Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2015 Sep 7;4:e08758. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08758

A neural command circuit for grooming movement control

Stefanie Hampel 1, Romain Franconville 1, Julie H Simpson 1,2,*, Andrew M Seeds 1,*
Editor: Alexander Borst3
PMCID: PMC4599031  PMID: 26344548

Abstract

Animals perform many stereotyped movements, but how nervous systems are organized for controlling specific movements remains unclear. Here we use anatomical, optogenetic, behavioral, and physiological techniques to identify a circuit in Drosophila melanogaster that can elicit stereotyped leg movements that groom the antennae. Mechanosensory chordotonal neurons detect displacements of the antennae and excite three different classes of functionally connected interneurons, which include two classes of brain interneurons and different parallel descending neurons. This multilayered circuit is organized such that neurons within each layer are sufficient to specifically elicit antennal grooming. However, we find differences in the durations of antennal grooming elicited by neurons in the different layers, suggesting that the circuit is organized to both command antennal grooming and control its duration. As similar features underlie stimulus-induced movements in other animals, we infer the possibility of a common circuit organization for movement control that can be dissected in Drosophila.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.001

Research organism: D. melanogaster

eLife digest

Many movements that animals perform regularly—including walking and grooming—consist of stereotyped sequences of muscle contractions. For example, a dog may scratch its side in response to a fleabite or because it is itchy. But how does the nervous system trigger such specific movements from among the repertoire of different movements that the animal could perform? It also remains unclear how such movements can be produced in a reliable, yet flexible manner.

Hampel et al. have now described the neural circuit that triggers and controls the stereotyped leg movements that the fruit fly Drosophila uses to groom its antennae. Such grooming movements are stereotyped yet have certain degrees of flexibility, which makes them ideal to study the neural circuits that underlie specific movements. Grooming further lends itself to this kind of investigation because it can be triggered by irritating the surface of the fly's body. There is also an extensive genetic toolkit that can be used to manipulate and observe the fruit fly's nervous system in detail.

Hampel et al. first identified sensory neurons in the flies' antennae that were needed to elicit grooming in response to irritating displacements of the antennae. Once these neurons were found, techniques—including those that allow specific neurons in the fly's brain to be precisely controlled—were then used to find other neurons that participate in the grooming process. This approach highlighted three groups of interneurons: two in the brain and one in the fly's equivalent of the spinal cord. Together these layers of sensory neurons and interneurons formed a circuit that triggered grooming whenever the antennae were disturbed.

Notably, activating different sensory or interneurons triggered bouts of antennal grooming of differing durations. This shows that the same neural circuit can both produce highly specific movements and modify the movements to provide flexibility. Neural circuits with similar features have been observed previously to induce other animal behaviors, for example, swimming in leeches. This suggests that this organization may be common in circuits that elicit movements. Additional experiments are now needed to validate whether similar circuits underlie other stereotyped movements in fruit flies and other animals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.002

Introduction

An animal may perform a particular movement in response to its environment and internal state, and many movements are selected from a repertoire of stereotyped motor patterns. This repertoire can include movements that serve particular purposes, such as feeding, grooming, song production, locomotion, and even coordinated facial poses for expressing different emotions (Grillner and Wallén, 2004; Grillner et al., 2005). Many of these movements are produced by neural networks called pattern generators, which control the precise timing of motor neuron activity to coordinate stereotyped patterns of muscle contractions (Pearson, 1993; Kiehn and Kullander, 2004). These neural networks are localized to distinct regions of the central nervous system (CNS), such as the hindbrain and spinal cord in vertebrates and ventral nerve cord in arthropods, and are capable of producing their respective movements even when experimentally isolated from the brain and sensory inputs (Grillner et al., 2005; Marder et al., 2005; Büschges et al., 2011). The activity of pattern generators can be initiated, or adapted to particular circumstances, in response to inputs from proprioceptive neurons, neuromodulators, or command-like neurons (Marder et al., 2005; Grillner, 2006; Ritzmann and Büschges, 2007; Blitz and Nusbaum, 2011; Harris-Warrick, 2011).

The proximal trigger of pattern generator activity arises from the command-like neurons, which can consist of individual neurons, or populations of neurons that induce or ‘command’ specific movements (Kupfermann and Weiss, 1978; Jing, 2009). Such neurons are also implicated in controlling different parameters of the movements that they induce, such as their speed or duration (Kupfermann and Weiss, 1978; Pearson, 1993; Kristan, 2008; Jing, 2009). However, the organizational principles underlying how such command circuitry can both initiate movements and control their parameters remain unclear. Moreover, in many cases in which the activity of command-like neurons has been experimentally manipulated (Jing, 2009), the behavioral impact of the manipulations was not assessed in intact and freely moving animals.

The development of neurogenetic tools in Drosophila has led to rapid progress in identifying command-like neurons. This progress has been enabled by the use of optogenetic and thermogenetic activation of specific, genetically targeted populations of neurons in freely moving adult flies (Flood et al., 2013a; Owald et al., 2015). For example, activation of specific neuronal types can elicit stereotyped movements such as feeding, locomotion, courtship song, or escape (Lima and Miesenböck, 2005; von Philipsborn et al., 2011; Flood et al., 2013b; Gao et al., 2013; Inagaki et al., 2014; Bidaye et al., 2014; von Reyn et al., 2014). Although some of these studies have led to the identification of individual neurons and groups of neurons that command their respective behaviors, the anatomical organization of and functional connections among such groups of neurons that control these movements has been largely unexplored.

Grooming movements (a.k.a. cleaning, scratching, or wiping reflexes) can be studied to determine the neuronal mechanisms by which specific movements are initiated and controlled. Grooming is ubiquitous among limbed animals as a means of protecting the body surface from different types of mechanical or chemical irritants (Sachs, 1988). Such sensory stimuli induce the movement of a limb to the irritated body part, which then scratches or wipes the surface (Stein, 1983; Dürr and Matheson, 2003). Because grooming movements can be predictably elicited by defined stimuli, they offer a way to access the sensory-connected neural circuitry that commands precisely targeted limb movements. In addition, grooming movements exhibit differing response durations, limb trajectories, stimulus-induced habituation, and can be suppressed, suggesting that the neural circuitry underlying these movements is subject to regulation and flexible control (Sherrington, 1906; Stein, 1983; Corfas and Dudai, 1989; Page and Matheson, 2009; Seeds et al., 2014). Therefore, the study of grooming movements may reveal basic principles of movement control, but little is known about the neuronal mechanisms governing their initiation and modulation.

We previously discovered that activating different neuronal populations in the fly nervous system could induce distinct grooming movements, such as grooming of the eyes, antennae, wings, thorax, or legs (Seeds et al., 2014). This raised the possibility that the functional organization of the neural circuitry controlling specific grooming movements could be defined. Here we examine a circuit that commands one of these grooming movements—antennal grooming. This movement involves the grasping and brushing of the antennae with the legs in response to different types of irritants (Robinson, 1996; Böröczky et al., 2013; Seeds et al., 2014). To deconstruct the neural circuitry underlying antennal grooming, we isolated a small number of GAL4-expressing transgenic lines that could elicit the appropriate leg movements when driving expression of the temperature-gated neuronal activator dTrpA1 (Hamada et al., 2008; Seeds et al., 2014). However, these lines expressed GAL4 in multiple neuronal subsets, making it difficult to determine which neurons were responsible for the grooming movement. In this work, we refine these GAL4 lines to identify the specific neurons that elicit antennal grooming. We show that these neurons are functionally connected to form a circuit that detects displacement of the antennae via mechanosensory neurons and then commands grooming through three different interneuronal classes. Our analysis of the complex organization of this circuit provides insight into how stereotyped movements are controlled.

Results

A group of Johnston's Organ mechanosensory neurons elicits antennal grooming

Given that grooming is induced by stimulation of the body surface, we first sought to identify sensory neurons that could relay such stimulation from the antennae to the brain. To this end, we revisited the GAL4 lines we identified in our previous behavioral screen and examined them for expression in the antennae (Seeds et al., 2014). One line expressed GAL4 in mechanosensory chordotonal neurons of the Johnston's Organ (JO, see below), and elicited antennal grooming when the targeted neurons were thermally activated by dTrpA1 (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A,B). Because this line also expressed in interneurons in the CNS (Figure 1—figure supplement 1C,D), we identified four additional GAL4 lines that target these sensory neurons by visually screening an image database of GAL4 line expression patterns (Jenett et al., 2012). Each of these lines elicited antennal grooming when used to thermogenetically activate the targeted neurons, further implicating this population of JO neurons in the behavior (Figure 1—figure supplement 1G–J, behavior not shown).

While our results were consistent with these sensory neurons being responsible for eliciting antennal grooming, the expression of GAL4 in other neurons allowed the possibility that they were contributing to this behavior. To examine whether the JO neurons elicit antennal grooming, we used the intersectional Split GAL4 (spGAL4) approach in an effort to restrict GAL4 activity to these neurons (Luan et al., 2006; Pfeiffer et al., 2010). Specifically, the activation domain (AD) of GAL4 is expressed in the genomic enhancer-driven pattern of one identified GAL4 line, while the DNA binding domain (DBD) of GAL4 is expressed in the pattern of another line (see ‘Materials and methods’). When both halves are co-expressed in the same cell, the activity of GAL4 is reconstituted. By co-expressing the AD and DBD in the patterns of different enhancer pairs, we observed reconstitution of GAL4 activity in the JO neurons (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A–E′). Five of these different AD/DBD pairs showed increased antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation (Figure 1A, pairs referred to as aJO-spGAL4-1 through aJO-spGAL4-5, Supplementary file 1 shows enhancer pairs used).

Figure 1. Sensory neurons that elicit antennal grooming.

(A) Grooming movements performed by flies in which aJO spGAL4 pairs drove expression of thermally activated dTrpA1. Movements were manually scored from 2 min of recorded video per fly (n ≥ 17 flies per spGAL4). Colors correspond to the percent of total time spent performing each movement. (B) Percent time flies spent antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation of neurons targeted by spGAL4 pairs, with or without their antennae (filled or open boxes, respectively). Bottom and top of the boxes indicate the first and third quartiles respectively; median is the red line; whiskers show the upper and lower 1.5 IQR; red dots are data outliers (n ≥ 17 for each box; asterisks show p < 0.0001, Kruskal–Wallis and post hoc Mann–Whitney U pairwise tests with Bonferroni correction). Dotted line marks the median of the intact control. (CF) aJO-spGAL4-1 driving expression of green fluorescent protein (GFP). Maximum intensity projections are shown. (C) Frontal view of the head (native GFP fluorescence, green; cuticle autofluorescence, magenta). Left bracket shows the third antennal segment. Right bracket marks the second antennal segment, which is shown in (D). Scale bar, 100 μm. (D) Second antennal segment co-stained with anti-GFP (green) and anti-Elav (magenta, marks neuronal nuclei) antibodies. White arrows show the ventral and dorsal aJO clusters. Scale bar, 25 μm. (E, F) Central nervous system (CNS) co-stained with anti-GFP (green) and anti-Bruchpilot (magenta) to visualize the aJO afferent projections into the ventral brain neuropile (E) and their specific targeting of the indicated antennal mechanosensory and motor center (AMMC) and subesophageal zone (SEZ) regions (arrows shown in F). Box in (E) indicates region shown in F. Scale bars, (E) 100 μm and (F) 25 μm. Prothoracic neuromeres (ProNm). Ventral nervous system (VNS). See also Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.003

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. GAL4 lines that target expression to sensory neurons from the antennae and elicit grooming.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Percent of total time three GAL4 lines expressing dTrpA1 spent antennal grooming, compared with controls. Box plots, statistics, and experimental conditions are as described in Figure 1A,B (n ≥ 10, asterisks represent: ***p < 0.001, *p < 0.01). R26B12-GAL4 and R18C11-GAL4 expression patterns are shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 1B,C. (B) Head GFP expression pattern of R39A11-GAL4 (native GFP fluorescence shown, green). The cuticular autofluorescence is shown in magenta. Scale bar, 100 μm. (C, D) Expression in (C) the CNS and (D) SEZ of R39A11-GAL4. Brains were co-stained with anti-GFP (green) and anti-bruchpilot (magenta). Scale bar, (D) 50 μm. (E, F) CNS images of control lines used in this study: (E) GAL4 and (F) spGAL4 controls do not show significant expression. Scale bars, 100 μm. (G, J) GFP expression patterns of the CNS of GAL4 lines that had expression in sensory neurons projecting from the antennae and displayed increased antennal grooming with dTrpA1. (G) R25F11-GAL4, (H) R52F12-GAL4, (I) R60E02-GAL4 (aJO-GAL4-1) and (J) R27H08-GAL4 (aJO-GAL4-2).
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. spGAL4 pairs that target expression to sensory neurons in the antennae and elicit grooming.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(AE) CNS expression patterns of spGAL4 line pairs that displayed increased antennal grooming with dTrpA1. (A′E′) Native GFP expression (green) in the antennae of corresponding spGAL4 lines shown in AE. The cuticular autofluorescence is shown in magenta. Scale bars, 50 μm.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. JO neurons projecting to zone C/E elicit antennal grooming.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(A, B) Co-expression of JO sensory neurons using aJO-LexA to express GFP (green) and (A) JO4-GAL4 or (B) JO31-GAL4 lines to express tdTomato (magenta). See Figure 4—figure supplement 1 for more details about aJO-LexA. aJO-LexA neurons have distinct projections in the ventral SEZ (middle and right panel, red arrows), but show overlapping arborizations with zone C/E neurons in the AMMC region (right panel, white arrows) driven by JO4-GAL4. Note in A (left and right panel) that aJO-LexA does not overlap with the zone A projections (blue arrow). Scale bar, 50 μm. (CG) CNS expression patterns of JO-GAL4 lines that target different zones in the AMMC. (C) JO4-GAL4 (zones A and C/E); (D) JO31-GAL4 (zones C/E); (E) JO3-GAL4 (zones A, B, C/E, D); (F) JO22-GAL4 (zone A), and (G) JO15-GAL4 (zone A and B). Scale bar, 100 μm. (H) Number of bouts per minute of antennal grooming that each JO-GAL4 line expressing CsChrimson performed with optogenetic activation, with or without their antennae (filled or open boxes, respectively). Box plots and statistics are described in Figure 1B. (n ≥ 4 flies per JO-GAL4, asterisks represent: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.)
Figure 1—figure supplement 4. Most stochastically labeled aJO neurons show projections to both the AMMC and ventral SEZ.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

(A) Overview of the aJO neuron population with AMMC, posterior, and ventral SEZ projections (white, yellow, and red arrows point to each projection respectively). (BF) Multicolor stochastic labeling of aJO neurons in five different brains. The full patterns shown in panels B, C, D, E, F, whereas the separated channels are shown in panels B′, B′′, C′, C′′, D′D′′′, E′E′′′. Cases where single neuron clones were obtained using this method are labeled with red numbers. (G, H) Lateral view of two neurons labeled with the same color from panel B′′ in (G) and from panel C in (H) show projections to the AMMC (white arrow), ventral SEZ (red arrow) and posterior SEZ (yellow arrow). (I) Table shows the analysis of the projection branches of individual neurons corresponding with the red numbers in the figure panels. The + or − signs indicate that the neurons either do or do not project to the indicated region respectively. Scale bars, 50 μm.

Confocal imaging confirmed that each spGAL4 pair targeted neurons within the second antennal segment, as revealed by staining for the neuronal protein Elav (Figure 1C,D, Figure 1—figure supplement 2A′–E′) (Kamikouchi et al., 2006). The anatomy of these neurons identifies them as subsets of the approximately 500 chordotonal neurons within the JO, a mechanosensory structure that detects antennal movements. All five pairs were expressed in two distinct clusters of 40–50 neurons each in the dorsal and ventral regions of the JO, and we designate these clusters collectively as the antennal grooming JO (aJO) (Figure 1D, Figure 1—figure supplement 2A′–E′). Although four of the pairs still targeted GAL4 activity to neurons outside of the JO (Figure 1—figure supplement 2B–E), aJO-spGAL4-1 expressed almost exclusively in the aJO, providing strong evidence that these sensory neurons elicit antennal grooming (Figure 1D,E). To independently evaluate the necessity of the aJO in antennal grooming, we tested whether amputation of the antennae, and thus removal of the JO, would abolish the thermogenetically elicited behavior of the spGAL4 pairs. Indeed, antennal amputation abolished grooming at temperatures that would normally activate dTrpA1 and elicit antennal grooming (Figure 1B). These experiments demonstrate that the aJO induces antennal grooming.

The JO comprises different subgroups of chordotonal neurons whose axons project to distinct zones (zones A–E) in the brain antennal mechanosensory and motor center (AMMC) (Kamikouchi et al., 2006; Yorozu et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2014). aJO axons enter the AMMC with zone C/E neurons (Figure 1—figure supplement 3A–B). However, unlike previously described C/E neurons that terminate in the AMMC, the aJO have three apparent projections within the ventral brain: the AMMC, the ventral subesophageal zone (SEZ), and the posterior SEZ (Figure 1E,F). To test whether each neuron within the aJO has all three projections, we performed multicolor stochastic labeling (Nern et al., 2015), which allows for visualization of individual neurons within the aJO population. Because each of the single neurons that we isolated projects from the AMMC to the ventral SEZ (Figure 1—figure supplement 4A–F,I), it would appear that the majority of neurons within the aJO have similar projections. A smaller subset appears to project from the AMMC to the posterior SEZ; however, we were unable to isolate individual cells to definitively show this (Figure 1—figure supplement 4G,H). In contrast to previously identified zone C/E-projecting neurons, we found no evidence of aJO neurons that project only to the AMMC. Because none of the previously described JO neurons project to the ventral SEZ (Kamikouchi et al., 2006; Matsuo et al., 2014), the aJO corresponds to a previously unrecognized set of neurons.

Given that aJO neurons project to zone C/E before passing to the SEZ, we tested whether activation of previously described C/E neurons could elicit antennal grooming. Indeed, activation of zone C/E neurons using published GAL4 drivers (Kamikouchi et al., 2006) elicited antennal grooming (Figure 1—figure supplement 3C–E,H). In contrast, activation of zone A or B neurons did not elicit antennal grooming (Figure 1—figure supplement 3F,G,H), indicating that only the zone C/E subpopulation elicits the behavior. Therefore, our data indicate that at least two types of zone C/E-projecting neurons are sufficient to induce grooming. The first terminates within the AMMC and constitutes a previously described set of JO neurons (Kamikouchi et al., 2006), whereas the second type are the aJO neurons that project into the AMMC and then ventrally to the SEZ.

Three different interneuron classes elicit antennal grooming

The muscles that control front leg movements necessary for antennal grooming are innervated by neurons residing in the most anterior region of the ventral nervous system (VNS), the prothoracic neuromeres (ProNm) (Figure 1E) (Burrows, 1996; Brierley et al., 2012). Because JO afferent projections terminate in the brain and do not project to the ProNm, where they would be positioned to activate leg movements, we reasoned that additional neurons must project to the ProNm to command grooming behavior. Therefore, we sought to identify interneurons that transmit the sensory signal to the ProNm.

Of the GAL4 lines that elicited antennal grooming in our previous screen (Seeds et al., 2014), two lack expression in antennal sensory neurons but have expression in interneurons within the brain (lines R26B12 and R18C11, behavioral analysis in Figure 1—figure supplement 1A, expression patterns in Figure 2—figure supplement 1A–C). A visual screen of the GAL4 line expression pattern database (Jenett et al., 2012) identified additional lines with interneuron projection patterns in the AMMC and SEZ that made them candidates for associating with the JO projections. Five of these lines elicited antennal grooming when thermogenetically activated (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). As each GAL4 line expressed in other neuronal populations (Figure 2—figure supplement 1D–H), we again generated spGAL4 lines using their respective enhancers to further restrict GAL4 activity to the behaviorally relevant interneurons. With this approach, we identified spGAL4 pairs that elicited antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation, and targeted interneurons that we designated aBN1, aBN2, and antennal descending neuron (aDN) for reasons described below (Figure 2A, Supplementary file 1). Of note, there were striking differences in the amounts of grooming elicited by these pairs (Figure 2A). For example, aDN1-spGAL4-1 flies spent 4.8% of their time grooming their antennae vs 66.1% for aBN2-spGAL4-2 (Figure 2B, black boxes).

Figure 2. Interneurons that elicit antennal grooming.

(A) Grooming movements performed by interneuron spGAL4 pairs expressing thermally activated dTrpA1. Data was obtained and displayed as described in Figure 1A. (B) Percent time flies spent antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation of interneurons targeted by spGAL4 pairs, with or without their antennae (filled or open boxes respectively). Box plots and statistics are described in Figure 1B. Asterisks represent the following p values: *p < 0.01, **p < 0.001, ***p < 0.0001 (n ≥ 9 flies per spGAL4). Black p value statistics show differences between control and spGAL4 flies with their antennae. Gray statistics show differences between each spGAL4 with and without the antennae. (CF) GFP expression patterns of spGAL4 lines: (C) aBN1-spGAL4-1, (D) aBN2-spGAL4-2, (E) aDN1-spGAL4-1, (F) aDN2-spGAL4-4. Images show maximum intensity projections of co-staining with anti-GFP (green) and anti-Bruchpilot (magenta). White arrows show cell bodies. Scale bars, 100 μm. (G) antennal descending neurons (aDNs) targeted by each spGAL4 pair. Circles represent one of three neurons in aDN-GAL4. Filled circles show which neurons are targeted by each spGAL4 (enhancer pairs listed). (H) Two aDN neurons are targeted in a triple spGAL4 combination expressing GFP (white arrows). No spGAL4 combinations were identified that exclusively target aDN3. Scale bar, 25 μm. (I) Graphical summary of neuronal expression patterns of the spGAL4 pairs. Green boxes indicate expression of the pair on the left (rows) in the indicated neurons or region listed above the grid (columns). Figure 2—figure supplement 2 shows the locations of these neurons. Black framing highlights antennal grooming neurons.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.009

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. GAL4 lines that elicit antennal grooming.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Grooming movements performed by GAL4 lines expressing thermally activated dTrpA1. Data was obtained and displayed as described in Figure 1A (n = 10 per line). Note: R11B11-GAL4 did not perform significantly increased antennal grooming, however, this line was able to target antennal grooming interneurons when its enhancer was used for generating spGAL4 intersections. (BH) Images of antennal grooming GAL4 lines expressing GFP. CNSs were co-stained with anti-GFP (green) and anti-bruchpilot (magenta). (B) R26B12-GAL4, (C) R18C11-GAL4 (aDN-GAL4), (D) R34C03-GAL4, (E) R11B11-GAL4, (F) R70H02-GAL4, (G) R71D01-GAL4, and (H) R76F12-GAL4. Inset in C shows higher magnification of three candidate antennal grooming interneuron cell bodies. White arrow in (B, C) show where projections from JO sensory neurons would enter the brain if the GAL4 line targeted expression to them. Scale bars, 100 μm.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. spGAL4 lines with interneuron expression that elicit antennal grooming.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(AI) spGAL4 lines expressing GFP (green) are co-stained with anti-GFP (green) with anti-bruchpilot (magenta). The activation domains (ADs) and DNA binding domains (DBDs) used are shown below each corresponding spGAL4 pair name. Arrows or lines indicate neurons or populations of neurons targeted by each pair. Summary expression data for all spGAL4 pairs are shown in Figure 2I. (A) aBN1-spGAL4-1, (B) aBN1-spGAL4-2, (C) aBN1/aDN1-spGAL4, (D) aBN2-spGAL4-1, (E) aBN2-spGAL4-2, (F) aDN1-spGAL4-1, (G) aDN2-spGAL4-2, (H) aDN2-spGAL4-3, and (I) aDN2-spGAL4-4. Insets in D, E show aBN2 cell bodies at higher magnification; (D) shows three cell bodies and (E) shows five. Scale bars, 100 μm. (J) Summary of neuronal types targeted by interneuron spGAL4 pairs. Abbreviations: abdominal ganglion cluster (AG-Cl), ascending cluster (AscCl), metathoracic neuromere cluster (MetaNmCl), mesothoracic neuromere cluster (MesaNmCl), ProNm cluster (ProNmCl), posterior cluster (pCl 1 + 2), dorsolateral cluster (dLatCl), lateral cells (latCells), VNS, central complex (CX), optic lobes (OL), aDN, antennal local interneurons (aBN), pars intercerebralis cluster (PI). Dashed lines show neuronal population on posterior side of the brain.

To validate that the spGAL4 lines target interneurons, we removed sensory neurons by amputating the antennae, and then measured the time spent grooming during thermogenetic activation. As anticipated, amputation did not abolish movements directed towards the antennal region, and some pairs significantly increased such movements relative to intact antennae (Figure 2B, aBN1/aDN1-spGAL4, aBN2-spGAL4-1, and aDN2-spGAL4-2/3/4 show increased grooming). Thus, we conclude that these spGAL4 pairs target interneurons that induce antennal grooming when activated. Moreover, the increased grooming associated with antennal amputation raises the possibility that grooming might be negatively influenced by sensory feedback from the antennae.

We next examined the anatomy of these interneurons in greater detail. Two classes that induce antennal grooming are located entirely in the brain, and we designate them antennal grooming brain interneurons 1 and 2 (aBN1 and aBN2). aBN1-spGAL4-1 targeted expression to aBN1, a single interneuron in the ventral brain (Figure 2C). Two other spGAL4 pairs also targeted aBN1 and were able to elicit antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation (aBN1-spGAL4-2, aBN1/aDN1-spGAL4, Figure 2I, Figure 2—figure supplement 2A–C). aBN2 was targeted by two spGAL4 pairs that each used the R26B12 enhancer, which itself expresses in a cluster of eight neurons with cell bodies in the posterior and ventrolateral brain. Each pair targets expression to either three or five of these neurons (aBN2-spGAL4-1, aBN2-spGAL4-2, Figure 2D,I, Figure 2—figure supplement 2D,E, Figure 4—figure supplement 2C,D).

The other interneuron class comprises aDNs that project from the brain to the VNS. Five spGAL4 pairs targeted single descending neurons with cell bodies located in the posteroventral SEZ (Figure 2E,F,I, Figure 2—figure supplement 2C,F–I). Given that the R18C11 enhancer was used to generate several of these pairs, and by itself targets expression to three aDNs when driving GAL4 (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C), we asked whether each spGAL4 pair targeted the same neuron or distinct neurons. By simultaneously combining the GAL4 DBD, expressed under control of the R18C11 enhancer, with two versions of the GAL4 AD, one expressed under control of the R76F12 enhancer and the other controlled by the R34C03 enhancer, we observed two aDNs (Figure 2G,H). This demonstrates that the spGAL4 pairs target distinct descending interneurons, and we named these aDN1 and aDN2. However, we did not identify a spGAL4 combination that exclusively expresses in the third R18C11-targeted aDN (named aDN3).

A putative circuit that elicits grooming in response to antennal displacement

The identified neuronal classes all have projections in the ventral brain (Figure 3A–D). aBNs project to the AMMC and SEZ, following the aJO projections (Figure 3B,C), whereas two aDNs (aDN1 and aDN2), and likely aDN3 project both to the SEZ and through the cervical connective to the ProNm (Figure 3D, only aDN1 shown). Manual and computational alignment of the projections from these classes suggested intimate associations and the potential to form a neural circuit (Figure 3E, Video 1, Video 2). Thus, we explored their functional relationships.

Video 1. Traced antennal grooming circuit.

Download video file (39.6MB, avi)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.012

Traced and manually aligned neurons are shown in different colors. aJO neurons (blue) project from the second antennal segment into the anterior brain. aBN1 (red) has cell bodies in the dorsal and posterior brain. aBN2 is shown in yellow. aDN1 is shown in green and sends descending projections to the VNS.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.012

Video 2. Computationally aligned antennal grooming circuit.

Download video file (10.3MB, mov)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.013

Computationally aligned neurons are shown in different colors. aJO neurons (blue), aBN1 (red), aBN2 (yellow) and aDN1 (green). The neuropil was stained with anti-bruchpilot (grey). See ‘Materials and methods’ for a description of how the computational alignment and rendering of images were done.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.013

Figure 3. Neurons that elicit antennal grooming have neurites in the AMMC and/or SEZ.

Figure 3.

(AD) spGAL4 pairs targeting each neuronal class in the ventral brain: (A) aJO-spGAL4-1, (B) aBN1-spGAL4-1, (C) aBN2-spGAL4-2, and (D) aDN1-spGAL4-1. aDN1 is shown as an example in (D), but there are additional aDNs (aDN2 and aDN3, see Figure 2F and Figure 2—figure supplement 2G–I). CNSs stained with anti-GFP (green) and anti-bruchpilot (magenta). Maximum intensity projections are shown from frontal and lateral views. Arrows show the different projection regions: AMMC (white), posterior SEZ (yellow), and ventral SEZ (red). Scale bars, 25 μm. (E) Traced neurons in different colors manually aligned (also shown in Video 1).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.008

We first asked whether the grooming elicited by aJO thermogenetic activation required the activity of the aBNs and aDNs by performing a behavioral epistasis test in which we genetically silenced the activity of the interneurons while activating the aJO (Figure 4A). For silencing the interneurons, we used the aBN spGAL4 pairs and aDN-GAL4 to target expression of the synaptic transmission blocker, tetanus toxin (TNT) (Sweeney et al., 1995). To genetically access the aJO independent of the interneurons, we employed the LexA binary transcriptional system so that the aJO enhancer R27H08 directed expression of LexA (Lai and Lee, 2006; Pfeiffer et al., 2010). The resulting aJO-LexA line targeted aJO neurons and also elicited antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A,B). Although aJO-LexA targets more JO neurons than the aJO spGAL4 pairs (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A,B), expression of TNT in the aJO by aJO-spGAL4-1 significantly decreased antennal grooming in response to thermogenetic activation by aJO-LexA. This showed that the aJO-spGAL4-1 neurons constituted a major portion of the grooming elicited with aJO-LexA (Figure 4B, blue boxes). However, given that aJO-LexA targets additional zone C/E neurons that could induce grooming (Figure 1—figure supplement 3C–E,H), it is possible that they are responsible for the residual grooming that occurs with expression of TNT in aJO-spGAL4-1 (Figure 4B, blue boxes). Therefore, we hereafter refer to the neurons targeted by aJO-LexA as aJO+C/E neurons. We next found that TNT expression in the interneurons aBN1 and aBN2 suppressed grooming when aJO+C/E neurons were thermogenetically activated (Figure 4B, red and yellow, respectively). However, grooming was not suppressed when TNT was expressed in the aDNs (Figure 4B, green boxes). We conclude that the aBNs are necessary for the grooming response to aJO+C/E activation, while additional descending neurons important for mediating antennal grooming may remain to be identified.

Figure 4. Functional relationships among putative antennal circuit components.

(A) Overview of experiments shown in (B, E). Grooming was induced by thermogenetic activation of Johnston's Organ (JO) neurons (dTrpA1) or by imposed displacements of the antennae. Synaptic release was blocked in different neuronal classes expressing tetanus toxin (TNT). (B) Antennal grooming performed by flies with thermogenetic activation of the aJO while inhibiting synaptic release in interneuron classes. The experiment was performed and data is displayed as described in Figure 1B (n ≥ 11 flies per spGAL4). (CE) To displace the antennae, iron powder was glued to the third antennal segment and the flies were tethered within an electromagnet. (C) Image of the electromagnet apparatus. (D) Tethered fly with iron powder on its antennae. Magnetic pulses were delivered to displace the third antennal segment at 1 Hz for 4 × 10 s periods, with 30 s rests between stimulations. Flies were recorded and their grooming movements were manually scored (see Figure 4—figure supplement 3D for ethograms). (E) The percent time that flies spent grooming their antennae within the total assay time is shown. The grooming responses to antennal movements were also tested while blocking synaptic release in the different neuronal types with TNT. Box plots and statistics are shown as described in Figure 1B (n ≥ 11 flies per line).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.014

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. aJO-, aBN2-, and aDN-LexA lines.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Percent of total time that LexA lines expressing thermally activated dTrpA1 spent antennal grooming. Box plots, statistics, and experimental conditions are as described in Figure 1A,B (n > 10; ***p < 0.001). (BD) CNSs co-stained with anti-GFP (green) and anti-bruchpilot (magenta). LexA lines are as follows: (B) aJO-LexA, (C) aBN2-LexA, and (D) aDN-LexA. Scale bar, 100 μm. Box insets show higher magnification of cell bodies of interneurons involved in antennal grooming; (C) shows four cell bodies and (D) shows three.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Co-expression of LexA lines with selected spGAL4 pairs.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(AF) Co-expression of LexA and spGAL4 lines. Left column: spGAL4 expression patterns. Middle column: LexA expression patterns. Right column: Merged expression patterns. (A, B) Co-expression of aJO-LexA (magenta) with (A) aJO-spGAL4-1 or (B) aJO-spGAL4-3 (green). Frontal views: Arrows point to JO projections that do not co-localize with aJO projections. (C, D) aBN2-LexA (magenta) expressed with (C) aBN2-spGAL4-1 or (D) aBN2-spGAL4-2 (green). (E, F) Co-expression of aDN-LexA with (E) aDN1-spGAL4-1 or (F) aDN2-spGAL4-2. Scale bar, 25 μm.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Testing of stimulus parameters for the antennal displacement assay.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

(AC) Flies were prepared as shown in Figure 4C,D (see ‘Materials and methods’) and tested for their grooming responses when different magnetic field frequencies (A) or voltages (B) were applied to displace their antennae. The grooming responses were recorded and manually scored. Plots are displayed as described in Figure 1B. (A) Box plots show the percent time that flies groomed their antennae when the magnetic field that was turned on and off at different frequencies (magnetic strength was set at 10 Volts). (B) Percent time that flies groomed their antennae when the magnetic field was applied at different voltages (frequency set at 1 Hz). Raw data used to generate the red box is shown in D. (C) Side view of a fly head showing the trajectories of the antennae before (grey) and after (green) the magnetic field was applied. Red asterisks and arrow show the measured distances. The values shown below the image represent the measured distances that the antennae moved at the indicated voltages. (D) Ethograms of grooming movements performed by 16 control flies (w1118; UAS-TeTxLC.tnt; pBPGAL4U) whose antennae were stimulated in 4 × 10 s periods by a magnetic field of 570 gauss (10 Volts) at a frequency of 1 Hz. The magnet was left off for 30 s between periods. These flies are the controls used for the experiment shown in Figure 4E.

We next sought to identify a sensory stimulus upstream of this putative grooming circuit. Because JO neurons detect antennal movements (Kamikouchi et al., 2009; Yorozu et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2014), we reasoned that the role of the aJO might be to elicit grooming in response to physical displacements of the antennae. To test this, we glued iron powder to the third antennal segments and tethered the flies within an electromagnetic behavioral set up (Figure 4C,D). Application of a magnetic field at 1 Hz caused visible displacements of the antennae (31 ± 6 μm) and elicited grooming of both the antennae and other head parts (Figure 4E, white boxes, Figure 4—figure supplement 3C,D, Video 3). The aJO was critical for this response because expression of TNT in these neurons significantly reduced antennal and head grooming in response to the magnetic field (Figure 4E, blue boxes, head grooming not shown). Given that activation of aJO elicits antennal grooming almost exclusively (Figure 1A, Figure 7—figure supplement 1A,B), the role of the aJO in head grooming is unclear.

Video 3. Grooming movements performed in response to displacements of the antennae.

Download video file (24.6MB, mov)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.018

The third antennal segments of a control fly were coated with iron powder, and the fly was tethered within the electromagnetic apparatus shown in Figure 4C,D. The infrared light positioned behind the fly shows when the magnetic field was applied to displace the antennae.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.018

We next examined the functional necessity of the different interneuron classes for grooming in response to antennal displacement. aBN1 was found to be necessary, as expression of TNT in aBN1-spGAL4-1 or aBN1-spGAL4-2 both reduced the grooming response (Figure 4E, red boxes). Intriguingly, expression of TNT targeted by the two different aBN2 spGAL4 pairs gave opposing results in this assay (Figure 4E, yellow boxes): aBN2-spGAL4-1 activity showed necessity for antennal grooming while aBN2-spGAL4-2 did not. This contrasted with our earlier finding that both spGAL4 pairs disrupted grooming when the aJO+C/E neurons were thermogenetically activated (Figure 4B, yellow boxes). This may reflect a functional difference between the specific subsets of aBN2 neurons targeted by these two spGAL4 pairs in the context of antennal displacement. Similar to the results for thermogenetic activation of aJO+C/E neurons, expression of TNT (or the inward rectifying potassium channel Kir) in all three aDNs did not block the grooming response to antennal displacement (Figure 4E, green box, Kir data not shown). Taken together, our results demonstrate that all identified neurons within the putative circuit elicit antennal grooming when activated, but not all are necessary for the grooming response to antennal displacement.

Functional connectivity among the antennal grooming neuronal classes

To address the potential for connectivity among these neurons, we first examined the relative proximities of their projections in the brain by visualizing expression of LexA in one neuronal class and spGAL4 in another. We found that the aBN1 co-localized with all major aJO projections, whereas aBN2 co-localized with the aJO, AMMC, and ventral SEZ projections (Figure 5A,B, arrows). Interestingly, co-visualization of aJO with aDN1 or aDN2 revealed that the aDNs have distinct projections: aDN1 co-localizes with the aJO in the AMMC and ventral SEZ, whereas aDN2 only associated with the most ventral SEZ projections (Figure 5C,D, arrows). We confirmed that aDN1 projects more dorsally than aDN2 by examining their relative projections within the aDN-LexA pattern (Figure 4—figure supplement 2E,F). In support of our conclusion that the aBNs and aDNs are in close proximity with the JO projections, GFP-positive staining indicates reconstitution across synaptic partner experiments reported membrane contact among these neurons (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A–D) (Feinberg et al., 2008; Gordon and Scott, 2009).

Figure 5. Antennal grooming neurons are in close proximity.

(AH) Co-expression in neuronal pairs using two binary expression systems (LexA and spGAL4) to express tdTomato or GFP in each neuronal class. Processed maximum intensity projections of frontal and lateral views are shown. See ‘Materials and methods’ about how images were processed (unprocessed images in Figure 5—figure supplement 2). Scale bars, 25 μm. (AD) Proximity between aJO-LexA targeted sensory projections (magenta) and the following interneuron spGAL4 pairs (green): (A) aBN1-spGAL4-1, (B) aBN2-spGAL4-1, (C) aDN1-spGAL4-1 and (D) aDN2-spGAL4-2. (EG) Proximity between aBN2-LexA targeted neurons (magenta) and the following interneuron spGAL4 pairs (green): (E) aBN1-spGAL4-1, (F) aDN1-spGAL4-1, (G) aDN2-spGAL4-2. (H) Proximity between aDN-LexA targeted neurons aDN (green) and aBN1-spGAL4-1 targeted neurons (magenta). Overlap between different projections of the LexA and spGAL4-targeted neurons is indicated by different colored arrows: (AC, E, F) AMMC projections white arrows, (A) posterior SEZ projection (yellow arrow), (BH) ventral SEZ projections (red arrows).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.019

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. GFP-positive staining indicates reconstitution across synaptic partner (GRASP) staining indicates close proximity of neurons involved in antennal grooming.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(AH) Expression of different GFP halves (spGFP11 and spGFP1-10) in putative connected neuronal pairs. aJO-LexA drove expression of spGFP11 in the JO neurons and spGFP1-10 expression was driven by the following spGAL4 lines: (A) aBN1-spGAL4-1, (B) aBN2-spGAL4-1, (C) aDN1-spGAL4-1, (D) aDN2-spGAL4-2, or (E) spGAL4 control. Brains were stained with an antibody that recognizes reconstituted GFP (green) (Gordon and Scott, 2009). The neuropil was stained with an anti-bruchpilot antibody (magenta). GRASP where neurons contacted each other. (FH) Controls where only GFP1-10 was expressed by: (F) aBN1-spGAL4-1 or (G) aBN2-spGAL4-1, or (H) aDN1-spGAL4-1. (I) White box marks the area shown in AH. Scale bar, 100 μm.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Co-staining indicates close proximity of neurons involved in antennal grooming.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

(AH) Unprocessed maximum intensity projections of co-stained images shown in Figure 5 (see ‘Materials and methods’).

We next examined projections of the interneuron types. For this, lines targeting LexA to either aBN2 or aDN were generated using the appropriate enhancers (Supplementary file 1). Both of these lines elicit antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation, and aDN-LexA targets three aDN neurons, while aBN2-LexA targets a cluster of four aBN2 neurons (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A,C,D, Figure 4—figure supplement 2C–F). Projections of aBN1 and aBN2 were closely associated in both the AMMC and ventral SEZ but lacked associations in the posterior SEZ (Figure 5E, arrows). Both aBN classes projected near the aDNs in the SEZ, but aDN1 also arborized near the dorsal projections of aBN2 (Figure 5F–H, arrows). We conclude that the projections of these sensory neurons and interneurons are in close proximity with each other, and potentially form a functionally connected circuit.

To test for functional connectivity among the neuronal classes, we measured calcium responses of the different interneurons when other neurons in the putative circuit were optogenetically activated. In isolated CNSs, we activated aJO+C/E neurons expressing the red light-inducible neuronal activator CsChrimson, while measuring fluorescence of the calcium responder GCaMP6s in the interneurons (Chen et al., 2013; Klapoetke et al., 2014). We detected significant calcium responses in aBN1, aBN2, and aDN1 (Figure 6A–C, Figure 6—figure supplement 1A–D, Figure 6—figure supplement 2, Figure 6—figure supplement 3), but only a weak response in aDN2 even with high-intensity red light (Figure 6D). Thus, aBN1, aBN2, aDN1 are likely downstream of aJO+C/E neurons, while aDN2 may be weakly or indirectly downstream of these sensory neurons.

Figure 6. Different antennal grooming neurons are functionally connected.

(AI) Dissected CNSs with different neuronal classes expressing CsChrimson (magenta) were activated with red light while changes in calcium in their putative downstream partners expressing GCaMP6 (green) were imaged (ΔF/F). Each tested neuronal pair is shown using circles and as traced pairs. The direction of the connection and whether it is excitatory or inhibitory is depicted with an arrow (excitatory) or ball and stick (inhibitory). Changes in fluorescence of GCaMP6s of multiple flies under similar stimulus conditions are shown on the right (average ± s.e.m., 3–5 flies tested with 9–21 trials per trace). Arrow below each trace shows when the red light pulse was delivered. Black traces show flies that were imaged without drug treatment, whereas orange and blue traces were imaged while the nervous system was bathed with mecamylamine or picrotoxin respectively. See ‘Materials and methods’, Figure 6—figure supplement 1, Figure 6—figure supplement 2, Figure 6—figure supplement 3, and Supplementary file 2 for detailed ‘Materials and methods’, stimulus conditions, and controls. (AD) aJO-LexA tested with the following interneuron spGAL4 pairs: (A) aBN1-spGAL4-1, (B) aBN2-spGAL4-1, (C) aDN1-spGAL4-1, and (D) aDN2-spGAL4-2. (E, F) aBN2-LexA tested with aBN1-spGAL4-1. (G) aBN1-spGAL4-1 tested with aDN-LexA. (H, I) aBN2-LexA tested with either (H) aDN1-spGAL4-1 or (I) aDN2-spGAL4-2.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.022

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Functional connectivity: controls, technical details, and raw data.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) Control experiments for CsChrimson/GCaMP6s activation. LexAop-CsChrimson was crossed with the control LexA driver, and GCaMP6s was expressed with aBN2-spGAL4-1 (top) or aDN2-spGAL4-2 (bottom). Flies were tested and imaging results are displayed as described in Figure 5. Average changes in fluorescence ±s.e.m. of multiple flies. (BD) Representative examples of the regions imaged for (B) aBN1, (C) aBN2, and (D) aDNs. Top panel: example average projections of an experimental run. Middle panel: regions of interest used for analysis (see ‘Materials and methods’). Lower panel: black rectangles show the approximate positions of the fields of view in each whole pattern. Scale bars, 10 μm.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2. Raw data for functional connectivity experiments (at low intensity red light).

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.

Raw data for experiments shown in Figure 6. All experiments shown were done in the absence of drugs. Red light intensity was set at 50 μW/mm2. Each column corresponds to the number of light pulses delivered, where each light pulse was 2 ms and the interpulse intervals were 18 ms. Each trace shown is the average of four responses recorded at ∼20 s intervals. Each row represents a different genotype and colored traces in a given row correspond to an individual CNS. Multiple runs were often performed for a given CNS and set of conditions (shown by the same colored traces in a row). Black boxes show which conditions were used to generate the average traces displayed in Figure 6. All traces within the black boxes were used to generate traces shown in Figure 6A–D. For Figure 6E–I, colored dots correspond to samples that were used to generate pre-drug treatment average traces and then used for the pharmacology experiments (traces with drugs are not shown). Traces of samples used for mecamylamine experiments are marked using blue, green, red dots, whereas, those for or picrotoxin are marked using black, grey, orange dots.
Figure 6—figure supplement 3. Raw data for functional connectivity experiments (at high intensity red light).

Figure 6—figure supplement 3.

Raw data for experiments shown in Figure 6. All experiments shown were done in the absence of drugs. Red light intensity was set betwen 290 to 700 μW/mm2. Each column corresponds to the number of light pulses delivered, where each light pulse was 2 ms and the interpulse intervals were 18 ms. Each trace shown is the average of four responses recorded at ∼20 s intervals. Each row represents a different genotype and colored traces in a given row correspond to an individual CNS. Multiple runs were sometimes performed for a given CNS and set of conditions (shown by the same colored traces in a row). Black boxes show which conditions were used to generate the average traces displayed in Figure 6. All traces within the black boxes were used to generate traces shown in Figure 6A–D. For Figure 6E–I, colored dots correspond to samples that were used to generate pre-drug treatment average traces and then used for the pharmacology experiments (traces with drugs are not shown). Traces of samples used for mecamylamine experiments are marked using blue, green, red dots, whereas, those for or picrotoxin are marked using black, grey, orange dots.

We next tested whether CsChrimson-mediated optogenetic activation of either aBN1 or aBN2 induced a calcium response in the other. Activation of aBN1 induced a calcium response in aBN2 (Figure 6E), whereas only high-intensity red light activation of aBN2 could cause a weak calcium response in aBN1 (Figure 6F). The latter response was inconsistent, as only three out of five flies tested showed increases in calcium in aBN1 (average trace of three trials are shown in Figure 6F, raw traces of all five trials shown in Figure 6—figure supplement 3). Further, because the aBN2-LexA driver used to express CsChrimson in aBN2 also expresses in neurons in other parts of the brain (Figure 4—figure supplement 1C), we cannot rule out these other neurons as causing the aBN1 calcium response. Therefore, our results strongly support excitation from aBN1 to aBN2, but we only find weak functional imaging-based evidence supporting the reverse connection. Nevertheless, the excitatory responses that we observed are cholinergic because they were suppressed by the cholinergic receptor antagonist mecamylamine (Figure 6E,F).

We next tested whether aBNs are upstream of the aDNs. Activation of aBN1 induced a calcium response in the aDNs, and this excitatory response is cholinergic as it was suppressed by mecamylamine (Figure 6G). Interestingly, activation of aBN2 caused a mecamylamine-sensitive excitatory response in aDN1 (Figure 6H), yet decreased the basal fluorescence of GCaMP6s in aDN2 in a manner that was alleviated by mecamylamine (Figure 6I). The latter observation leads us to propose that aBN2 excites an unidentified neuron that then inhibits aDN2. We tested this possibility by applying picrotoxin, a GABA and inhibitory glutamate receptor blocker (Cleland, 1996; Liu and Wilson, 2013). In the presence of picrotoxin, activation of aBN2 abolished the decreased calcium responses in aDN2 (Figure 6I). This corroborated the presence of an inhibitory neuron (IN) that impinges on aDN2 that is stimulated by the activity of aBN2. Thus, the aDNs are subject to both feedforward excitation and indirect inhibition from aBN2. Taken together, these data demonstrate functional connectivity between the JO, aBNs, and aDNs and imply that they form a circuit that relays antennal sensory information through the brain and conveys it to the VNS.

A circuit whose components elicit different durations of antennal grooming

Our results motivated a circuit model that depicts the functional connectivity among the JO neurons, aBNs, and aDNs (Figure 7A,B). The model includes putative reciprocal excitation between the aBNs (Figure 5E, Figure 6E,F) and feedforward inhibition of aDN2 mediated by aBN2 and an unknown IN (Figure 6I). The proposed outputs of this circuit are the parallel descending commands to the VNS (Figure 2), where the antennal grooming pattern-generating circuitry is expected to reside (see ‘Discussion’). We propose that the aDNs can act in parallel because experiments to thermogenetically activate aDN1 or aDN2 alone indicate that they are each sufficient to induce antennal grooming (Figure 2B). The presence of these features within the circuit suggests complex processing and raises the question of whether they influence the grooming output. Given that aBNs and aDNs induce different amounts of grooming with thermogenetic activation (Figure 2A,B), we postulated that one feature of the circuit is to control the duration of antennal grooming.

Figure 7. A circuit whose components elicit different antennal grooming durations.

(A) The antennal grooming circuit (lateral view of tracings). Specific colors represent each neuron type shown in B. (B) Wiring diagram of the circuit. Lightning bolt represents mechanical stimulation of the antennae. Arrows represent excitatory cholinergic functional connections and the ball and stick indicates an inhibitory (picrotoxin sensitive) connection from an unidentified inhibitory neuron (IN). Note: JO neurons were previously reported to be cholinergic (Yasuyama and Salvaterra, 1999; Salvaterra and Kitamoto, 2001). Arrow to the gray oval surrounding the aDNs indicates that aBN1 provides excitatory input for aDN, but it is not known for which aDN(s). Gray dashed arrows indicate relatively weak and/or inconsistent connections (JO to aDN2 and aBN2 to aBN1). Text on the left highlights putative circuit connectivity features. Dashed arrow from aDN3 depicts presumed descending command. (C) The different neuronal classes induce distinct grooming responses. Ethograms of manually scored video showing antennal grooming induced with red light sensitive CsChrimson expressed in different spGAL4 pairs (aJO-spGAL4-1, aBN1-spGAL4-1, aBN2-spGAL4-1, aDN1-spGAL4-1, and aDN2-spGAL4-2). Ethograms of individual flies are stacked on top of each other. The gray bars indicate presentation of red light. Colors correspond to the wiring diagram (B) and indicate which neuronal class expressed csChrimson. Control flies did not perform antennal grooming (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). See Video 4, Video 5, and Video 6 for representative examples. (D) Histograms representing the fraction of flies that were performing antennal grooming in C within one-second time bins. (E) The proposed organization of antennal grooming circuitry.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.026

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. CsChrimson activation of different neuronal classes.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) Stacked ethograms of grooming movements performed by the spGAL4 pairs indicated expressing CsChrimson (10–15 flies shown for each spGAL4 pair). Gray bars indicate when the red light was on. Colors correspond to grooming movements performed. (B) Percent of total time that each spGAL4 line spent grooming their antennae. Box plots and statistics are described in Figure 1B.

Our results raise the possibility that aBN1 and aBN2 are reciprocally excitatory, although the aBN2 to aBN1 excitatory connection was only weakly supported. One prediction of reciprocal excitation between the aBNs is that activation of either neuronal class will induce grooming that persists in the absence of stimulation (Major and Tank, 2004; Li et al., 2006). To test this, we exploited CsChrimson's tight temporal control to activate these neurons briefly with red light, and then examined the dynamics of the grooming responses after red light cessation. Activation of either aBN1 or aBN2 elicited long antennal grooming durations that could persist for tens of seconds beyond red light cessation (Figure 7C,D, Figure 7—figure supplement 1A, Video 4). In contrast, activation of the aDNs, which are downstream of potential aBN-induced reciprocal excitation, should not induce persistent grooming (Figure 7B). Indeed, activation of aDN1 or aDN2 caused grooming that did not outlast the red light activation (Figure 7C,D, Figure 7—figure supplement 1A, Video 5). We observed similar results using additional spGAL4 pairs that target aBN1, aBN2, and aDN2 (not shown). Of note, activation of aDN2-elicited antennal grooming that lasted until the stimulus ended (Figure 7C,D), whereas activation of aDN1 elicited about 50% fewer grooming bouts that terminated prior to the red light turning off (Figure 7C,D, Figure 7—figure supplement 1A,B). This raises the intriguing possibility that the parallel aDNs induce different durations of antennal grooming. We next found that red light activation of the aJO, which is upstream of the aBNs, could also elicit grooming that persisted after red light cessation (Figure 7C,D, Video 6). This suggests that the aJO might excite the aBNs, which then induce persistent grooming. Taken together, our data indicate that the circuit produces persistent antennal grooming through aBN1 and aBN2. Future experiments will test whether the persistence is caused by reciprocal excitation or alternate mechanisms (see ‘Discussion’). We concluded that our data describe an antennal grooming circuit whose components have the potential to modulate the duration of grooming, possibly mediated by their connectivity.

Video 4. Grooming in response to red light stimulation of CsChrimson-expressing aBN1 neurons.

Download video file (35.8MB, mov)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.028

CsChrimson was expressed in aBN1 using aBN1-spGAL4-1. The infrared light in the bottom right hand corner shows when the red light was on to activate aBN1. Note that grooming persists upon cessation of the red light.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.028

Video 5. Grooming in response to red light stimulation of CsChrimson-expressing aDN2 neurons.

Download video file (23.9MB, mov)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.029

CsChrimson was expressed in aDN2 using aDN2-spGAL4-2. The infrared light in the bottom right hand corner shows when the red light was on to activate aDN2. Note that grooming does not persist upon cessation of the red light.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.029

Video 6. Grooming in response to red light stimulation of CsChrimson-expressing aJO neurons.

Download video file (49.5MB, mov)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.030

CsChrimson was expressed in the aJO using aJO-spGAL4-1. The infrared light in the bottom right hand corner shows when the red light was on to activate the aJO. Note that grooming persists upon cessation of the red light.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.030

Discussion

Identification of a circuit that induces a specific grooming movement

This work provides the first description of a neural circuit that evokes a grooming movement: antennal displacements are detected by specific mechanosensory neurons that excite different interneuronal classes to elicit the front leg movements that constitute antennal grooming. We specifically identified JO neurons that project from the second antennal segment into the ventral brain, two brain interneuron classes (aBNs) that project within this ventral brain region, and descending neurons (aDNs) that project both within the ventral brain and descend to the VNS (Figure 7A,B,E). These neuronal classes are functionally connected with the common purpose of specifically inducing antennal grooming.

How does activation of this circuit elicit grooming of the antennae? Grooming movements are ultimately produced by pattern generators in the VNS that control the front and hind legs (Berkowitz and Laurent, 1996; Burrows, 1996). The aDNs project to the region of the VNS that should control front leg grooming movements, the ProNm. This suggests that the aDNs might connect to and activate neural circuits that generate coordinated antennal grooming movements (Figure 7B,E), but such circuitry remains to be identified. The tools for targeting aDNs acquired here may provide a means to identify this antennal grooming circuitry by examining their synaptic partners. Identification of this circuitry will provide a valuable example of how nervous systems achieve such exquisite specificity in targeting grooming movements to the site of a stimulus.

Biological functions of the antennal grooming circuit

In this work, we examined a role for the identified circuit in detecting displacements of the antennae and eliciting grooming. This was based both on our discovery that the JO induces grooming, and that different JO neurons can detect antennal movements to elicit specific behavioral responses (Kamikouchi et al., 2009; Yorozu et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2014). Further, insects can encounter conditions in their natural environments, such as static electricity or unexpected mechanical disruptions that can move the antennae and cause different aversive behavioral responses (Hunt et al., 2005; Newland et al., 2008; Jackson et al., 2011; Matsuo et al., 2014). We have observed that flies show increased grooming of their antennae in chambers with high levels of static, and that this grooming was ablated using antistatic agents (unpublished observations). This led us to find that imposed deflections of the antennae induce grooming through the JO. Because such deflections likely disrupt normal antennal functions, such as hearing (Göpfert and Robert, 2002), grooming may provide a means of restoring them to their proper position and function.

Another function of grooming is to remove debris, parasites, or other substances from the body surface (Sachs, 1988; Böröczky et al., 2013; Zhukovskaya et al., 2013). We have shown that dust on the antennae induces grooming in flies (Seeds et al., 2014), however, it seems unlikely that small particles of dust could cause the large displacements that induce grooming in this work. This is supported by our observations that expression of TNT in the aJO does not cause defects in antennal dust removal (unpublished data), but does disrupt the grooming response to antennal displacements (Figure 4E). Therefore, dust might be detected by different sensory neurons, such as the mechanosensory bristle neurons found on the surface of the antennae. Bristle neurons are good candidates for stimulating grooming, given that mechanical stimulation of bristles on other body parts can induce site directed grooming responses (Vandervorst and Ghysen, 1980; Corfas and Dudai, 1989). Like JO neurons, the antennal bristle mechanosensory neurons send projections to the AMMC, and possibly to the SEZ (Homberg et al., 1989; Melzig et al., 1996), where they could induce grooming by interacting with the interneurons described in this work. This raises the possibility that multiple different sensory neuron types feed into the circuit to induce grooming. However, we have not yet identified spGAL4 lines that specifically target the antennal bristle mechanosensory neurons to allow us to test this hypothesis.

A scenario whereby different types of stimuli, such as antennal deflections or dust can cause grooming could explain some of the complexity of the circuit that we observe. Our data suggest multiple possible routes of feedforward excitation through the circuit layers to induce antennal grooming. For example, the function of at least a subset of the aBN2 neurons was not necessary for grooming in response to physical displacement of the antennae, whereas they were necessary in response to thermogenetic activation of the JO. Thus, feedforward excitation likely has multiple routes through the antennal grooming circuit, but why would this be? One possibility is that each route reflects specific features of the sensory stimulus. Different stimuli might be sensed by distinct JO neurons or mechanosensory bristle neurons on the antennae that engage different ensembles of neurons within the circuit to produce specific grooming responses. Therefore, it may be revealing to test whether different stimuli coopt specific circuit components to fine–tune parameters of grooming, such as short vs persistent grooming durations.

The finding that the antennal grooming circuit can induce persistent grooming raises the question as to its biological role. Our results do not reveal a natural stimulus that induces persistence, as displacements of the antennae rarely induced persistent grooming (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Persistence was previously described in the grooming responses of other animals, where it was found to be at least partially dependent on the nature of the stimulus (e.g., stimulus strength, duration, or frequency) (Sherrington, 1906; Stein, 2005). Thus, future work will explore the possibility that specific stimulus parameters can evoke persistent antennal grooming in flies. Additionally, persistence indicates the presence of a mechanism that produces long lasting neural activity within the grooming sensorimotor response. Such persistent activity could provide a mechanism by which temporal summation of successive stimuli is achieved. That is, successive, sub-threshold stimuli have been shown to be ‘remembered’ or summed to elicit grooming, although the neuronal mechanisms have not been identified (Sherrington, 1906; Stein, 2005; Guzulaitis et al., 2013). Therefore, future experiments will examine the role of specific features of the antennal grooming circuit in producing persistent grooming, and possible temporal summation (Figure 7B, possible circuit features discussed below).

A subset of JO neurons detects antennal displacements and elicits grooming

aJO mechanosensory neurons are critical for the grooming response to antennal movements and sufficient to induce grooming. This reveals a new role for the JO, which was previously implicated in hearing, gravitaxis, and wind-induced suppression of locomotion (Kamikouchi et al., 2009; Yorozu et al., 2009). These behaviors are mediated by JO neurons that are localized to distinct regions of the mechanosensory structure and respond to high frequency vibrations (e.g., sounds) or more tonic movements (e.g., wind) (Kamikouchi et al., 2006; Kamikouchi et al., 2009; Yorozu et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2014). Thus, the JO is functionally organized such that different groups of mechanosensory neurons mediate different behaviors. Our finding that the JO elicits antennal grooming supports this idea, and demonstrates an even greater diversity of JO functions than previously thought.

Our data indicate that JO-induced antennal grooming is likely restricted to neurons projecting to zone C/E. Previously characterized JO neurons projecting to this zone are less sensitive to smaller movements of the antennae and adapt slowly to mechanical stimuli (Kamikouchi et al., 2009; Yorozu et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2014). The antennal displacement distance that induces grooming is within the range shown to induce calcium responses in previously described zone C/E neurons (Yorozu et al., 2009). This suggests that aJO neurons might also be activated by such large displacements, given that they are critical for the subsequent grooming response and project to zone C/E.

Two different populations of C/E-projecting neurons can induce antennal grooming; one that projects only to the AMMC, and the other (aJO) that projects to the AMMC and SEZ. This raises the question of whether both populations interact with the antennal grooming circuitry described in this work. The aBNs and aDN1 have neurites within the AMMC where they could plausibly receive excitatory inputs from both C/E neuron populations. Additionally, because neurons within zone C/E are implicated in other behaviors like wind-induced suppression of locomotion, it remains to be determined whether these JO neurons are multifunctional, or whether specific subpopulations within this group are responsible for distinct behaviors. Future work will be required to determine how these two populations of JO sensory neurons interact with the antennal grooming circuitry.

A command circuit that elicits variable grooming durations

The antennal grooming circuitry consists of at least three different neuronal classes. As our extensive screening efforts identified multiple GAL4 lines for each class, we presume that we have uncovered a major portion of the neurons that elicit antennal grooming. Indeed, these neurons are sufficient to form a functionally connected circuit that extends from the JO to the VNS. However, additional descending neurons are likely involved given that TNT expression in the aDNs failed to disrupt the grooming response to aJO activation. We also found evidence for an unidentified neuron downstream of aBN2 that inhibits aDN2.

The neuronal classes in the circuit are each sufficient to elicit antennal grooming, similar to command-like neurons that evoke specific movements (also termed decision neurons or higher order neurons) (Kupfermann and Weiss, 1978; Pearson, 1993; Kristan, 2008; Jing, 2009). aBN1 and a subset of aBN2 neurons could potentially be more specifically termed command neurons, which are defined as being necessary and sufficient for initiating a specific movement, and fire in response to the movement-initiating sensory input (Kupfermann and Weiss, 1978). However, in order to definitively call these neurons command neurons, we need to test whether they are active in response to imposed movements of the antennae. Another term to describe collections of neurons that induce a behavior is a command system (Kupfermann and Weiss, 1978; Jing, 2009). However, a command system does not necessarily consist of functionally connected neurons. Therefore, given that these neurons constitute a functionally connected circuit, it may be appropriate to refer to them collectively as a command circuit for antennal grooming.

When the antennal grooming command circuit is compared with those that were previously identified in other systems, it emerges that circuits can consist of different layers that each elicit the specific movement, but with different durations (Frost et al., 2001; Kristan et al., 2005; Pirri and Alkema, 2012). In such cases, neurons have been found within particular layers that either elicit a movement that persists beyond their initial activation, or must be continually activated to continue the movement. These neuronal types have been termed trigger and gating neurons respectively (Stein, 1978). In the marine mollusc (Tritonia diomedia) and in the leech (Hirudo medicinalis), trigger neurons immediately downstream of sensory neurons induce persistent swimming motor patterns (named Tr1 in both animals) (Frost et al., 2001; Kristan et al., 2005). Gating neurons then induce swimming downstream of Tr1 in both animals (named DRI in the mollusc and 204/205 in the leech). This organization is strikingly similar to the layers of neurons that induce antennal grooming identified here; the aBNs induce persistent grooming and might be considered trigger neurons, whereas aDN2 could serve as a gating neuron. Taken together, the use of trigger and gating neurons might constitute a common organization in circuits that command specific movements downstream of a sensory stimulus.

The layered and complex organization of the antennal grooming circuit raises the question as to its function. We discussed above the possibility that the circuit may have different possible routes of feedforward excitation. Such organization may provide different points at which the circuit can be modulated, thus allowing for flexible control of different movement parameters. In the antennal grooming circuit, flexible control of movement duration could be provided by the parallel aDNs (Figure 7B), which appear to elicit different amounts of antennal grooming when activated. Artificially activated aDN1 elicits isolated grooming bouts that terminate despite continued activation, whereas activated aDN2 elicits grooming throughout the activation period. These observations are reminiscent of two descending neurons in molluscs that initiate biting movements during feeding, as one induces longer protraction durations whereas the other induces shorter durations (Jing and Weiss, 2005). This ability to generate distinct movement parameters, such as duration, may be a general feature of descending neurons that induce the same movement. Other parameters that might be differentially controlled are illustrated by locomotor systems, wherein descending neurons can elicit movement while controlling parameters such as speed and direction (Dubuc et al., 2008; Roberts et al., 2010; Mullins et al., 2011; Puhl et al., 2012; El Manira and Grillner, 2014).

The aDNs are also impinged upon by feedforward excitation and inhibition (Figure 7B), possibly to control which ones are active. For example, aBN2 provides differential control by exciting aDN1 and inhibiting aDN2. As there are multiple aDNs, it may be that feedforward excitation could impinge on them at the same time to induce hybrid durations of antennal grooming. Such an effect has been described for the two descending neurons controlling mollusc bite protraction, as activating the two together produces intermediate durations (Jing and Weiss, 2005). Thus, we will further test whether the aDNs similarly produce flexible durations of antennal grooming.

The persistent grooming induced by brief excitation of the aBNs provides another mechanism for controlling grooming duration (Figure 7B). This is reminiscent of previously described grooming responses that were sustained despite stimuli cessation, which Sherrington referred to as afterdischarge (Sherrington, 1906; Stein, 2005). Persistence is not limited to grooming responses, as it has been described in behaviors as diverse as locomotion and courtship song (Stein, 1978; Kristan et al., 2005; Inagaki et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2015). In locomotion, reciprocal excitation among reticulospinal neurons has even been implicated in persistent tactile-induced swimming (Li et al., 2006). Although our work only provides weak evidence for reciprocal excitation between the aBNs, a mechanism whereby reciprocal connections maintain their excitation offers a plausible explanation for the persistence that we observe. Alternatively, aBN1 and aBN2 could have intrinsic membrane properties that allow them to produce prolonged responses to brief excitation (Major and Tank, 2004). Thus, future experiments will examine whether reciprocal excitation or intrinsic membrane properties of the aBNs produce persistent grooming.

In this section we have discussed how the complex organization of the antennal grooming circuit might control movement duration, however, it could also control parameters that our current analysis methods cannot detect. The level of behavioral analysis presented in this work reveals that the induced trajectories of the legs are specific for the antennae rather than other head parts; however, it does not allow for the detection of finer antennal grooming movements. Grooming is characterized by an initial targeting of the legs to the stimulated body part, followed by cyclic movements that groom the region (Dürr and Matheson, 2003). Higher resolution analysis of the leg kinematics could help resolve the boundary limits on the head to which the antennal grooming movements are confined during these two phases, and allow for determining how stereotyped these movements are. Furthermore, such analysis could elucidate how the legs interact with the antennal region to perform the grooming movements. Thus, higher resolution analysis would facilitate testing possible roles for the circuit in controlling different variables of antennal grooming, such as the limb trajectories or speed.

Neurons that command specific movements in insects

Recent work indicates that command-like neurons may constitute a common means of eliciting specific movements in insects. Tools that allow for both acute control of neuronal activity and precise genetic targeting of specific neurons in fruit flies have enabled experiments that merge behavioral analyses with real time neuronal manipulations. Such experiments demonstrate that activation of specific neurons can elicit distinct movements, such as escape, locomotion, or courtship song (Lima and Miesenböck, 2005; von Philipsborn et al., 2011; Gao et al., 2013; Flood et al., 2013b; Inagaki et al., 2014; Bidaye et al., 2014; von Reyn et al., 2014). Additionally, work in crickets, grasshoppers, and locusts has revealed specific neurons that elicit courtship stridulation or flight (Pearson et al., 1985; Hedwig, 1994, 1996, 2000). Given our discovery of a circuit that specifically elicits antennal grooming, it would appear that the use of dedicated neurons to command specific movements is a common mechanism of behavioral control in insects. However, these command-like neurons may be embedded in larger, more dynamic neural networks in which neurons that elicit one movement also participate in other movements (Kupfermann and Weiss, 1978; Kristan, 2008). For example, in the leech several of the neurons involved in commanding swimming were found to also be excited during stimulus-induced shortening movements (Shaw and Kristan, 1997). Such findings indicate that neurons that can command one movement might also participate in the production of other movements, with their specific output subject to the collective activities of multiple different neurons within a given network (Kristan et al., 2005; Kristan, 2008). Thus, future experiments that examine whether the antennal grooming command circuit participates in controlling additional movements, may further elucidate the properties of neural networks that enable the performance of specific movements.

Materials and methods

Fly strains and rearing conditions

Flies were reared on cornmeal and molasses food at 21°C and 50% relative humidity on a 16/8 hr light/dark cycle. 5–8 day old males were used for all experiments, except for those in Figure 6 that were performed with 2–8 day-old flies. Stocks used in this study are listed in Supplementary file 1 and Supplementary file 3.

Identification of genetic reagents targeting neurons that induce antennal grooming

Three GAL4 lines that induced antennal grooming with thermogenetic activation using dTrpA1 were identified by screening over 1500 randomly selected GAL4 lines (R39A11, R26B12, and R18C11-GAL4) (Seeds et al., 2014). To identify additional lines, we visually screened through an image database of GAL4 expression patterns (Jenett et al., 2012) for those with expression in neurites close to aJO projections. Selected lines were crossed to UAS-dTrpA1 and screened for increased antennal grooming at 30–32°C. For pattern refinement and co-expression studies, the enhancers of GAL4 lines that exhibited increased antennal grooming were used to generate spGAL4 and LexA reagents, which were constructed as described previously (Pfeiffer et al., 2010) and produced by Gerald Rubin's lab. DBDs were inserted into the attP2 landing site (on chromosome 3), ADs were inserted into attP40 (on chromosome 2), and LexAs were inserted into attP40. Supplementary file 1 lists the enhancer identities (Jenett et al., 2012) used to generate the spGAL4, LexA, and GAL4 lines. Control flies contain the DNA elements used for generating the different GAL4, spGAL4 halves, or LexA collections, but lack enhancers to drive their expression (images of control lines crossed to UAS-GFP are shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1E,F). R27H08 (aJO-LexA) was identified in a screen of existing LexA lines (Pfeiffer et al., 2010) that targeted the aJO and could induce antennal grooming (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A,B).

Behavioral analysis procedures

The camera setup and methods for recording the behavior of flies expressing dTrpA1 in different neuronal classes were described previously (Seeds et al., 2014). Behavior was recorded at 35 frames per second for 2 min at 30–31°C. For amputation experiments, the entire antennae of 2 day-old males were severed with forceps and flies were allowed to recover for 4 days. The amputation did not damage the rest of the head or lead to mortality of the animals. CsChrimson experiments were performed in the dark, and flies were visualized for recording using an 850-nm infrared light source at 2 mW/cm2 intensity (Mightex, Toronto, CA), which flies cannot see. For CsChrimson activation, we used 656-nm red light at 27 mW/cm2 intensity (Mightex). The red light stimulus parameters were delivered using a NIDAQ board controlled through Labview (National Instruments, Austin, TX). Red light frequency was 5 Hz for 5 s (0.1-s on/off), and 30-s interstimulus intervals (total of 3 stimulations). Grooming movements were manually scored as previously described (Seeds et al., 2014), with the exceptions listed below. Manual scoring of prerecorded video was performed with VCode software and the data was analyzed in MATLAB (MathWorks Incorporated, Natick, MA).

Modifications of previously described scoring of grooming movements: Antennal grooming: The legs grasp and brush the antennae, with the head often tilted forward. When the antennae were amputated, ‘antennal grooming’ was scored when the legs were directed towards the area where antennae used to be. Proboscis grooming: The legs sweep down the proboscis when it is extended, or the tip when it is retracted. Ventral head: Legs sweep the area between the antennae and the ventral side of the head, as well as below the eyes towards the ventral bottom of the head. Leg rubbing: This movement was not included in this study; however, leg rubbing was often associated with the activated movements. Movement start times were scored one frame before the specific body part was first touched and ended two frames after that body part was last touched. The time interval between the previous and the next movement was scored as standing.

Antennal displacement assay

Third antennal segments were coated with a mixture of iron powder (Atlantic Equipment Engineers, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 325 mesh) and UV cured glue (Kemxert Corp., York, PA) and then left to recover for 12 hr. The presence of the powder on the antennae did not cause increased antennal grooming after the recovery period. Flies were tethered using a pin that was glued to their thoraces and then positioned on a 6 mm diameter air-supported ball (Seelig et al., 2010) within a custom made electromagnet (Figure 4C,D, Supplementary file 4). The automated stimulation parameters of the electromagnet (frequency, on/off durations) were delivered via a NIDAQ board controlled through MATLAB. Voltage was controlled using a Power Supply (B&K Precision Corporation, Yorba Linda, CA). The magnetic field was applied to move the antennae while the induced movements were recorded. Movement responses were manually scored as described above. The percent time the flies spent grooming their antennae was calculated through the duration of the experiment.

The stimulus parameters used for our experiments were determined using control flies (w1118; UAS-TNT; pBPGAL4U). The percent time that control flies groomed when the magnetic field was applied was measured at different frequencies and voltages to determine the optimal stimulus conditions (Figure 4—figure supplement 3A,B). Flies performed an intermediate amount of grooming at 1 Hz and 10 V. Based on these results, antennal deflection experiments were performed at 1 Hz for 10 s (0.5 s on/off) at 10 V, with a 30 s wait time between stimulations (total of 4 stimulations). The magnetic field strength at 10 V was measured at 570 Gauss. Grooming was not induced with flies that lacked iron powder on their antennae and were exposed to the magnetic field (data not shown). The antennal displacement caused by the magnetic field was measured by recording video of the antenna from a side view (Figure 4—figure supplement 3C). Antennal displacement was calculated by measuring movement of the distal tip of the third antennal segment before and after the magnetic field was applied. Pixels were calibrated to physical distances using a known standard.

Statistical methods

Behavioral data was analyzed with nonparametric statistical tests. First, we performed a Kruskal–Wallis (ANOVA) test to compare more than three genotypes with each other. Next we performed a post-hoc Mann–Whitney U test and applied Bonferroni correction.

Immunostaining and image analysis

Dissection and staining was performed as previously described (Hampel et al., 2011), with modifications listed below. 5–8 day-old males were used for all dissections, except for the stochastic labeling experiments using MCFO-1 (Nern et al., 2015) in Figure 1—figure supplement 4A–H, where 1–2 day-old flies were dissected. For Figure 1E,F, Figure 2C–F, Figure 3A–D, Figure 5A–H, Figure 1—figure supplement 1F, Figure 1—figure supplement 2A–E, Figure 2—figure supplement 2A–I, Figure 4—figure supplement 1B–D, Figure 4—figure supplement 2A–F, and Figure 5—figure supplement 2A–H additional treatment was performed to clear the tissue: After immunohistochemistry, tissue samples were post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS for 4 hr at room temperature, followed by four 30 min washes in PBT. Before mounting the CNS on a poly-L-lysine (P1524; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) coated cover slip, the tissues were washed in PBS for 15 min to remove Triton. After mounting the tissues on the poly-L-lysine-coated cover slip, they were dehydrated through a series of ethanol dilutions (30%, 50%, 75%, 95%, and 3 × 100%) for 10 min each, followed by an incubation series in 100% xylene (Fisher Scientific, Fair Lawn, NJ) three times for 5 min each in Coplin jars. Afterwards tissues were embedded in DPX, a xylene-based mounting solution (Electron Microscopy Sciences, Hatfield, PA, Cat#13512) and allowed to dry for 48 hr before imaging.

For Figure 2H, Figure 1—figure supplement 1C–E,G–J, Figure 1—figure supplement 3A–G, Figure 1—figure supplement 4A–H, Figure 2—figure supplement 1B–H, and Figure 5—figure supplement 1A–I stained tissue samples were mounted after the immunohistochemistry in Vectashield (Vector Laboratories, Inc. Burlingame, California) and allowed to incubate for an hour before imaging.

Antibodies used: rabbit anti-GFP (1:500, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, #A11122), chicken anti-GFP (1:2000, Abcam, Cambridge, MA, #ab13970), mouse anti-GFP (1:200, Sigma, #G6539), mouse mAb anti-nc82 (1:50, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, University of Iowa), rat anti-DN-cadherin (1:20, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank), rabbit anti-RFP (to detect tdTomato; 1:1000, Clontech Laboratories, Inc., Mountain View, CA, #632496), rat anti-flag (Novus Biologicals, LLC, Littleton, CO, #NBP1-06712), rabbit anti-HA (Cell Signaling Technology, Danvers, MA, #3724S), mouse anti-V5 (AbD Serotec, Kidlington, England, #MCA1360), AlexaFluor-488 (1:500; goat anti-rabbit, goat anti-chicken, goat anti-mouse; Thermo Fisher Scientific), AlexaFluor-568 (1:500; goat anti-mouse, goat anti-rat; Thermo Fisher Scientific), AlexaFluor-633 (1:500; goat anti-rat; Thermo Fisher Scientific).

Confocal stacks of stained CNS and antennae were imaged on a Zeiss LSM710 confocal microscope with a Plan-Apochromat 20×/0.8 M27 objective and a Plan-Apochromat 63×/1.4 oil immersion objective. To visualize the neuronal classes together as shown in Video 2, confocal images of different split-GAL4 lines were computationally aligned from individual specimens to one brain sample of our own collection with the Computations Morphometry Toolkit CMTK (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/cmtk/) (Jefferis et al., 2007) and assembled in FluoRender, (Wan et al., 2009) a suite of tools for viewing and analyzing image data.

Image preparation, analysis of overlap, and adjustment of brightness and contrast were performed with Fiji (http://fiji.sc/). Figure 5 shows maximum projections of confocal stacks that were modified using the 3D viewer plugin in Fiji to crop contaminating neurons and background noise. The same images are shown in Figure 5—figure supplement 2A–H with only brightness and contrast adjusted. Confocal stacks of brains imaged with a 63× objective were used to reconstruct each neuronal class shown in Figure 3E. We traced each neuron from different brains with neuTube software (Feng et al., 2015) and assembled the neuronal circuit manually.

GCaMP6s imaging and analysis of calcium responses

Male flies were dissected in saline containing 103 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 5 mM TES, 8 mM trehalose dihydrate, 10 mM glucose, 26 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 2 mM CaCl2, 4 mM MgCl2, and bubbled with carbogen. The brain and VNS were placed on a poly-lysine-coated coverslip. Dissections were visualized using minimal illumination to avoid activation of CsChrimson. The preparation was continuously perfused in saline at 60 ml/hr. Imaging was done using a two-photon scanning microscope (Bruker, Billerica, MA) with an excitation wavelength of 920 nm. Imaging fields of view were chosen as to contain a distinctive process of the candidate post-synaptic neuron, which included processes at the midline for the aDNs and aBN2, and vertical running processes close to the midline for aBN1.

CsChrimson was excited with 2 ms pulses of 590-nm light via an LED shining through the objective. Instantaneous powers measured out of the objective ranged between 50 μW/mm2 and 800 μW/mm2. Trains were delivered at 50 Hz. Experiments usually started with a 50-pulse train. If no response was observed, the power was raised progressively until one occurred or the maximum power was reached. Each experimental run consisted of 4 repeats lasting for approximately 20 s. Runs were repeated approximately every 2 min. When postsynaptic responses were observed, we did not see any desensitization. See Supplementary file 2 for stimulus conditions used for data shown in Figure 6. For blocking nicotinic or GABAergic/glutamatergic transmission, mecamylamine (50 μM) or picrotoxin (30 μM) were administered through a perfusion line for 3–10 min, followed by a drug-free wash (drugs from Sigma). Pharmacology experiments were run using samples that showed consistent responses. Stimulus conditions were chosen that elicited reliable transient responses. Average traces shown in Figure 6E–I are from those trials where pharmacology experiments were done (trials used to generate average traces shown in Figure 6—figure supplements 2, 3). Average traces shown in Figure 6A–D correspond to all flies and trials at the indicated stimulus conditions shown in Figure 6—figure supplements 2, 3.

ΔF/F0 was calculated (F0 is the average signal before the stimulation) for each video in a region of interest (ROI) obtained as follows: the average projection of the video was calculated and pixels of the projection were clustered by a k-means algorithm between ROI and non-ROI pixels. Of note, the selection method relies only on average intensity and not activity, because we wanted to use the same detection method for responsive and non-responsive runs. This also relies on selecting fields of view that unambiguously contain only the neuron of interest. Analysis was run in Julia (http://julialang.org/).

Acknowledgements

We thank: Heather Dionne, Barret Pfeiffer, and Gerald Rubin for providing spGAL4 lines and pJFRC26, pJFRC34, and pJFRC48 flies; Eric Hoopfer for MATLAB code used to analyze behavioral data; Karen Hibbard and Don Hall for stock-building support; Vivek Jayaraman for advice and reagents for calcium imaging experiments (author Romain Franconville is a member of the Jayaraman lab); Rebecca Johnston, Gudrun Ihrke, Jennifer Jeter, Gina DePasquale, and Joanna Hausenfluck for protocols and immunohistochemistry on selected spGAL4 lines; Jeff Jordan, Lakshmi Ramasamy, and Steven Sawtelle for constructing the electromagnet and optogenetic setups; Carmen Robinett, Claire McKellar, Scott Sternson, and Bill Kristan for editing and comments on the manuscript; Emily Nielson for administrative support; TJ Florence for help with antennal displacement measurements. Jeremy Cohen for advice related to antistatic agents; The Howard Hughes Medical Institute supported this work.

Funding Statement

The funder had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • Howard Hughes Medical Institute (HHMI) to Julie H Simpson.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

SH, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

AMS, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

RF, Performed calcium imaging experiments and analyzed the calcium imaging data, Drafting or revising the article.

JHS, Conception and design, Drafting or revising the article.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1.

Enhancer identities used to target different neuronal classes.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.031

elife08758s001.pdf (46.1KB, pdf)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.031
Supplementary file 2.

Stimulus conditions used for the experiment shown in Figure 6.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.032

elife08758s002.pdf (54KB, pdf)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.032
Supplementary file 3.

Stocks used in this study.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.033

elife08758s003.pdf (138KB, pdf)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.033
Supplementary file 4.

Design plans for the fly electromagnetic stimulation rig.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.034

elife08758s004.pdf (1MB, pdf)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.034

References

  1. Berkowitz A, Laurent G. Central generation of grooming motor patterns and interlimb coordination in locusts. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:8079–8091. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-24-08079.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bidaye SS, Machacek C, Wu Y, Dickson BJ. Neuronal control of Drosophila walking direction. Science. 2014;344:97–101. doi: 10.1126/science.1249964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blitz DM, Nusbaum MP. Neural circuit flexibility in a small sensorimotor system. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21:544–552. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Böröczky K, Wada-Katsumata A, Batchelor D, Zhukovskaya M, Schal C. Insects groom their antennae to enhance olfactory acuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2013;110:3615–3620. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212466110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brierley DJ, Rathore K, VijayRaghavan K, Williams DW. Developmental origins and architecture of Drosophila leg motoneurons. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2012;520:1629–1649. doi: 10.1002/cne.23003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burrows M. The neurobiology of an insect brain. 1st edition. Oxford University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  7. Büschges A, Scholz H, El Manira A. New moves in motor control. Current Biology. 2011;21:R513–R524. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Svoboda K, Kim DS. Ultrasensitive Fluorescent Proteins for Imaging Neuronal Activity. Nature. 2013;499(7458):295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cleland TA. Inhibitory glutamate receptor channels. Molecular Neurobiology. 1996;13:97–136. doi: 10.1007/BF02740637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Corfas G, Dudai Y. Habituation and dishabituation of a cleaning reflex in normal and mutant Drosophila. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9:56–62. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-01-00056.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dubuc R, Brocard F, Antri M, Fénelon K, Gariépy JF, Smetana R, Ménard A, Le Ray D, Viana Di Prisco G, Pearlstein E, Sirota MG, Derjean D, St-Pierre M, Zielinski B, Auclair F, Veilleux D. Initiation of locomotion in lampreys. Brain Research Reviews. 2008;57:172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dürr V, Matheson T. Graded limb targeting in an insect is caused by the shift of a single movement pattern. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;90:1754–1765. doi: 10.1152/jn.00416.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. El Manira A, Grillner S. Motion with direction and balance. Neuron. 2014;83:515–517. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Feinberg EH, VanHoven MK, Bendesky A, Wang G, Fetter RD, Shen K, Bargmann CI. GFP reconstitution across synaptic partners (GRASP) defines cell contacts and synapses in living nervous systems. Neuron. 2008;57:353–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Feng L, Zhao T, Kim J. neuTube 1.0: a new design for efficient neuron reconstruction software based on the SWC format. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;2 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0049-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Flood TF, Gorczyca M, White BH, Ito K, Yoshihara M. A large-scale behavioral screen to identify neurons controlling motor programs in the Drosophila brain. G3. 2013a;3:1629–1637. doi: 10.1534/g3.113.006205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Flood TF, Iguchi S, Gorczyca M, White B, Ito K, Yoshihara M. A Single pair of interneurons commands the Drosophila feeding motor program. Nature. 2013b;499:83–87. doi: 10.1038/nature12208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Frost WN, Hoppe TA, Wang J, Tian LM. Swim initiation neurons in Tritonia Diomedea. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2001;41:952–961. doi: 10.1093/icb/41.4.952. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gao S, Xie L, Kawano T, Po MD, Guan S, Zhen M, Pirri JK, Alkema MJ. The NCA sodium leak channel is required for persistent motor circuit activity that sustains locomotion. Nature Communications. 2015;6:6323. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gao XJ, Potter CJ, Gohl DM, Silies M, Katsov AY, Clandinin TR, Luo L. Specific kinematics and motor-related neurons for aversive chemotaxis in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2013;23:1163–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gordon MD, Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron. 2009;61:373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Göpfert MC, Robert D. The mechanical basis of Drosophila audition. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2002;205:1199–1208. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.9.1199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Grillner S. Biological pattern generation: the cellular and computational logic of networks in motion. Neuron. 2006;52:751–766. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grillner S, Hellgren J, Ménard A, Saitoh K, Wikström MA. Mechanisms for selection of basic motor programs—roles for the striatum and pallidum. Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28:364–370. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Grillner S, Wallén P. Innate versus learned movements—a false dichotomy? Progress in Brain Research. 2004;143:3–12. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(03)43001-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Guzulaitis R, Alaburda A, Hounsgaard J. Increased activity of pre-motor network does not change the excitability of motoneurons during protracted scratch initiation. Journal of Physiology. 2013;591:1851–1858. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.246025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hamada FN, Rosenzweig M, Kang K, Pulver SR, Ghezzi A, Jegla TJ, Garrity PA. An internal thermal sensor controlling temperature preference in Drosophila. Nature. 2008;454:217–220. doi: 10.1038/nature07001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hampel S, Chung P, McKellar CE, Hall D, Looger LL, Simpson JH. Drosophila brainbow: a recombinase-based fluorescence labeling technique to subdivide neural expression patterns. Nature Methods. 2011;8:253–259. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Harris-Warrick RM. Neuromodulation and flexibility in central pattern generator networks. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21:685–692. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hedwig B. A cephalothoracic command system controls stridulation in the acridid grasshopper Omocestus viridulus L. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994;72:2015–2025. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.4.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hedwig B. A descending brain neuron elicits stridulation in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus (De Geer) Die Naturwissenschaften. 1996;83:428–429. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hedwig B. Control of cricket stridulation by a command neuron: efficacy depends on the behavioral state. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83:712–722. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.2.712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Homberg U, Christensen TA, Hildebrand JG. Structure and function of the deutocerebrum in insects. Annual Review of Entomology. 1989;34:477–501. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.34.010189.002401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hunt EP, Jackson CW, Newland PL. ‘Electrorepellancy’ behaviour of periplaneta americana exposed to friction charged dielectric surfaces. Journal of Electrostatics. 2005;63:853–859. doi: 10.1016/j.elstat.2005.03.081. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Inagaki HK, Jung Y, Hoopfer ED, Wong AM, Mishra N, Lin JY, Tsien RY, Anderson DJ. Optogenetic control of Drosophila using a red-shifted channelrhodopsin reveals experience-dependent influences on courtship. Nature Methods. 2014;11:325–332. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jackson CW, Hunt E, Sharkh S, Newland PL. Static electric fields modify the locomotory behaviour of cockroaches. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2011;214:2020–2026. doi: 10.1242/jeb.053470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jefferis GS, Potter CJ, Chan AM, Marin EC, Rohlfing T, Maurer CR, Jr, Luo L. Comprehensive maps of Drosophila higher olfactory centers: spatially segregated fruit and pheromone representation. Cell. 2007;128:1187–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.01.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jenett A, Rubin GM, Ngo TT, Shepherd D, Murphy C, Dionne H, Pfeiffer BD, Cavallaro A, Hall D, Jeter J, Iyer N, Fetter D, Hausenfluck JH, Peng H, Trautman ET, Svirskas RR, Myers EW, Iwinski ZR, Aso Y, DePasquale GM, Enos A, Hulamm P, Lam SC, Li HH, Laverty TR, Long F, Qu L, Murphy SD, Rokicki K, Safford T, Shaw K, Simpson JH, Sowell A, Tae S, Yu Y, Zugates CT. A GAL4-Driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jing J. Encyclopedia of neuroscience. Volume 2. Command Systems; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  40. Jing J, Weiss KR. Generation of variants of a motor act in a modular and hierarchical motor network. Current Biology. 2005;15:1712–1721. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kamikouchi A, Inagaki HK, Effertz T, Hendrich O, Fiala A, Göpfert MC, Ito K. The neural basis of Drosophila gravity-sensing and hearing. Nature. 2009;458:165–171. doi: 10.1038/nature07810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kamikouchi A, Shimada T, Ito K. Comprehensive classification of the auditory sensory projections in the brain of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;499:317–356. doi: 10.1002/cne.21075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kiehn O, Kullander K. Central pattern generators deciphered by molecular genetics. Neuron. 2004;41:317–321. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00042-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, Morimoto TK, Chuong AS, Carpenter EJ, Tian Z, Wang J, Xie Y, Yan Z, Zhang Y, Chow BY, Surek B, Melkonian M, Jayaraman V, Constantine-Paton M, Wong GK, Boyden ES. Independent Optical Excitation of Distinct Neural Populations. Nature Methods. 2014;11(3):338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kristan WB. Neuronal decision-making circuits. Current Biology. 2008;18:R928–R932. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kristan WB, Jr, Calabrese RL, Friesen WO. Neuronal control of leech behavior. Progress in Neurobiology. 2005;76:279–327. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kupfermann I, Weiss KR. The command neuron concept. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1978;1:3–10. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00059057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lai SL, Lee T. Genetic mosaic with dual binary transcriptional systems in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:703–709. doi: 10.1038/nn1681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Li WC, Soffe SR, Wolf E, Roberts A. Persistent responses to brief stimuli: feedback excitation among brainstem neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:4026–4035. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4727-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lima SQ, Miesenböck G. Remote control of behavior through genetically targeted photostimulation of neurons. Cell. 2005;121(1):141–152. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2005.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Liu WW, Wilson RI. Glutamate is an inhibitory neurotransmitter in the Drosophila olfactory system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2013;110:10294–10299. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220560110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Luan H, Peabody NC, Vinson CR, White BH. Refined spatial manipulation of neuronal function by combinatorial restriction of transgene expression. Neuron. 2006;52:425–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Major G, Tank D. Persistent neural activity: prevalence and mechanisms. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004;14:675–684. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Marder E, Dirk B, Schulz DJ, Taylor AL. Invertebrate central pattern generation moves along. Current Biology. 2005;15:R685–R699. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Matsuo E, Yamada D, Ishikawa Y, Asai T, Ishimoto H, Kamikouchi A. Identification of novel vibration- and deflection-sensitive neuronal subgroups in Johnston's organ of the fruit fly. Frontiers in Physiology. 2014;5:179. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Melzig J, Buchner S, Wiebel F, Wolf R, Burg M, Pak WL, Buchner E. Genetic depletion of histamine from the nervous system of Drosophila eliminates specific visual and mechanosensory behavior. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1996;179:763–773. doi: 10.1007/BF00207355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mullins OJ, Hackett JT, Buchanan JT, Friesen WO. Neuronal control of swimming behavior: comparison of vertebrate and invertebrate model systems. Progress in Neurobiology. 2011;93:244–269. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nern A, Pfeiffer BD, Rubin GM. Optimized tools for multicolor stochastic labeling reveal diverse stereotyped cell arrangements in the fly visual system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2015;112:E2967–E2976. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1506763112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Newland PL, Hunt E, Sharkh SM, Hama N, Takahata M, Jackson CW. Static electric field detection and behavioural avoidance in cockroaches. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2008;211:3682–36890. doi: 10.1242/jeb.019901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Owald D, Lin S, Waddell S. Light, heat, action: neural control of fruit fly behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2015;370:20140211. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Page KL, Matheson T. Functional recovery of aimed scratching movements after a graded proprioceptive manipulation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:3897–3907. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0089-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pearson KG. Common principles of motor control in vertebrates and invertebrates. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1993;16:265–297. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.001405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pearson KG, Reye DN, Parsons DW, Bicker G. Flight-initiating interneurons in the locust. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1985;53:910–925. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.4.910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pfeiffer BD, Ngo TT, Hibbard KL, Murphy C, Jenett A, Truman JW, Rubin GM. Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics. 2010;186:735–755. doi: 10.1534/genetics.110.119917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pirri JK, Alkema MJ. The neuroethology of C. elegans escape. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2012;22:187–193. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Puhl JG, Masino MA, Mesce KA. Necessary, sufficient and permissive: a single locomotor command neuron important for intersegmental coordination. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:17646–17657. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2249-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ritzmann RE, Büschges A. Adaptive motor behavior in insects. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17:629–636. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Roberts A, Li WC, Soffe SR. How neurons generate behavior in a hatchling amphibian tadpole: an outline. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2010;4:16. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Robinson WH. Antennal grooming and movement behaviour in the german cockroach, Blattella germanica (L.) 1996 Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Urban Pests. [Google Scholar]
  70. Sachs BD. The development of grooming and its expression in adult animals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1988;525:1–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb38591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Salvaterra PM, Kitamoto T. Drosophila cholinergic neurons and processes visualized with Gal4/UAS–GFP. Gene Expression Patterns. 2001;1:73–82. doi: 10.1016/S1567-133X(01)00011-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Seeds AM, Ravbar P, Chung P, Hampel S, Midgley FM, jr, Mensh BD, Simpson JH. A suppression hierarchy among competing motor programs drives sequential grooming in Drosophila. eLife. 2014;3:e02951. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Seelig JD, Chiappe ME, Lott GK, Dutta A, Osborne JE, Reiser MB, Jayaraman V. Two-photon calcium imaging from head-fixed Drosophila during optomotor walking behavior. Nature Methods. 2010;7:535–540. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Shaw BK, Kristan WB., jr The neuronal basis of the behavioral choice between swimming and shortening in the leech: control is not selectively exercised at higher circuit levels. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:786–795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-02-00786.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sherrington CS. The integrative action of the nervous system. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons; 1906. [Google Scholar]
  76. Stein PS. Motor systems, with specific reference to the control of locomotion. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1978;1:61–81. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.01.030178.000425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Stein PS. The vertebrate scratch reflex. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology. 1983;37:383–403. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Stein PS. Neuronal control of turtle hindlimb motor rhythms. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2005;191:213–229. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0568-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sweeney ST, Broadie K, Keane J, Niemann H, O'Kane CJ. Targeted expression of tetanus toxin light chain in Drosophila specifically eliminates synaptic transmission and causes behavioral defects. Neuron. 1995;14:341–351. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90290-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Vandervorst P, Ghysen A. Genetic control of sensory connections in Drosophila. Nature. 1980;286:65–67. doi: 10.1038/286065a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. von Philipsborn AC, Liu T, Yu JY, Masser C, Bidaye SS, Dickson BJ. Neuronal control of Drosophila courtship song. Neuron. 2011;69:509–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. von Reyn CR, Breads P, Peek MY, Zheng GZ, Williamson WR, Yee AL, Leonardo A, Card GM. A Spike-Timing Mechanism for Action Selection. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17(7):962–970. doi: 10.1038/nn.3741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wan Y, Otsuna H, Chien CB, Hansen C. An interactive visualization tool for multi-channel confocal microscopy data in neurobiology research. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics. 2009;15:1489–1496. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2009.118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Yasuyama K, Salvaterra PM. Localization of choline acetyltransferase-expressing neurons in Drosophila nervous system. Microscopy Research and Technique. 1999;45:65–79. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0029(19990415)45:2&#x0003c;65::AID-JEMT2&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Yorozu S, Allan W, Fischer BJ, Dankert H, Kernan MJ, Kamikouchi A, Ito K, Anderson DJ. Distinct sensory representations of wind and near-field sound in the Drosophila brain. Nature. 2009;458:201–205. doi: 10.1038/nature07843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Zhukovskaya M, Yanagawa A, Forschler B. Grooming behavior as a mechanism of insect disease defense. Insects. 2013;4:609–630. doi: 10.3390/insects4040609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2015 Sep 7;4:e08758. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08758.035

Decision letter

Editor: Alexander Borst1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled “A neural command circuit for grooming movement control” for peer review at eLife. Your submission has been favorably evaluated by K VijayRaghavan (Senior Editor), a Reviewing Editor, and three reviewers.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

All reviewers agree that this is an extremely thorough characterization of the neural circuits underlying an interesting behavior in Drosophila. For example, one of the reviewers said: “The authors have done an impressive job in leveraging the state-of-the-art genetic/neural circuit tools in Drosophila to determine which neurons are necessary/sufficient for this behavior and how they functionally connect with each other. Moreover, they have a nicely supported model for functional interactions in the circuit, including the role of inhibition – this is terrific!”. Another reviewer commented: “This study will be highly interesting for neuroscientists in the field of motor control as well as for scientists studying animal behavior in general”. A third reviewer also stated: “The authors identify novel chordotonal sensory neurons innervating Johnston's Organ (JO) in the second antennal segment, and 3 classes of interneurons involved in this behavior, including 2 classes of local neurons (LNs) and several classes of descending neurons (DNs). This clearly required a heroic effort, and the authors are to be congratulated for their work”.

Remarkably, optogenetic activation of any of these classes of neurons drives grooming of the antenna, and not of other body parts (at least during photostimulation) (Figure 7–figure supplement 1). This observation offers a fascinating insight into the fundamental question of how the fly's brain directs its appendages to groom the precise part of the body surface from which it is receiving a sensory irritant or input: evidently the sensory neurons that innervate that part of the body surface activate a motor circuit that controls limb movement with precisely the right trajectory to direct them to the location from which the sensory input was received. The data presented here show that deeper layers of the circuit exhibit a similar level of functional specificity. It is almost as if this part of the fly's sensory-motor system is built like a marionette: a dedicated, position-specific sensory-interneuron-motor loop for each body part that needs to be groomed. While there were hints of this in the original paper, the present study demonstrates this much more clearly and the degree of specificity uncovered, at all layers of the circuit, is remarkable. Although the results of this paper do not yet explain how this sensory information is converted to such a specific limb trajectory through 3-D space at the motor level, they provide an important step in this direction. It is, therefore, surprising that the authors do not raise this interesting point at all in their Introduction and Discussion. They are encouraged to do so.

In summary, the authors have taken an important step forward in deconstructing a circuit that may ultimately explain a fascinating problem in sensory-motor coordination. They are encouraged to downplay the reciprocal excitation/persistent activity part of their story, which is the weakest, and emphasize the aspects of their findings that are most relevant to the grooming system, namely the remarkable functional specificity they have uncovered. The circuit architecture they have defined certainly hints at a more complex function and regulation than would be expected from a simple feed-forward stimulus-response reflex pathway, and this is interesting as well. Above all, they should make clear to the reader (especially the non-specialist) the limitations of the methods they have employed, and the parts of the story that are rock-solid vs. those that are less so.

Essential revisions:

1) Which sensory neurons of the JO induce grooming?

1A) It is unclear what distinguishes antennal movements for song, wind, and gravity perception (what these antennal movements are known to be important for) and antennal movements for grooming. Is it a much larger displacement of the antenna? Or is it the context? The authors show a plot where they increase the strength of the magnetic field (measured in volts) and show more grooming. They could easily measure the displacement of the antenna (using high speed video or a laser doppler vibrometer) and determine what range of antennal displacements (measured in nm) leads to grooming – this information is critical for knowing what strength of the stimulus activates the antennal grooming circuits. This is important because the receptor neurons for their behavior are likely overlapping with the receptor neurons for song, wind, and gravity detection, as the authors mention.

1B) The authors interpret their findings to that effect that a specific subpopulation of JO neurons is exclusively responsible for providing sensory information specifically for antennal grooming. I agree that activation of these sensory neurons (SN) shows that they are sufficient to induce grooming. This, however, might be a general and unspecific stimulation of the JO which, in turn, elicits a general grooming response. You show in Figure 4E that silencing aJO neurons significantly reduces grooming; while this suggests that aJO is involved in eliciting grooming it only shows sufficiency, not necessity. Also, this is still consistent with a general decrease of JO sensitivity. Therefore, the following questions need to be addressed to support the conclusion that these SNs are specific for grooming: (i) did activation of the other previously described subpopulations of JO SNs (e.g. those described to be specific for hearing, gravitaxis, or wind sensitivity) fail to induce grooming? (ii) if aJO neurons are indeed the sole JO neurons necessary for grooming then ablation of all non-aJO neurons should not result in a decrease in grooming behavior; (iii) it may have slipped my attention, but was the role of antennal exteroceptors, e.g. tactile hairs, tested? All of these points have to be addressed before you can definitely say that aJO neurons are necessary and sufficient for grooming, i.e. part of the grooming circuit. As it stands now, you have shown that grooming can be induced by stimulating at least part of the JO.

1C) The authors show that the activity of several (but not all) of the classes of neurons they have described is required for this behavior. That is reassuring, at one level. However inspection of the video suggests that the magnitude of the antennal displacements (while not explicitly measured) produced by the magnet must be far greater than those normally produced by the application of dust. This raises the important question of whether the neurons described here are required for dust-evoked grooming. The authors do not comment on this point, and should. Presumably, they have tried this experiment, and apparently it did not work (otherwise why would they have gone to the trouble of constructing the elaborate magnet-evoked assay). The authors need to be up front with their readers about this, raise the issue and discuss it. There may be an interesting reason for the discrepancy. For example, is the artificial stimulus used here actually mimicking the antennal displacements produced by grooming behavior itself, rather than by the small particles that typically evoke grooming? If so, could the circuit uncovered here be part of a mechanism for feedback-control of grooming behavior? There is some hint of this, briefly touched on by the authors in passing, because amputation of the antenna actually causes an increased level of grooming behavior produced by thermogenetic activation of some of the interneuron classes. In any case, in the interests of full disclosure the authors need to discuss this important issue.

2) The Introduction is not broad enough. For example, the authors need to provide more background on CPGs, patterned motor behaviors, and grooming, and to place the behavior they are studying in the context of the larger body of work. For example, the authors only cite papers on stick insect walking when introducing stereotyped motor patterns – they need to invoke behaviors like song production, feeding, etc. and compare/contrast their behavior with other well-studied motor patterns. In addition, the authors also don't provide enough background on their previous study (Seeds et al., 2014) – this study is in many ways a follow-up, but they expect the reader to have familiarity with this paper already.

3) Where do individual aJOs project? The authors don't report on any single cell clones to show that individual aJOs innervate all 3 brain regions mentioned (AMMC, pSEZ and vSEZ) or that individual aJOs only innervate one region. If this experiment was not possible for some technical reason can the authors indicate this in the text of the manuscript?

4) Functional connectivity versus synaptic connectivity: The data presented clearly show functional connectivity between neurons. However, there is only weak evidence (it is not ‘likely’), that functionally connected neurons are postsynaptic to each other since synaptic delays are known to be small relative to the temporal resolution of the GCaMP6s used. Their approach detects functional connectivity but not synaptic connectivity – which is fine. However, the authors should make this clear throughout the manuscript (and in the Abstract).

5) Resolution of the behavior: Given the authors' approach to scoring the behavior manually (and lumping all of antennal grooming into one category) it is not possible to examine whether activity of different neurons in the antennal grooming circuit leads to different types/aspects of behavior or different timing of the behavior or differences in trial-to-trial reproducibility. Given that the authors suggest in their Introduction and Discussion that this behavior is a good model for understanding the contribution of sensory drive and modulation to stereotyped motor programs, it would help to know how stereotyped the behavior is. The authors should add a paragraph to their Discussion of the future value of examining the grooming behavior of Drosophila at higher resolution (think Dragonfly or Drosophila flight/steering, Drosophila or bird song production, ant path navigation, or arm reaching). In other words, there is much more to get out of this system now that the circuit is mapped.

6) The differentiation of the classes of antennal grooming descending neurons (aDN) appears to be less specific compared to the two aLN-classes. For example, no information is given on aDN2 in Figure3 and its legend. In general throughout the text the number of DN-classes that are referred to is changing from two (in the subsection “Three different interneuron classes elicit antennal grooming”), to one (in the subsection “A putative circuit that elicits grooming in response to antennal displacement”) and to three (also in the aforementioned subsection). In order to justify the summary Figure 7B, it is important that the authors allow the reader to keep clear track for all data available on DN classes leading to this scheme.

7) In the third paragraph of the subsection “Connectivity among the antennal grooming neuronal classes”, based on the neuroanatomical analysis of the branching patterns of the described sensory and brain interneurons, the authors conclude specific connectivity between them. I agree that their data show that these neurons are likely to belong to the same circuit, but I do not see proof of an explicit synaptic connectivity between them: (i) the anatomical analysis of the branching patterns has not the scale to allow for that – a scale of 25µm as given in the figures is not sufficient to resolve connectivity (e.g. see Figure 5) and (ii) the same holds for the calcium transients recorded (e.g. see Figure 6). Such explicit interpretation is not justified. A terminology like “neurons downstream” or “neurons upstream” is appropriate.

8) With regards to the subsection “A command circuit that elicits variable grooming durations”, all neural circuits are composed of “multiple neurons”. It is not clear which point the authors want to make. Specifically, information is lacking on how the new findings relate to previous findings on neurons that serve command-like functions in insect behavior (work on courtship behavior in locusts and crickets; mostly Hedwig and coauthors) and in the fruit fly? With respect to the fruit fly I was missing a discussion on what the new study adds to the previous work on command neurons in the control of fruit fly courtship behavior (e.g. von Philippsborn et al., 2011), aversive motor behavior (Gao et al., 2013) and walking direction (Bidaye et al., 2014)?

9) Is there really a reciprocal excitation loop in this circuit, and is it responsible for persistent grooming behavior evoked by optogenetic stimulation? The authors place a great deal of emphasis on the finding of an apparent reciprocal excitation loop between the aLN1 and aLN2 local interneurons (Figure 7B), and invoke it to explain their interesting observation that activation of these interneurons can produce persistent grooming (Figure 7C). Unfortunately, due to limitations in the specificity of the reagents used to perform these experiments, the evidence of reciprocal excitation is unconvincing and it is very difficult to correlate the physiological (imaging) and behavioral data. Firstly, the evidence of reciprocal connectivity is weak. The degree of anatomical overlap between aLN1 and aLN2 neurons is very limited (Figure 5E – by the way, please indicate with dashed outlines the location of the AMMC, posterior and ventral SEZ on these figures for the readers' benefit). Although the evidence for functional connectivity from LN1→LN2 neurons is convincing (Figure 6F), the evidence for the reciprocal connection (Figure 6G) is not. Apparently (based on the information provided in Figure 6–figure supplement 3), to test for the latter connection the authors used an LN2-LexA driver to express CsChrimson in these neurons (in contrast to the reverse experiment where they used an LN1-GAL4 driver to express the opsin). This LN2-LexA driver is extremely broadly expressed, in the central brain and optic lobes (Figure 4–figure supplement 1C). Without specific 2-photon targeted activation of the LN2 neurons themselves, it is difficult to be confident that the excitatory drive on the LN1 neurons in Figure 6G is indeed due to input from the aLN2 cells.

10) Furthermore, the magnitude of the average LN1 response to “LN2 stimulation” in Figure 6G is much weaker than that of the LN1 → LN2 stimulation in Figure 6F. Close inspection of the supplemental data indicates that even this weak average recording is misleading, because only a fraction of the flies tested, and only a small fraction of trials, yielded any response at all (Figure 6–figure supplement 3, LN2 to LN1). This is very different from a result in which the neurons showed a weak response in most of the flies in most of the trials. This is all the more puzzling given that there are more aLN2 than aLN1 cells (Figure 7B), and that the activation of neurons in the aLN2-spGAL4-2 line has the strongest effect to promote grooming of any of the lines characterized in the paper (Figure 2B and 7C). Thus, the evidence for an LN2→LN1 connection that is relevant to grooming is, in my opinion, weak and unconvincing. At the very least, the circuit diagram in Figure 7B should be re-drawn to show that the connection from aLN2→aLN1 is far weaker and more questionable than the reverse connection (perhaps using a dashed line and question mark), and this issue and associated caveats should be clearly spelled out in the Discussion.

11) In addition, if the authors really want to make the case that this putative reciprocal excitatory loop is responsible for the persistent grooming behavior they observe, then they should perform an epistasis experiment in which they demonstrate that persistent grooming triggered by aLN2 activation (but perhaps not grooming per se) is blocked by inactivating aLN1 cells, and/or vice-versa.

Finally, the authors should point out that in their antennal displacement assay, only a small fraction of stimulation trials elicited persistent grooming, and that even in those cases the persistence appears to be in the grooming of the eyes/head, not the antenna (Figure 4–figure supplement 3). So the biological relevance of this persistence phenomenon, interesting though it may be, is not obvious.

eLife. 2015 Sep 7;4:e08758. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08758.036

Author response


[…] In summary, the authors have taken an important step forward in deconstructing a circuit that may ultimately explain a fascinating problem in sensory-motor coordination. They are encouraged to downplay the reciprocal excitation/persistent activity part of their story, which is the weakest, and emphasize the aspects of their findings that are most relevant to the grooming system, namely the remarkable functional specificity they have uncovered. The circuit architecture they have defined certainly hints at a more complex function and regulation than would be expected from a simple feed-forward stimulus-response reflex pathway, and this is interesting as well. Above all, they should make clear to the reader (especially the non-specialist) the limitations of the methods they have employed, and the parts of the story that are rock-solid vs. those that are less so.

We thank the reviewers for their kind and thoughtful comments regarding this work. The manuscript is revised to better emphasize the discovery of a circuit that induces a highly specific grooming movement. The possible role of reciprocal excitation as the cause of persistent grooming is now deemphasized, but the manuscript retains some focus on the circuit as a potential mediator of grooming duration. We have added additional experiments related to the antennae and Johnston’s organ requested by the reviewers, including two full figure supplements for Figure 1 (Figure 1–figure supplements 3 and 4), and an additional panel for Figure 4–figure supplement 3. We revised the Abstract, and the Introduction and Discussion are expanded to include topics that the reviewers requested. Finally, we have renamed the interneurons aLN1 and aLN2 to aBN1 and aBN2 as suggested by the reviewers. Our responses to each requested revision are listed below.

Essential revisions:

1) Which sensory neurons of the JO induce grooming?

Responses to specific queries for question 1 are addressed independently (i.e. 1A, 1B (parts i, ii, iii), and 1C). At the end, these responses are assembled into a cohesive conclusion. Additionally, because question 3 also fits nicely with this question, we incorporated our response to it with question 1 conclusions.

1A) It is unclear what distinguishes antennal movements for song, wind, and gravity perception (what these antennal movements are known to be important for) and antennal movements for grooming. Is it a much larger displacement of the antenna? Or is it the context? The authors show a plot where they increase the strength of the magnetic field (measured in volts) and show more grooming. They could easily measure the displacement of the antenna (using high speed video or a laser doppler vibrometer) and determine what range of antennal displacements (measured in nm) leads to grooming – this information is critical for knowing what strength of the stimulus activates the antennal grooming circuits. This is important because the receptor neurons for their behavior are likely overlapping with the receptor neurons for song, wind, and gravity detection, as the authors mention.

We measured how far the antennae are displaced when the electromagnet was driven at two different voltages (5 and 10V). Five volts induces limited grooming whereas ten volts induces near the maximum amount of grooming (Figure 4–figure supplement 3B). The measured displacements at these voltages are 11 +/- 3 μm (5V) and 31 +/- 6 μm (10V) (new data added as Figure 4–figure supplement 3C). The displacement distance at ten volts is within the range shown to induce calcium responses in JO neurons projecting to zones C/E (Yorozu 2009). Interestingly, aJO neurons identified in this work project to zone C/E (new data added as Figure 1–figure supplement 3A-B), suggesting that they too might be activated by such displacements. However, because these results demonstrate that the amplitude of the stimulus is capable of inducing previously described zone C/E neurons, this experiment does not resolve how the stimulus induces grooming behavior versus wind-induced suppression of locomotion.

The measurement of antennal displacement magnitude is now in the Results text and incorporated into Figure 4–figure supplement 3C. Additionally, we mention in the Discussion that this displacement magnitude is likely to cause calcium responses in previously described zone C/E neurons and provide limited speculation on the implications. Finally, text was added to the Materials and methods that describe how we measured the antennal displacements.

1B) The authors interpret their findings to that effect that a specific subpopulation of JO neurons is exclusively responsible for providing sensory information specifically for antennal grooming. I agree that activation of these sensory neurons (SN) shows that they are sufficient to induce grooming. This, however might be a general and unspecific stimulation of the JO which, in turn, elicits a general grooming response. You show in Figure 4E that silencing aJO neurons significantly reduces grooming; while this suggests that aJO is involved in eliciting grooming it only shows sufficiency, not necessity. Also, this is still consistent with a general decrease of JO sensitivity. Therefore, the following questions need to be addressed to support the conclusion that these SNs are specific for grooming: (i) did activation of the other previously described subpopulations of JO SNs (e.g. those described to be specific for hearing, gravitaxis, or wind sensitivity) fail to induce grooming?

We tested whether activation of previously described JO subpopulations induces grooming. Excitation of zone C/E neurons using previously published GAL4 drivers elicits antennal grooming, whereas excitation of zone A/B neurons did not (new data added as Figure 1–figure supplement 3C-H). We added a co-stain of aJO with two previously described C/E GAL4 lines to show that aJO neurons described in this study also project to zone C/E (new data added as Figure 1–figure supplement 3A-B, also discussed above in reference to reviewers' comment 1A). Taken together, our data demonstrates that a subset of JO neurons (zone C/E) are involved in inducing antennal grooming. The fact that activation of neurons projecting to other zones does not induce grooming argues against the possibility that antennal grooming is a non-specific effect of generally activating any JO neurons.

(ii) If aJO neurons are indeed the sole JO neurons necessary for grooming then ablation of all non-aJO neurons should not result in a decrease in grooming behavior.

Our results indicate that expression of TNT in the aJO almost completely blocks the grooming response to imposed movements of the antennae (Figure 4E). Therefore, we described these neurons as being necessary for this response. We believe that calling aJO neurons necessary does not imply that they are the only sensory neurons on the antennae that induce grooming in response to antennal deflections. However, we changed two sentences in the manuscript to now state that the aJO neurons are critical rather than necessary. A sentence in the third paragraph of the subsection “A putative circuit that elicits grooming in response to antennal displacement” now states: “The aJO was critical for this response because expression of TNT in these neurons significantly reduced antennal and head grooming in response to the magnetic field (Figure 4E, blue boxes, head grooming not shown).” The first sentence in the subsection “A distinct class of JO neurons detects antennal displacements and elicits grooming” now reads: “aJO mechanosensory neurons are critical for the grooming response to antennal movements and sufficient to induce grooming.”

(iii) It may have slipped my attention, but was the role of antennal exteroceptors, e.g. tactile hairs, tested? All of these points have to be addressed before you can definitely say that aJO neurons are necessary and sufficient for grooming, i.e. part of the grooming circuit. As it stands now, you have shown that grooming can be induced by stimulating at least part of the JO.

The bristle mechanosensory neurons on the antennae have not been tested for their role in grooming because we have not yet acquired spGAL4 lines that can target them. We added text to address the bristle mechanosensory neurons in the Discussion (for further information, see our comments below for 1C).

1C) The authors show that the activity of several (but not all) of the classes of neurons they have described is required for this behavior. That is reassuring, at one level. However inspection of the video suggests that the magnitude of the antennal displacements (while not explicitly measured) produced by the magnet must be far greater than those normally produced by the application of dust. This raises the important question of whether the neurons described here are required for dust-evoked grooming. The authors do not comment on this point, and should. Presumably, they have tried this experiment, and apparently it did not work (otherwise why would they have gone to the trouble of constructing the elaborate magnet-evoked assay). The authors need to be up front with their readers about this, raise the issue and discuss it. There may be an interesting reason for the discrepancy. For example, is the artificial stimulus used here actually mimicking the antennal displacements produced by grooming behavior itself, rather than by the small particles that typically evoke grooming? If so, could the circuit uncovered here be part of a mechanism for feedback-control of grooming behavior? There is some hint of this, briefly touched on by the authors in passing, because amputation of the antenna actually causes an increased level of grooming behavior produced by thermogenetic activation of some of the interneuron classes. In any case, in the interests of full disclosure the authors need to discuss this important issue.

We discuss this in the first two paragraphs of a new subsection of the Discussion called “Biological functions of the antennal grooming circuit.” In brief, the large deflections that induce grooming described in this paper are likely to be caused by non-debris stimuli such as static electricity or unexpected displacements of the antennae. We cite several papers that support this idea. We also address the role of this circuitry in the removal of debris, such as dust from the antennae. Different sensory neuron types other than the JO neurons may play a role in detecting debris on the antennae and inducing grooming (e.g. antennal mechanosensory bristles). However, we have not yet found lines that specifically target the bristle neurons to test this possibility.

3) Where do individual aJOs project? The authors don't report on any single cell clones to show that individual aJOs innervate all 3 brain regions mentioned (AMMC, pSEZ and vSEZ) or that individual aJOs only innervate one region. If this experiment was not possible for some technical reason can the authors indicate this in the text of the manuscript?

We added a multicolor stochastic labeling experiment to the manuscript. This revealed that the majority of neurons within the aJO have similar projections. Most of the cells project from the AMMC to the ventral SEZ, and a smaller subset projects from the AMMC to the posterior SEZ (new data added as Figure 1–figure supplement 4). In contrast to previously identified zone C/E-projecting neurons, we found no evidence of aJO neurons that project only to the AMMC. Text was added to the first Results section to describe this experiment. Additionally, we added more discussion that considers the implications of this result (see conclusions below).

Conclusions for questions 1 and 3:

A) Activation of JO neurons targeted by previously described GAL4 lines that express in the different JO regions revealed that C/E-projecting neurons induce grooming, whereas A/B-projecting subtypes do not (new data added as Figure 1–figure supplement 3C-H). Additionally, newly added co-stains show that aJO neurons project into zone C/E (new data added as Figure 1–figure supplement 3A-B). Therefore, JO-induced antennal grooming is likely restricted to neurons projecting to zone C/E, which argues against the possibility raised by the reviewers that the grooming is caused by non-specific activation of any JO neurons.

B) We find evidence that at least two subsets of zone C/E JO neurons induce grooming. The previously described C/E neurons show no obvious projections to the SEZ, but instead terminate in the AMMC (Kamikouchi, 2006). In contrast most, if not all of the aJO neurons project into the AMMC and then to the SEZ, with none appearing to terminate in the AMMC (Figure 1–figure supplement 4). This indicates that the aJO projections belong to a previously undescribed group, leading us to conclude that two different populations of C/E-projecting neurons can elicit antennal grooming – one that projects only to the AMMC, and the other (aJO) that projects to the AMMC and SEZ. Future work will be required to determine how these two populations of JO sensory neurons interact with the antennal grooming circuitry.

C) The antennal displacement distance that induces grooming (31 +/- 6 μm) is within the range shown to induce calcium responses in previously described C/E JO neurons. Therefore, aJO neurons might also be activated by such large displacements, coupled with our finding that they are critical for the grooming response to these displacements.

Overview of major revisions:

A) We revised and extended the Results and Discussion to incorporate the new results and draw the conclusions mentioned above. Particularly, we added text to the Results section (last two paragraphs) called “A group of Johnston’s Organ mechanosensory neurons elicits antennal grooming” and Discussion section, “A distinct class of JO neurons detects antennal displacements and elicits grooming”.

B) Because there are likely two populations of zone C/E JO neurons that induce grooming, we no longer say aJO neurons are responsible for antennal grooming in the Abstract. Instead we state, “Mechanosensory chordotonal neurons detect displacements of the antennae and excite three different classes of functionally connected interneurons, which include two classes of brain interneurons and different parallel descending neurons.”

C) Given that additional zone C/E neurons induce grooming, and that aJO-LexA may target some of these non-aJO neurons (Figure 1–figure supplement 3A, B, Figure 4–figure supplement 2A, B), we now refer to this LexA line as targeting aJO+C/E neurons in the Results. We have revised our conclusions to more generally state that zone C/E neurons elicit antennal grooming, rather than just the aJO neurons. This required us to modify some text in the Results, Discussion, and labels in Figures 5-7. The main part of the manuscript that addresses this is in the second paragraph of the Results section called “A putative circuit that elicits grooming in response to antennal displacement.”

2) The Introduction is not broad enough. For example, the authors need to provide more background on CPGs, patterned motor behaviors, and grooming, and to place the behavior they are studying in the context of the larger body of work. For example, the authors only cite papers on stick insect walking when introducing stereotyped motor patterns – they need to invoke behaviors like song production, feeding, etc. and compare/contrast their behavior with other well-studied motor patterns. In addition, the authors also don't provide enough background on their previous study (Seeds et al. 2014) – this study is in many ways a follow-up, but they expect the reader to have familiarity with this paper already.

We now provide additional background information on stereotyped movement patterns, CPGs, the study of neurons that induce specific movements in Drosophila, and additional information on our previous work. However, on the latter point, our previous work was primarily focused on how grooming is organized into a sequence of movements, which is not what we intended this paper to be about. The only point that we want to introduce from this previous work is the finding that activating different subsets of neurons induces specific grooming movements. This sets up the identification of neurons that specifically elicit antennal grooming. We have added two additional sentences to the last paragraph of the Introduction that more completely describes this point from our previous work. Additionally, the reviewers asked us to compare and contrast grooming with other stereotyped motor patterns. We felt that this was better addressed in more detail in the Discussion, framed in the context of what we learned from the results presented in the paper. The last section of the Discussion discusses the antennal grooming circuit in the context of other command-like neurons that have been described in flies. We hope that the reviewers are satisfied with this approach.

4) Functional connectivity versus synaptic connectivity: The data presented clearly show functional connectivity between neurons. However, there is only weak evidence (it is not ‘likely’), that functionally connected neurons are postsynaptic to each other since synaptic delays are known to be small relative to the temporal resolution of the GCaMP6s used. Their approach detects functional connectivity but not synaptic connectivity – which is fine. However, the authors should make this clear throughout the manuscript (and in the Abstract).

We made numerous changes throughout the manuscript to make clear that our evidence supports functional connectivity among the neurons. Where appropriate, we now use the terminology suggested by the reviewers, such as “neurons downstream” or “neurons upstream” (suggested in question 7). In other places we now refer to the neurons as “functionally connected” rather than connected.

5) Resolution of the behavior: Given the authors' approach to scoring the behavior manually (and lumping all of antennal grooming into one category) it is not possible to examine whether activity of different neurons in the antennal grooming circuit leads to different types/aspects of behavior or different timing of the behavior or differences in trial-to-trial reproducibility. Given that the authors suggest in their Introduction and Discussion that this behavior is a good model for understanding the contribution of sensory drive and modulation to stereotyped motor programs, it would help to know how stereotyped the behavior is. The authors should add a paragraph to their Discussion of the future value of examining the grooming behavior of Drosophila at higher resolution (think Dragonfly or Drosophila flight/steering, Drosophila or bird song production, ant path navigation, or arm reaching). In other words, there is much more to get out of this system now that the circuit is mapped.

We have added a paragraph to the Discussion that now addresses this important point. In brief, we discuss that the antennal grooming circuit might control parameters that our current analysis methods cannot detect. The level of behavioral analysis presented in this work does not allow for the detection of fine antennal grooming movements. Thus, higher resolution analysis of the kinematics of the leg movements may allow for testing the roles of the circuit in controlling different variables of antennal grooming. This text can be found in the last paragraph of the Discussion called “A command circuit that elicits variable grooming durations.”

6) The differentiation of the classes of antennal grooming descending neurons (aDN) appears to be less specific compared to the two aLN-classes. For example, no information is given on aDN2 in Figure3 and its legend. In general throughout the text the number of DN-classes that are referred to is changing from two (in the subsection “Three different interneuron classes elicit antennal grooming”), to one (in the subsection “A putative circuit that elicits grooming in response to antennal displacement”) and to three (also in the aforementioned subsection). In order to justify the summary Figure 7B, it is important that the authors allow the reader to keep clear track for all data available on DN classes leading to this scheme.

We made several revisions to the text to ensure that our logic for stating that there are three aDNs is consistent and clear throughout. First, we added text to the last sentence of the last paragraph of the Results section called “Three different interneuron classes elicit antennal grooming” that should help establish that there are three aDNs. This text reads: “However, we did not identify a spGAL4 combination that exclusively expresses in the third R18C11-targeted aDN (named aDN3)” Second, we added text to the Figure 2H legend that mentions aDN3. This text reads: “No spGAL4 combinations were identified that exclusively target aDN3.” Third, we revised text to the second sentence of the first paragraph of the Results section called “A putative circuit that elicits grooming in response to antennal displacement”, which now explicitly mentions all three aDNs. The sentence reads: “aBNs project to the AMMC and SEZ, following the aJO projections (Figure 3B, C), whereas two aDNs (aDN1 and aDN2), and likely aDN3 project both to the SEZ and through the cervical connective to the ProNm (Figure 3D, only aDN1 shown).” Fourth, to avoid confusion about why only aDN1 is shown in Figure 3, we added text to the legend stating: “aDN1 is shown as an example in (D), but there are additional aDNs (aDN2 and aDN3, see Figure 2F, H and Figure 2–figure supplement 2G-I)”. Finally, we added text that further justifies the way the aDNs are depicted in the Figure 7B in the first paragraph of the Results section called “A circuit whose components elicit different durations of antennal grooming.” This text reads: “We propose that the aDNs can act in parallel because experiments to thermogenetically activate aDN1 or aDN2 alone indicate that they are each sufficient to induce antennal grooming (Figure 2B).”

7) In the third paragraph of the subsection “Connectivity among the antennal grooming neuronal classes”, based on the neuroanatomical analysis of the branching patterns of the described sensory and brain interneurons, the authors conclude specific connectivity between them. I agree that their data show that these neurons are likely to belong to the same circuit, but I do not see proof of an explicit synaptic connectivity between them: (i) the anatomical analysis of the branching patterns has not the scale to allow for that – a scale of 25µm as given in the figures is not sufficient to resolve connectivity (e.g. see Figure 5) and (ii) the same holds for the calcium transients recorded (e.g. see Figure 6). Such explicit interpretation is not justified. A terminology like “neurons downstream” or “neurons upstream” is appropriate.

See question 4 for the revisions that we made to the manuscript. Of note, we incorporated the reviewers’ suggestion to use terminology, such as neurons upstream or downstream.

8) With regards to the subsection “A command circuit that elicits variable grooming durations”, all neural circuits are composed of “multiple neurons”. It is not clear which point the authors want to make.

We agree with the reviewers that we did not sufficiently describe the significance of circuits that consist of multiple neurons. We clarified this point in the third paragraph of the subsection “A command circuit that elicits variable grooming durations” (in the Discussion). We describe how the antennal grooming circuit and other well-described circuits are organized such that some neurons induce movements that persist beyond their initial activation, whereas other neurons must be continually activated to continue the movement. We point out that this type of organization appears to be common among sensory-connected neuronal circuits that induce specific movements.

Specifically, information is lacking on how the new findings relate to previous findings on neurons that serve command-like functions in insect behavior (work on courtship behavior in locusts and crickets; mostly Hedwig and coauthors) and in the fruit fly? With respect to the fruit fly I was missing a discussion on what the new study adds to the previous work on command neurons in the control of fruit fly courtship behavior (e.g. von Philippsborn et al., 2011), aversive motor behavior (Gao et al., 2013) and walking direction (Bidaye et al., 2014)?

We added a new section to the Discussion entitled “Neurons that command specific movements in insects.” This section describes how work in flies and other insects suggests that the use of dedicated neurons to command specific movements is a common mechanism of behavioral control in insects. We then insert the caveat that these command-like neurons may be embedded in larger, more dynamic neural networks in which neurons that elicit one movement also participate in other movements. We cite work from leech that supports this possibility.

9) Is there really a reciprocal excitation loop in this circuit, and is it responsible for persistent grooming behavior evoked by optogenetic stimulation? The authors place a great deal of emphasis on the finding of an apparent reciprocal excitation loop between the aLN1 and aLN2 local interneurons (Figure 7B), and invoke it to explain their interesting observation that activation of these interneurons can produce persistent grooming (Figure 7C). Unfortunately, due to limitations in the specificity of the reagents used to perform these experiments, the evidence of reciprocal excitation is unconvincing and it is very difficult to correlate the physiological (imaging) and behavioral data.

Questions 9, 10, and 11 all relate to the strength of evidence supporting aBN1/aBN2 reciprocal functional connections. We address the specific points for each of these questions and then summarize the revisions related to all of them at the end.

Firstly, the evidence of reciprocal connectivity is weak. The degree of anatomical overlap between aLN1 and aLN2 neurons is very limited (Figure 5E – by the way, please indicate with dashed outlines the location of the AMMC, posterior and ventral SEZ on these figures for the readers' benefit). Although the evidence for functional connectivity from LN1→LN2 neurons is convincing (Figure 6F), the evidence for the reciprocal connection (Figure 6G) is not. Apparently (based on the information provided in Figure 6–figure supplement 3), to test for the latter connection the authors used an LN2-LexA driver to express CsChrimson in these neurons (in contrast to the reverse experiment where they used an LN1-GAL4 driver to express the opsin). This LN2-LexA driver is extremely broadly expressed, in the central brain and optic lobes (Figure 4–figure supplement 1C). Without specific 2-photon targeted activation of the LN2 neurons themselves, it is difficult to be confident that the excitatory drive on the LN1 neurons in Figure 6G is indeed due to input from the aLN2 cells.

The reviewers requested that we used dashed outlines to mark the different projection regions. We realized with this comment that there was confusion about the meaning of AMMC, posterior and ventral SEZ in this context. We did not intend for these to refer to distinct regions of the neuropile, but to the three major regions defined by the aJO projections shown in Figure 3A (colored arrows). Therefore, defining specific regions using dashed outlines would be arbitrary and misleading. We have tried to make this clearer in Figure 5 by adding the same colored arrows shown in Figure 3A to identify each projection region containing overlap between the different neuronal pairs. We added text to the Figure 5 legend that describes what the arrows mean. This text reads: “Overlap between different projections of the LexA and spGAL4-targeted neurons is indicated by different colored arrows: (A-C, E, F) AMMC projections white arrows, (A) posterior SEZ projection (yellow arrow), (B-H) ventral SEZ projections (red arrows).”

We agree that the aBN2-LexA driver expresses in additional neurons, raising the possibility that aBN2 is not the upstream cause of the aBN1 increased calcium response shown in Figure 6F. Therefore we added text in the Results that describes this caveat. This text is in the fourth paragraph of the Results section called “Functional connectivity among the antennal grooming neuronal classes”, and states: “Further, because the aBN2-LexA driver used to express CsChrimson in aBN2 also expresses in neurons in other parts of the brain (Figure 4–figure supplement 1C), we cannot rule out these other neurons as causing the aBN1 calcium response.”

10) Furthermore, the magnitude of the average LN1 response to “LN2 stimulation” in Figure 6G is much weaker than that of the LN1 → LN2 stimulation in Figure 6F. Close inspection of the supplemental data indicates that even this weak average recording is misleading, because only a fraction of the flies tested, and only a small fraction of trials, yielded any response at all (Figure 6–figure supplement 3, LN2 to LN1). This is very different from a result in which the neurons showed a weak response in most of the flies in most of the trials. This is all the more puzzling given that there are more aLN2 than aLN1 cells (Figure 7B), and that the activation of neurons in the aLN2-spGAL4-2 line has the strongest effect to promote grooming of any of the lines characterized in the paper (Figure 2B and 7C). Thus, the evidence for an LN2→LN1 connection that is relevant to grooming is, in my opinion, weak and unconvincing.

We revised the manuscript to more completely describe the data that contributes to the average traces shown in Figure 6. Text was added to the second paragraph of the “GCaMP6s imaging and analysis of calcium responses” section of the Materials and methods. This more explicitly describes which imaging trials were used for data shown in Figure 6. We also labeled the trials from the raw imaging data shown in Figure 6–figure supplements 2-3 that contribute to the average traces shown in Figure 6.

At the very least, the circuit diagram in Figure 7B should be re-drawn to show that the connection from aLN2→aLN1 is far weaker and more questionable than the reverse connection (perhaps using a dashed line and question mark), and this issue and associated caveats should be clearly spelled out in the Discussion.

Figure 7B is revised to weaken our statement about a functional connection between aBN2 and aBN1. We redrew the connection from aBN2 to aBN1 using a dotted gray line to emphasize that the connection is not as well supported as the other connections in the diagram. Text was added to the figure legend that describes the meaning of the gray arrow. We also added text to the Figure 7B Results section to explain the aBN2 to aBN1 connection caveats (discussed more below).

11) In addition, if the authors really want to make the case that this putative reciprocal excitatory loop is responsible for the persistent grooming behavior they observe, then they should perform an epistasis experiment in which they demonstrate that persistent grooming triggered by aLN2 activation (but perhaps not grooming per se) is blocked by inactivating aLN1 cells, and/or vice-versa.

The manuscript is now revised to deemphasize the putative reciprocal excitation between aBN1 and aBN2. We describe the caveats associated with the experiment to test the aBN2 to aBN1 functional connectivity. The major revisions are described below.

Description of major revisions for questions 9-11:

A) We removed mention of the reciprocal excitation from the Abstract.

B) We added description of the caveats associated with the aBN2 to aBN1 functional connectivity experiment, and explicitly state that the connection is only weakly supported by the data. This description is found in the fourth paragraph of the Results section called “Functional connectivity among the antennal grooming neuronal classes.” As the reviewers pointed out, we now indicate that only high-intensity red light activation of aBN2 could cause a weak calcium response in aBN1. We mention that this response was inconsistent, as only three out of five flies tested showed increases in calcium in aBN1. We further point out that the aBN2-LexA driver also expresses in neurons in other parts of the brain (discussed above).

C) We removed most of the discussion of reciprocal excitation as an underlying mechanism causing the persistence. However, reciprocal excitation is briefly mentioned in the sixth paragraph of the Discussion section called “A command circuit that elicits variable grooming durations” as one of two possible mechanisms that could cause the persistence. This text reads: “Although our work only provides weak evidence for reciprocal excitation between the aBNs, a mechanism whereby reciprocal connections maintain their excitation offers a plausible explanation for the persistence that we observe. Alternatively, aBN1 and aBN2 could have intrinsic membrane properties that allow them to produce prolonged responses to brief excitation. Thus, future experiments will examine whether reciprocal excitation or intrinsic membrane properties of the aBNs produce persistent grooming (Major and Tank, 2004).”

Finally, the authors should point out that in their antennal displacement assay, only a small fraction of stimulation trials elicited persistent grooming, and that even in those cases the persistence appears to be in the grooming of the eyes/head, not the antenna (Figure 4–figure supplement 3). So the biological relevance of this persistence phenomenon, interesting though it may be, is not obvious.

The fourth paragraph of the Discussion section entitled “Biological functions of the antennal grooming circuit” addresses the possible biological relevance of persistent grooming. We discuss how our results do not identify a natural stimulus that induces reliable persistent grooming. However, persistence was previously described in the grooming responses of other animals, where it was shown to be at least partially dependent on the nature of the stimulus (e.g. stimulus strength, duration, or frequency). Thus, future work will explore the possibility that specific stimulus parameters can evoke persistent antennal grooming in flies.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1.

    Enhancer identities used to target different neuronal classes.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.031

    elife08758s001.pdf (46.1KB, pdf)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.031
    Supplementary file 2.

    Stimulus conditions used for the experiment shown in Figure 6.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.032

    elife08758s002.pdf (54KB, pdf)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.032
    Supplementary file 3.

    Stocks used in this study.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.033

    elife08758s003.pdf (138KB, pdf)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.033
    Supplementary file 4.

    Design plans for the fly electromagnetic stimulation rig.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08758.034

    elife08758s004.pdf (1MB, pdf)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.08758.034

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES