Abstract
The demands of modern psychophysical studies require precise stimulus delivery and flexible platforms for experimental control. Here, we describe PsyAcoustX, a new, freely available suite of software tools written in the MATLAB® environment to conduct psychoacoustics research on a standard PC. PsyAcoustX provides a flexible platform to generate and present auditory stimuli in real time and record users’ behavioral responses. Data are automatically logged by stimulus condition and aggregated in an exported spreadsheet for offline analysis. Detection thresholds can be measured adaptively under basic and complex auditory masking tasks and other paradigms (e.g., amplitude modulation detection) within minutes. The flexibility of the module offers experimenters access to nearly every conceivable combination of stimulus parameters (e.g., probe-masker relations). Example behavioral applications are highlighted including the measurement of audiometric thresholds, basic simultaneous and non-simultaneous (i.e., forward and backward) masking paradigms, gap detection, and amplitude modulation detection. Examples of these measurements are provided including the psychoacoustic phenomena of temporal overshoot, psychophysical tuning curves, and temporal modulation transfer functions. Importantly, the core design of PsyAcoustX is easily modifiable, allowing users the ability to adapt its basic structure and create additional modules for measuring discrimination/detection thresholds for other auditory attributes (e.g., pitch, intensity, etc.) or binaural paradigms.
Keywords: experiment design software, psychoacoustics, psychometric, auditory perception, forward masking, temporal modulation, temporal effect, gap detection
Introduction
Modern psychoacoustics provides a variety of experimental methodologies to probe the auditory perceptual system with the goal of establishing a link between physical stimuli and their corresponding percepts/sensations. Controlled stimulus manipulations are used to parametrically evaluate the performance of the sensory–perceptual system. Psychoacoustic studies typically involve the precise measurement of detection/discrimination thresholds that approach the limits of listeners’ hearing sensitivity. As such, auditory behavioral experiments have historically required dedicated, highly precise hardware for stimulus delivery and response collection. However, with the ubiquity and flexibility of modern computing, flexible PC-based platforms are now available for the control of rigorous auditory perceptual experiments.
Several commercial software platforms are currently available for auditory research. Both E-prime® (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.)1 and Presentation® (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.)2 are widely used suites for neuropsychological research. However, both of these packages are available only on the Windows® platform and contain no source code. These platforms are also limited to experimental designs with simple presentation paradigms; auditory stimuli must be pre-made or rendered in external software and then imported as WAV files. This shortcoming makes it cumbersome to implement adaptive tracking rules and forced choice procedures and does not allow the measurement of auditory thresholds—both common in psychoacoustics research (Green and Swets, 1966; Levitt, 1971; Macmillan and Creelman, 2005).
A handful of free software packages are now available for auditory behavioral research. Most of these packages, including WhisPER (Ciba et al., 2009) are tailored only to perceptual audio evaluation (e.g., for sound engineers) and collect only subjective listener judgments. Of the psychophysical packages implementing quantitative response collection, Psycon3 is based on the Auditory Syntax (AUX) scripting language (Kwon, 2011) and requires some background programming to run. The platform-independent program PsychoPy (Peirce, 2007), although largely geared toward visual psychophysics research, is able to generate and present auditory stimuli. However, this platform requires some user knowledge of the Python programming language and external auxiliary libraries to handle its graphical and input/output (I/O) engines. Other packages, (MLP Toolbox; Grassi and Soranzo, 2009) 4 provide access to psychoacoustic paradigms but limit the user to selected or built-in tasks. Moreover, task modifications require alterations to the source code, rather than through the convenience of a graphical user interface (GUI). Perhaps the most widely used, freely available package is the Psychtoolbox5 (Brainard, 1997). The Psychtoolbox is a collection of scripts implemented in MATLAB® (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) that provides access to hardware interfaces (e.g., monitor and sound card), millisecond timing, low-latency audio, and carries a large community of users and support forums. While the Psychtoolbox has garnered a vibrant history of development and is widely used by both behavioral and cognitive neuroscientists, psychoacoustic applications are limited.
Here, we present a new, point-and-click application able to execute typical psychoacoustic paradigms including a wide variety of forward/simultaneous masking paradigms, auditory detection tasks, and temporal processing measures. PsyAcoustX is a freely available, open source platform for psychoacoustics research that uses a common GUI and adaptive tracking rules to measure behavioral thresholds for various psychoacoustic phenomena6. PsyAcoustX can be run entirely via its extensive GUI, which provides full access to stimulus generation, calibration, subject logging, and data file I/O. As such, it requires little to no overhead of background programming knowledge. Nevertheless, PsyAcoustX was developed under the MATLAB® programming language to allow maximum flexibility and the development of extensions to the base package by the end user. We first discuss software/hardware system requirements, calibration, and provide an overview of PsyAcoustX’s GUI interface. We then highlight several applications of the program’s engine and illustrate typical data that can be obtained from current modules available in PsyAcoustX.
Materials and Methods
Hardware Requirements
PsyAcoustX requires only the MATLAB® base license and the functions of the Signal Processing Toolbox to run. Auditory stimuli are generated as digital waveforms within MATLAB® and output through the PC’s native soundcard and corresponding headphone port. In our laboratories, we use the pro-audio LynxTWO soundcard (Lynx Studio Technology, Inc.) and either ER-2 (Etymotic Research) or E-A-RTONE-5A headphones (Aearo Corp.). Other headphone and soundcard arrangements are possible assuming the user adequately accounts for the frequency response of the signal chain (e.g., correcting for any frequency shaping produced by headphones). However, a soundcard with quantization bit depth of 24-bits is recommended to take advantage of the largest possible dynamic range.
PsyAcoustX was designed in MATLAB® 2011 and has been tested for compatibility through the 2015a release. Data presented herein and those published in our previous studies (Bidelman and Syed Khaja, 2014; Bidelman et al., 2014; Roverud and Strickland, 2015) were collected using MATLAB® version 2013b, or earlier. Importantly, the timing of stimulus presentation in the PsyAcoustX GUI is not dependent on CPU speed. This was made possible by generating stimuli in single experimental trials as contiguous digital waveforms (e.g., see Figure 3). This approach has made it possible to create temporally precise auditory stimuli with millisecond resolution—e.g., as would be necessary for a gap detection threshold (GDT) paradigm (Fitzgibbons, 1983; Florentine et al., 1999)—that would be impossible to implement in the presence of any lag/jitter from the CPU. While there are no specific minimum PC hardware requirements for PsyAcoustX, 4 GB of memory is recommended to provide ample space to properly load stimuli to RAM during runtime execution and allow maximum fluidity of the program’s GUI.
Calibration
PsyAcoustX has a dedicated function which allows the user to calibrate the system output. Detailed calibration procedures are provided in the manual accompanying PsyAcoustX’s source code (see Supplementary Material). Briefly, the GUI allows the user to calibrate the system in reference to a 10-s, 1-kHz sinusoid played at a user-defined reference amplitude. MATLAB’s native audio functions (e.g., audioplayer.m) clip signal values greater than digital full scale (i.e., ±1); a calibration reference root-mean-squared amplitude of 0.4 is recommended to avoid audio clipping. The corresponding acoustic output of the sound hardware chain (i.e., MATLAB® → sound card → headphones) is then measured using a sound pressure level (SPL) meter and spectrum analyzer to ensure that distortion is within acceptable limits (i.e., low total harmonic distortion). The user can then set a variable (“CaldB”) in the systemInfo.mat file to this measured system SPL. “CaldB” represents the maximum obtainable output of PsyAcoustX (and full signal path) before distortion.
Overview of the GUI Interface and Program Layout
PsyAcoustX provides a fully functional, point-and-click GUI interface. This makes it possible for the end user to conduct a multitude of psychoacoustics paradigms without having to script or program (although user-based routines are easy to incorporate in the MATLAB® programming language). A screenshot of PsyAcoustX’s home window for the masking module is shown in Figure 1. Basic operations including subject enrollment, experiment creation, setting stimulus parameters, calibration, and run functions (detailed below) are available directly from the program’s home window.
Users can build auditory stimuli based on the needs of their paradigm, using the Stimulus generation window (Figure 2). PsyAcoustX was originally designed for auditory masking experiments and we have included a large number of tunable stimulus parameters for users to adjust properties of the probe, maskers, and control sounds as well as their temporal and spectral relations to one another (e.g., masker-probe delay). Precursor signals (Bacon and Healy, 2000; Jennings et al., 2009; Roverud and Strickland, 2014), suppression (Duifhuis, 1980), and notched noise (Patterson, 1976; Glasberg and Moore, 1990; Jennings and Strickland, 2012) options are also available for more complex masking paradigms. Additionally, we have included a toggle to implement a secondary high-frequency masker to limit off-frequency listening (Patterson and Nimmo-Smith, 1980; O’Loughlin and Moore, 1981; Jennings and Strickland, 2012). In our experience, these options promote maximum flexibility and make it possible to run a myriad of masking-based protocols.
Multiple stimulus conditions can be loaded into a single experiment, allowing the researcher to define all experimental conditions, run these conditions in a random order, and repeat conditions as necessary (see “Enroll Subject Feature”). Once the desired parameters are selected and condition blocks loaded, the program plots a spectrogram in PsyAcoustX’s home window to allow the user to verify the time course and spectral details of the stimuli (Figure 3). PsyAcoustX also allows the participant to play an example of the stimulus prior to starting the experiment to familiarize him/herself with the task.
PsyAcoustX uses a common experimental engine for all of its experimental modules. Experiments are implemented as a three-interval forced choice (3IFC) design with a 2-down—1-up adaptive tracking rule (Levitt, 1971). That is, one interval contains the probe while the other two do not (i.e., “noise” intervals). PsyAcoustX’s response window is shown in Figure 4. The response box includes lights to visualize the presentation order of the 3IFC task and another graphical light for feedback (green = correct; red = incorrect). The 3IFC paradigm provides a simple three button response interface for subjects. The common tracking rule provides a consistent criterion performance level (i.e., 71%; Levitt, 1971) for all experimental data. Thresholds for a given task are measured adaptively. Tracking on the probe level or masker level is possible within the masking module of PsyAcoustX. In other words, this allows the researcher to fix either the probe or masker while measuring threshold. One could, for example, measure either a masking pattern (Egan and Hake, 1950) or a psychophysical tuning curve (PTC; Moore, 1978) depending on whether threshold is measured by varying the probe, or masker, respectively.
During data collection, a small window (run tracker panel) displays the stimulus parameters, the subjects’ response history, the level of the dependent variable (e.g., probe level), and the threshold and standard deviation of the previous run. If two monitors are available on the PC system, the end user can position this window so it is not visible to the subject. This window is useful when monitoring the subject’s progress within a run, and when keeping a paper record of experimental data.
Data Management
Participants’ data are stored in “test” files and “completed” files, in the TXT format. Test files specify the stimulus parameter used while acquiring a given threshold measurement. Completed files contain a copy of the stimulus parameters, the subject’s responses for each individual trial in the adaptive track, and the measured threshold and standard deviation. Data management occurs automatically when subjects are enrolled in an experiment using the enroll subject window. This feature enables automatic test file and completed file generation. This feature also stores the information of test/completed files in Excel® and MATLAB® formats, to facilitate GUI automation and data export. Test files can also be saved and loaded manually using menu items in the stimulus generation window of PsyAcoustX.
Enroll Subject Feature
One of the more convenient features of PsyAcoustX is the ability to “enroll” a subject and store various experimental conditions. This feature allows the user to enter all possible values assumed by a given stimulus parameter during an experiment. When enrolling a subject (done using the menu bar in PsyAcoustX’s home window), the enroll subject window appears (Figure 5), which contains editable fields for all possible stimulus parameters. Most of these fields will accept single or multiple entries. For example, consider an experiment that involves measuring PTCs for several probe levels using a 4000-Hz probe. In the probe level and masker frequency fields, the user can enter all probe levels (e.g., 50, 55, 60, 65, and 70 dB SPL), and masker frequencies (e.g., 2000, 3000, 3500, 4000, 4200, and 4400 Hz), as well as other settings (e.g., probe frequency = 4000 Hz). Once the user submits the desired stimulus parameters, PsyAcoustX confirms the user-selected conditions in a table. After confirming the settings, PsyAcoustX then creates an array of folders and files associated with the subject, the experiment, and the conditions to be measured in the experiment. Moreover, these files and folders are automatically updated to track the participant’s progress during data collection.
If multiple subjects are to be run on the same experiment, the end user can save the experiment’s stimulus parameters as a “preset,” and load this preset when enrolling additional subjects. When experimental conditions are created using the enroll subject window, the user has three options for subsequent runtime. First, the user can select to continue with data collection, which will result in PsyAcoustX loading the next experimental condition and prompting the subject to start the run. Second, the user can select to start with a “warm up” condition. In this case, PsyAcoustX will load the next experimental condition, but will not save the data, nor flag the condition as completed after the subject’s thresholds are collected. This warm-up feature is useful to acclimatize subjects at the beginning of a testing session. Finally, the user can select to run a single user-defined condition. In this case, PsyAcoustX allows the user to navigate to a manually generated test file to run a single condition.
Results and Discussion
We now demonstrate some of the functional capabilities of the PsyAcoustX program. Various psychoacoustic paradigms are introduced and representative data are presented to illustrate the program’s flexibility.
Audiometric Hearing Thresholds
Auditory experiments typically involve first measuring listeners’ thresholds for long tones in quiet at octave frequencies from 250 to 8000 Hz, as would be done clinically, to rule out confounds of hearing acuity in psychoacoustic tasks. In PsyAcoustX, it is straightforward to measure these “audiometric” thresholds by turning off all maskers and tracking the probe frequency of interest. Detection thresholds are then measured adaptively based on PsyAcoustX’s stock 3IFC paradigm with a two-down one-up tracking rule, which converges on 71% correct performance (Levitt, 1971). Figure 6 shows the average audiometric thresholds for n = 17 normal hearing listeners. Additionally, the minimum audible pressure (MAP; Killion, 1978) is shown, representing auditory thresholds measured under headphone listening. As is illustrated in the figure, audiometric thresholds measured through PsyAcoustX agree well with typical hearing thresholds measured via other hardware. That is, once calibrated, PsyAcoustX can provide a tool for threshold measurements that is similar to audiological-grade systems. Subtle discrepancies in thresholds measured with PsyAcoustX and MAP curves are due to differences in the calibration reference between measures. MAP curves represent SPLs in the ear canal at each individual measurement frequency. Conversely, thresholds measured with PsyAcoustX are based on a calibration reference at 1 kHz and do not account for the frequency response of the headphones, or ear canal acoustics. We should stress that PsyAcoustX is intended for experimental purposes and not for diagnostic testing; proper audiological measurements typically require a sound booth, a quiet testing environment, calibrated earphones, and normative data.
Simultaneous Masking
PsyAcoustX can be used to measure thresholds in simultaneous masking. Here, we present simultaneous masking data on a frequently studied effect called “overshoot” or the “temporal effect” (Zwicker, 1965). Thresholds for a short sinusoidal probe improve as the probe’s temporal position approaches the temporal center of a gated, broadband masker (e.g., Bacon, 1990; Strickland, 2001). The magnitude of this improvement (i.e., overshoot) is the difference between thresholds measured at the onset of the masker, versus the temporal center. PsyAcoustX was used to measure overshoot for a 4000-Hz probe at several probe levels (50, 60, 70, 80, and 90 dB SPL). The probe was delayed by 2-ms (short delay) or 198-ms (long delay) from the onset of a noise masker (filtered Gaussian noise, 100–8000 Hz). Masker spectrum level at threshold was the dependent variable.
Figure 7A shows the masker spectrum level at threshold as a function of probe level for n = 17 normal-hearing ears for the short and long delay conditions. Overshoot from these measurements is shown in Figure 7B, computed as the difference in detection threshold between the long and short delay (i.e., long–short). Consistent with previous studies, listeners are better at detecting the probe (i.e., they experience less masking) in the long relative to the short delay condition. Participants show ∼4–12 dB release from masking when the probe is temporally centered in the masker compared to when it occurs concurrent with its onset (Figure 7B). Data from Strickland (2004) are also included for comparison.
Temporal Masking: Forward and Backward Masking
Temporal (i.e., non-simultaneous) masking is commonly used as an assay of temporal resolution. Temporal masking can also provide a psychoacoustic estimate of the basilar membrane’s compressive non-linearity (Oxenham and Plack, 1997; Nelson et al., 2001), and cochlear frequency selectivity (Moore, 1978; Shera et al., 2002). In forward masking, the interfering masker (e.g., tone or noise) precedes the probe; whereas in backwards masking, the masker follows the probe. Forward masking is thought to reflect direct physiological mechanisms of cochlear processing, e.g., the recovery from short-term adaptation or “ringing” of the basilar membrane (Harris and Dallos, 1979; Moore, 2003). Physiological mechanisms of backward masking are not well understood but are thought to result from “top–down” mechanisms such as selective attention and/or listeners’ confusion between the probe and masker (Wright, 1998; Moore, 2003).
Both forward and backward masking paradigms are available in PsyAcoustX. Temporal masking functions were measured according to the parameters described by Elliott (1971) (10-ms, 1-kHz probe masked by 70-dB, 50-ms wideband noise). In this example, we varied Δt, the time delay between the masker and probe. Representative temporal masking data from a normal hearing listener are shown in Figure 8. Temporal masking functions show the threshold elevation (in dB) for the probe when it precedes (Figure 8A, backwards) and follows (Figure 8B, forward) the masker. Consistent with data in the literature (Elliott, 1971), Figure 8 shows the consistent asymmetry observed in temporal masking studies between forward and backward conditions. Masking is more effective and persists for longer masker-probe intervals (Δt) in the forward compared to the backward case. The improved backward masking thresholds in our data relative to those of Elliott (1971) are likely attributable to the extensive musical training of our listener (first author), which is known to reduce the effects of backward masking (Strait et al., 2010).
Psychophysical Tuning Curves
The peripheral auditory system (i.e., cochlea) is typically conceived of as bank of overlapping bandpass filters that performs a spectral decomposition on the incoming sound (Fletcher, 1940; Patterson and Moore, 1986). In humans, the magnitude response of the auditory filters can be estimated using a number of psychophysical techniques. In the simplest simultaneous masking approach (Moore, 1978), the detection of a low-level probe is measured in the presence of a masking tone. The probe encourages the participant to “listen” at a specific cochlear location, or characteristic frequency (CF), while the masker interferes with the detection of the probe (Moore, 1978). Detectability of the probe varies dependent on the spectral proximity to the masker. The tuning (i.e., frequency selectivity) of a given cochlear location (i.e., CF) can then be estimated by plotting the masked probe threshold as a function of masker frequency to derive the so-called PTC. PTCs can be measured using simultaneous and forward masking approaches (for comparisons, see Moore, 1978; Bidelman et al., 2014) and/or using tonal or noise maskers. Although details are beyond the scope of the present report, each of these approaches has various strengths/weakness and controls for other extraneous factors, e.g., cochlear suppression and/or beating cues (Abbas and Sachs, 1976; Moore, 1978; Oxenham and Shera, 2003).
Relevant to the present work, we have successfully used PsyAcoustX in our recent studies to estimate cochlear tuning in human listeners via PTCs (Bidelman and Syed Khaja, 2014; Bidelman et al., 2014). Here, we present PTCs from a representative normal hearing listener (first author), measured at probe frequencies of 500 and 2000 Hz. PTCs were measured using forward masking with a 300-ms pure tone masker followed immediately by a 35-ms probe tone (0 ms masker-probe delay; for details, see Bidelman et al., 2014). We used 10 masker frequencies (i.e., five below and above the probe frequency). Masked threshold as a function of masker frequency provides an estimate of the PTC function, that is, the listeners’ auditory filter shape at a given CF.
Forward masked PTCs are shown in Figure 9. PTCs show the typical “V-shape” with a low-frequency tail, highly selective tip, and steep high-frequency skirt characteristic of auditory filters measured via psychophysical paradigms (Moore, 1978; Sek et al., 2005; Jennings and Strickland, 2012; Bidelman et al., 2014). It should be noted that other approaches to estimating auditory filters are also possible in PsyAcoustX. The notched noise method is another common technique to measure auditory filter bandwidths via masking. In this approach, the detectability of a probe is measured amidst noise with a notch of varying spectral width to derive the shape of the auditory filters (Patterson, 1976; Glasberg and Moore, 1990; Jennings and Strickland, 2012). In PsyAcoustX, the generation of both symmetric and asymmetric notched noise are possible, allowing the user to measure the inherent asymmetries in auditory filter profiles (Unoki et al., 2006).
Gap Detection
By nature, hearing involves decoding and interpreting changes in acoustic stimuli over time. The ability of the auditory system to follow these changes is known as “temporal resolution.” One measure of temporal resolution involves detecting a temporal gap in an otherwise steady-state stimulus, often called a “marker.” The minimum audible gap duration is known as the gap detection threshold (GDT) (Fitzgibbons, 1983; Florentine and Buus, 1984; Florentine et al., 1999).
GDTs and other temporal aspects of hearing may deteriorate with age and with cochlear hearing loss; however, some methodological factors complicate this simple interpretation (for review, see Reed et al., 2009). Regardless, gap detection remains a popular measurement of temporal resolution in hearing research. GDTs for a single listener measured using PsyAcoustX are shown in Figure 10. The 500-ms marker was a broadband noise (100–8000 Hz) presented at several SPLs, with the temporal gap positioned centrally within the marker. Data from Florentine and Buus (1984) are also provided for comparison. Consistent with their study, GDTs improved rapidly with increasing marker level and then remained relatively constant.
Amplitude Modulation Detection
Gap detection thresholds provide only a single estimate of a listener’s auditory temporal resolution (Fitzgibbons, 1983; Florentine and Buus, 1984; Florentine et al., 1999). Moreover, due to the short duration stimuli used in typical GDT paradigms, stimuli may be confounded by additional cues of spectral splatter. This can improve perceptual detection and thus, can overestimate a listener’s temporal resolution thresholds (Moore, 2003).
To circumvent acoustic issues of GDT paradigms and provide a more complete, functional description of auditory temporal resolution, some psychoacousticians measure the temporal modulation transfer function (TMTF; Viemeister, 1979; Strickland and Viemeister, 1996; Dau et al., 1997). TMTFs measure the ability to follow or resolve amplitude fluctuations in an ongoing carrier. Often, a carrier signal (e.g., sinusoidal tone, fc) is modulated by another sinusoid (fm). Wideband noise carriers are often used to prevent altering the long-term power spectrum of the modulated stimulus and to prevent listeners from detecting audible sidebands resulting from the fm (Burns and Viemeister, 1981). The modulation depth needed for a listener to just detect amplitude fluctuations is then recorded as a function of the modulation frequency. TMTFs typically resemble a low-pass filter with cutoff frequency of ∼100 Hz; listeners are more sensitive at detecting amplitude modulations at low compared to high modulation frequencies (Viemeister, 1979).
A representative TMTF recorded in PsyAcoustX is shown in Figure 11. This TMTF was measured from a normal hearing listener (first author) in response to gated 500-ms sinusoidally amplitude modulated (SAM) noise (high-pass filtered at 80 Hz) presented at 60 dB SPL. PsyAcoustX was used to track the modulation depth at threshold (in dB) for various fms ranging from 4 to 1000 Hz (e.g., Viemeister, 1979; their Figure 6). Thresholds measured for TMTFs represent the smallest amplitude modulation that the listener can reliably detect at each modulation frequency.
Conclusion
The current report presents PsyAcoustX, a new open-source, MATLAB® -based software suite for psychoacoustics research. PsyAcoustX is advantageous compared to other platforms as it does not require dedicated hardware or programming knowledge. In addition, stimuli are generated adaptively within the program according to the participant’s response. These features make it possible for users to execute highly precise psychophysical paradigms entirely via a GUI interface and measure behavioral thresholds adaptively. Current applications available through PsyAcoustX’s engine were presented including measurement of audiometric thresholds, simultaneous and non-simultaneous masking paradigms, PTCs, temporal overshoot, gap detection, and amplitude modulation detection. While the current version of the software is geared toward psychoacoustics research and masking and temporal resolution paradigms under monaural listening, users can easily extend the base package to accommodate any number of conceivable psychoacoustic paradigms (e.g., pitch discrimination). Future iterations of the program could also extend the GUI to include binaural auditory tasks (e.g., spatial masking paradigms, and interaural time/intensity discrimination).
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Development of PsyAcoustX and preparation of this work was supported by NIH–NIDCD R01-DC008327 (ES). PsyAcoustX is freely available from the authors or via Supplementary Material associated with this article. Subsequent development and newer versions of PsyAcoustX are available on the program’s website: https://sites.google.com/site/psyacoustx/. Windows® and Excel® are registered trademarks of the Microsoft Corporation; MATLAB® is a registered trademark of The MathWorks, Inc.; Presentation® is a registered trademark of Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.; E-prime® is a registered trademark of Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
Footnotes
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01498
References
- Abbas P. J., Sachs M. B. (1976). Two-tone suppression in auditory-nerve fibers: extension of a stimulus-response relationship. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 59 112–122. 10.1121/1.380841 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bacon S. P. (1990). Effect of masker level on overshoot. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 88 698–702. 10.1121/1.399773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bacon S. P., Healy E. W. (2000). Effects of ipsilateral and contralateral precursors on the temporal effect in simultaneous masking with pure tones. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 1589–1597. 10.1121/1.428443 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bidelman G. M., Schug J. M., Jennings S. G., Bhagat S. P. (2014). Psychophysical auditory filter estimates reveal sharper cochlear tuning in musicians. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 136 EL33–EL39. 10.1121/1.4885484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bidelman G. M., Syed Khaja A. (2014). Spectrotemporal resolution tradeoff in auditory processing as revealed by human auditory brainstem responses and psychophysical indices. Neurosci. Lett. 572 53–57. 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.04.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10 433–436. 10.1163/156856897X00357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burns E. M., Viemeister N. F. (1981). Played–again SAM: further observations on the pitch of amplitude-modulated noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 70 1655–1660. 10.1121/1.387220 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ciba S., Wlodarski A., Maempel H.-J. (2009). WhisPER – A new tool for performing listening tests. Paper Presented at the 126th AES Convention Munich. Paper 7449 Munich. [Google Scholar]
- Dau T., Kollmeier B., Kohlrausch A. (1997). Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation. I. Detection and masking with narrow-band carriers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 2892–2905. 10.1121/1.420344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duifhuis H. (1980). Level effects in psychophysical two-tone suppression. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 67 914–927. 10.1121/1.383971 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egan J. P., Hake H. W. (1950). On the masking pattern of a simple auditory stimulus. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1950 622–630. 10.1121/1.1906661 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott L. L. (1971). Backward and forward masking. Audiology 10 65–76. 10.3109/00206097109072544 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgibbons P. J. (1983). Temporal gap detection in noise as a function of frequency, bandwidth, and level. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 74 67–72. 10.1121/1.389619 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher H. (1940). Auditory patterns. Rev. Mod. Phys. 12 47–61. 10.1103/RevModPhys.12.47 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Florentine M., Buus S. (1984). Temporal gap detection in sensorineural and simulated hearing impairments. J. Speech Hear. Res. 27 449–455. 10.1044/jshr.2703.449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Florentine M., Buus S., Geng W. (1999). Psychometric functions for gap detection in a yes–no procedure. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106 3512–3520. 10.1121/1.428204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasberg B. R., Moore B. C. (1990). Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data. Hear. Res. 47 103–138. 10.1016/0378-5955(90)90170-T [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grassi M., Soranzo A. (2009). MLP: a MATLAB toolbox for rapid and reliable auditory threshold estimations. Behav. Res. Methods 41 20–28. 10.3758/BRM.41.1.20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green D. M., Swets J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Harris D. M., Dallos P. (1979). Forward masking of auditory nerve fiber responses. J. Neurophysiol. 42 1083–1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jennings S. G., Strickland E. A. (2012). Auditory filter tuning inferred with short sinusoidal and notched-noise maskers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134 2497–2513. 10.1121/1.4746029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jennings S. G., Strickland E. A., Heinz M. G. (2009). Precursor effects on behavioral estimates of frequency selectivity and gain in forward masking. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125 2172–2181. 10.1121/1.3081383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killion M. C. (1978). Revised estimates of minimum audible pressure: where is the “missing 6 dB”? J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63 1501–1508. 10.1121/1.381844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwon B. J. (2011). AUX: a scripting language for auditory signal processing and software packages for psychoacoustic experiments and education. Behav. Res. Methods 44 361–373. 10.3758/s13428-011-0161-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 49 467–477. 10.1121/1.1912375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macmillan N. A., Creelman C. D. (2005). Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Moore B. C. J. (1978). Psychophysical tuning curves measured in simultaneous and forward masking. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63 524–532. 10.1121/1.381752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore B. C. J. (2003). Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson D. A., Schroder A. C., Wojtczak M. (2001). A new procedure for measuring peripheral compression in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110 2045–2064. 10.1121/1.1404439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Loughlin B. J., Moore B. C. J. (1981). Off-frequency listening: effects on psychoacoustical tuning curves obtained in simultaneous and forward masking. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 69 1119–1125. 10.1121/1.385691 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oxenham A. J., Plack C. J. (1997). A behavioral measure of basilar-membrane nonlinearity in listeners with normal and impaired hearing. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 101 3666–3675. 10.1121/1.418327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oxenham A. J., Shera C. A. (2003). Estimates of human cochlear tuning at low levels using forward and simultaneous masking. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 4 541–554. 10.1007/s10162-002-3058-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson R. D. (1976). Auditory filter shapes derived with noise stimuli. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 59 640–654. 10.1121/1.380914 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson R. D., Moore B. C. J. (1986). “Auditory filters and excitation patterns as representations of frequency resolution,” in Frequency Selectivity in Hearing ed. Moore B. C. J. (London: Academic; ) 123–127. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson R. D., Nimmo-Smith I. (1980). Off-frequency listening and auditory-filter asymmetry. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 67 229–245. 10.1121/1.383732 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peirce J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. J. Neurosci. Methods 162 8–13. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed C. M., Braida L. D., Zurek P. M. (2009). Review of the literature on temporal resolution in listeners with cochlear hearing impairment: a critical assessment of the role of suprathreshold deficits. Trends Amplif. 13 4–43. 10.1177/1084713808325412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roverud E., Strickland E. A. (2014). Accounting for nonmonotonic precursor duration effects with gain reduction in the temporal window model. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 135 1321–1334. 10.1121/1.4864783 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roverud E., Strickland E. A. (2015). Exploring the source of the mid-level hump for intensity discrimination in quiet and the effects of noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 137 1318–1335. 10.1121/1.4908243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sek A., Alcantara J., Moore B. C. J., Kluk K., Wicher A. (2005). Development of a fast method for determining psychophysical tuning curves. Int. J. Audiol. 44 408–420. 10.1080/14992020500060800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shera C. A., Guinan J. J., Jr., Oxenham A. J. (2002). Revised estimates of human cochlear tuning from otoacoustic and behavioral measurements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 3318–3323. 10.1073/pnas.032675099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strait D. L., Kraus N., Parbery-Clark A., Ashley R. (2010). Musical experience shapes top-down auditory mechanisms: evidence from masking and auditory attention performance. Hear. Res. 261 22–29. 10.1016/j.heares.2009.12.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strickland E. A. (2001). The relationship between frequency selectivity and overshoot. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109 2062–2073. 10.1121/1.1357811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strickland E. A. (2004). The relationship between precursor level and the temporal effect. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123 946–954. 10.1121/1.2821977 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strickland E. A., Viemeister N. F. (1996). Cues for discrimination of envelopes. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99 3638–3646. 10.1121/1.414962 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unoki M., Irino T., Glasberg B., Moore B. C., Patterson R. D. (2006). Comparison of the roex and gammachirp filters as representations of the auditory filter. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120 1474–1492. 10.1121/1.2228539 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viemeister N. F. (1979). Temporal modulation transfer functions based upon modulation thresholds. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66 1364–1380. 10.1121/1.383531 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright B. A. (1998). “Specific language impairment: abnormal auditory masking and the potential for its remediation through training,” in Psychological and Physiological Advances in Hearing eds Palmer A. R., Reese A., Summerfield A. Q., Mediss R. (London: Whurr Publishing; ). [Google Scholar]
- Zwicker E. (1965). Temporal effects in simultaneous masking by white-noise bursts. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 37 653–663. 10.1121/1.1909389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.