The study reported by Dolan et al1 in this issue of the Journal of Graduate Medical Education epitomizes the type of research that is sorely needed in medical education. The authors conducted a randomized controlled trial in which they assigned internal medicine residents at a large academic practice to receive either the standard curriculum in fracture prevention, or a standard curriculum plus repeated, spaced practice on the material over a 3- to 6-month period. They found that the intervention with repeated practice produced better retention of knowledge 10 months later, and improved the quality of clinical care (bone density screening rates, appropriate use of bisphosphonates, but not FRAX score reporting) provided by the residents during the study. This study is innovative and important in that it assesses the long-term consequences of an educational intervention, and measures its impact on both knowledge and clinical practice.
In education at all levels and in all fields, we tend to focus on short-term outcomes. That is, learning is generally assessed during, or immediately after, the time it occurs (eg, questions posed to students during a lecture, quizzes at the end of class, or final examination after a course). Short-term outcomes are important for many reasons, such as formative and summative assessment. However, when we focus exclusively on short-term outcomes, we make an assumption that is often false: we assume that short-term performance is a good predictor of performance over longer periods of time. Unfortunately, mastery demonstrated during or immediately after learning can be easily lost in the following weeks and months without continued practice.2,3 For example, numerous studies have found that a substantial portion of the basic knowledge and skills acquired in medical school is forgotten by the time individuals enter graduate training, let alone practice.4–7 Similar patterns occur in graduate medical education, with residents forgetting knowledge and skills that are fundamental to their training.8–12
One conclusion that can be drawn from studies that assess long-term outcomes is that we can be shortsighted in how we approach educational interventions. We often devote a substantial amount of effort to facilitating initial learning, but comparatively little effort to maintaining it. For example, a recent study13 assessed the short-term and long-term consequences of redesigning a lecture-based, preclinical pediatrics course to incorporate team-based learning, a pedagogical approach that promotes active learning. Through comparing a group that received the lecture-based version of the course to a group that received the team-based learning version, the study tracked student knowledge of core concepts from the course over time. When knowledge was measured after the end of course, the students who had taken the team-based learning version performed substantially better than students in the lecture-based version. However, when students were given a follow-up knowledge assessment prior to their clerkship, the learning gains in the team-based learning group had disappeared, and the 2 groups performed at the same level. Such findings demonstrate how devoting substantial effort to facilitating initial learning can yield benefits, but those benefits can be easily lost without efforts to maintain knowledge and skills afterward.
Given that learning gains can be easily lost in the absence of continued practice, how can we help medical students, residents, and other health professionals maintain the knowledge and skills that they acquire during training? The intervention implemented by Dolan and colleagues1 provides a possible template. The intervention incorporates several mechanisms known to promote long-term retention and deeper understanding: retrieval practice, feedback, and spaced repetitions.14,15 Retrieval practice refers to the act of retrieving information from memory (eg, solving a practice problem or answering a test question), which is a potent learning event.16,17 Providing feedback increases the benefits of retrieval practice by correcting errors,18 maintaining correct responses,19 and increasing understanding.20 When such practice is spaced or distributed over time, it is more effective than massed practice (eg, cramming).21,22 Numerous studies have shown the efficacy of interventions that incorporate these 3 mechanisms on the retention and transfer of knowledge in undergraduate, graduate, and continuing medical education.23–26 To our knowledge, however, the research reported by Dolan and colleagues1 is the first that links such an intervention to improved quality of clinical care.
Of course, it is challenging to assess long-term outcomes and determine the amount of continued practice needed to maintain knowledge and skills that are not regularly used in clinical practice. However, here too Dolan and colleagues1 present a potential solution: the use of technology. Advances in technology are providing educators with powerful new tools that are rapidly changing how people learn both inside and outside the classroom. At a minimum, technology can make providing continued practice more flexible, efficient, inexpensive, and scalable through automation. Yet, the real promise lies in adaptive technology that provides a personalized experience for each learner because it has the potential to exponentially increase the effectiveness and impact of educational interventions.27 Technology is also transforming the way in which health records are kept, providing new opportunities to link learning in the classroom to practice in clinical settings. The ability to assess the impact of educational interventions on clinical care is critical to improving medical education.
In conclusion, we think that the innovative approach taken by Dolan and colleagues1 represents the future of medical education. The training required to become a health professional is substantial. In order to ensure that the learning that occurs during each phase of training is retained over time, more effort must be devoted to maintaining the knowledge and skills acquired. We must also assess long-term outcomes, including the quality of clinical care, so that we can accurately judge the effectiveness of educational interventions. With medical interventions, health professionals routinely follow up with their patients: a surgery is never assumed to be a success immediately after the operation, and a drug is never presumed to be effective based solely on the initial administration. The same approach should be applied to interventions in medical education.
Footnotes
Both authors are in the Department of Educational Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin. Andrew C. Butler, PhD, is Assistant Professor of Human Development, Culture, & Learning Sciences; and Nathaniel D. Raley, BA, is a Graduate Research Assistant.
References
- 1.Dolan BM, Yialamas MA, McMahon GT. A randomized educational intervention trial to determine the effect of online education on the quality of resident-delivered care. J Grad Med Educ. 2015;7(3):376–381. doi: 10.4300/JGME-D-14-00571.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bjork RA. Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In: Metcalfe J, Shimamura A, editors. Metacognition: Knowing About Knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1994. pp. 185–205. In. eds. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bjork RA. Institutional impediments to effective training. In: Druckman D, Bjork RA, editors. Learning, Remembering, Believing: Enhancing Human Performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1994. pp. 295–306. In. eds. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Custers EJ, ten Cate OT. Very long-term retention of basic science knowledge in doctors after graduation. Med Educ. 2011;45(4):422–430. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2010.03889.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Custers EJ. Long-term retention of basic science knowledge: a review study. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2010;15(1):109–128. doi: 10.1007/s10459-008-9101-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ling Y, Swanson DB, Holtzman K, Bucak SD. Retention of basic science information by senior medical students. Acad Med. 2008;83(suppl 10):82–85. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e318183e2fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sullivan PB, Gregg N, Adams E, Rodgers C, Hull J. How much of the paediatric core curriculum do medical students remember? Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2013;18(3):365–373. doi: 10.1007/s10459-012-9375-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Picciano A, Winter R, Ballan D, Bimberg B, Jacks M, Laing E. Resident acquisition of knowledge during a noontime conference series. Fam Med. 2003;35(6):418–422. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Winter RO, Picciano A, Bimberg B, Chae M, Chae S, Jacks M, et al. Resident knowledge acquisition during a block conference series. Fam Med. 2007;39(7):498–503. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fitzgerald JD, Wenger NS. Didactic teaching conferences for IM residents: who attends, and is attendance related to medical certifying examination scores? Acad Med. 2003;78(1):84–89. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200301000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cacamese SM, Eubank KJ, Hebert RS, Wright SM. Conference attendance and performance on the in-training examination in internal medicine. Med Teach. 2004;26(7):640–644. doi: 10.1080/09563070400005446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pollack R, Baker RJ. The acquisition of factual knowledge and the role of the didactic conference in a surgical residency program. Am Surg. 1988;54(9):531–534. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Emke AR, Butler AC, Larsen DP. Effects of team-based learning on short-term and long-term retention of factual knowledge. Med Teach. 2015 Apr;21:1–6. doi: 10.3109/0142159X.2015.1034663. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Larsen DP, Butler AC. Test-enhanced learning. In: Walsh K, editor. Oxford Textbook of Medical Education. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 443–452. In. ed. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Larsen DP, Butler AC, Roediger HL., 3rd Test-enhanced learning in medical education. Med Educ. 2008;42(10):959–966. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03124.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Butler AC. Repeated testing produces superior transfer of learning relative to repeated studying. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2010;36(5):1118–1133. doi: 10.1037/a0019902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Roediger HL, 3rd, Butler AC. The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(1):20–27. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Butler AC, Roediger HL., 3rd Feedback enhances the positive effects and reduces the negative effects of multiple-choice testing. Memory Cogn. 2008;36(3):604–616. doi: 10.3758/mc.36.3.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Butler AC, Karpicke JD, Roediger HL., 3rd Correcting a meta-cognitive error: feedback enhances retention of low confidence correct responses. J Exp Psychol Learn Memory Cogn. 2008;34(4):918–928. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Butler AC, Godbole N, Marsh EJ. Explanation feedback is better than correct answer feedback for promoting transfer of learning. J Educ Psychol. 2013;105(2):290–298. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cepeda NJ, Pashler H, Vul E, Wixted JT, Rohrer D. Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: a review and quantitative synthesis. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(3):354–380. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dempster FN. Spacing effects and their implications for theory and practice. Educ Psychol Rev. 1989;1(4):309–330. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Larsen DP, Butler AC, Roediger HL., 3rd Repeated testing improves long-term retention relative to repeated study: a randomized controlled trial. Med Educ. 2009;43(12):1174–1181. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Larsen DP, Butler AC, Lawson AL, Roediger HL., 3rd The importance of seeing the patient: test-enhanced learning with standardized patients and written tests improves clinical application of knowledge. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2013;18(3):409–425. doi: 10.1007/s10459-012-9379-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Larsen DP, Butler AC, Roediger HL., 3rd Comparative effects of test-enhanced learning and self-explanation on long-term retention. Med Educ. 2013;47(7):674–682. doi: 10.1111/medu.12141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Larsen DP, Butler AC, Aung WY, Corboy JR, Friedman DI, Sperling MR. The effects of test-enhanced learning on long-term retention in AAN annual meeting courses. Neurology. 2015;84(7):748–754. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Butler AC, Marsh EJ, Slavinsky JP, Baraniuk RG. Integrating cognitive science and technology improves learning in a STEM classroom. Educ Psychol Rev. 2014;26(2):331–340. [Google Scholar]