furthermore, the use of an acute stressor may have limited the findings – more chronic forms of stress may have yielded a different result. The order of tasks was not counterbalanced, as our aim was to examine the effects of stress on subsequent exposure to drug cues. This limited our ability to examine the effects of exposure to drug cues on subsequent stress. In addition, the study did not include a neutral-cue condition. This would have allowed us to investigate the effects of stress alone, the cue alone, and the interaction of stress and cues. Finally, the assessments of substance use relied heavily on participant self-report and blood levels of opioids were not obtained. Despite these limitations, the study utilized a well-controlled human laboratory paradigm, had participants stay overnight in the hospital prior to testing, and included subjective, physiological, and neuroendocrine measures.
In summary, the findings demonstrate that the psychosocial stress task employed elicited salient subjective, physiological and neuroendocrine responses from PO dependent participants, in particular. Exposure to the stressor was also associated with an increase in craving among PO dependent participants. However, exposure to the stressor did not potential response to the drug cue. Further preclinical and clinical investigations are needed to expand the current findings and increase understanding of the link between stress and cues in hopes of better addressing the growing epidemic of PO dependence.
Table 4.
ANCOVA examining stress-drug cue interaction
PO Group | Control Group | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stress | No Stress | Stress | No Stress | ||||||||
Adjusted Mean Difference |
SEM | Adjusted Mean Difference |
SEM | Adjusted Mean Difference |
SEM | Adjusted Mean Difference |
SEM | P for Drug Main Effect |
P for Stress Main Effect |
P for interaction |
|
Stress | 4.17a | 0.56 | 0.86b | 0.55 | 2.41c | 0.55 | 0.04b | 0.55 | 0.266 | < 0.001 | 0.046 |
Crave | 2.26a | 0.43 | −0.25b | 0.43 | 0.00b | 0.04 | 0.00b | 0.04 | 0.022 | 0.004 | 0.004 |
Resist | 0.28a | 0.28 | 0.0a | 0.27 | 0.06a | 0.27 | 0.06a | 0.27 | 0.93 | 0.35 | 0.35 |
Anger | 3.31a | 0.52 | −0.39b | 0.51 | 0.71c | 0.5 | −0.41b | 0.5 | 0.017 | < 0.001 | 0.013 |
Sad | 1.08a | 0.44 | −0.48a | −0.03a | 0.43 | −0.03a | 0.43 | 0.467 | 0.073 | 0.073 | |
Happy | −1.44a | 0.59 | 0.13a | 0.58 | 0.19a | 0.56 | 0.25a | 0.58 | 0.161 | 0.16 | 0.194 |
Market Value | 20.22a | 4.35 | −0.28a | 4.12 | −1.56a | 4.06 | −1.56a | 4.06 | 0.108 | 0.100 | 0.100 |
HR | 37.3a | 3.43 | 24.41b | 3.21 | 38.34a | 3.14 | 20.1b | 3.14 | 0.914 | <0.001 | 0.833 |
GSR | 1.66a | 3.96 | 0.63a | 3.6 | 4.87a | 3.67 | 8.06a | 3.56 | 0.18 | 0.769 | 0.564 |
Cortisol | 0.14a | 0.04 | −0.02b | 0.04 | 0.05a | 0.04 | −0.05b | 0.04 | 0.151 | 0.003 | 0.431 |
DHEA | 106.06a | 35.5 | 18.03a | 31.01 | 21.9a | 33.91 | 4.06b | 30.7 | 0.165 | 0.108 | 0.283 |
BP | 5.8a | 1.69 | 1.67b | 1.67 | 5.44a | 1.7 | −0.54b | 1.64 | 0.47 | 0.003 | 0.576 |
Note. Means are adjusted for days since last use. Superscripts denote significant differences.
SEM = standard error of the mean. HR = heart rate. GSR = galvanic skin response. DHEA = dehydroepiandrosterone. BP = blood pressure.
Highlights.
Stress and cues implicated in the initiation and maintenance of drug abuse
Participants completed stress or no-stress conditions and then drug cue paradigm
Heightened stress reactivity and craving observed in PO group compared to controls
Exposure to stress did not potentiate craving in response to drug cues
Stress response is not by enhancing the incentive salience of drug cues.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) to S. E. Back (K23 DA021228), J.L. McCauley (DA036566) and K. T. Brady (K24 DA00435), and a grant from the Department of Veteran Affairs Clinical Sciences Research and Development to D. F. Gros (CX000845). The authors would also like to acknowledge support from NIAAA T32 grant number AA007474 (Woodward, J.). The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of NIDA, the Department of Veterans Affairs, or the United States government. We would like to thank the following individuals for their assistance with study design and data collection: Dr. Ronald See, Dr. Elizabeth Cox, Ms. Mary Ashley Mercer, Ms. Katie Lawson, and Ms. Emily Hartwell.
Role of Funding Source:
NIDA, NIAAA, and Veterans Affairs Clinical Sciences Research and Development had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributors:
Sudie E. Back was principal investigator of the project and lead author of the manuscript as a whole. Daniel F. Gros wrote the introduction section and provided significant editing of the entire manuscript through the revision process. Matthew Price completed the analyses and authored the results section and related tables. Steve LaRowe and Julianne Flanagan authored the discussion section. Kathleen T. Brady supervised the development and completion of the project and related manuscript. Charles Davis aided in the coding and interpretation of the medication and biological findings. Maryanne Jaconis created the figures and assisted with editing the manuscript. Jenna L. McCauley contributed to the introduction and discussion sections.
Authors Disclosures
Conflict of Interest:
There are no conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic And Statistical Manual Of Mental Disorders. 4th Ed Text Revision) Washington D. C: APA; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Back SE, Gros DF, McCauley JL, Hellmuth J, Cox EQ, Barth KS, Brady KT. Laboratory-induced cue reactivity among individuals with prescription opioid dependence. Addict. Behav. 2014;39:1217–1223. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Back SE, Hartwell K, DeSantis SM, Saladin M, McRae-Clark AL, Price KL, Moran-Santa Maria MM, Baker NL, Spratt E, Kreek MJ, Brady KT. Reactivity to laboratory stress provocation predicts relapse to cocaine. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010;106:21–27. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Back SE, Waldrop AE, Saladin ME, Yeatts SD, Simpson A, McRae AL, Upadhyaya HP, Contini Sisson R, Spratt EG, Allen J, Kreek MJ, Brady KT. Effects of gender and cigarette smoking on reactivity to psychological and pharmacological stress provocation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:560–568. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchmann AF, Laucht M, Schmid B, Wiedemann K, Mann K, Zimmermann US. Cigarette craving increases after a psychosocial stress test and is related to cortisol stress response but not to dependence scores in daily smokers. J. Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:247–255. doi: 10.1177/0269881108095716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffalari DM, See RE. Footshock stress potentiates cue-induced cocaine-seeking in an animal model of relapse. Physiol. Behav. 2009;98:614–617. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady KT, Back SE, McRae AL, Waldrop AE, Anton RF, Upadhyaya HP, .Saladin ME, Randall PK. Cold pressor task reactivity: Predictors of alcohol use among alcohol-dependent individuals with and without comorbid PTSD. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2006;30:938–946. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00097.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady KT, Sinha R. Co-occurring mental and substance use disorders: The neurobiological effects of chronic stress. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:1483–1493. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breese GR, Chu K, Dayas CV, Funk D, Knapp DJ, Koob GF, Le DA, O’Dell LE, Overstreet DH, Roberts AJ, Sinha R, Valdez GR, Weiss F. Stress enhancement of craving during sobriety: a risk for relapse. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2005;29:185–195. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000153544.83656.3c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breese GR, Sinha R, Heilig M. Chronic alcohol neuroadaptation and stress contribute to susceptibility for alcohol craving and relapse. Pharmacol. Thera. 2011;129:149–171. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2010.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruchas MR, Land BB, Chavkin C. The dynorphin/kappa opioid system as a modulator of stress-induced and pro-addictive behaviors. Brain Res. 2010;1314:44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calcaterra S, Glanz J, Binswanger IA. National trends in pharmaceutical opioid related overdose deaths compared to other substance related overdose deaths: 1999–2009. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;131:263–270. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.11.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter BL, Tiffany ST. Cue-reactivity and the future of addiction research. Addiction. 1999;94:349–351. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childress A, Ehrman R, McLellan AT, O’Brien C. Conditioned craving and arousal in cocaine addiction: a preliminary report. NIDA Res. Monogr. 1988;81:74–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childress AR, McLellan AT, O'Brien CP. Conditioned responses in a methadone population: a comparison of laboratory, clinic, and natural settings. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 1986;3:173–179. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(86)90018-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childs E, de Wit H. Effects of acute psychosocial stress on cigarette craving and smoking. Nicotine Tob. Res. 2010;12:449–453. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daughters SB, Richards JM, Gorka SM, Sinha R. HPA axis response to psychological stress and treatment retention in residential substance abuse treatment: a prospective study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;105:202–208. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drummond DC, Cooper T, Glautier SP. Conditioned learning in alcohol dependence: implications for cue exposure treatment. Br. J. Addict. 1990;85:725–743. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enoch MA. The role of early life stress as a predictor for alcohol and drug dependence. Psychopharmacology. 2011;214:17–31. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1916-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Biometrics Research. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002. Structured Clinical Interview For DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. [Google Scholar]
- Foley P, Kirschbaum C. Human hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis responses to acute psychosocial stress in laboratory settings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010;35:91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken IH, Kroon LY, Hendriks VM. Influence of individual differences in craving and obsessive cocaine thoughts on attentional processes in cocaine abuse patients. Addict. Behav. 2000;25:99–102. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(98)00112-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garland EL, Froeliger B, Zeidan F, Partin K, Howard MO. The downward spiral of chronic pain, prescription opioid misuse, and addiction: cognitive, affective, and neuropsychopharmacologic pathways. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013;37:2597–2607. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garland EL, Gaylord SA, Boettiger CA, Howard MO. Mindfulness training modifies cognitive, affective, and physiological mechanisms implicated in alcohol dependence: results of a randomized controlled pilot trial. J. Psychoactive Drugs. 2010;42:177–192. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2010.10400690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gossop M. The development of a Short Opiate Withdrawal Scale (SOWS) Addict. Behav. 1990;15:487–490. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(90)90036-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gros DF, Haren WB. Open trial of brief behavioral activation psychotherapy for depression in an integrated VA primary care setting. Prim. Care Companion CNS Disord. 2011;13 doi: 10.4088/PCC.11m01136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall AJ, Logan JE, Toblin RL, Kaplan JA, Kraner JC, Bixler D, Crosby AE, Paulozzi LJ. Patterns of abuse among unintentional pharmaceutical overdose fatalities. JAMA. 2008;300:2613–2620. doi: 10.1001/jama.2008.802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SM, Sinha R. Stress-related factors in cannabis use and misuse: implications for prevention and treatment. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 2009;36:400–413. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2008.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamboj SK, Joye A, Das RK, Gibson AJ, Morgan CJ, Curran HV. Cue exposure and response prevention with heavy smokers: a laboratory-based randomised placebo-controlled trial examining the effects of D-cycloserine on cue reactivity and attentional bias. Psychopharmacology. 2012;221:273–284. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2571-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khantzian EJ. The self-medication hypothesis of addictive disorders: focus on heroin and cocaine dependence. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1985;142:1259–1264. doi: 10.1176/ajp.142.11.1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test' - a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28:76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF. Brain stress systems in the amygdala and addiction. Brain Res. 2009;1293:61–75. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.03.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosten TR, Scanley BE, Tucker KA, Oliveto A, Prince C, Sinha R, Potenza MN, Skudlarski P, Wexler BE. Cue-induced brain activity changes and relapse in cocaine-dependent patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:644–650. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuehn BM. Opioid prescriptions soar. JAMA. 2007;297:249–253. doi: 10.1001/jama.297.3.249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee FH, Raja SN. Complementary and alternative medicine in chronic pain. Pain. 2011;152:28–30. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Qin W, Yuan K, Li J, Wang W, Li Q, Wang Y, Sun J, von Deeneen KM, Liu Y, Tian J. Interaction between dysfunctional connectivity at rest and heroin cues-induced brain responses in male abstinent heroin-dependent individuals. PLoS One. 2011;6:e23098. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Weiss F. Additive effects of stress and drug cues on reinstatement of ethanol seeking: exacerbation by history of dependence and role of concurrent activation of corticotrophin-releasing factor and opioid mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:7856–7861. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-18-07856.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lunn MPT, Hughes RAC, Wiffen PJ. Duloxetine for treating painful neuropathy or chronic pain. Cochrane Database System. Rev. 2009;4:CD007115. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007115.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marissen MA, Franken IH, Blanken P, van den Brink W, Hendriks VM. Cue exposure therapy for the treatment of opiate addiction: results of a randomized controlled clinical trial. Psychother, Psychosomat. 2007;76:97–105. doi: 10.1159/000097968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHugh RK, Nielsen S, Weiss RD. Prescription drug abuse: from epidemiology to public policy. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 2015;48:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2014.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McRae-Clark AL, Carter RE, Price KL, Baker NL, Thomas S, Saladin ME, Giarla K, Nicholas K, Brady KT. Stress- and cue-elicited craving and reactivity in marijuana-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 2011;218:49–58. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2376-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McRae AL, Saladin ME, Brady KT, Upadhyaya H, Back SE, Timmerman MA. Stress reactivity: biological and subjective responses to the cold pressor and Trier Social Stressors, Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2006;21:377–385. doi: 10.1002/hup.778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien CP, Childress AR, Ehrman R, Robbins SJ. Conditioning factors in drug abuse: can they explain compulsion? J. Psychopharmacol. 1998;12:15–22. doi: 10.1177/026988119801200103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paliwal P, Hyman SM, Sinha R. Craving predicts time to cocaine relapse: further validation of the now and brief versions of the cocaine craving questionnaire. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;93:252–259. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulozzi LJ, Budnitz DS, Xi Y. Increasing deaths from opioid analgesics in the United States. Pharmacoepidemiol. Drug Safe. 2006;15:618–627. doi: 10.1002/pds.1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowbotham MC. Washington, DC: FDA; 2015. What Alternatives Are There To The Use Of Opioid Analgesics In The Treatment Of Chronic Pain In Light Of Existing Evidence And Its Limitations? www.fda.gov/downloads/Drugs/NewsEvents/UCM307837.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Ray LA, Lunny K, Bujarski S, Moallem N, Krull JL, Miotto K. The effects of varenicline on stress-induced and cue-induced craving for cigarettes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;131:136–142. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schank JR, Pickens CL, Rowe KE, Cheng K, Thorsell A, Rice KC, Shaham Y, Heilig M. Stress-induced reinstatement of alcohol-seeking in rats is selectively suppressed by the neurokinin 1 (NK1) antagonist L822429. Psychopharmacology. 2011;218:111–119. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2201-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- See RE. Neural substrates of conditioned-cued relapse to drug-seeking behavior. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2002;71:517–529. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00682-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt A, Walter M, Gerber H, Seifritz E, Brenneisen R, Wiesbeck GA, Riecher-Rossler A, Lang UE, Borgwardt S. Normalizing effect of heroin maintenance treatment on stress-induced brain connectivity. Brain. 2015;138:217–228. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.INI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1998;59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiffman S. Relapse following smoking cessation: a situational analysis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1982;50:71–86. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.50.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel S. Drug anticipation and drug addiction. The 1998 H. David Archibald Lecture. Addictiion. 1999;94:1113–1124. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.94811132.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel S, Ramos B. Applying laboratory research: drug anticipation and the treatment of drug addiction. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2002;10:162. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.10.3.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R. The role of stress in addiction relapse. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2007;9:388–395. doi: 10.1007/s11920-007-0050-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology. 2001;158:343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R, Catapano D, O’Malley S. Stress-induced craving and stress response in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 1999;142:343–351. doi: 10.1007/s002130050898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R, Fox HC, Hong KA, Bergquist K, Bhagwagar Z, Siedlarz K. Enhanced negative emotion and alcohol craving, and altered physiological responses following stress and cue exposure in alcohol dependent individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1198–1208. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snelleman M, Schoenmakers TM, van de Mheen D. The relationship between perceived stress and cue sensitivity for alcohol. Addict. Behav. 2014;39:1884–1889. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (Form Y) Palo Alto, CA: Mind Garden, Inc.; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Stewart J. Stress and relapse to drug seeking: studies in laboratory animals shed light on mechanisms and sources of long- term vulnerability. Am. J. Addict. 2003;12:1–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 6th Edition. Boston: Allyn and Bacon; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Thor KB, Kirby M, Viktrup L. Serotonin and noradrenaline involvement in urinary incontinence, depression, and pain: scientific basis for overlapping clinical efficacy from a single drug, duloxetine. Int. J. Clin. Pract. 2007;61:1349–1355. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-1241.2007.01433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffany ST. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol. Rev. 1990;97:147. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas SE, Randall PK, Brady K, See RE, Drobes DJ. An acute psychosocial stressor does not potentiate alcohol cue reactivity in non-treatment-seeking alcoholics. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2011;35:464–473. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01363.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter M, Gerber H, Kuhl HC, Schmid O, Joechle W, Lanz C, Brenneisen R, Schachinger H, Riecher-Rossler A, Wiesbeck GA, Borgwardt SJ. Acute effects of intravenous heroin on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis response: a controlled trial. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2013;33:193–198. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31828393cb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wei LJ, Lachin JM. Properties of the urn randomization in clinical trials. Control. Clin. Trials. 1988;9:345–364. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(88)90048-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Shiffman S. Coping and substance abuse: a conceptual framework. In: Shiffman S, Wills TA, editors. Coping And Substance Use. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985. pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]