Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Personal Disord. 2015 May 25;6(4):336–346. doi: 10.1037/per0000125

Mapping the association of global executive functioning onto diverse measures of psychopathic traits

Arielle R Baskin-Sommers 1, Inti A Brazil 2,3, Jonathan Ryan 1, Nathaniel J Kohlenberg 4, Craig S Neumann 5, Joseph P Newman 4
PMCID: PMC4593757  NIHMSID: NIHMS723405  PMID: 26011576

Abstract

Psychopathic individuals display a callous-coldhearted approach to interpersonal and affective situations and engage in impulsive and antisocial behaviors. Despite early conceptualizations suggesting that psychopathy is related to enhanced cognitive functioning, research examining executive functioning (EF) in psychopathy has yielded few such findings. It is possible that some psychopathic trait dimensions are more related to EF than others. Research using a two-factor or four-facet model of psychopathy highlights some dimension-specific differences in EF, but this research is limited in scope. Another complicating factor in teasing apart the EF-psychopathy relationship is the tendency to use different psychopathy assessments for incarcerated versus community samples. In this study, an EF battery and multiple measures of psychopathic dimensions were administered to a sample of male prisoners (N=377). Results indicate that using the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R), the independent effect of Factor2 was related to worse EF, but neither the independent effect of Factor1 nor the unique variance of the Factors (1 or 2) were related to EF. Using a four facet model, the independent effects of Facet2 (Affect) and Facet4 (Antisocial) were related to worse EF, but when examining the unique effects, only Facet2 remained significant. Finally, the questionnaire-based measure, Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire-Brief, of Fearless Dominance was related to better EF performance, whereas PCL-R Factor1 was unrelated to EF. Overall, the results reveal the complex relationship among EF and behaviors characteristic of psychopathy-related dimensions. Moreover, they demonstrate the interpersonal and affective traits measured by these distinct assessments are differentially related to EF.

Keywords: Psychopathic Traits, Interpersonal-Affective, Fearless Dominance, Executive Function


The dynamic control of behavior occurs through cognitive processes that encompass executive functioning (EF), which involves task monitoring, rule learning, response inhibition, and planning. Conventional wisdom highlights the importance of a person’s ability to exert EF in order to regulate the expression of violent behavior, inappropriate drug use, harmful antisocial behavior, shortsighted reward seeking, as well as engage in prosocial behaviors, such as complex social interactions (e.g., making inferences about others behaviors and preferences) (Rueda, Posner & Rothbart, 2005). Not surprisingly, when individuals engage in behaviors leading to unfavorable outcomes, EF dysfunctions are commonly implicated in these behavioral disturbances. However, a robust connection between EF and maladaptive behavior is not apparent for all subtypes of individuals displaying this type of behavior; namely, psychopathic individuals. Psychopathic individuals display a callous-coldhearted approach to interpersonal and affective situations and chronically engage in impulsive and antisocial behaviors (Cleckley, 1941). However, results are equivocal with regard to the relationship between deficits in EF and psychopathy (Hart, Forth & Hare, 1990; Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Smith, Arnett & Newman, 1992).

While there is little support for a generalized EF deficit in psychopathy, deficits within the EF components of inhibition and rule learning have been reported (Bagshaw, Gray & Snowden, 2014; Fisher & Blair, 1998; Newman & Howland, 1987; Snowden, Gray, Pugh, & Atkinson, 2013). These findings are further supported by results from meta-analyses; but such deficits appear ultimately related to antisocial behavior, more broadly, and therefore are not unique to psychopathy (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Ogilvie, Stewart, Chan & Shum, 2011). Moreover, though there is evidence for psychopathy-related deficits in inhibition and rule learning, there is also evidence of non-significant or negligible relationships between these components of EF and psychopathy (Dolan, 2012; Sellbom & Verona, 2007; see also Maes & Brazil for review, 2013). There are a number of factors that may contribute to the inconsistency in the research findings on EF in psychopathy. Two such factors may be how psychopathy is operationalized across studies (e.g., a single broad syndrome versus lower-level trait dimensions) and the selection of measures that are utilized for each study (e.g., interview versus questionnaire). In an attempt to reconcile some of the methodological issues that plague research on EF as a function of psychopathy, the present study examined the relationship between separate trait dimensions of psychopathy and compared different methods of assessing these dimensions.

Psychopathic trait dimensions and executive functioning

Across studies, psychopathic individuals have demonstrated deficient, superior, and normative EF performance. Consequently, a question remains about how to understand these diverse findings. As noted above, it is possible that certain dimensions of psychopathy (i.e., specific traits) are more related to EF dysfunctions than others. The potential for specific traits to account for the EF and psychopathy relationship could mean that some relationships would be suppressed or obscured when examining psychopathy as a broad clinical syndrome. In light of this, some investigators advocate parsing psychopathy into dimensional traits so that a more nuanced assessment of relevant correlates may be identified. Because the impulsive and antisocial lifestyle symptoms apply to most forms of disinhibition, it is the interpersonal-affective trait dimensions that distinguish psychopathy from other traits and disorders (e.g., low constraint, antisocial personality disorder). The existence of these distinguishing trait dimensions contributes to the common conceptualization of psychopathy through a lens of dual characteristics: one that captures the interpersonal-affective trait dimensions and the other that captures the impulsive-antisocial trait dimensions of psychopathy.

Such dual-trait conceptualizations of psychopathy are predicated on the two-factor model of Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare et al., 1990). Here, Factor1 represents the distinguishing interpersonal (charm, grandiosity, and deceitfulness) and affective (lack of remorse, empathy, and emotional depth) traits of psychopathy, which reflect low anxiety and deficient emotion processing (Neumann, Johansson & Hare, 2013; Patrick, 2007). In contrast, Factor2 describes the impulsive and chronic antisocial tendencies associated with psychopathy that are attributed to a deficit in behavioral inhibition and control (Hare & Neumann, 2010). Broadly speaking, research distinguishing these Factors in terms of EF seems to indicate superior performance is primarily related to Factor1, while inferior EF is associated with Factor2 (Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989; Patrick, 2007; Sadeh & Verona, 2008). However, a recent review pointed out that there is little support for a generalized impairment in EF in relation to the two Factors of psychopathy (Maes & Brazil, 2013).

Following the logic that parsing psychopathy into trait dimensions affords a detailed understanding of the complex EF-behavior relationships, some investigators have proposed that further division of the Factors into facets (Facet1: Interpersonal; Facet2: Affective; Facet3: Lifestyle; Facet4: Antisocial) may lead to an even more specific understanding of these traits and related correlates (Hare & Neumann, 2008; Neumann, Hare, & Pardini, 2014; Neumann & Pardini, 2014). Each of these facets represents a cluster of related symptoms with the potential for different etiological correlates. Interpersonal traits reflect a tendency to engage in impression management, to be grandiose, to use pathological lying, and to be conning and manipulative; affective traits tap the individuals propensity towards a lack of remorse, shallow affect, callousness, and a failure to accept responsibility; the lifestyle traits relate to stimulation seeking, impulsivity, irresponsibility, a parasitic orientation, and a lack of realistic goals; and, the antisocial traits reflect poor anger control, early behavior problems, serious criminal behavior, violation of conditional release, and criminal versatility (Hare & Neumann, 2008; Vitacco, Neumann, Caldwell, Leistico, & Van Rybroek, 2006).

Currently, only a handful of studies examined the relationship between psychopathy facets and EF, with Facet 2 (Affective) relating to deficient inhibition (Feilhauer, Cima, Korebrits & Kunert, 2012; c.f., Sadeh & Verona, 2008) and rule learning (Mahmut, Homewood & Stevenson, 2008). Along with Facet 2, Facet 4 (Antisocial) has been related to deficient inhibition (Feilhauer et al. 2012). Despite these results, there are inconsistent results with regard to EF, including within inhibition and rule learning (see Maes & Brazil, 2013 for review). The dimensional models represented by the Factors and facet approach to psychopathy may provide a means to understand the relationship between specific traits and functions; but, the transition away from a broad syndromal representation to the dimensional trait level creates an issue related to how those traits are operationalized and measured across samples. Thus, the issue of mode of measurement may further influence the complex relationship between psychopathic dimensions and EF performance.

Psychopathic trait dimensions and issues of measurement

Early work with the PCL-R was conducted in correctional settings, given the high prevalence of psychopathy in offender samples. However, the basic construct of psychopathy and its replicable two-factor structure has been extended to other populations, including community and undergraduate samples (e.g., Seara-Cardoso & Viding, 2014). Using non-incarcerated offender populations necessitated a shift in assessment, since the PCL-R relies on the availability of collateral information, such as criminal records. Consequently, a number of self-report questionnaire measures emerged, purporting to capture psychopathic trait dimensions in non-offender samples. The Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ) uses scales to estimate the factors of psychopathy within a broad inventory of normal personality functioning (Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen, & Krueger, 2003; Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, Hicks, & Iacono, 2005). Specifically, MPQ subscales, Fearless Dominance (FD) and Impulsive-Antisociality (IA), which were a derivative of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996), supposedly map onto PCL Factor1 and Factor2 dimensions and relevant external correlates, respectively (c.f., Neumann, Malterer & Newman, 2008; Malterer, Lilienfeld. Neumann & Newman, 2010; Lynam & Miller, 2012). The FD domain, like Factor1, tends to be positively related to sociability, narcissism, and adventure seeking, but negatively related to internalizing disorders and fearfulness (Benning et al., 2005; Hicks & Patrick, 2006). By contrast, IA is positively correlated with externalizing disorders, impulsivity, trait anxiety, but negatively correlated with socialization. While, the development of different assessments for psychopathic dimensions has many advantages, a controversy has emerged regarding the extent to which different measures, such as PCL-R and MPQ-derived subscales tap the same construct (Miller & Lynam, 2012).

As noted above, interpersonal-affective traits are central to the conceptualization of psychopathy. However, especially with the development of alternative assessment measures, there is a lack of consensus about what constitutes these unique traits of psychopathy. One view posits that the interpersonal-affective traits of psychopathy are associated with certain features of healthy or adaptive functioning, such as a relative lack of anxiety and fear, charm, boldness, and social poise (Lilienfeld et al., 2012). Consistent with this view, MPQ-derived measures of psychopathy containing orthogonal factors (FD and IA), are widely used with non-offender populations, and lack explicit assessment of antisociality or criminality. By contrast, another view argues that psychopathy, including the interpersonal-affective traits, is inherently linked to maladaptive features that interface with antisocial behavior (Lynam & Miller, 2012; Neumann, Hare, & Newman, 2007). Moreover, Lynam and Miller (2012) go on to suggest that FD provides an assessment of extraversion, which may have some association with traits of psychopathy, but is not an essential feature of the construct. Conversely, the PCL-R, which contains highly correlated factors that tap both interpersonal-affective and impulsive-antisocial traits (Factor1 and Factor2), is widely used with incarcerated criminal populations, but has been criticized for eliminating the “positive-adjustment features” (Skeem, Polaschek, Patrick & Lilienfeld, 2011) of psychopathy.

The divergence in the conceptualization of psychopathy broadly, and interpersonal-affective traits more specifically, has resulted in contradictory findings on key correlates of these trait dimensions. One domain where heterogeneity of the construct has lead to mixed results relates to EF. On the one hand, it makes intuitive sense that individuals with interpersonal-affective traits would display enhanced EF abilities, given their ability to exert social dominance and to manipulate other people for their own benefit. In fact, some evidence supports the proposition that these traits are associated with intact or enhanced brain, physiological and executive functioning (Carlson & Thai, 2010; Gao & Raine, 2010; Neumann & Pardini, 2014; Sellbom & Verona, 2007; see Maes & Brazil, 2013). Notably, the majority of these studies were completed using community and undergraduate samples. On the other hand, when considering interpersonal-affective traits within the context of antisociality, often within incarcerated samples, there is little evidence that Factor1 is associated with enhanced EF, and in fact, individuals high on these traits may reflect deficient EF (Bagshaw, Gray & Snowden, 2014; Gao & Raine, 2009; Mol, van Den Bos, Derks & Egger, 2009; Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Ogilvie et al., 2011). The differences in conceptualization of the phenotype and the methodology used to measure these dimensions may contribute to these contradictory findings and perspectives. Thus, the construct of interest may not only be obfuscated, but also is its relationship to important external correlates.

The Present Study

In order to begin to address these concerns, the goals of the current study were twofold. First, using a sample of incarcerated male offenders, we examined the relationship between psychopathic trait dimensions and performance on an EF battery, in order to evaluate whether EF impairments are related to specific dimensions, rather than the syndrome of psychopathy as defined by the aggregation of diverse trait dimensions. We examined the relationships the using traditional two-factor model, and also the four facet model to assess if by further parsing psychopathic trait dimensions, a more nuanced relationship with EF emerged. Second, we examined the consistency across measures of the association between psychopathic trait dimensions and performance on an EF battery. MPQ-derived FD is most often used in student samples, which can generally be regarded as a subset of high functioning individuals. Thus, based on the scale development and type of samples evaluated, it is plausible that as scores on FD increase certain adaptive features that intersect with EF also increase. In contrast, PCL-R Factor1 emerged from work in correctional settings and is often used to assess offenders; individuals that may be less educated, are in a less enriched environment, and experience profound inhibitory problems as evidenced by their incarceration. In this type of sample and based on the information used to rate interpersonal-affective traits of the PCL-R, the link to the maladaptive features of the construct may be more prominent, and consequently, there may be no, or even a negative, relationship between PCL-R Factor1 and EF.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited from medium-security correctional institutions in Wisconsin. A prescreen of institutional files and assessment materials were used to exclude individuals who had performed below the fourth-grade level on a standardized measure of reading (Wide Range Achievement Test-III; Wilkinson, 1993), who scored below 70 on a brief measure of IQ (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III; Wechsler, 1997), who had diagnoses of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or psychosis, not otherwise specified (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997), or who had a history of medical problems (e.g., uncorrectable auditory or visual deficits; head injury with loss of consciousness greater than 30 minutes) that may impact their comprehension of the materials or performance on the EF tasks. Additionally, all participants were between the ages of 18 and 45 because antisocial behavior has been found to change with advancing age (Steffensmeier, Allan, Harer & Streifel, 1989). The final sample consisted of 377 male participants (Table 1). All participants provided written informed consent according to the procedures set forth by the University of Wisconsin–Madison Human Subjects Institutional Review Board.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations (N=377)

Variable Mean Standard
Deviation
Range Correlations

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
Demographic
1. Age 30.97 6.97 27.00 -- −.08 .07 −.02 −.08 −.12* −.17* .03 −.06 −.07 −.05 −.06
2. WAIS-III 98.95 12.79 63.70 -- .59* .00 −.09 .21* .01 .38* .14* .33* .51* .56*
3. WRAT-III 45.29 6.10 33.00 -- −.05 −.13* .13* −.01 .39* .19* .19* .34* .46*
Individual Differences
4. Factor1 8.93 3.13 16.00 -- .54* .26* .29* .10 −.12* −.09 .03 −.03
5. Factor2 12.61 3.96 18.00 -- .14* .34* .07 −.02 −.19* −.02 −.07
6. Fearless
Dominance
.06 .58 2.99 -- −.12* .20* .11* .17* .18* .27*
7. Impulsive
Antisociality
−.04 .62 3.38 -- −.08 −.08 −.10 .03 −.10
Executive Function (D-KEFS)
8. Letter Fluency 9.91 3.38 17.00 -- .09 .13* .26* .62*
9. Color-Word 10.32 2.74 18.00 -- .07 .10 .53*
10. Tower 9.89 2.53 17.00 -- .23* .60*
11. Proverb 10.44 2.80 13.00 -- .67*
12. Composite 0.00 0.65 3.20 --

Note: WAIS-III= Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III; WRAT-III= Wide Range Achievement Test-III; Factor1= Psychopathy Checklist-Revised Factor1; Factor2= Psychopathy Checklist-Revised Factor2; Composite=SEM-derived composite of Color-Word, Tower and Proverb

*

p<.05

Measures

Psychopathy Checklist–Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003)

PCL-R ratings were completed using information from prison files and a semi-structured interview that lasted approximately 60 minutes. Based on information gathered from the interview and file review, the 20 items of the PCL-R were rated 0, 1, or 2, reflecting the degree to which a trait was present: significantly (2), moderately (1), or not at all (0). Early work with the PCL-R revealed a replicable two-factor structure (Hare et al., 1990) with Factor 1 items assessing Interpersonal–Affective traits (e.g., glib, callous) and Factor 2 items relating to Impulsive–Antisocial behavior (e.g., irresponsible, criminality). Using Confirmatory Factor Analysis, subsequent work also identified a four facet model of the PCL-R (Hare & Neumann, 2008; Neumann, Kosson, & Salekin, 2007). More specifically, Factor1 was separated into Interpersonal (e.g., manipulation of others, pathological lying) and Affective (e.g., callousness, shallow affect) facets, while Factor2 was separated into Lifestyle (e.g. sensation seeking, impulsivity) and Antisocial (e.g. criminality, early behavior problems) facets. Inter-rater reliability for PCL-R Factor scores based on 42 dual ratings was .94 and .91, respectively.

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire-Brief Form (MPQ-B; Patrick, Curtin, & Tellegen, 2002)

The MPQ-B is a 155 item self-report questionnaire that consists of 11 primary trait scales. The FD and IA dimensions of psychopathy are calculated as linear combinations of specific standardized (i.e., z-scored) MPQ-B primary trait scales. Specifically, FD is calculated as (.34*zSocial Potency) + (−.42*zStress Reaction) + (−.21*zHarm Avoidance). IA is calculated as (.16*zAggression) + (.31*zAlienation) + (−.13*zTraditionalism) + (−0.29*zControl) + (−.15*zSocial Closeness) (Benning et al., 2003). Prior research suggests that in prisoners, FD is selectively related to Factor1 and IA is preferentially associated with Factor2 of the PCL-R. The MPQ-B has shown good internal consistency and validity (see Patrick et al., 2002). For the present study Chronbach’s alpha for MPQ-B FD and MPQ-B IA were .98 and .83, respectively.

Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS; Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001)

The D-KEFS was developed to assess components of EF through well-established tests. Four of the eight tests from the D-KEFS were selected for this study based on their demonstrated utility in previous research to assess different aspects of executive functions (working memory, inhibition, planning and rule learning, and abstraction): Letter Fluency, Color-Word, Tower, and Proverbs tests. Each of these tests generates a summary score based on age norms, and additional contrast and percentile scores. As noted below, the use of the D-KEFS was to assess a general aptitude for regulation and problem solving (Crawford, Garthwaite, Sutherland & Borland, 2011).

EF comprises a broad set of cognitive processes such as “planning, organizational skills, selective attention and inhibitory control, and optimal cognitive-set maintenance” (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000, p. 114). Given the range of EF processes, identification of a clear factor structure of EF has been challenging, though recent research across a diversity of EF batteries suggests that several correlated factors often emerge. Furthermore, correlated EF factor domains provide evidence for a general EF factor, which may reflect a central executive function (see example with D-KEFS in Latzman, Elkovitch, Young & Clark, 2010). Evidence for a general EF factor has also been provided by Li et al. (2014) based on a sample of adolescents (N= 142, 12-18 years) who were administered the D-KEFS. Moreover, as noted by Crawford et al. (2011) the use of a general EF composite also has the advantage of providing greater reliability of EF assessment, given that the individual D-KEFS scores have lower reliability. Consistent with results using the D-KEFS, Kramer et al. (2014) examined a large diverse sample (N=1248) of adults who were administered the NIH-EXAMINER battery and also found evidence for three EF factors (control, working memory, and fluency), which could be accounted for in terms of a general EF factor. Together, these factor analytic and empirical findings are consistent with research on brain development that reveals a dynamic process of emerging interactions between different brain regions (Johnson, 2003), and thus it is reasonable to expect that different EF processes can be represented in terms of a general central EF factor.

Data Analysis

In order to evaluate the relationship between EF and psychopathic trait dimension, our analysis of the D-KEFS data occurred in two stages. The first stage involved identification of a theoretically and empirically meaningful latent EF factor(s) (see D-KEFS section above). Since there are a number of D-KEFS scaled, contrast, and percentile scores that can be used for indexing a given EF ability for each measure, there is considerable redundancy in the EF variable set. Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) has been regularly used in the EF literature given that it allows investigators to succinctly summarize a large set of observed variables, as well as isolate variables that make little contribution to a given factor (Neumann et al., 2007). In EFA, variables are allowed to freely cross-load onto any number of factors, and in the case of neuropsychological data cross-loadings may simply reflect method factors (e.g., timed tests) or redundancy in related scores.

Using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2000), exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was run using scaled, contrast, and percentile scores from the Letter Fluency, Color-Word, Tower, and Proverbs tests. We also included WAIS-IQ and WRAT-Reading to insure we were not simply uncovering a general IQ/education factor. Given that previous factor analytic research on EF assessments has found that not all EF tasks load significantly on a given factor, and that many EF scores show substantial cross-loadings on several factors (cf. Latzman & Markon, 2010), we also ran an EFA on only a subset of EF variables that loaded significantly and in a theoretically coherent manner in the initial EFA. Then, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to provide a strict test of model fit for the reduced EF variable subset identified from the EFA. In other words, CFA was conducted to test a model that specified the respective EF variables to load only onto their respective factors (i.e., no cross-loadings).

Model fit was evaluated according to several criteria: the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean-squared error of approximation (RMSEA), and the relative chi-square index (the ratio of the chi-square statistic to the degrees of freedom). The CFI, which adjusts for the degrees of freedom, compares the fit of the model against the (unstructured) null model; with scores over .90 (maximum=1) representing acceptable fit (Bentler, 1992). The RMSEA takes into account model complexity and a fit less than .05 is considered a good fit, and a fit less than .08 is acceptable. Finally, for the ratio of the chi-square statistic to the degrees of freedom, values less than 5 indicate an acceptable model (Bollen, 1989).

From the initial EFA, nine variables emerged as an indicator for one of three coherent factors (Color-Word: performance on Inhibition trials and performance on the Inhibition-Switch trials; Tower: time to first move, ratio of time per move, and ratio of accuracy per move; Proverb: performance on the common and uncommon proverbs, total accuracy on the proverbs, and accuracy on the abstract proverbs). After the initial EFA was conducted with the entire variable set and a viable subset of variables identified (i.e., those which loaded primarily onto a theoretically meaningful factor), a second EFA was conducted with the subset of EF variables, and this EFA was associated with good model fit, CFI=.95, RMSEA=.06, χ2/df (68.31/25)=2.73, for a three-factor solution.

Following this EFA, the results indicated good fit for a three-factor CFA model, (CFI=.96, RMSEA=.05, χ2/df (56.47/24)=2.35), and all of the EF variables loaded significantly onto their respective factor and the three EF factors were significantly inter-correlated (p’s<.01-.001). As it turns out, the three first-order EF factors could be loaded onto a second-order (super-ordinate) factor and this hierarchical CFA model was statistically identical to the initial CFA model with three correlated first-order factors. The equivalent super-ordinate model is referred to as an alternative equivalent model (Neumann, Vitacco, Hare & Wupperman, 2005). In this case, the super-ordinate factor provides evidence for a broad EF factor consistent with the previous literature, and moreover, that the subset of EF variables could be used to form a unidimensional composite scale, which has significant advantages when mathematically representing psychological processes (Smith, McCarthy, & Zapolski, 2009; see also Crawford et al., 2011; Latzman & Markon, 2010).

For the second stage of analysis, the results from the EFA/CFA were used to compute an EF composite measure by standardizing (z-score) each identified EF scale score (see 9 scales above) and combining the standardized measures as an arithmetic mean. Then, multiple separate regression models were run, using psychopathic traits, as continuous predictors of the EF composite. More specifically, PCL-R Factors were entered as standardized (z-score) continuous variables, both the independent and simultaneous (i.e., unique effects) models. Similar analyses were conducted using the four facets in place of the Factors. Finally, analyses were conducted to compare EF performance based on the measurement of psychopathic trait dimensions using questionnaire versus interview measures (e.g., MPQ-B versus PCL-R). For these analyses, measures of psychopathic trait dimensions were entered as continuous variables independently and simultaneously to directly compare different psychopathy measures.

Results

Sample characteristics and bivariate correlations are presented in Table 1. Below we present analyses that examine, (1) the relationship between psychopathic trait dimensions and EF, and (2) the impact of potential measurement differences for understanding the relationship between psychopathic trait dimensions and EF.

Psychopathic trait dimensions and EF performance

First, using the two-factor model, PCL-R Factor1 was negatively, but non-significantly related to EF performance, F(1,374)=2.62, Beta= −.09, p=.08, ηp2<.01 (Figure1.A). Conversely, PCL-R Factor2 was significantly and negatively related to EF performance, F(1,352)=4.69, Beta= −.12, p=.03, ηp2=.01. However, when PCL-R Factors1 and 2 were entered simultaneously, neither Factor was significantly related to EF performance (unique effects of Factor1: F(1,352)=.76, Beta= −.06, p=.39, ηp2=.01; unique effects of Factor2: F(1,352)=1.84, Beta= −.09, p=.18, ηp2=.01)1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A. PCL-R F1 was unrelated to the EF composite (p>.05) B. MPQ-Estimated FD was significantly and positively related to the EF composite (p < .01).

Second, using the four facet approach, the Interpersonal facet was non-significant and negative, F(1,374)=.46, Beta= −.04, p=.50, ηp2<.01, the Affect facet was significant and negative, F(1,374)=6.15, Beta= −.13, p=.01, ηp2=.02, the Lifestyle facet was non-significant and negative, F(1,374)=1.74, Beta= −.07, p=.19, ηp2<.01, and the Antisocial facet was significant and negative, F(1,373)=6.00, Beta= −.13, p=.02, ηp2=.02. However, when all four facets were entered simultaneously, only the Affect facet effect remained significant (Interpersonal: F(1,346)=.38, Beta= .03, p=.54, ηp2<.01, Affect: F(1,346)=4.49, Beta= −.13, p=.04, ηp2=.01, Lifestyle: F(1,346)=.04, Beta= −.01, p=.84, ηp2<.01, Antisocial: F(1,346)=1.50, Beta= −.08, p=.22, ηp2<.01).

PCL-R trait dimensions supplemental analysis

Given the extensive evidence for a latent four facet PCL-based model of psychopathy (Neumann, Hare, & Pardini, 2014), and that latent variable methods provide rigorous control of measurement error, we also conducted a structural equation (SEM) model in-line with the four facet regression reported above. Specifically, we used the PCL-R items as indicators of their respective factors (Interpersonal, Affective, Lifestyle, & Antisocial) and specified the PCL-R factors as predictors of the super-ordinate EF factor. The results indicated acceptable fit for the SEM (CFI=.90; RMSEA=.04, robust weighted least squares-χ2/df (770.74/399)=1.93). Consistent with the regression results reported above, the Affective facet (standardized parameter: −.43, p < .01) was negatively related to EF performance. Additionally, unlike the regression results, the Interpersonal facet (standardized parameter: .24, p < .05) was positively related to the EF performance (see Figure 2). SEM formally accounts for measurement error (unlike linear regression) and, thus, it is possible to obtain more precise parameter estimates, lower standard errors, and thus, more significant results (e.g., significant Interpersonal and Affect facet effects using SEM).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Structural equation model of PCL-R facets predicting EF performance.

The influence of measurement on the relationship between psychopathic trait dimensions and EF performance

A second goal of the present study was to compare measurements methods of psychopathic trait dimensions. PCL-R Factor1 and MPQ-B FD were only correlated at r=.26 reinforcing the idea that these measure may be tapping a somewhat similar construct, but are far from equivalent (see Benning et al., 2005). Consistent with the proposal that certain traits of psychopathy are related to adaptive characteristics, MPQ-B FD was significantly and positively related to the EF composite (F(1,351)=11.10, Beta= .23, p<.01, ηp2=.03; Figure1.B)2. There were no significant effects involving MPQ-B IA (Beta= −.08, p=.14). Unique effects (i.e., simultaneous) models indicated that the FD effect remained significant (F(1,351)=11.10, Beta= .23, p<.01, ηp2=.03), while the IA effects remained non-significant (Beta= −.05, p=.34). Finally, comparison (i.e., simultaneous regression) of PCL-R Factor1 and FD indicated that PCL-R Factor1 was negatively and significantly related to EF performance F(1,328)=4.10, Beta= −.18, p=.03, ηp2=.02, whereas FD remained positively and significantly related to EF performance F(1,328)=10.99, Beta= .28, p<.01, ηp2=.033,4.

Discussion

Although there has been substantial research regarding EF in psychopathy, the overall pattern of results lacks consistency. While EF may be weakly and inconsistently related to the broad construct of psychopathy, there is some evidence that it may be significantly associated with specific dimensions that capture aspects of psychopathy (see Maes & Brazil, 2013; Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000). Moreover, the type of measures used to define these dimensions may influence the emerging associations with external correlates. Clarifying these associations can yield important insights into the specific dimensions associated with psychopathy, and, in the process, clarify the correlates responsible for characteristic behaviors.

Psychopathic trait dimensions and EF performance

A potential advantage of examining psychopathy in terms of various trait dimensions is that relationships may be revealed that are often not apparent when examining total psychopathy scores (i.e., the clinical syndrome). Consistent with previous research linking general antisocial behavior with deficient EF, the independent effect of Factor2 and the Antisocial facet were negatively related to EF performance. The association between antisocial behavior and poor EF corroborates previous suggestions (e.g., Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000). Individuals with EF deficits are less able to override maladaptive response inclinations in order to maintain more appropriate and personally beneficial behavior. Consequently, they are at higher risk for persistent rule breaking and committing acts of violence. Thus, deficits in EF may underlie the lack of prosocial behavior and some decision-making deficits that have been found to characterize antisocial behavior (e.g., Yechiam et al., 2008; Radke, Brazil, Scheper, Bulten, & De Bruijn, 2012).

However, when controlling for Factor1 traits (e.g., callousness) or the Affective facet, the association between antisocial behavior and EF became non-significant. On the one hand, there are potential pitfalls of partialling the independent effects of one variable from another (Lynam, Hoyle & Newman, 2006), in the sense that removing interpersonal and affective traits from antisocial behavior may be artificially splitting traits that within an individual are important in combination for informing behavior, particularly in offenders. Furthermore, recent work suggests that the shared variance between psychopathy-related traits might be of great explanatory value in characterizing the interplay between psychopathy and cognitive functioning (Maes & Brazil, 2015; Brazil, 2015). On the other hand, examination of specific relationships with particular traits may become statistically meaningful while controlling for other aspects of psychopathy. In this case, the Affect facet of psychopathy appears most robustly related to EF deficits (see also the Supplemental analysis where the PCL-R Interpersonal facet was positively related to EF performance). Though somewhat surprising, it is possible that difficulty inhibiting dominant responses, rule learning, and abstract reasoning for individuals high on the Affect facet may influence their ability to callously disregard their own well-being, the well-being of others, and chronically fail to engage in responsible behaviors. This interpretation is consistent with evidence that the Affect facet of psychopathy prospectively predicts aggressive and violent behavior (Vitacco, Neumann & Jackson, 2005). These findings suggest that affective dimension of psychopathy may be uniquely related to EF and understanding that association can help clarify the specific EF dysfunctions associated with these dimensions, but not psychopathy as a clinical syndrome, per se.

Comparison of psychopathy measures and EF performance

In extant literature, PCL-R Factor1 and FD are often used interchangeably to assess the interpersonal-affective traits of psychopathy. However, the pattern of results from the current study demonstrates that the interpersonal-affective traits of the respective measures are only modestly correlated and are differentially related to EF performance. Results indicate that better EF performance was associated with higher scores on MPQ-B FD, whereas PCL-R Factor1 did not predict EF performance. Thus, the general claim that interpersonal-affective traits are associated with higher EF can neither be wholly supported nor refuted (Maes & Brazil, 2013; but see Footnote 3). Rather, consistent with previous research, the suitability of this claim is dependent on the operative assessment tool.

As noted in the Introduction, the construct of FD is derived from a normal personality trait model. For example, FD components, such as fearlessness and low stress reaction may tap a propensity toward decreased general arousal and anxiety, which would enhance a person’s “capacity to remain calm and focused in pressured or threatening situations, rapid recovery from stressful events, high self-assurance and social efficacy, and a tolerance for unfamiliarity and danger” (Skeem et al., 2011, p. 106). In turn, such freedom from negative affect may alleviate demands on EF, and leave more capacity available for screening out interfering information and inhibitory control (i.e., superior performance on EF measures). PCL-R Factor1, by contrast, places more emphasis on the antisocial features inherent to the construct of psychopathy (Hare & Neumann, 2010). Factor1 items, such as glibness, manipulativeness, shallow affect, failure to accept responsibility, and pathological lying are often rated based on criminal activities or behaviors that are contradictory to social mores (e.g., theft, sexual crimes, covering up indiscretions). While these traits may be related to better EF functioning, they don’t appear as closely linked as the traits comprising FD.

Consistent with this conceptualization and the nature of the Factor and four facet models (Hare & Neumann, 2008), the recently proposed Triarchic model decomposes symptoms related to psychopathy into three clusters: Meanness, Boldness, and Disinhibition (Patrick, Fowles & Krueger, 2009). Within this model, the interpersonal-affective traits of psychopathy appear to be differentially related to the Triarchic subcomponents. While the Triarchic subcomponents show a range of significant correlations with the Interpersonal, Lifestyle, and Antisocial facets of the PCL-R, they are only modestly linked with the Affect facet of the PCL-R (Venables, Hall & Patrick, 2014). Moreover, the Triarchic subcomponents appear differentially related to measures that assess these interpersonal-affective traits. That is, Boldness (emotional resiliency, social assertiveness) aligns with FD and is presumed to reflect individual differences in reactivity of the brain’s core defensive system. By contrast, Meanness (lack of empathy, exploitativeness) is more aligned with PCL-R Factor1 and seems to reflect a “biologically based predatory orientation entailing aggressive resource seeking without concern for others” (Patrick & Drislane, 2014, p.4). Alternatively, both FD and PCL-R Factor1 may be related to low arousal (reactivity) and anxiety, but for different reasons relating to the nature of their link with EF. Specifically, FD and its related behaviors may be dependent upon enhanced top-down control (i.e. EF) which facilitates their ability to control negative emotions and navigate surroundings (Bishop, 2007), whereas PCL-R Factor1 traits may be unrelated to top-down control, but driven by deficits in bottom-up processes that undermine the experience of negative affect (Patrick, 2007). Regardless of the specific etiological model, the conceptualizations of FD and PCL-R Factor1 items seem to capture phenotypically similar constructs, but may reflect different underlying psychobiological processes.

While the findings from the present study are intriguing, a few limitations should be mentioned. First, given that this study was conducted within a prison setting, the pattern of results may not generalize to populations outside of correctional institutions. This issue is particularly important given that FD was developed and largely validated in a community sample. Researchers in the future may want to conduct a large-scale study in the community using FD and the PCL-Screening Version (a community analogue to the PCL-R). Second, the present sample was limited to male offenders, thus it is unclear if how gender may impact the relationship between measures of interpersonal-affective traits and EF. Finally, the measure of EF was selected to represent a broad spectrum of EF processes. It is possible that more nuanced relationships within a subset of EF processes exist between various psychopathic trait dimensions. Despite these limitations, the present study illustrates that not all psychopathy-related dimensions and measures are alike, and differ in their relationships with EF in important and measurable ways.

Skeem and colleagues (2011) have suggested that exclusively favoring the PCL-R over other measures of psychopathy may negatively impact research, because they believe the measure is eclipsing the construct itself (c.f. Hare & Neumann, 2010). Therefore, they propose that an optimal study would include multiple measures of psychopathic dimensions. While this may be a more comprehensive approach, it then becomes paramount to discuss – and if possible, reconcile –the differences across measures, such that these measures are not discussed as if they are interchangeable.

The present study shows that specific dimensions are related to EF dysfunction and that those relationships are dependent on measure (e.g., FD may be associated with adaptive functions, whereas PCL-R Factor1 is not). Therefore, caution is needed when suggesting that psychopathy broadly, or interpersonal-affective traits specifically, are positively related to EF (Maes & Brazil, 2013). And, most critically, greater clarity is needed about the theoretical arguments and assessment measures used to substantiate an adaptive versus maladaptive view of interpersonal-affective traits of psychopathy. Ultimately, clarifying the interaction between measure, construct, and functioning will promote our understanding of psychopathy, particularly interpersonal-affective traits, from clinical, cognitive, and psychobiological perspectives.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the Wisconsin Department of Corrections, and especially Dr. Kevin Kallas and Warden Tom Nickel, for their continued support of this research program.

This paper received funding from NIDA.

Footnotes

1

For all models, we examined if Race/Ethnicity, IQ, and/or Reading scores impacted the results. None of the effects reported in the main text were altered by the inclusion of Race/Ethnicity. When IQ was included as a continuous covariate, the independent effect of Factor1 was significant F(1,368)=5.23, p=.02, ηp2=.01. However, the addition of Reading scores yielded a non-significant effect, F(1,316)=2.05, p=.15, ηp2<.01, consistent with the findings reported in the main text. For Factor2, inclusion of IQ resulted in a negative, but non-significant effect, F(1,347)=1.69, p=.19, ηp2<.01. With both Reading and IQ scores entered as covariates, there was no significant relationship between Factor2 and EF performance, F(1,298)=.32, p=.57, ηp2<.01. Finally, for the unique effects model, none of the effects changed (i.e, neither Factor1 nor 2 were related to EF performance). Finally, with the inclusion of IQ the Affect facet effect remained significant (F(1,368)=4.60, p=.03 ηp2=.01) and the Antisocial facet effect was non-significant (F(1,346)=2.99, p=.09, ηp2<.01). When Reading scores were included both of these effects were non-significant. In the unique effects model for the facets, the inclusion of IQ and/or Reading scores resulted in no significant effects of the facets on EF performance.

2

One goal of the present study is to compare measures of psychopathic trait dimensions. While the selection of the PCL-R and MPQ-B as measures of psychopathic trait dimensions represent prominent examples of scales that may be differentially related to external correlates, a number of other measures of psychopathic traits that have emerged. Therefore, we also examined the relationship between psychopathic trait dimensions and EF using another self-report questionnaire, the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI; Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996). The PPI is a self-report questionnaire that includes eight subscales that independently assess Factor1 and Factor2. Like the MPQ-B, the PPI estimates psychopathic trait dimensions using a variety of personality traits. Social Potency, Coldheartedness, Fearlessness, Impulsive Non-conformity, and Stress Immunity comprise Factor 1 (PPI I), whereas Machiavellian Egocentricity, Blame Externalization, and Carefree Nonplanfulness items comprise Factor 2 (PPI-II). The PPI shows good convergent validity with other self-report measures of psychopathy (Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996; Tonnaer, Cima, Sijtsma, Uzieblo & Lilienfeld, 2013). Results for the PPI were consistent with the results for FD. PPI-I was associated with better EF performance (independent model: F(1,368)=7.78, p<.01, ηp2=.02; unique model: F(1,368)=7.65, p<.01, ηp2=.02 ) and PPI-II was unrelated to EF(p’s>.55). Comparing PCL-R Factor1 and PPI-I, Factor1 was negatively and significantly related to EF performance F(1,368)=7.63, p=.01, ηp2=.02, whereas PPI-I remained positively and significantly related to EF performance F(1,368)=12.00, p<.01, ηp2=.03. Therefore, much like FD, higher scores on PPI-I seems to indicate better EF performance and may tap more adaptive traits than PCL-R Factor1.

3

Statistics based on the normal distribution (frequentist statistics) are incapable of assessing whether the model for the null-hypothesis (H0) predicting the absence of a correlation is more probable than the alternative model (H1) predicting the presence of a correlation. Therefore, we computed log-transformed Bayes Factors (BF10) to assess whether H0 or H1 is more likely using the R software package (Wetzels & Wagemakers, 2012). The BF10 was 0.049 for the correlation between EF and Factor1, indicating that the lack of relationship between EF and Factor1 is about 21 times more likely to be true. For the FD-EF correlation, the BF10 was 57602, thus providing decisive evidence in favor of the positive relationship between these two variables. These results substantiate the validity of the main findings from an alternative statistical framework.

4

Since the MPQ-B and relatedly the PPI do not have clear item-to-factor latent structures, it was not possible to test a SEM of the MPQ-B/EF relations. Moreover, serious questions have been raised regarding the latent variable integrity of the PPI two-factor model, and by association scales like the MPQ-B (see Neumann, Uzieblo, Crombez & Hare, 2013 for example).

References

  1. Bagshaw R, Gray NS, Snowden RJ. Executive function in psychopathy: The tower of London, Brixton Spatial Anticipation and the Hayling Sentence Completion Tests. Psychiatry Research. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Benning SD, Patrick CJ, Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Iacono WG. Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: a step toward community epidemiological investigations. Assessment. 2005;12(1):3–18. doi: 10.1177/1073191104271223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benning SD, Patrick CJ, Hicks BM, Blonigen DM, Krueger RF. Factor structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: validity and implications for clinical assessment. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15(3):340–350. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.3.340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bentler P. On the fit of models to covariances and methodology to the bulletin. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:400–404. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.3.400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bishop SJ. Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11:307–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bollen K. Structural equations with latent variables. Wiley; New York: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brazil IA. Considering new insights into antisociality and psychopathy. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2(2):115–116. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00125-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carlson SR, Thái S. ERPs on a continuous performance task and self-reported psychopathic traits: P3 and CNV augmentation are associated with Fearless Dominance. Biological Psychology. 2010;85(2):318–330. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cleckley HM. The Mask of Sanity: An Attempt to Reinterpret the So-called Psychopathic Personality. Mosby: 1941. [Google Scholar]
  10. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH, Sutherland D, Borland N. Some supplementary methods for the analysis of the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System. Psychological assessment. 2011;23(4):888–898. doi: 10.1037/a0023712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS) Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  12. Dolan M. The neuropsychology of prefrontal function in antisocial personality disordered offenders with varying degrees of psychopathy. Psychological medicine. 2012;42(08):1715–1725. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Feilhauer J, Cima M, Korebrits A, Kunert HJ. Differential associations between psychopathy dimensions, types of aggression, and response inhibition. Aggressive behavior. 2012;38(1):77–88. doi: 10.1002/ab.20415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders (SCID) American Psychiatric Press; Washington, DC: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  15. Fisher L, Blair RJR. Cognitive impairment and its relationship to psychopathic tendencies in children with emotional and behavioral difficulties. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 1998;26(6):511–519. doi: 10.1023/a:1022655919743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gao Y, Raine A. Successful and unsuccessful psychopaths: A neurobiological model. Behavioral Sciences & the Law. 2010;28:194–210. doi: 10.1002/bsl.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gao Y, Raine A. P3 event-related potential impairments in antisocial and psychopathic individuals: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychology. 2009;82(3):199–210. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Giancola PR, Tarter RE. Executive Cognitive Functioning and Risk for Substance Abuse. Psychological Science. 1999;10(3):203–205. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hare RD. Manual for the Revised Psychopathy Checklist. 2nd ed Multi-Health Systems; Toronto, Ontario, Canada: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hare RD, Neumann CS. Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:217–246. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hare RD, Neumann CS. The role of antisociality in the psychopathy construct: comment on Skeem and Cooke (2010) Psychological Assessment. 2010;22(2):446–454. doi: 10.1037/a0013635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hare RD, Harpur TJ, Hakstian AR, Forth AE, Hart SD, Newman J. The revised Psychopathy Checklist: Reliability and factor structure. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1990;2(3):338–341. [Google Scholar]
  23. Harpur TJ, Hare RD, Hakstian AR. Two-factor conceptualization of psychopathy: Construct validity and assessment implications. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of consulting and clinical Psychology. 1989;1(1):6–17. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hart SD, Forth AE, Hare RD. Performance of criminal psychopaths on selected neuropsychological tests. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1990;99(4):374–379. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.99.4.374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hart SD, Hare RD. Discriminant validity of the Psychopathy Checklist in a forensic psychiatric population. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989;1(3):211–218. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hicks BM, Patrick CJ. Psychopathy and Negative Emotionality: Analyses of Suppressor Effects Reveal Distinct Relations With Emotional Distress, Fearfulness, and Anger-Hostility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:276–287. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Johnson MH. Development of human brain functions. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;54(12):1312–1316. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00426-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kramer JH, Mungas D, Possin KL, Rankin KP, Boxer AL, Rosen HJ, Bostrom A, Sinha L, Berhel A, Widmeyer M. NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2014;20(01):11–19. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Latzman RD, Elkovitch N, Young J, Clark LA. The contribution of executive functioning to academic achievement among male adolescents. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2010;32(5):455–462. doi: 10.1080/13803390903164363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Latzman RD, Markon KE. The factor structure and age-related factorial invariance of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Assessment. 2010;17(2):172–184. doi: 10.1177/1073191109356254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Li JJ, Chung TA, Vanyukov MM, Scott WD, Ferrell R, Clark DB. A Hierarchical Factor Model of Executive Functions in Adolescents: Evidence of Gene-Environment Interplay. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2014;21(01):1–12. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714001039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lilienfeld SO, Andrews BP. Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1996;66:488–524. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lilienfeld SO, Patrick CJ, Benning SD, Berg J, Sellbom M, Edens JF. The role of fearless dominance in psychopathy: Confusions, controversies, and clarifications. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2012;3(3):327–340. doi: 10.1037/a0026987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lynam DR, Hoyle RH, Newman JP. The perils of partialling cautionary tales from aggression and psychopathy. Assessment. 2006;13(3):328–341. doi: 10.1177/1073191106290562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lynam DR, Miller JD. Fearless dominance and psychopathy: A response to Lilienfeld et al. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2012;3(3):341–353. doi: 10.1037/a0028296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Maes JHR, Brazil IA. No clear evidence for a positive association between the interpersonal-affective aspects of psychopathy and executive functioning. Psychiatry Research. 2013;210(3):1265–1274. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Maes JH, Brazil IA. Distraction from cognitive processing by emotional pictures: Preliminary evidence for an association with interactions between psychopathy-related traits in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;75:53–58. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mahmut MK, Homewood J, Stevenson RJ. The characteristics of non-criminals with high psychopathy traits: Are they similar to criminal psychopaths? Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42(3):679–692. [Google Scholar]
  39. Malterer MB, Lilienfeld SO, Neumann CS, Newman JP. Concurrent Validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory With Offender and Community Samples. Assessment. 2010;17(1):3–15. doi: 10.1177/1073191109349743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Miller JD, Lynam DR. An examination of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory’s nomological network: A meta-analytic review. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2012;3(3):305–326. doi: 10.1037/a0024567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mol B, Van Den Bos P, Derks Y, Egger J. Executive functioning and the two-factor model of psychopathy: No differential relation? International Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;119(1):124–140. doi: 10.1080/00207450802324861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Morgan AB, Lilienfeld SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000;20:113–136. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00096-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. Sixth edition Muthén & Muthén; Los Angeles, CA: 1998-2010. [Google Scholar]
  44. Neumann CS, Hare RD. Psychopathic traits in a large community sample: links to violence, alcohol use, and intelligence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76(5):893–899. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.5.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Neumann CS, Hare RD, Newman JP. The super-ordinate nature of the psychopathy checklist-revised. Journal of personality disorders. 2007;21(2):102–117. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.2.102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Neumann CS, Hare RD, Pardini DA. Antisociality and the Construct of Psychopathy: Data From Across the Globe. Journal of personality. 2014 doi: 10.1111/jopy.12127. DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Neumann CS, Johansson PT, Hare RD. The Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R), low anxiety, and fearlessness: a structural equation modeling analysis. Personality Disorders. 2013;4:129–37. doi: 10.1037/a0027886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Neumann CS, Kosson D, Salekin R. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis of the psychopathy construct: Methodological and conceptual issues. In: Herve H, Yuille J, editors. The Psychopath: Theory, Research, and Practice. Lawrence Erlbaum; New York: 2007. pp. 79–104. [Google Scholar]
  49. Neumann CS, Malterer MB, Newman JP. Factor structure of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI): findings from a large incarcerated sample. Psychological assessment. 2008;20(2):169–174. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.20.2.169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Neumann CS, Pardini D. Factor structure and construct validity of the Self-Report Psychopathy (SRP) scale and the Youth Psychopathic Traits inventory (YPI) in young men. Journal of personality disorders. 2014;28(3):419–433. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2012_26_063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Neumann CS, Uzieblo K, Crombez G, Hare RD. Understanding the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI) in terms of the unidimensionality, orthogonality, and construct validity of PPI-I and-II. Personality disorders. 2013;4(1):77–79. doi: 10.1037/a0027196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Neumann CS, Vitacco MJ, Hare RD, Wupperman P. Re-construing the “Reconstruction” of psychopathy: A comment on Cooke, Michie, Hart & Clarke. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2005;19:624–640. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2005.19.6.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Newman JP, Howland EW. The effects of incentives on Wisconsin Card Sorting Task performance in psychopaths. 1987. Unpublished manuscript.
  54. Ogilvie JM, Stewart AL, Chan RCK, Shum DHK. Neuropsychological Measures of Executive Function and Antisocial Behavior: A Meta-Analysis*. Criminology. 2011;49(4):1063–1107. [Google Scholar]
  55. Patrick CJ. Getting to the Heart of Psychopathy. In: Herv H, Yuille JC, editors. The Psychopath: Theory, Research, and Practice. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; Mahwah, NJ, US: 2007. pp. 207–252. [Google Scholar]
  56. Patrick CJ, Curtin JJ, Tellegen A. Development and validation of a brief form of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment. 2002;14:150–163. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.14.2.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Patrick CJ, Drislane LE. Triarchic model of psychopathy: Origins, operationalizations, and observed linkages with personality and general psychopathology. Journal of personality. 2014 doi: 10.1111/jopy.12119. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Patrick CJ, Fowles DC, Krueger RF. Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and psychopathology. 2009;21(03):913–938. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Radke S, Brazil IA, Scheper I, Bulten BH, De Bruijn ER. Unfair offers, unfair offenders? Fairness considerations in incarcerated individuals with and without psychopathy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:406. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00406. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ross SR, Benning SD, Adams Z. Symptoms of Executive Dysfunction are Endemic to Secondary Psychopathy: An Examination in Criminal Offenders and Noninstitutionalized Young Adults. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2007;21:384–399. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.4.384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28:573–594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sadeh N, Verona E. Psychopathic personality traits associated with abnormal selective attention and impaired cognitive control. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(5):669–680. doi: 10.1037/a0012692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Seara-Cardoso A, Viding E. Functional neuroscience of psychopathic personality in adults. Journal of Personality. 2014 doi: 10.1111/jopy.12113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sellbom M, Verona E. Neuropsychological correlates of psychopathic traits in a non-incarcerated sample. Journal of Research in Personality. 2007;41:276–294. [Google Scholar]
  65. Skeem JL, Polaschek DLL, Patrick CJ, Lilienfeld SO. Psychopathic Personality: Bridging the Gap Between Scientific Evidence and Public Policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2011;12:95–162. doi: 10.1177/1529100611426706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Smith SS, Arnett PA, Newman JP. Neuropsychological differentiation of psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. Personality and Individual Differences. 1992;13(11):1233–1243. [Google Scholar]
  67. Smith GT, McCarthy DM, Zapolski TC. On the value of homogeneous constructs for construct validation, theory testing, and the description of psychopathology. Psychological Assessment. 2009;21:272–284. doi: 10.1037/a0016699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Snowden RJ, Gray NS, Pugh S, Atkinson G. Executive function as a function of sub-clinical psychopathy. Personality and Individual Differences. 2013;55(7):801–804. [Google Scholar]
  69. Steffensmeier DJ, Allan EA, Harer MD, Streifel C. Age and the Distribution of Crimt. American Journal of Sociology. 1989;94:803–831. [Google Scholar]
  70. Tonnaer F, Cima M, Sijtsma K, Uzieblo K, Lilienfeld SO. Screening for psychopathy: Validation of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Short Form with reference scores. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2013;35(2):153–161. [Google Scholar]
  71. Venables NC, Hall JR, Patrick CJ. Differentiating psychopathy from antisocial personality disorder: A triarchic model perspective. Psychological medicine. 2014;44(05):1005–1013. doi: 10.1017/S003329171300161X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Vitacco MJ, Neumann CS, Caldwell MF, Leistico AM, Van Rybroek GJ. Testing factor models of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version and their association with instrumental aggression. Journal of personality assessment. 2006;87(1):74–83. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Vitacco MJ, Neumann CS, Jackson RL. Testing a four-factor model of psychopathy and its association with ethnicity, gender, intelligence, and violence. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2005;73(3):466–476. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed Harcourt Assessment; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  75. Wetzels R, Wagenmakers E-J. A Default Bayesian Hypothesis Test for Correlations and Partial Correlations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2012;19:1057–1064. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0295-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wilkinson GS. Wide Range Achievement Test. 3rd ed Psychological Assessment Resources; Lutz, FL: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  77. Yechiam E, Kanz JE, Bechara A, Stout JC, Busemeyer JR, Altmaier EM, Paulsen JS. Neurocognitive deficits related to poor decision making in people behind bars. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15(1):44–51. doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.1.44. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.1.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES