Abstract
The ability to taste bitterness evolved to safeguard most animals, including humans, against potentially toxic substances, thereby leading to food rejection. Nonetheless, bitter perception is subject to individual variations due to the presence of genetic functional polymorphisms in bitter taste receptor (TAS2R) genes, such as the long-known association between genetic polymorphisms in TAS2R38 and bitter taste perception of phenylthiocarbamide. Yet, due to overlaps in specificities across receptors, such associations with a single TAS2R locus are uncommon. Therefore, to investigate more complex associations, we examined taste responses to six structurally diverse compounds (absinthin, amarogentin, cascarillin, grosheimin, quassin, and quinine) in a sample of the Caucasian population. By sequencing all bitter receptor loci, inferring long-range haplotypes, mapping their effects on phenotype variation, and characterizing functionally causal allelic variants, we deciphered at the molecular level how a subjects’ genotype for the whole-family of TAS2R genes shapes variation in bitter taste perception. Within each haplotype block implicated in phenotypic variation, we provided evidence for at least one locus harboring functional polymorphic alleles, e.g. one locus for sensitivity to amarogentin, one of the most bitter natural compounds known, and two loci for sensitivity to grosheimin, one of the bitter compounds of artichoke. Our analyses revealed also, besides simple associations, complex associations of bitterness sensitivity across TAS2R loci. Indeed, even if several putative loci harbored both high- and low-sensitivity alleles, phenotypic variation depended on linkage between these alleles. When sensitive alleles for bitter compounds were maintained in the same linkage phase, genetically driven perceptual differences were obvious, e.g. for grosheimin. On the contrary, when sensitive alleles were in opposite phase, only weak genotype-phenotype associations were seen, e.g. for absinthin, the bitter principle of the beverage absinth. These findings illustrate the extent to which genetic influences on taste are complex, yet arise from both receptor activation patterns and linkage structure among receptor genes.
Author Summary
Human bitter taste is believed to protect us from the ingestion of poisonous substances, thereby shaping food rejections. Bitter perception differs, however, across individuals, due to genetic variations in the ~25 bitter taste receptor (TAS2R) genes. A famous example known since the 1930s is the inherited bitter taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide, which is associated with genetic polymorphisms in a single TAS2R gene. Yet, such simple receptor-substance associations do not reflect the full complexity of bitter perception, since individual bitter substances frequently activate several TAS2Rs. Here, we provide an in-depth analysis of the genetic variability influencing human bitter taste. While each study subject carried a different set of genetic polymorphisms, we found that most variations reside in just six blocks, each harboring only one to five haplotypes. Thus, besides simple associations between taste and TAS2R gene polymorphisms, we revealed complex associations dependent on linkage between several high- and low-sensitivity alleles. Indeed, subjects carried either sensitive or insensitive alleles for receptors sensitive to grosheimin, a bitter compound in artichoke, or at least one sensitive allele for receptors specific for absinthin, the bitter principle of absinth. In short, simple associations and complex genomic linkage determine sensitivity to selected dietary bitter compounds.
Introduction
Bitter taste perception plays a fundamental role in dietary preferences and behaviours, by shaping aversions to foods and drinks. Indeed, averse responses to bitterness are instinctive and drive rejection and avoidance behaviours widely observed in animal models, but also in human infants [1–5]. They are hypothesized to originate with bitter perception’s role as a warning sensor against potentially harmful substances contained in the diet, such as toxins released by plants to deter herbivores, and they belong to diverse chemical classes including acetogenins, alkaloids, flavonoids, phenylpropanes, terpenoids, and thiol compounds [6–9]. These rejection behaviours mediated by bitter perception show evolutionary trends, with responses depending on the occurrence of bitter substances in animal typical diets [10]. Occasionally, bitter substances known to possess desirable pharmacological activities are also deliberately ingested (e.g., [11, 12]), nevertheless acceptation of bitter phytonutrient in food remains challenging (for review see [13]).
Despite the importance of bitter taste in shaping nutritional behaviours and guarding against toxin ingestion, bitter responses in humans vary profoundly. The classic example of phenotypic diversity in humans is threshold sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide (PTC), which differs by up to 10,000-fold among individuals. Such variation is due to a constellation of interacting factors including environmental effects, age, gender, experience and genetics (for reviews see [13–15]). Particularly strong effects have been found to arise from polymorphism in TAS2R genes, which encode a series of ~25 G protein-coupled receptors expressed in taste buds [16–20]. In the case of PTC perception, polymorphism in TAS2R38 accounts for more than 55% of observed phenotypic variance [17, 21]. Moreover, genetic polymorphisms occurring at TAS2R loci are common, with numerous high-frequency alleles [22], suggesting the presence of functionally important receptor variants. Major changes in receptor activity due to such variants have been observed in a few other cases, i.e., TAS2R9, TAS2R16, TAS2R43, and TAS2R31 [23–27]. In addition, gene association studies suggested functional polymorphic alleles at other TAS2R loci, e.g. TAS2R4 or TAS2R13 [28–30].
An essential aspect of interactions between TAS2Rs and bitter compounds relates to the overlapping agonist profiles, with most TAS2Rs responding to multiple agonists and many agonists stimulating multiple receptors [31]. This combinatorial activation pattern, together with the distribution of TAS2R genes among only four cytogenic locations, which can lead to false-positive genotype-phenotype associations arising from sites in linkage with the causal variants, have so far prevented the full elucidation of bitter perception’s molecular underpinnings [26, 32–34]. To establish a genetic basis for the observed perceptual differences in the population, we used in this study an integrative approach, sequencing all known TAS2R loci in humans, inferring long-rang haplotypes, mapping their effects on perception of several chemically diverse compounds, and functionally characterizing all allelic variants associated with shifts in perception.
Results
SNP and haplotype diversity
Genetic diversity was assessed by determining whole-gene genotypes of the 25 members of the TAS2R gene family and corresponding copy number variations (Figs 1 and 2, S1 Table). Across the 48 Caucasian subjects, a total of 93 coding SNPs (cSNPs), including 65 missense SNPs and 2 nonsense SNPs, were identified. Three indels, including 1 rare three-nucleotide in-frame deletion and 2 major deletions spanning TAS2R43 or TAS2R45 locus, were also detected. Genes harboured a mean of 4 cSNPs with a mean of ~1 synonymous and ~3 non-synonymous SNPs per gene. The mean number of haplotypes within genes was 3.5 with a range of 1 to 6, which recombined to form a mean number of 5.6 genotypes within genes with a range of 1 to 13.
When only common cSNPs were considered (i.e., with frequency ≥ 0.05), a total of 67 SNPs, including 45 missense SNPs and 2 nonsense SNPs, and 2 indels were identified, all of which have been reported previously [22, 26, 36–40]. Sequenced genes harboured a mean of ~1 synonymous and ~2 non-synonymous common SNPs per gene. While some genes harboured no SNPs (e.g., TAS2R10 and TAS2R39), others harboured many. Genes located in the proximal region of the cluster at 12p13 harboured also highest number of SNPs, with 8 at TAS2R31 and -42, and 9 at TAS2R20.
Of these 45 common missense cSNPs, 27 corresponded to amino acid positions localised in TAS2R transmembrane (TM) domains, with 9 SNPs affecting amino acids in TM VI and 6 in TM V (Fig 3). The remaining SNPs were distributed in sequence areas coding for extracellular domains, intracellular domains, and COOH terminal region, with 8, 5, and 5 SNPs, respectively. In addition, 2 nonsense cSNPs result in a premature stop codon in the reading frame and therefore to a putatively non-functional, truncated receptor variant.
These common cSNPs recombined across genes to form a total of 59 coding haplotypes, with a mean of 2.2 per gene and a range of 1 to 4. Again, genes localised to the proximal region of 12p13 were most diverse, with 4 haplotypes at TAS2R20, -31, and -50. As with common SNPs, most within-gene haplotypes have been reported previously [22, 26, 36–40]. Two common haplotypes were newly identified: one at TAS2R14 (frequency = 0.34), and one at TAS2R42 (frequency = 0.30).
Long-range haplotypes
High levels of genetic diversity were observed although our population sample, only Caucasian subjects, was relatively homogeneous with respect to ethnic and geographic origin. Thus, chromosomal spatial relations, linkage and block structures among genes might be important contributors to TAS2R-mediated phenotypes. Indeed, the 25 members of the TAS2R gene family are restricted to just three cytogenetic locations 5p15, 7q31-7q35, and 12p13 (Fig 1). Position 5p15 contains a single gene, TAS2R1, 7q31-35 contains 9 genes distributed across a ~20.5 Mb region, and 12p13 contains 15 genes distributed across a ~400 kb region. Further, most of the TAS2Rs at 7q31-7q35 (TAS2R3, -4, -5, -38, -39, -40, -60, and -41) reside in a 1.8 Mb sub-region.
Estimates of pairwise D′ and r2 revealed extensive linkage in both the 7q31-7q35 and 12p13 regions. Mean values of D′ and r2 between common cSNPs were 0.66 and 0.52 across 12p13, and associated with low p-values (Fig 4). Nearly identical trends were observed between TAS2R loci, with mean D′ and r2 values of 0.66 and 0.37. Similarly, across the 1.8 Mb TAS2R3-38 cluster, mean values were 0.76 and 0.74 for common cSNPs and 0.60 and 0.63 between TAS2R loci. These trends indicate that TAS2Rs are tightly connected in long-range haplotypes spanning multiple genes.
Haplotype block partitions and corresponding long-range haplotypes were then determined for the 25 TAS2R genes (Fig 5). Analyses inferring haplotype blocks identified six blocks distributed across the TAS2R clusters on chromosomes 7 and 12. These corresponded to blocks found in the region by previous studies [34, 43, 44]. Two were found on chromosome 7, encompassing the TAS2R3-5 and TAS2R39-60 regions, each of which harboured two long-range haplotypes with frequencies at or above 0.05. Four blocks were found on chromosome 12, encompassing the TAS2R7-10, TAS2R13-14, TAS2R50-19, and TAS2R31-42 regions, which harboured 1, 3, and 5 long-range haplotypes respectively. In addition, whereas blocks on chromosome 7 were flanked by recombination hot spots, we identified no hot spots in the TAS2R13-42 region on chromosome 12, which seems to indicate that these latter haplotype blocks are determined by linkage disequilibrium decay [45].
Genotype-phenotype associations
Genotype-phenotype association analyses were conducted to elucidate genetic liability for taste phenotypes; phenotype data consisting of individual detection and recognition thresholds for the bitter tastants, as well as concentrations for perceived weak, moderate, strong, and very strong intensities inferred from intensity ratings. Subsequent functional assays provided further insights into the identification of causal TAS2Rs.
Amarogentin
Bitter taste responses to amarogentin exhibited high variance across subjects on both threshold and suprathreshold measures (Fig 6A). Recognition thresholds ranged 11-fold across subjects, with the most sensitive individuals reporting bitterness at a concentration of 1.2x10-8 M, the least sensitive reporting it at 2.0x10-7 M, and a modal number at 4.0x10-8 M. Concentrations rated as weakly bitter by subjects ranged 17-fold, from 1.2x10-8 to 3.0x10-7 M with a mode at 5.0x10-8 M. In contrast to recognition thresholds and concentrations perceived as weak, the distribution of concentrations perceived as strongly bitter was narrow, ranging less than 4-fold, from 6.0x10-8 to 3.0x10-7 M and a mode between 1.4x10-7 and 2.0x10-7 M. Distributions of concentrations perceived as moderately and very strongly bitter exhibited analogous properties (S2 Fig).
Association analyses targeting perceived weak intensity of amarogentin revealed significant signals localized to the region spanning TAS2R31 to TAS2R42, with the lowest p-values occurring at TAS2R30, -31, -43, and -46 (Fig 6B; S2 Table). Associations with recognition threshold and perceived strong intensity showed similar patterns. However, while significant at the 0.05 level, these did not reach significance at the experiment-wide significance threshold, i.e., when corrected for multiple testing. Association analyses targeting detection thresholds, perceived moderate intensity, and very strong intensity yielded similar results (S2 Fig; S2 Table). Numerous non-synonymous variants in the TAS2R31-42 block were thus observed among the variants significantly associated with amarogentin perception, indicating that phenotypes are likely driven by polymorphism in the corresponding TAS2Rs. However, within-block LD across the implicated sites was high, raising the possibility that in addition to true associations arising from functional effects, spurious associations were present as the result of linkage between contributory and irrelevant sites. Numerous SNPs in TAS2Rs at 12p13 were also significant at 0.05, but not when corrected. These patterns might be also explained by high within-block LD, accompanied by between-block linkage decay which reduces spurious association signals with distance. Hence, the mechanistic properties of these variants could not be discerned from genotype-phenotype association data alone. To resolve these ambiguities we examined substitution effects using an in vitro molecular approach.
In a previous study, we utilized heterologous expression assays to ascertain the response of every known TAS2R in humans to a chemical library of synthetic and natural compounds [31]. The resulting inventory identified six TAS2Rs showing baseline responses to amarogentin: TAS2R1, -4, -39, -43, -46, -30, and -50. Thus, three TAS2Rs (TAS2R43, -46, and -30) previously shown to exhibit baseline responses to amarogentin also harboured SNPs associated with amarogentin sensitivity in our subjects, specifically implicating these sites as sources of variation in both receptor function and downstream measures of amarogentin sensitivity.
In vitro assays revealed extensive variation in receptor response arising from substitutions associated with amarogentin perception phenotypes (Fig 6C). TAS2R30 harboured two common receptor variants (i.e., with frequency ≥ 0.05) exhibiting divergent responses, TAS2R30-H1 and-H2. TAS2R30-H1 was highly sensitive and responsive to amarogentin (threshold = 1.0x10-8 M; EC50 = 4.1x10-7 M; maximum amplitude = 0.50 ΔF/F) while TAS2R30-H2 was less sensitive, showing a slightly reduced threshold and severely reduced maximal signal amplitude (threshold = 3.0x10-7 M; EC50 = 3.9x10-7 M; maximum amplitude = 0.12 ΔF/F). In contrast to TAS2R30, TAS2R43 variants exhibited essentially no response to amarogentin, with both TAS2R43-H1 and-H2 only being activated at the highest artefact-free concentration of 3.0x10-4 M. Receptor variants of TAS2R46 exhibited major differences in amarogentin sensitivity, nonetheless only from the relatively high concentration of 3.0x10-5 M. TAS2R46-H1 was highly sensitive and responsive (threshold = 3.0x10-5 M; EC50 = 6.7x10-5 M; maximum amplitude = 0.57 ΔF/F), whereas TAS2R46-H3 was moderately activated (threshold = 1.0x10-4 M). TAS2R46-H2, which is characterized by a premature stop codon resulting in a severe truncation of the receptor, was not activated at any concentration.
Patterns of activation across TAS2R30, -43, and -46 variants suggest that bitter taste sensitivity to amarogentin is likely solely driven by functional genetic polymorphisms in TAS2R30. Of all tested receptor variants, only TAS2R30-H1 was highly responsive to amarogentin at concentrations matching thresholds in subjects. Further, the second receptor variant of TAS2R30,-H2, exhibited weak activation regardless the concentration range, explaining the presence of low-sensitivity subjects and overall patterns of association. In contrast to TAS2R30, activation of TAS2R43 or TAS2R46 occurred at concentrations 3000-fold or 300-fold higher than TAS2R30, respectively, and far exceeding observed threshold phenotypes. TAS2R43 and TAS2R46 failed thus to explain observed patterns of phenotypic variation.
Quassin
Like amarogentin, genotype-phenotype associations and functional analyses indicate that functional genetic polymorphisms in TAS2R30 are also responsible for the bitter taste sensitivity of quassin, as expected by the high value of the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between these two tastants (0.87; p < 0.001). As with amarogentin, distributions of threshold recognition and concentrations perceived as weak were broad, ranging 38-fold (from 2.6x10-8 M to 1.0x10-6 M) and 26-fold (from 3.9x10-8 M to 1.0x10-6 M), respectively, while distribution of concentrations perceived as strong was narrow, ranging 8-fold (from 1.3x10-7 M to 1.0x10-6 M) (S3 Fig). In vitro, activation pattern of TAS2Rs differ slightly between the two chemically distinct tastants amarogentin and quassin, with quassin activating only TAS2R30 and TAS2R46. TAS2R30-H1 and-H2 exhibited divergent responses to quassin, with-H1 responding in ranges matching phenotypic recognition threshold. TAS2R46 variants also exhibited divergent activation patterns, with TAS2R46-H1 being most sensitive,-H3 being intermediate, and-H2 being least sensitive, but these responded at concentrations exceeding phenotypic recognition thresholds, suggesting that they are unable to explain observed phenotypes.
Grosheimin
Taste responses to grosheimin exhibited broad phenotypic distributions (Fig 7A). Recognition thresholds ranged 25-fold, from 2.0x10-6 M to 5.0x10-5 M, with distinct modes at 9.9x10-6 M and 2.2x10-5 M. Bimodality is a classic feature of threshold responses to bitter substances including phenylthiocarbamide (PTC), propylthiouracil (PROP), goitrin, saccharin, acesulfame potassium, and aloin, suggesting that genetic effects on grosheimin response are likely strong [25, 26, 46]. The distribution of concentrations corresponding to perceived weak intensity was also broad, ranging from 2.9x10-6 M to 5.0x10-5 M (17-fold) with a single mode at 1.5x10-5 M. In contrast, the distribution of concentrations corresponding to perceived strong intensity was narrow, ranging only 5-fold (from 9.9x10-6 M to 5.0x10-5 M) with a single mode at 3.3x10-5 M. Distribution of detection thresholds was similar to that of recognition threshold; distributions of concentrations perceived as weak, moderate, strong, and very strong changed gradually from a broad to a relatively narrow phenotypic distribution (S2 Fig).
SNPs significantly associated with grosheimin response at the experiment-wide significance threshold were found at multiple loci, depending on the specific phenotype (Fig 7B; S2 Table). Two loci, TAS2R19 and -31, harboured polymorphisms associated with recognition threshold. In addition to being found at TAS2R19 and -31, SNPs significantly associated with weak intensity occurred at TAS2R30 and -46; the whole-gene deletion at TAS2R43 was also significant. Besides these associations, numerous SNPs spanning the TAS2R50-19 and TAS2R31-42 haplotype blocks were associated with strong intensity response. Additional SNPs in TAS2R genes located at 12p13 were also significant at the 0.05 level for recognition threshold, perceived weak and strong intensities, i.e., uncorrected for multiple testing. Tests for association with detection threshold, and perceived moderate and very strong intensities were consistent with these observations (S2 Fig; S2 Table). As previously mentioned for amarogentin and quassin, LD was high between SNPs associated with grosheimin response, leaving the specific causative sites unclear. However, in our published inventory of receptor-agonist relationships across the complete TAS2R family, only TAS2R43 and -46 were activated by grosheimin, and both harboured variants associated with grosheimin perception in our subjects [31].
In vitro assays targeting TAS2R43 and -46 confirmed the presence of functional variation at these loci (Fig 7C). TAS2R43 harboured two receptor variants, TAS2R43-H1 and-H2, both of which were responsive to grosheimin. However, TAS2R43-H2 was responsive at lower concentrations than did-H1 (threshold = 3.0x10-6 M; EC50 = 6.9x10-6 M vs. threshold = 1.0x10-5 M; EC50 = 1.6x10-5 M), and exhibited stronger responses (maximum amplitude = 0.81 ΔF/F vs. 0.60 ΔF/F). Both receptor variants were activated in the concentration range perceived as bitter by human subjects. A third allele of TAS2R43 characterized by complete deletion of the gene (TAS2R43-Δ) was also found at high frequencies, suggesting that it could be an important driver of phenotypic associations [25, 26]. Among the three observed TAS2R46 variants, TAS2R46-H1 was highly responsive to grosheimin (threshold = 1.0x10-6 M; EC50 = 5.6x10-6 M; maximum amplitude = 0.87 ΔF/F), and-H3 was moderately responsive (threshold = 3.0x10-6 M; EC50 = 7.6x10-6 M; maximum amplitude = 0.50 ΔF/F). TAS2R46-H2, a truncated, six-transmembrane variant of the receptor, was not activated at any concentration. Moreover, responsive TAS2R43 and -46 variants were activated at concentrations perceived as bitter by human subjects. Thus, both receptors contributed to the variance of grosheimin perception.
A key aspect of variation in TAS2R43 and -46 was that linkage disequilibrium spanned gene loci, such that their functional and phenotypic contributions were non-independent. Inspection of the long-range haplotypes in the TAS2R31-42 block (Fig 5) revealed that the most common haplotype (frequency = 0.30) harboured both the deleted allele at TAS2R43 and the truncated non-functional allele at TAS2R46. In contrast, all other haplotypes harboured sensitive and non-sensitive alleles in varying combinations. The second most common (frequency = 0.22) harboured a moderately sensitive (TAS2R43-H1) and a sensitive allele (TAS2R46-H1), and the third most common (frequency = 0.20) a deleted (TAS2R43-Δ) and a moderately sensitive allele (TAS2R46-H3). A final haplotype (frequency = 0.16) harboured two sensitive alleles (TAS2R43-H2 and TAS2R46-H1). Thus, while TAS2R43 and -46 make functionally independent contributions to phenotype they co-segregate, resulting in positively correlated associations.
Quinine
Quinine elicited also phenotypic responses driven by variation in multiple TAS2Rs. Threshold recognition exhibited a broad distribution (25-fold, from 1.6x10-6 M to 4.0x10-5 M), followed by perceived weak intensity (17-fold, from 2.3x10-6 M to 4.0x10-5 M). The distribution of perceived strong intensity was narrower (5-fold, from 7.9x10-6 M to 4.0x10-5 M) (S3 Fig). However, our genotype-phenotype associations failed, in line with previously reported studies, to identify SNPs explaining phenotypic variation, due to the high linkage disequilibrium spanning functionally distinct TAS2R loci [32, 33]. In vitro, quinine elicited numerous TAS2R-mediated responses, with TAS2R30, -31, -43, and -46 responding only at the second or the last artefact-free concentration (1.0x10-5 M or 3.0x10-5 M). Receptor variants of TAS2R30, -43, and -46 are activated similar to tastants already presented. Additionally, TAS2R31 harboured one sensitive (TAS2R31-H2), two intermediates (-H1 and-H4), and one insensitive variants (-H3). Hence, several TAS2Rs contribute to phenotypic variation.
Absinthin
The distribution of recognition thresholds for absinthin was broad, ranging 40-fold, from 5.2x10-8 M to 2.0x10-6 M (Fig 8A). Two modes were present, at 2.6x10-7 M and one at 5.9x10-7 M. The distribution of concentrations eliciting perceived weak intensity was narrower, ranging 15-fold, from 1.2x10-7 M to 2.0x10-6 M with a single mode at 3.9x10-7 M. The distribution of concentrations eliciting perceived strong intensity was yet narrower, ranging 5-fold, from 4.0x10-7 M to 2.0x10-6 M with a single mode at 1.3x10-6 M. Distributions of detection threshold and concentrations perceived as moderately and very strongly bitter exhibited largely analogous properties, with a detection threshold lower than recognition threshold, and means for perceived moderate, strong, and very strong intensities shifted gradually upward relative to perceived weak bitterness (S2 Fig).
While our previous inventory of receptor-agonist interactions identified TAS2R30 and -46 as responsive to absinthin, and both TAS2R30 and -46 harboured coding variations in our sample, association analyses found, however, no SNPs significantly associated with perception phenotypes (Figs 8B and S2; S2 Table).
Functional assays targeting TAS2R30 and -46 in our sample confirmed that both receptors are capable of responding to absinthin (Fig 8C). As noted earlier, TAS2R30 existed in two variants, TAS2R30-H1 and-H2. As with amarogentin and quassin, TAS2R30-H1 was strongly activated by absinthin (threshold = 3.0x10-7 M; EC50 = 3.1x10-6 M; maximum amplitude = 0.41 ΔF/F) while H2 was less sensitive, with a severely reduced maximal signal amplitude (threshold = 1.0x10-6 M; EC50 = 1.3x10-6 M; maximum amplitude = 0.15 ΔF/F). Likewise, three variants of TAS2R46 exhibited responses mirroring their responses to amarogentin and quassin. TAS2R46-H1 (threshold = 3.0x10-6 M; EC50 = 1.1x10-5 M; maximum amplitude = 0.63 ΔF/F) was more sensitive than-H3 (threshold = 1.0x10-5 M; EC50 = 1.4x10-5 M; maximum amplitude = 0.48 ΔF/F), and TAS2R46-H2 showed no response at any concentration.
The lack of association between absinthin perception and SNPs at TAS2R30 and -46, in spite of the presence of functional diversity among these receptors, was explained by long-range haplotypes in the TAS2R31-42 region (Fig 5). Two common long-range haplotypes coded the sensitive receptor variant TAS2R30-H1, combined with the non-functional TAS2R46-H2 or the moderately sensitive variant TAS2R46-H3, for the first (frequency = 0.30) and the third (frequency = 0.20) haplotype, respectively. The two other common long-range haplotypes, the second (frequency = 0.22) and the fourth (frequency = 0.16), coded the sensitive receptor variant TAS2R46-H1 and the insensitive TAS2R30-H2. Thus, while TAS2R30 and -46 both harboured alleles with variable sensitivity, such that their individual contributions to phenotype varied, their organization in long-range haplotypes resulted in 96% of subjects carrying exactly two copies of a high-sensitivity allele of at least one of the two genes, and 4% of subjects carrying one copy. This pattern explains the lack of genotypic association. This clarifies also the low concentration perceived as bitter by human subjects when compared with in vitro activation.
Cascarillin
Cascarillin elicited patterns of phenotypic and functional responses similar to those elicited by absinthin, but differing in important respects. As with absinthin, threshold recognition exhibited a broad distribution (17-fold, from 2.3x10-6 M to 4.0x10-5 M). Distribution of perceived weak intensity was narrower (11-fold, from 3.5x10-6 M to 4.0x10-5 M), followed by perceived strong intensity (S3 Fig). In vitro, the minimum concentration required to elicit response to cascarillin in functional assays was slightly higher (3.0x10-5 M). Indeed, receptor variants of TAS2R30 and -46 are activated in similar ways by absinthin and cascarillin, with TAS2R30 harbouring sensitive and insensitive variants (TAS2R30-H1 and-H2) and TAS2R46 harboring sensitive, intermediate, and insensitive variants (TAS2R46-H1,-H3, and-H2, respectively) (S3 Fig). Again, genotype-phenotype associations between variation at TAS2R30 and -46 failed to identify SNPs explaining phenotypic variation, likely as the result of linkage disequilibrium between responsive and nonresponse alleles of TAS2R30 and -46, since all subjects carried at least one functional allele.
Discussion
Bitter taste perception has long been known to have major heritable components. Common dominant and recessive alleles shaping sensitivity to specific chemical compounds were identified as early as the 1930s (for review see [47]). It is now known that much of this heritability is due to coding variation in TAS2R genes altering receptor affinity, which shapes perception phenotypes. In the classic case, patterns of bitter sensitivity are driven by strong, single-gene effects [17, 21, 25, 26]. However, such associations with only one TAS2R locus appear to be rare. Relationships between TAS2R variation and perception are likely most often complex due to high levels of genetic diversity, linkage between loci and overlap in receptor-agonist interactions, with some agonists stimulating multiple receptors and some receptors responding to multiple agonists [26, 31]. This hinders elucidation of bitter perception’s molecular underpinnings and, consequently, their downstream effects on aversion and ingestive behaviours [34]. Therefore, until now only a limited number of TAS2R genes have been implicated in the modification of ingestive behavior (for a review see [48]). Our findings reveal the extent to which linkage constrains both, variation in TAS2R genes and patterns of overlap in receptor-agonist affinity, and the impact of these factors on bitter taste phenotypes.
Human TAS2Rs are highly diverse, with per nucleotide heterozygosity significantly higher than genome-wide averages, elevated rates of non-synonymous substitution, and fixation indices (FST values) indicative of substantial population differentiation [22]. This suggests that combinatorial variation across loci could, in principle, be extremely high. However, patterns of linkage disequilibrium in our sample of the Caucasian population demonstrate that such diversity is limited. While a total of 93 SNPs were present, which could in principle recombine to form more than 7 quintillion (7.71 x 1030) different combinations, most variation resided in just six blocks, each harbouring just 1 to 4 haplotypes. This finding has two implications. First, it suggests that while humans harbour 25 functional TAS2R loci, each of which encodes a receptor with alleles responsive to different ranges of agonists, phenotypic responses are likely correlated across compounds regardless of whether they are mediated by the same receptor. Second, linkage disequilibrium and block structures spanning loci are likely major sources of spurious genotype-phenotype associations. Because blocks often span several loci, yet few long-range haplotypes are present, true genotype-phenotype associations will most often be accompanied by false positives arising from sites in LD with the causal variants. This problem is compounded by the prevalence of non-synonymous variants in TAS2Rs, which stand out as potential functional receptor candidates, making them difficult to rule out as causal. These issues were evident in our association analyses, which were able to localize signals for amarogentin, grosheimin, and quassin at the block level but not with respect to specific SNPs (Figs 7, 8 and S1–S3).
Functional assays characterizing the kinetic properties of individual TAS2R variants in our sample were successful in resolving the positions of sites shaping phenotypes, revealing complex variation in response across loci and alleles (Figs 6–8; S2 Table). Within each haplotype block implicated in associations, we found at least one locus harboring functionally polymorphic alleles corresponding to receptor variants activated in the concentration range perceived as bitter by subjects, thus pinpointing the sites underlying variable perception of four tastants. However, the mechanisms underlying variation in sensitivity varied from locus to locus. At TAS2R30, the high- and low-sensitivity receptor variants, which differed drastically in response to amarogentin and quassin, were distinguished by a single amino acid position (L252F) in the third extracellular domain of the receptor, which is hypothesized to interact directly with agonists. High- and low-sensitivity TAS2R46 variants also differed at a single amino acid position (L228M); however, this site, located in the sixth transmembrane domain of the receptor, is highly conserved among TAS2Rs and thought to be essential to the basic functionality of the receptor, which likely explains the strong effects of TAS2R46-L228M on phenotype. TAS2R46 also harbored a common allele coding a premature stop codon (W250X), resulting in the production of a severely truncated, dysfunctional receptor [22]. At TAS2R43, the high- and low-sensitivity receptor variants differed at position W35S in the first intracellular domain and H212R in the fifth transmembrane domain; by considering the high degree of sequence similarity between TAS2R43 and TAS2R31, the sole presence of amino acid substitution W35S, corresponding to the deleterious substitution TAS2R31-R35W, likely explains the severe impaired functionality of the receptor [26]. Beyond these substitutions, TAS2R43 harboured a high-frequency whole-gene deletion allele completely lacking an open reading frame, and unable to produce protein at all [25, 26].
In addition to identifying simple phenotypic associations with alleles at single loci, our analyses revealed associations arising from linkage disequilibrium across loci, demonstrating the complexity of relationships between TAS2R variation and phenotypic response. In the case of amarogentin and quassin, several loci (TAS2R30, -43, and -46) harboured both high- and low-sensitivity alleles, suggesting that loci could individually contribute up- or downward shifts in phenotype. However, only TAS2R30 harboured receptor variants responsive across the threshold ranges of subjects, indicating that it alone accounts for most variability in perception. This relationship is similar to long known patterns of bitter sensitivity driven by strong, single-gene effects. In the case of grosheimin, two loci (TAS2R43 and -46) harboured both high- and low-sensitivity alleles. However, these were maintained in the same linkage phase such that the sensitive allele of TAS2R43 was linked with the sensitive allele of TAS2R46 and the insensitive allele of TAS2R43 was linked with the insensitive allele of TAS2R46. Thus, while both loci harboured variation able to explain observed phenotypes, their contributions were strongly correlated. In the case of absinthin and cascarillin, again two loci (TAS2R30 and -46) harboured both high- and low-sensitivity alleles. However, linkage disequilibrium maintained these in opposite phase such that the sensitive allele of TAS2R30 was linked with the insensitive allele of TAS2R46, and the insensitive allele of TAS2R30 was linked with the sensitive allele of TAS2R46. Thus, most subjects carried at least one allele sensitive to absinthin and cascarillin, explaining both the low mean threshold response to these compounds in subjects and the weak statistical associations for individual loci. These findings, together with prior evidence of extensive overlap in sensitivity across loci and agonists, suggest that while strong associations between a single TAS2R locus and phenotype may occur, they are likely uncommon. Moreover, linkage between TAS2R loci can cause confounds resulting in both false positive and false negative results in association analyses. Thus, dissecting genetic effects on bitter taste sensitivity through association analysis alone is likely to be inaccurate in most situations.
An essential aspect of TAS2R diversity in our sample of the Caucasian population, which has been broadly observed in population genetic studies, was that diversity is extremely high. In total we identified 93 SNPs, of which 67 SNPs were common, with frequencies above 5% (Fig 3). Moreover, every subject harboured a different allele combination at the SNP positions. Thus, inherited variation in taste responses was not a rarity as is the case for many phenotypes, such as diseases, but the norm. Further, our European sample, though ethnically homogeneous, captured most TAS2R variation found to date in worldwide populations [22, 40]. For example, beyond the 93 SNPs found in our subjects, Kim et al. (2005) reported only 10 additional common SNPs (5 synonymous and 5 non-synonymous) across a panel of 55 Africans, Asians, Europeans, and Native Americans. Hence, differences at TAS2R loci between individuals from ethnically diverse populations are modest in comparison to differences among individuals from the same population, consistent with more general observations on large-scale data sets [49, 50]. These patterns suggest that the association trends in our data are likely not restricted to Europeans, but relevant in most populations.
Yet, even if receptor polymorphism and genomic structure dominantly shape bitter taste perception, further studies need to be performed to enable a complete comprehension of variation in bitter taste perception, taking into account that other relevant factors may also play a significant role. Indeed, besides receptor-agonist interactions, differences in taste receptor gene expression levels could also contribute to individual differences in bitter taste perception. The importance of polymorphisms in the putative promoter regions of taste genes further indicates overlapping genetic influences on receptor expression and functionality [51, 52]. In addition, differences at peripheral level, e.g. in taste signaling cascade components [53], may also influence taste perception, as well as differences in signal transmission by afferent taste nerves and signal processing at central level. Hormones may also, at the level of the individual, impact bitter taste perception, e.g. hunger-satiety hormones as well as sex hormones (for review see [54, 55]).
Nonetheless, deciphering receptor activation patterns and linkage structure among TAS2R genes is an important prerequisite to establish a solid basis to assess bitter taste variations in the population. This may pave the way to evaluate the consequences of these variations in food rejection and ultimately help to improve public health.
Material and Methods
Ethics statement
This work was conducted in accordance to the Declaration of Helsinki on Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Potsdam (Germany) through decision 11 / 27. Session / 2009. All participants gave written informed consent.
Subjects
The subject panel was composed of 48 unrelated Caucasian subjects (39 women, 9 men; age range 21 to 59 years, mean age 30.6 years), recruited at the German Institute of Human Nutrition Potsdam-Rehbruecke (Germany). All were pre-screened to avoid inclusion of individuals with health problems and overt taste pathologies; pregnant and breast-feeding women were also excluded. Each subject participated in the entire course of the study, which included one training session and nine experimental sessions. Visits consisted of DNA collection and psychophysical tests for genotyping and phenotypic analyses, respectively. General taste abilities were also assessed for the bitter, salty, sour, and sweet taste during the first session.
Taste compounds
Six structurally diverse bitter substances, found at low concentrations in various beverages, and known to exhibit pharmacological properties at high concentrations, were used to probe phenotypic and molecular responses (S1 Fig). These included absinthin (a dimeric sesquiterpene lactone), amarogentin (a secoiridoid glycoside), cascarillin (a diterpene lactone), grosheimin (a sesquiterpene lactone), quassin (a triterpene lactone), and quinine (a quinoline alkaloid). Absinthin, cascarillin, and grosheimin were isolated from crude vegetable material as detailed in previous studies [31, 56]. Amarogentin and quassin were purchased from Chromadex Inc. (Irvine, CA, USA), quinine hydrochloride from Sigma-Aldrich Co. (St. Louis, MO, USA). Salicin, sodium chloride, citric acid, and sucrose (Sigma-Aldrich Co.) were used as reference tastants for assessing bitter, salty, sour, and sweet perception, respectively.
Genotype analysis
Gene locations and coding sequences of the 25 TAS2R genes were obtained from genomic scaffolds 1103279188109, 1103279188381, 1103279188228, and 1103279188408 of the whole genome assembly released by the Venter Institute human reference genome as well as from all subjects in the present study [57] (Fig 1). Corresponding amino acid sequences were aligned according the modified version of the Feng-Doolittle progressive alignment algorithm (Align X, Vector NTI; Life Technologies, Carlsbad, CA, USA) [58]. Alignment was then manually adjusted on the basis of previous in silico and in vitro experiments, in order to conserve structural and functional key domains, e.g., transmembrane domains and the conserved glycosylation site [41, 42, 59]. A neighbour-joining tree with bootstrap values was then constructed from aligned sequences using Clustal X [60].
Genomic DNA was obtained from saliva samples collected using Oragene DNA self-collection kits (Oragene DNA; DNA Genotek Inc., Kanata, Canada), and purified using prepIT-L2P kits (prepIT-L2P; DNA Genotek Inc.). Complete nucleotide sequences of every TAS2R coding region were then obtained for all subjects. Locus-specific primers localized in the flanking regions of each gene, ~100 bp upstream of the start codon and ~100 bp downstream of the stop codon, were designed using the Primer-Blast tool or obtained from previous studies [22, 26, 40, 61]. Corresponding DNA fragments of at least ~1 kbp were amplified by PCR using a high-throughput polymerase (Advantage 2 polymerase mix; Takara Bio Inc., Otsu, Japan). Amplified DNA fragments were sequenced by capillary electrophoresis of both forward and reverse strands (Eurofins MWG Operon, Ebersberg, Germany). Reads were assembled and trimmed to remove low-quality sequence (Vector NTI; Life Technologies). All single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were then identified and individual genotypes were determined. Departures from the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium were tested to rule out genotyping problems.
Copy number determination
Previously reported major deletions at TAS2R43 and -45 loci, ~39k b and ~32 kb in length, respectively, were characterised by multiplex PCR reactions. These were performed using primer sets targeted within, outside, and spanning the deleted regions such that amplifications produced alternate products in deleted and non-deleted alleles (S4 Fig). Gel separation and sequencing of the resulting fragments (Advantage 2 polymerase; Takara Bio Inc.) revealed whether a subject carried zero, one or two copies of each gene.
Haplotype estimation
DNA sequence and copy-number data were jointly used to call genotypes. Haplotypes were then either directly ascertained for homozygous individuals or inferred using PHASE, which utilizes Bayesian algorithms to resolve haplotypes in heterozygous individuals [62, 63]. In cases of uncertain phase, haplotypes were confirmed by comparison with previously published data or identified by cDNA cloning and sequence analysis [26]. Linkage disequilibrium measures were then obtained by calculating D’, r2 and corresponding p-values between multi-allelic loci for both SNPs and genes [64, 65]. Haplotype block partitions were generated according to the four gamete rule with a 5% cut-off score, and manually adjusted for TAS2R19 and TAS2R42, which SNPs spanned several blocks [66, 67]. Corresponding long-range haplotypes were then inferred [62, 63]. Algorithms for visual representation were specifically implemented in Matlab, based on the population genetics and evolution toolbox (Matlab; The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA) [68].
Phenotype analysis
Experiments were conducted at the sensory analysis laboratory of the German Institute of Human Nutrition Potsdam-Rehbruecke, according to good practice guidelines [69, 70]. Detection and recognition thresholds were assessed using a procedure adapted from the norm ISO 13301:2002 of the International Organization for Standardization: a four-alternative ascending forced-choice procedure, followed by yes-no questions about the bitter taste quality of the quoted samples [71]. Perceived bitter taste intensities were rated on the general Labelled Magnitude Scale (gLMS; [72–74]). Concentration series consisted of geometric sequences of twelve steps, with a 1.5 common ratio and the following concentration ranges: 2.3x10-8–2.0x10-6M, 3.5x10-9–3.0x10-7 M, 4.6x10-7–4.0x10-5 M, 5.8x10-7–5.0x10-5 M, 1.2x10-8–1.0x10-6 M, 4.6x10-7–4.0x10-5 M, for absinthin, amarogentin, cascarillin, grosheimin, quassin, and quinine, respectively. For each 4-AFC test, four coded samples containing 10 ml solution were presented simultaneously; one containing the bitter tastant diluted in mineral water (Evian; Danone, Paris, France) and three containing mineral water only. At each concentration, subjects were challenged to identify the different sample, specify whether the quoted sample tasted bitter, and rate the perceived bitter intensity. Tests were performed with nose clips and oral rinsing, with a 45 s pause between concentrations. Training sessions were first used to familiarize subjects with the experimental procedures, and secondly, to assess general taste abilities. Concentration series consisted of geometric sequences of six steps, with a 1.5 common ratio and the following concentration ranges: 3.5x10-5–2.0x10-3M, 6.9x10-4–4.0x10-2 M, 2.6x10-4–1.5x10-2 M, 6.9x10-4–4.0x10-2 M, for salicin, sodium chloride, citric acid, and sucrose used as reference tastants for bitter, salty, sour, and sweet taste, respectively. Following training, subjects tested each tastant in triplicate over nine sessions. Latin square designs extended for first-order carry-over effects were used to counterbalance presentation order of the test compounds over the test sessions, across subjects and for each test compound, as well as presentation order of the samples at each concentration across both repetitions and subjects. Sensory sessions were monitored and data automatically collected (Fizz; Biosystèmes, Couternon, France).
Detection and recognition probabilities of the bitter samples were analysed per repetition and subject. Relationships between probabilities (detection or recognition probabilities) and concentrations were fitted by a logistic regression model using the maximum-likelihood method. Threshold value and slope of the logistic curves were then obtained per repetition and subject, using a self-implemented toolbox (Matlab; The MathWorks Inc.). No repetition effect was observed. Where required, outliers were discarded according to the Peirce’s criterion [75, 76]. Perceived intensities, expressed in percentage of the scale length, were analysed similarly. Concentrations were subsequently inferred for weak, moderate, strong, and very strong intensities, corresponding respectively to 6, 17, 34.7, and 52.5% of the scale length.
Genotype-phenotype association analyses
General linear mixed model analyses were performed at SNP, gene, and LD block level (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Concentrations corresponding to detection thresholds, recognition thresholds, or perceived intensities were treated as dependent variables following a log-normal distribution. Genotype or haplotype with frequencies above 0.05 were used as independent variables and subjects as random variables. For analyses at the SNP level, probability values were assessed at an uncorrected significance level of 0.05, and at an experiment-wide significance threshold required to keep a significance level of 0.05, thus correcting for multiple comparisons [77]. For analyses at gene-specific level and LD block level, a Bonferroni correction for multiple corrections was applied.
Functional analyses of TAS2R variants
Allelic responses to agonists were quantified using in vitro heterologous expression assays designed in previous studies of TAS2R receptor function, which successfully mimic responses in vivo with respect to both magnitude and concentration range [17, 23, 25, 26, 46]. DNA fragments containing TAS2R coding sequences were first amplified from genomic DNA by PCR using a proofreading polymerase (PfuUltra II Fusion HotStart DNA Polymerase; Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) and cloned into a plasmid vector according to the manufacturer’s protocol (Zero Blunt TOPO PCR Cloning Kit; Life Technologies). A second PCR was performed using specific cloning primers to facilitate subcloning into the expression vector, which was then modified to add an N-terminal signal for cell surface localisation and a C-terminal epitope for immunocytochemical detection to the receptor sequence (Fast Link DNA Ligation Kit; Epicentre, Illumina Inc., Madison, WI, USA) (pcDNA5/FRT Mammalian Expression Vector; Life Technologies). Empty vector was used as a negative control [23, 31].
Functional assays were carried out in HEK 293T cells stably expressing the G protein chimera Gα16gust44 and transiently transfected with TAS2R alleles subcloned into the expression vector [78]. Calcium imaging was performed using an automated fluorometric imaging plate reader by exposing transfected cells to test compounds dissolved in assay buffer or to assay buffer alone (FLIPR Tetra; Molecular Devices, LLC, Sunnyvale, CA, USA). Changes in cytosolic calcium levels were monitored by measuring fluorescence intensity of a calcium-sensitive dye previously added (Fluo-4 AM; Life Technologies). Six replicates were carried out for each bitter stimulus, on separate experimental days, with each replicate consisting of concentration series of each bitter tastant.
Prior to final analysis, fluorescence data from functional assays, expressed in relative fluorescence units (RFU), underwent three corrections. First, a correction calculated from baseline values was applied to compensate for well-to-well fluctuations. A second correction calculated from negative control values was applied to correct for receptor independent artefacts and signal drift. A third correction calculated from positive control values was applied to facilitate comparison of data obtained from different experimental days. Fluorescence ratios ((F-F0)/F0), obtained by subtracting the background fluorescence from fluorescence peak height and then dividing the difference by the background fluorescence, were then used for data analysis. Variance analyses were performed, followed by Bonferroni multiple comparisons tests (SPSS 20; IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Threshold response values were then defined as the first concentration eliciting a significant activation of the receptor, with empty vector acting as a negative control. Finally, dose-response curves were fitted to the Hill equation by nonlinear regression in order to determine half maximal effective concentrations (EC50) and maximal amplitude [79] (SigmaPlot; Systat Software Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Prof. Giovanni Appendino for kindly supplying bitter tastants, Dr. Anne Brockhoff for providing expert advice on the calcium imaging technique, and Mrs. Renate Schröder and Mrs. Stefanie Schultz for their valuable technical assistance.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.
References
- 1. Berridge KC. Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000; 24(2): 173–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Pfaffmann C, Norgren R. Sensory affect and motivation. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1977; 290(1): 18–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Rosenstein D, Oster H. Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Dev. 1988; 59: 1555–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Schwartz C, Issanchou S, Nicklaus S. Developmental changes in the acceptance of the five basic tastes in the first year of life. Br J Nutr. 2009; 102(9): 1375–85. 10.1017/S0007114509990286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Steiner JE. Facial expressions of the neonate infant indicating the hedonics of foodrelated chemical stimuli In: Weiffenbach JM, editor. Taste and Development: the genesis of sweet preference. Washington DC, USA: Government Printing Office; 1977. p. 173–89. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Brown PD, Morra MJ. Control of Soil-Borne Plant Pests Using Glucosinolate-Containing Plants In: Donald LS, editor. Advances in Agronomy. Volume 61 New York, USA: Academic Press; 1997. p. 167–231. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Garcia J, Hankins WG. The evolution of bitter and the acquisition of toxiphobia. In: Denton DA, Coghlan JP, editors. Proceedings of the 5th International Symposium on Olfaction and Taste. Melbourne, Australia: Academic Press; 1975. p. 39.
- 8. Mauricio R, Rausher MD. Experimental manipulation of putative selective agents provides evidence for the role of natural enemies in the evolution of plant defense. Evolution. 1997; 51(5): 1435–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Whittaker RH, Feeny PP. Allelochemics: Chemical Interactions between Species. Science. 1971; 171(3973): 757–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Glendinning JI. Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? Physiol Behav. 1994; 56(6): 1217–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Koshimizu K, Ohigashi H, Huffman MA. Use of Veronia-amygdalina by wild chimpanzee—possible roles of its bitter and related constituents. Physiol Behav. 1994; 56(6): 1209–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Faiz MA, Bin Yunus E, Rahman MR, Islam F, Hoque MG, et al. ; South East Asian Quinine Artesunate Malaria Trial (SEAQUAMAT) group. Artesunate versus quinine for treatment of severe falciparum malaria: a randomised trial. Lancet. 2005; 366(9487): 717–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Drewnowski A, Gomez-Carneros C. Bitter taste, phytonutrients, and the consumer: a review. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000; 72(6): 1424–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Tepper BJ. Nutritional Implications of Genetic Taste Variation: The Role of PROP Sensitivity and Other Taste Phenotypes. Annu Rev Nutr. 2008; 28(1): 367–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Ventura AK, Worobey J. Early Influences on the Development of Food Preferences. Curr Biol. 2013; 23(9): R401–R8. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Adler E, Hoon MA, Mueller KL, Chandrashekar J, Ryba NJP, Zuker CS. A novel family of mammalian taste receptors. Cell. 2000; 100(6): 693–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Bufe B, Breslin PAS, Kuhn C, Reed DR, Tharp CD, Slack JP, et al. The molecular basis of individual differences in phenylthiocarbamide and propylthiouracil bitterness perception. Curr Biol. 2005; 15(4): 322–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Chandrashekar J, Mueller KL, Hoon MA, Adler E, Feng LX, Guo W, et al. T2Rs function as bitter taste receptors. Cell. 2000; 100(6): 703–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Matsunami H, Montmayeur JP, Buck LB. A family of candidate taste receptors in human and mouse. Nature. 2000; 404(6778): 601–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Shi P, Zhang JZ, Yang H, Zhang YP. Adaptive diversification of bitter taste receptor genes in mammalian evolution. Mol Biol Evol. 2003; 20(5): 805–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Kim UK, Jorgenson E, Coon H, Leppert M, Risch N, Drayna D. Positional cloning of the human quantitative trait locus underlying taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide. Science. 2003; 299(5610): 1221–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Kim U, Wooding S, Ricci D, Jorde LB, Drayna D. Worldwide haplotype diversity and coding sequence variation at human bitter taste receptor loci. Hum Mutat. 2005; 26(3): 199–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Bufe B, Hofmann T, Krautwurst D, Raguse JD, Meyerhof W. The human TAS2R16 receptor mediates bitter taste in response to beta-glucopyranosides. Nat Genet. 2002; 32(3): 397–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Dotson CD, Zhang L, Xu H, Shin YK, Vigues S, Ott SH, et al. Bitter Taste Receptors Influence Glucose Homeostasis. PLoS One. 2008; 3(12). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Pronin AN, Xu H, Tang H, Zhang L, Li Q, Li X. Specific Alleles of Bitter Receptor Genes Influence Human Sensitivity to the Bitterness of Aloin and Saccharin. Curr Biol. 2007; 17(16): 1403–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Roudnitzky N, Bufe B, Thalmann S, Kuhn C, Gunn HC, Xing C, et al. Genomic, genetic and functional dissection of bitter taste responses to artificial sweeteners. Hum Mol Genet. 2011; 20(17): 3437–49. 10.1093/hmg/ddr252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Soranzo N, Bufe B, Sabeti PC, Wilson JF, Weale ME, Marguerie R, et al. Positive selection on a high-sensitivity allele of the human bitter-taste receptor TAS2R16. Curr Biol. 2005; 15(14): 1257–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Allen AL, McGeary JE, Hayes JE. Polymorphisms in TRPV1 and TAS2Rs Associate with Sensations from Sampled Ethanol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014; 38(10): 2550–60. 10.1111/acer.12527 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Dotson CD, Wallace MR, Bartoshuk LM, Logan HL. Variation in the Gene TAS2R13 is Associated with Differences in Alcohol Consumption in Patients with Head and Neck Cancer. Chem Senses. 2012; 37(8): 737–44. 10.1093/chemse/bjs063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Risso D, Morini G, Pagani L, Quagliariello A, Giuliani C, De Fanti S, et al. Genetic signature of differential sensitivity to stevioside in the Italian population. Genes and Nutrition. 2014; 9(3). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Meyerhof W, Batram C, Kuhn C, Brockhoff A, Chudoba E, Bufe B, et al. The Molecular Receptive Ranges of Human TAS2R Bitter Taste Receptors. Chem Senses. 2010; 35(2): 157–70. 10.1093/chemse/bjp092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Ledda M, Kutalik Z, Souza Destito MC, Souza MM, Cirillo CA, Zamboni A, et al. GWAS of human bitter taste perception identifies new loci and reveals additional complexity of bitter taste genetics. Hum Mol Genet. 2014; 23(1): 259–67. 10.1093/hmg/ddt404 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Reed DR, Zhu G, Breslin PAS, Duke FF, Henders AK, Campbell MJ, et al. The perception of quinine taste intensity is associated with common genetic variants in a bitter receptor cluster on chromosome 12 Hum Mol Genet. 2010; 19(21): 4278–85. 10.1093/hmg/ddq324 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Hayes JE, Wallace MR, Knopik VS, Herbstman DM, Bartoshuk LM, Duffy VB. Allelic Variation in TAS2R Bitter Receptor Genes Associates with Variation in Sensations from and Ingestive Behaviors toward Common Bitter Beverages in Adults. Chem Senses. 2011; 36(3): 311–9. 10.1093/chemse/bjq132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.International Standing Committee on Human Cytogenetic Nomenclature. ISCN 2009: An International System for Human Cytogenetic Nomenclature. 1st ed. Shaffer LG, Slovak ML, Campbell LJ, editors. Basel, Switzerland: Karger; 2009. 138 p.
- 36. Altshuler DM, Durbin RM, Abecasis GR, Bentley DR, Chakravarti A, Clark AG et al. ; 1000 Genomes Project Consortium. An integrated map of genetic variation from 1,092 human genomes. Nature. 2012; 491(7422): 56–65. 10.1038/nature11632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Campbell MC, Ranciaro A, Zinshteyn D, Rawlings-Goss R, Hirbo J, Thompson S, et al. Origin and Differential Selection of Allelic Variation at TAS2R16 Associated with Salicin Bitter Taste Sensitivity in Africa. Mol Biol Evol. 2013; 31(2): 288–302. 10.1093/molbev/mst211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Altshuler D, Brooks LD, Chakravarti A, Collins FS, Daly MJ, Donnelly P et al. ; International Haplotype Map (HapMap) Consortium. A haplotype map of the human genome. Nature. 2005; 437(7063): 1299–320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Thalmann S, Behrens M, Meyerhof W. Major haplotypes of the human bitter taste receptor TAS2R41 encode functional receptors for chloramphenicol. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2013; 435(2): 267–73. 10.1016/j.bbrc.2013.04.066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Wooding S, Kim UK, Bamshad MJ, Larsen J, Jorde LB, Drayna D. Natural selection and molecular evolution in PTC, a bitter-taste receptor gene. Am J Hum Genet. 2004; 74(4): 637–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Born S, Levit A, Niv MY, Meyerhof W, Behrens M. The Human Bitter Taste Receptor TAS2R10 Is Tailored to Accommodate Numerous Diverse Ligands. J Neurosci. 2013; 33(1): 201–13. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3248-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Brockhoff A, Behrens M, Niv MY, Meyerhof W. Structural requirements of bitter taste receptor activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010; 107(24): 11110–5. 10.1073/pnas.0913862107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Campa D, De Rango F, Carrai M, Crocco P, Montesanto A, Canzian F, et al. Bitter Taste Receptor Polymorphisms and Human Aging. PLoS One. 2012; 7(11): e45232 10.1371/journal.pone.0045232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Wall JD, Pritchard JK. Haplotype blocks and linkage disequilibrium in the human genome. Nat Rev Genet. 2003; 4(8): 587–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Altshuler D, Brooks LD, Chakravarti A, Collins FS, Daly MJ, Donnelly P et al. ; International Haplotype Map (HapMap) Consortium. A second generation human haplotype map of over 3.1 million SNPs. Nature. 2007; 449(7164): 851–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Wooding S, Gunn H, Ramos P, Thalmann S, Xing C, Meyerhof W. Genetics and Bitter Taste Responses to Goitrin, a Plant Toxin Found in Vegetables. Chem Senses. 2010; 35(8): 685–92. 10.1093/chemse/bjq061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Wooding S. Phenylthiocarbamide: A 75-Year Adventure in Genetics and Natural Selection. Genetics. 2006; 172(4): 2015–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Hayes JE, Feeney EL, Allen AL. Do polymorphisms in chemosensory genes matter for human ingestive behavior? Food Quality and Preference. 2013; 30(2): 202–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Stephens JC, Schneider JA, Tanguay DA, Choi J, Acharya T, Stanley SE, et al. Haplotype Variation and Linkage Disequilibrium in 313 Human Genes. Science. 2001; 293(5529): 489–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Witherspoon DJ, Wooding S, Rogers AR, Marchani EE, Watkins WS, Batzer MA, et al. Genetic Similarities Within and Between Human Populations. Genetics. 2007; 176(1): 351–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Fushan AA, Simons CT, Slack JP, Manichaikul A, Drayna D. Allelic Polymorphism within the TAS1R3 Promoter Is Associated with Human Taste Sensitivity to Sucrose. Curr Biol. 2009; 19(15): 1288–93. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Lipchock SV, Mennella JA, Spielman AI, Reed DR. Human bitter perception correlates with bitter receptor messenger RNA expression in taste cells. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013; 98(4): 1136–43. 10.3945/ajcn.113.066688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Fushan A, Simons C, Slack J, Drayna D. Association between common variation in genes encoding sweet taste signaling components and human sucrose perception. Chem Senses. 2010; 35(7): 579–92. 10.1093/chemse/bjq063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Loper HB, La Sala M, Dotson C, Steinle N. Taste perception, associated hormonal modulation, and nutrient intake. Nutr Rev. 2015; 73(2): 83–91. 10.1093/nutrit/nuu009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Faas MM, Melgert BN, de Vos P. A Brief Review on How Pregnancy and Sex Hormones Interfere with Taste and Food Intake. Chemosens Percept. 2010; 3(1): 51–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Brockhoff A, Behrens M, Massarotti A, Appendino G, Meyerhof W. Broad tuning of the human bitter taste receptor hTAS2R46 to various sesquiterpene lactones, clerodane and labdane diterpenoids, strychnine, and denatonium. J Agric Food Chem. 2007; 55(15): 6236–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Levy S, Sutton G, Ng PC, Feuk L, Halpern AL, Walenz BP, et al. The Diploid Genome Sequence of an Individual Human. PLoS Biol. 2007; 5(10): 2113–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Feng DF, Doolittle RF. Progressive sequence alignment as a prerequisitetto correct phylogenetic trees. J Mol Evol. 1987; 25(4): 351–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Reichling C, Meyerhof W, Behrens M. Functions of human bitter taste receptors depend on N-glycosylation. J Neurochem. 2008; 106(3): 1138–48. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05453.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Jeanmougin F, Thompson JD, Gouy M, Higgins DG, Gibson TJ. Multiple sequence alignment with Clustal X. Trends Biochem Sci. 1998; 23(10): 403–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Ye J, Coulouris G, Zaretskaya I, Cutcutache I, Rozen S, Madden T. Primer-BLAST: A tool to design target-specific primers for polymerase chain reaction. BMC Bioinform. 2012; 13(1): 134–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Stephens M, Donnelly P. A Comparison of Bayesian Methods for Haplotype Reconstruction from Population Genotype Data. Am J Hum Genet. 2003; 73(5): 1162–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Stephens M, Smith NJ, Donnelly P. A new statistical method for haplotype reconstruction from population data. Am J Hum Genet. 2001; 68(4): 978–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Weir BS, Cockerham CC. Testing hypotheses about linkage disequilibrium with multiple alleles. Genetics. 1978; 88(3): 633–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Zaykin DV, Pudovkin A, Weir BS. Correlation-Based Inference for Linkage Disequilibrium With Multiple Alleles. Genetics. 2008; 180(1): 533–45. 10.1534/genetics.108.089409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Barrett JC, Fry B, Maller J, Daly MJ. Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics. 2005; 21(2): 263–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Wang N, Akey JM, Zhang K, Chakraborty R, Jin L. Distribution of Recombination Crossovers and the Origin of Haplotype Blocks: The Interplay of Population History, Recombination, and Mutation. Am J Hum Genet. 2002; 71(5): 1227–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Cai JJ. PGEToolbox: A Matlab Toolbox for Population Genetics and Evolution. J Hered. 2008; 99(4): 438–40. 10.1093/jhered/esm127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.ISO 6658:2005. Sensory analysis—Methodology—General guidance. Vernier, Geneva (Switzerland): International Organization for Standardization; 2005. p. 1–20.
- 70. Lawless HT, Heymann H. Sensory Evaluation of Food: Principles and Practices: Springer US; 1999. 819 p. [Google Scholar]
- 71.ISO 13301:2002. Sensory analysis—Methodology—General guidance for measuring odour, flavour and taste detection thresholds by a three-alternative forced-choice (3-AFC) procedure. Vernier, Geneva (Switzerland): International Organization for Standardization; 2005. p. 1–27.
- 72. Green BG, Dalton P, Cowart B, Shaffer G, Rankin K, Higgins J. Evaluating the ‘Labeled Magnitude Scale’ for Measuring Sensations of Taste and Smell. Chem Senses. 1996; 21(3): 323–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Green BG, Shaffer GS, Gilmore MM. Derivation and evaluation of a semantic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties. Chem Senses. 1993; 18(6): 683–702. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Bartoshuk LM, Duffy VB, Green BG, Hoffman HJ, Ko CW, Lucchina LA, et al. Valid across-group comparisons with labeled scales: the gLMS versus magnitude matching. Physiol Behav. 2004; 82(1): 109–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Gould BA. On Peirce's Criterion for the Rejection of Doubtful Observations, with tables for facilitating its application. Astron J. 1855; 4(83): 81–7. [Google Scholar]
- 76. Peirce B. Criterion for the rejection of doubtful observations. Astron J. 1852; 2(45): 161–3. [Google Scholar]
- 77. Nyholt DR. A Simple Correction for Multiple Testing for Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms in Linkage Disequilibrium with Each Other. Am J Hum Genet. 2004; 74(4): 765–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Ueda T, Ugawa S, Yamamura H, Imaizumi Y, Shimada S. Functional Interaction between T2R Taste Receptors and G-Protein α Subunits Expressed in Taste Receptor Cells. J Neurosci. 2003; 23(19): 7376–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Hill AV. The possible effects of the aggregation of the molecules of haemoglobin on its dissociation curves. J Physiol. 1910; 40(Suppl): iv–vii. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.