Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addiction. 2015 Aug 22;110(11):1844–1852. doi: 10.1111/add.13079

Personality and smoking: Individual-Participant Meta-Analysis of 9 cohort studies

Christian Hakulinen 1, Mirka Hintsanen 2, Marcus R Munafò 3, Marianna Virtanen 4, Mika Kivimäki 5, G David Batty 6, Markus Jokela 7
PMCID: PMC4609271  NIHMSID: NIHMS711884  PMID: 26227786

Abstract

Aims

To investigate cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between personality and smoking, and test whether sociodemographic factors modify these associations.

Design

Cross-sectional and longitudinal individual-participant meta-analysis.

Setting

Nine cohort studies from Australia, Germany, UK and US.

Participants

A total of 79,757 men and women (mean age = 51 years).

Measurements

Personality traits of the Five-Factor Model (extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness and openness to experience) were used as exposures. Outcomes were current smoking status (current smoker, ex-smoker, and never smoker), smoking initiation, smoking relapse, and smoking cessation. Associations between personality and smoking were modeled using logistic and multinomial logistic regression, and study-specific findings were combined using random-effect meta-analysis.

Findings

Current smoking was associated with higher extraversion (odds ratio per 1 standard deviation increase in the score: 1.16; 95% confidence interval: 1.08–1.24), higher neuroticism (1.19; 1.13–1.26), and lower conscientiousness (0.88; 0.83–0.94). Among nonsmokers, smoking initiation during the follow-up period was prospectively predicted by higher extraversion (1.22; 1.04–1.43) and lower conscientiousness (0.80; 0.68–0.93), whereas higher neuroticism (1.16; 1.04–1.30) predicted smoking relapse among ex-smokers. Among smokers, smoking cessation was negatively associated with neuroticism (0.91; 0.87–0.96). Sociodemographic variables did not appear to modify the associations between personality and smoking.

Conclusions

Adult smokers have higher extraversion, higher neuroticism and lower conscientiousness personality scores than non-smokers. Initiation into smoking is positively associated with higher extraversion and lower conscientiousness, while relapse to smoking among ex-smokers is association with higher neuroticism.


Despite the known harmful effects of smoking on health (1), around 31% of men and 11% of women worldwide continue to smoke tobacco regularly (2). In the United States, 67 % of regular smokers have considered quitting smoking, and 52 % had attempted to do so during the past year (3). While there are many effective smoking cessation programs, such as behavioral support and pharmacological treatments (4), people's attempts to quit smoking tend not to be successful over the long term (3, 5).

Several psychological and social risk factors for smoking have been identified, including parental socioeconomic status, parental smoking, and peer smoking (6). Previous research has also reported differences in personality characteristics – which refers to individual differences in feelings, thoughts, and actions (7) – between smokers and non-smokers (8, 9). A meta-analysis of 25 published cross-sectional studies of extraversion and neuroticism (34,738 non-smokers and 12,764 smokers) reported that smokers had higher neuroticism and higher extraversion than non-smokers (10). Another meta-analysis of published cross-sectional studies on health correlates of conscientiousness (n=46,725) reported that smoking was more common among individuals with low compared with high conscientiousness (11). In addition, a cross-sectional association between low agreeableness and current smoking was reported in a meta-analysis that was based on nine published studies (n = 4,730) (12). In sum, current smokers are characterized by high neuroticism, high extraversion, low agreeableness, and low conscientiousness.

The role of personality in future smoking behaviors has also been examined. In prospective studies, high neuroticism has been shown to be associated with smoking initiation in some (1315), but not in all studies (16). Low conscientiousness has been shown to be associated with smoking initiation (13, 16), while the evidence of the association between high openness to experience and smoking initiation is mixed with both positive and null findings (13, 16). Results from two small-scale smoking cessation programs suggests that low neuroticism and low openness to experience may be associated with higher odds of smoking cessation (17, 18), and that higher conscientiousness might predict abstinence from smoking (17).

In sum, the majority of studies on smoking and personality have been cross-sectional and have focused only on some of the personality traits of the five-factor model instead of examining them all together. Even fewer longitudinal studies have assessed all the major dimensions of personality in relation to different smoking behaviors, including smoking initiation, relapse, and cessation. These studies have been carried out with relatively small samples. Thus, large-scale studies are needed to establish robustness of the associations between personality and smoking behaviors. Furthermore, it remains unclear whether sociodemographic factors might modify the association between personality and smoking behavior. For example, while some studies suggest that the association between personality and smoking is stronger among women than men (16), other studies report no gender differences (13, 14). The association between conscientiousness and smoking has been reported to be weaker among older compared with younger age groups (11), but this finding has not been replicated. Education has been linked to smoking behaviors (19), and it has been suggested that there is an interaction between education and personality on smoking (20). In addition, the association of psychological distress (a concept closely related to high neuroticism) with smoking has been suggested to differ between ethnic groups (21). However, further research is needed to clarify whether these sociodemographic characteristics are of importance in the relation between personality and smoking. Most previous studies have also not examined subgroup differences with regard to other smoking behaviors besides current smoking.

The aim of the present study was to examine associations between personality traits of the five-factor model (extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience) and smoking behavior in cross-sectional and longitudinal settings. More specifically, we examined whether personality traits predict smoking initiation, smoking relapse, and smoking cessation, and whether sociodemographic factors modify these associations. To achieve all this, we pooled data from nine large cohort studies for an individual-participant meta-analysis of 79,757 participants. Individual-participant meta-analysis is seen as the gold standard approach to evidence synthesis and it is an effective way to reduce the potential problem of publication bias (22), from which the previous meta-analyses based on publish studies might suffer. Based on previous research we hypothesized that higher extraversion and neuroticism, and lower agreeableness and conscientiousness, would be related to higher probability of smoking and smoking initiation, smoking relapse, and with lower probability of smoking cessation.

Methods and Materials

Data were selected by searching the data collections of the Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR; http://www.icpsr.umich.edu/icpsrweb/ICPSR/) and the Economic and Social Data Service (http://ukdataservice.ac.uk/) to identify eligible large-scale cohort studies that have measurements of personality and smoking. To be eligible for inclusion, studies needed to be open access datasets, have a sufficiently large sample size (n>1000), had to include information on participant's smoking status, and personality assessed with at least the brief 15-item questionnaire or with more comprehensive questionnaires based on the Five-Factor Model of personality.

The following cohort studies met the inclusion criteria: the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health), the British Household Panel Survey (BHPS), the German Socio-Economic Panel Study (GSOEP), the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey, the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS), the National Child Development Study (NCDS) the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study graduate (WLSG) sample, and the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study sibling (WLSS) sample. All these studies are well-characterized longitudinal cohort studies with large sample sizes. However, Add Health and NCDS did not have follow-up data on smoking after the assessment of personality, and thus these cohort studies were included only in cross-sectional analyses. All the cohort studies have been approved by the relevant local ethics committees. Full details of the cohort studies and used measures are provided in the Online Supplementary Appendix.

Measures

The Five-Factor Model personality traits were assessed with standardized questionnaire instruments. These instruments measure the following five higher-order personality traits that sum up individual variation in several, more precise, personality dispositions: extraversion (e.g., sociability and sensitivity to positive emotions), neuroticism (e.g., low emotional stability and proneness to anxiety), agreeableness (e.g., cooperativeness and trust toward other people), conscientiousness (e.g., self-control and allegiance to social norms), and openness to experience (e.g., curiosity and open-mindedness) (23).

Current smoking at baseline was measured with different questions across cohort studies that were categorized as follows: 0 = never-smoker; 1=ex-smoker; 2 = current smoker. At the follow-up, the same procedure was followed, except that current smoking was categorized as follows: 0 = non-smoker; 1 = current smoker. Sociodemographics were harmonized across cohort studies as follows: marital status (0=single, 1=married/cohabiting), race/ethnicity (0=white, non-Hispanic; 1=other), and educational level (0=primary education, 1=secondary education, 2=tertiary education).

Statistical analysis

Cross-sectional associations between personality traits and current smoking in the total sample and within different subgroups were examined using multinomial logistic regression (0 = never-smoker; 1=ex-smoker; 2 = current smoker), where never-smokers were used as a comparison group. Odds ratios (ORs) were calculated for personality z-scores (Standard Deviation [SD] = 1). Longitudinal associations between personality traits and smoking were analyzed in three separate analyses. First, the association between personality traits and smoking initiation among never-smokers was examined. Second, the association between personality traits smoking relapse among ex-smokers was investigated. Third, the association between personality traits and smoking cessation among baseline smokers was examined. All models were adjusted for sex, age at baseline, and ethnicity/nationality. Longitudinal analyses were further adjusted for follow-up period in months.

To examine whether the association between personality traits and smoking behaviors differed between sociodemographic groups, we carried out stratified analyses by sex (men vs women), age groups (under 40 years, between 40 to 65 years, or over 65 years), marital status (single vs married/cohabiting), race/ethnicity (white vs other), and educational level (primary, secondary, or tertiary education). The study-specific results were then pooled together by subgroup using meta-analysis and then heterogeneity across subgroups was examined using the I2 statistic. In addition to these subgroup analyses, longitudinal analyses were conducted separately according to the length of follow-up (i.e., short (4 years or less on average) vs long (4 years or more on average)) to examine whether the follow-up time would moderate the association between personality and smoking behaviors.

Meta-analysis was performed using the two-step approach, all models were first fitted separately within each cohort studies and the results from the individual cohort studies were then pooled together by using random-effects meta-analysis. Heterogeneity in the effect sizes was examined using the I2 estimates. Additional sensitivity analyses were performed to examine whether covariates and their interactions with personality traits explained heterogeneity. Sensitivity analyses were done by first pooling all data together, and then using one-step individual-participant meta-analysis (i.e., logistic multilevel mixed-effects regression analysis). Meta-analysis was performed with the metan package of Stata, version 13.1, software (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas) and the sensitivity analyses were performed using R package lme4 (24).

Results

The total sample included 79,757 participants (age range 15–104, mean age 50.8 years) and 52,684 participants were included in the longitudinal analysis (follow-up mean: 5.2 years; follow-up range: from 15 months to 157 months). Characteristics of the samples are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive characteristics of the 9 cohort studies

Add Health BHPS GSOEP HILDA HRS MIDUS NCDS WLSG WLSS
Participants
  Cross-sectional 5010 10456 14075 10980 13822 6259 8674 6566 3905
  Longitudinal - 8979 10052 8379 12590 4634 - 5230 2820
Follow-up time (months) - 36.1 (1.2) 48.1 (1.6) 48.0 (1.3) 35.3 (16.0) 107.5 (6.3) - 134.0 (4.2) 135.8 (6.7)
Age 29.0 (1.8) 47.3 (17.7) 50.4 (16.1) 43.7 (17.9) 67.2 (10.4) 46.8 (12.9) 50.3 (0.5) 54.1 (0.5) 53.1 (7.3)
Sex
  Men 45.9 (2,299) 45.8 (5,695) 47.1 (6,628) 46.8 (5,135) 40.7 (5,635) 47.5 (2,971) 48.2 (4,181) 46.4 (3,044) 46.7 (1,824)
  Women 54.1 (2,711) 54.2 (5,666) 52.9 (7,447) 53.2 (5,845) 59.3 (8,197) 52.5 (3,288) 51.8 (4,493) 53.6 (3,522) 53.3 (2,081)
Education
  Primary 33.4 (1,675) 28.5 (2,983) 15.8 (2,190) 34.4 (3,780) 18.5 (2,563) 8.7 (545) 18.3 (1,591) - 5.2 (204)
  Secondary 53.6 (2,683) 57.1 (5,963) 63.9 (8,854) 44.4 (4,874) 55.2 (7,633) 59.3 (3,704) 61.6 (5,340) 72.0 (4,730) 64.1 (2,504)
  Tertiary 13.0 (652) 14.3 (1,505) 20.3 (2,808) 21.2 (2,326) 26.2 (3,622) 32.0 (1,997) 20.1 (1,743) 28.0 (1,836) 30.7 (1,197)
Nationality / ethnicity
  Majority 72.0 (3,608) 92.7 (9,692) 92.9 (13,076) 79.2 (8,701) 78.0 (10,791) 88.9 (5,567) 98.0 (8,498) 100.0 (6,566) 100.0 (3,905)
  Minority 28.0 (1,402) 7.3 (764) 7.1 (999) 20.8 (2,279) 22.0 (3,041) 11.1 (692) 2.0 (176) - -
Marital status
  Married/cohabiting 41.5 (2,081) 54.5 (5,695) 65.5 (9,216) 59.3 (6,514) 63.9 (8,839) 67.7 (4,234) 69.9 (6,064) 83.2 (5,462) 80.9 (3,135)
  Single 58.5 (2,929) 45.5 (4,759) 34.5 (4,859) 40.7 (4,466) 36.1 (4,990) 32.3 (2,022) 30.1 (2,610) 16.8 (1,103) 19.1 (738)
Baseline smoking
  Never-smoker 54.6 (2,737) 44.6 (4,664) 48.1 (6,767) 50.4 (5,529) 43.2 (5,972) 48.7 (3,051) 47.0 (4,079) 45.5 (2,990) 44.3 (1,729)
  Ex-smoker 12.3 (615) 22.9 (2,397) 25.0 (3,512) 28.7 (3,152) 43.6 (6,034) 29.5 (1,844) 31.2 (2,708) 36.7 (2,412) 38.6 (1,506)
  Smoker 33.1 (1,658) 32.5 (3,395) 27.0 (3,796) 20.9 (2,299) 13.2 (1,826) 21.8 (1,364) 21.8 (1,887) 17.7 (1,164) 17.2 (670)
Follow-up smoking
  Non-smoker 74.2 (6,661) 75.2 (7,559) 50.9 (4,268) 43.5 (5,478) 51.2 (2,374) - 74.9 (3,917) 74.4 (2,099)
  Ex-smoker - - - 31.9 (2,675) 45.3 (5,707) 33.6 (1,556) - 18.5 (968) 18.3 (517)
  Smoker - 25.8 (2,318) 24.8 (2,493) 17.1 (1,436) 11.2 (1,405) 15.2 (704) - 6.6 (345) 7.2 (204)

Note. Because of missing data in covariates, numbers of covariate frequencies may not add up to the total number of participants with personality and baseline smoking data.

Add Health, National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health; BHPS, British Household Panel Survey; GSOEP, German Socio-Economic Panel Study; HILDA, Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia; HRS, Health and Retirement Study; MIDUS, Midlife in the United States; NCDS, National Child Development Study; WLSG, Wisconsin Longitudinal Study Graduate Sample; WLSS, Wisconsin Longitudinal Study Sibling Sample.

Current-smoking status

Cross-sectional analyses, where the association between personality and current smoking status was examined, are presented in Figure 1. Higher extraversion (pooled OR 1.16; 95% CI 1.08–1.24) and higher neuroticism (OR 1.19; 95% CI 1.13–1.26) were associated with an increased risk of smoking. These associations were, however, not consistent across studies (I2=90% for extraversion; I2=87% for neuroticism), suggesting high heterogeneity between studies (Supplement Figure 1). In addition, lower conscientiousness was associated with lower likelihood of smoking (pooled OR 0.88; 95% CI 0.83–0.94), which was also not consistent across individual studies (I2=90%).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Cross-sectional associations between personality traits and current smoking status at the baseline. Values are odds ratios per 1 standard deviation increment in personality trait. Personality traits are adjusted for each other in addition to sex, age and race/ethnicity.

Similar results were found when ex-smokers where compared with never-smokers; higher extraversion (pooled OR 1.13; 95% CI 1.08–1.17), higher neuroticism (OR 1.13; 95% CI 1.07–1.19), and lower conscientiousness (pooled OR 0.93; 95% CI 0.90–0.97) were associated with an increased likelihood of being an ex-smoker. In addition, lower agreeableness (pooled OR 0.90; 95% CI 0.85–0.94) and higher openness to experience (OR 1.07; 95% CI 1.04–1.12) were also associated with an increased likelihood of being an ex-smoker. However, I2 values suggested that there was high heterogeneity in the associations across studies (I2 values between 72% and 90%; for study specific associations see Supplement Figure 2). Although individual studies suggested some statistically significant cross-sectional associations for agreeableness and openness to experience, the meta-analysis suggested no pooled associations for these two traits.

Smoking initiation, relapse, and cessation

Figure 2 presents the associations of the personality traits with (1) smoking initiation among baseline non-smokers, (2) smoking relapse among baseline ex-smokers, and (3) smoking cessation among baseline smokers. Higher extraversion (pooled OR 1.22; 95% CI 1.04–1.43) and lower conscientiousness (pooled OR 0.80; 95% CI 0.68–0.93) were consistently associated with higher odds of smoking initiation (Supplement Figure 3). Higher neuroticism (pooled OR 1.16; 95% CI 1.04–1.30) was associated with higher odds of smoking relapse among ex-smokers (Supplement Figure 4). Higher neuroticism was consistently associated with lower odds of smoking cessation among those who smoked at baseline (pooled OR 0.91; 95% CI 0.87–0.96) (Supplement Figure 5).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Longitudinal associations between personality traits and smoking initiation, smoking relapse among non-smokers at the baseline, and smoking cessation among smokers at the baseline. Values are odds ratios per 1 standard deviation increment in personality trait. Personality traits are adjusted for each other in addition to sex, age, race/ethnicity, and follow-up time.

Sub-group and sensitivity analyses

Cross-sectional sub-group analyses between personality traits and current smoking status are presented in Supplemental Table 1 and 2. No significant sources of heterogeneity, which would explain the large heterogeneity found in the main analysis, were found in sub-group analyses. However, the associations between extraversion and neuroticism with smoking at baseline did not remain statistically significant among participants older than 65 years (OR 1.02; 95% CI 0.87–1.17; OR 1.07; 95% CI 0.93–1.21; respectively).

Longitudinal sub-group analyses between personality traits and smoking initiation, smoking relapse, and smoking cessation are presented in Supplemental Tables 3–5, respectively. The earlier sub-group findings between personality traits and smoking at baseline were not replicated in the longitudinal sub-group analyses. However, high extraversion predicted smoking relapse only among studies with long follow-up (OR 1.20; 95% CI 1.01–1.42), whereas high agreeableness predicted smoking relapse among studies with short-follow-up (OR 1.17; 95% CI 1.05–1.31).

Additional sensitivity analyses suggested that results from the two-step and one-step individual participant meta-analysis were similar (Supplemental Table 6). Observed heterogeneity between studies in the association between neuroticism and relapse was reduced 94 % when moderators and interactions between personality traits and moderators were included in the one-step multilevel logistic regression model.

Discussion

In an individual-participant meta-analysis of nine cohort studies higher neuroticism, higher extraversion, and lower conscientiousness were associated with increased probability of smoking. However, whereas higher extraversion and lower conscientiousness were associated with smoking initiation, only high neuroticism was associated with smoking relapse, indicating that personality is differently associated with smoking initiation and relapse. Among those smoking at baseline, smoking cessation was predicted by lower neuroticism but not by extraversion or conscientiousness.

Many of the present results are in agreement with previously published data. In a meta-analysis of 25 published cross-sectional studies (total n > 47,000) investigating extraversion and neuroticism, smoking was associated with higher neuroticism and higher extraversion (10) The effect size for extraversion was larger in the previously published meta-analysis (OR 1.41; 95% CI 1.29–1.57; transformed from Cohen's d=0.19; 95% CI 0.14 to 0.25) compared with our current study (pooled OR 1.16; 95% CI 1.08–1.24). Similarly, the effect size for neuroticism was slightly larger (OR 1.24; 95% CI 1.08–1.44; transformed from Cohen's d=0.12; 95% CI 0.04 to 0.20) than that observed in our present study (pooled OR 1.19; 95% CI 1.13–1.26). In another meta-analysis of published studies examining health correlates of conscientiousness, (n = 47,000), higher conscientiousness was associated with lower likelihood of smoking (11). Again, the effect size was considerably larger in this meta-analysis (OR 0.60; 95% CI 0.58–0.62; transformed from a correlation based effect size r=−0.14; 95% CI −0.13 to −0.15) compared with our current study (pooled OR 0.88; 95% CI 0.83–0.94). However, whereas a previous meta-analysis with 4,730 participants found an association between low agreeableness and current smoking, this association was not found in the current study (12). In addition, contrary to prior longitudinal evidence (1316), neuroticism and openness to experience were not associated with smoking initiation.

The effect sizes tended to be lower in our analyses than in the two previous meta-analyses (10, 11). For example, the effect estimate for the association between conscientiousness and smoking was 32% lower in our study compared with the earlier meta-analysis based on published studies (11). Several reasons might explain why our results differed in terms of magnitude from those in previous meta-analyses (10, 11). First, meta-analyses based on published data can be affected by publication bias, which is caused by selective publishing of positive findings, and can artificially inflate effect estimates (25). In the current study, data were obtained from two public databases and the analyses were preplanned, thus the final results were not influenced by the results from individual cohort studies. This procedure is likely to reduce the problem of selectively publishing significant findings only. Indeed, similar differences between published and unpublished studies have been also found in previous IPD meta-analyses of psychosocial factors and health (26), including the association between personality and all-cause mortality (27).

Our analyses indicated that there was heterogeneity in the results between the cohort studies. Some heterogeneity can be naturally expected as included cohort studies were from different countries and used different sampling methods. However, our sensitivity analyses suggested that observed between-study heterogeneity in neuroticism-smoking relapse association was substantially reduced when moderators and interactions between personality traits and moderators were included in the sensitivity analyses. Thus, it is likely that the subgroup differences are of importance in individual cohort studies, but they are not so consistent that they would be seen at the meta-analytic level. However, it is also likely there are, for example, some socio-cultural and biological factors, which we were not able to measure, that could explain the observed heterogeneity across studies. Further research is needed to identify these factors.

Different psychological processes may underlie smoking initiation, smoking relapse, and smoking cessation (2830). Our findings show that personality is also differentially associated with some of these smoking behaviors; higher extraversion and lower conscientiousness were associated with smoking initiation, whereas lower neuroticism was associated with smoking cessation and higher neuroticism was associated with smoking relapse. These findings are plausible. High extraversion is related to sensation seeking and sociability, and as smoking is often a social activity, individuals with higher extraversion might start smoking and smoke more just because they are more social. High neuroticism, in turn, reflects low emotional stability and high proneness to anxiety and stress. Given that smoking may represent a strategy to relieve stress (31), the stress-proneness and higher levels of negative emotions among neurotic individuals may explain their higher odds of smoking relapse. We also found that high neuroticism was associated with a lower likelihood of smoking cessation. This may also be related to their stress-proneness. Furthermore, smoking cessation introduces withdrawal symptoms, and these symptoms may be experienced more strongly by individuals with high neuroticism.

Previous individual-participant meta-analyses have identified conscientiousness as the central health related personality trait. Low conscientiousness has been found to predict obesity (32), diabetes (33), cardiovascular disease and stroke (34) and all-cause mortality (27), and many unfavorable health behaviors (11, 35). Cancer appears to be one of the few health outcomes that is not predicted by low conscientiousness—or by any other personality trait (36). High conscientiousness reflects good self-control and capacity for long-term planning, so the lower smoking behavior associated with conscientiousness is likely to reflect the greater adherence to healthy lifestyle and public health recommendations.

There have been repeated calls to include personality information in health behavior interventions (37, 38). Our results suggest that although the magnitude of the personality-smoking relationship might be smaller than previously reported, personality is clearly associated with smoking behavior. In particular, increased attention and support to individuals high on the personality dimension neuroticism could improve the outcome of smoking cessation interventions. A recent study suggests that interventions targeted to adolescents who display high anxiety sensitivity and hopelessness (i.e., high neuroticism) may be effective in preventing and reducing problematic drinking (39). Our findings imply that this could also be the case in interventions promoting smoking cessation. In addition, as neuroticism is related to depressive symptoms (40, 41) and depression is highly co-morbid with smoking (42), personality-informed interventions to reduce smoking could also support those with depressive symptoms. Further research should also investigate whether the success in current smoking cessations programs vary depending on individuals personality dispositions.

Some methodological limitations need to be acknowledged. Smoking status was self-reported, which might lead to the underestimation of smoking prevalence (43). It is possible, for example, that individuals classified as ex-smokers were smokers relatively long-time ago, or that current smokers respond as being ex-smokers due to social desirability. The study cohort included mainly middle-aged Caucasian participants and thus results might not be generalizable to other ethnical groups. Current study also contained relatively few initiators, which might bias the results. Although, the Five-Factor model is one of the most used conceptualization of personality structure, and it has considerable empirical support (7), it has also been criticized; its structure and usefulness at the individual level has been questioned (44) and it has also been seen too broad to capture all the possible variation in personality traits (45). Personality was measured with different instruments of the five-factor traits in different cohort studies, which could have introduced heterogeneity in the associations. However, different instruments of the five-factor traits have been shown to correlate strongly with each other, suggesting that this may not have been a major source of heterogeneity in the current meta-analysis (7, 46).

In conclusion, this individual-participant meta-analysis showed that high extraversion, high neuroticism and low conscientiousness are associated with smoking behavior, although the effect sizes were lower than those reported in previous meta-analyses, which were based on published data. Smoking cessation was predicted only by low neuroticism, suggesting that behavioral, emotional and cognitive dispositions related to this personality dimension may be particularly relevant for interventions.

Supplementary Material

Supp MaterialS1

Acknowledgements

M.R.M. is a member of the United Kingdom Centre for Tobacco and Alcohol Studies, a UKCRC Public Health Research: Centre of Excellence. Funding from British Heart Foundation, Cancer Research UK, Economic and Social Research Council, Medical Research Council, and the National Institute for Health Research, under the auspices of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration, is gratefully acknowledged. M.V. is supported by the Academy of Finland (258598, 265174). M.K. is supported by the UK Medical Research Council (K013351), the Economic and Social Research Council, the Academy of Finland, and the US National Institutes of Health (R01HL036310, R01AG034454). G.D.B. is a member of the Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research Centre funded by Alzheimer Scotland, and the University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (G0700704/84698). Funding from the BBSRC, EPSRC, ESRC, and MRC is gratefully acknowledged.

Footnotes

Disclosure: The authors state no conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Lim SS, Vos T, Flaxman AD, Danaei G, Shibuya K, Adair-Rohani H, et al. A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. The lancet. 2013;380:2224–60. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61766-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ng M, Freeman MK, Fleming TD, Robinson M, Dwyer-Lindgren L, Thomson B, et al. Smoking prevalence and cigarette consumption in 187 countries, 1980–2012. JAMA. 2014;311:183–92. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.284692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Quitting smoking among adults—United States, 2001–2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2011;60:1513–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hartmann Boyce J, Stead LF, Cahill K, Lancaster T. Efficacy of interventions to combat tobacco addiction: Cochrane update of 2012 reviews. Addiction. 2013;108:1711–21. doi: 10.1111/add.12291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kotz D, West R. Explaining the social gradient in smoking cessation: it's not in the trying, but in the succeeding. Tob Control. 2009;18:43–6. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.025981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tyas SL, Pederson LL. Psychosocial factors related to adolescent smoking: a critical review of the literature. Tob Control. 1998;7:409–20. doi: 10.1136/tc.7.4.409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ. Paradigm shift to the integrative big-five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 114–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cherry N, Kiernan K. Personality scores and smoking behaviour. A longitudinal study. Br J Prev Soc Med. 1976;30:123–31. doi: 10.1136/jech.30.2.123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Terracciano A, Costa PT. Smoking and the Five Factor Model of personality. Addiction. 2004;99:472–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00687.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Munafo MR, Zetteler JI, Clark TG. Personality and smoking status: A meta-analysis. Nicotine Tobacco Res. 2007;9:405–13. doi: 10.1080/14622200701188851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:887–919. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Malouff JM, Thorsteinsson EB, Schutte NS. The five-factor model of personality and smoking: A meta-analysis. J Drug Educ. 2006;36:47–58. doi: 10.2190/9EP8-17P8-EKG7-66AD. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Turiano NA, Whiteman SD, Hampson SE, Roberts BW, Mroczek DK. Personality and substance use in midlife: Conscientiousness as a moderator and the effects of trait change. Journal of research in personality. 2012;46:295–305. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Welch D, Poulton R. Personality influences on change in smoking behavior. Health Psychology. 2009;28:292–9. doi: 10.1037/a0013471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Munafo MR, Black S. Personality and smoking status: a longitudinal analysis. Nicotine Tob Res. 2007;9:397–404. doi: 10.1080/14622200701188869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hampson SE, Goldberg LR, Vogt TM, Dubanoski JP. Forty years on: teachers' assessments of children's personality traits predict self-reported health behaviors and outcomes at midlife. Health psychology. 2006;25:57–64. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.del Río EF, López-Durán A, Rodríguez-Cano R, Martínez Ú , Martínez-Vispo C, Becoña E. Facets of the NEO-PI-R and smoking cessation. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;80:41–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hooten WM, Wolter TD, Ames SC, Hurt RD, Vickers KS, Offord KP, et al. Personality correlates related to tobacco abstinence following treatment. The International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine. 2005;35:59–74. doi: 10.2190/N9F1-1R9G-6EDW-9BFL. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hiscock R, Bauld L, Amos A, Fidler JA, Munafò M. Socioeconomic status and smoking: a review. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1248:107–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06202.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chapman B, Fiscella K, Duberstein P, Kawachi I. Education and smoking: confounding or effect modification by phenotypic personality traits? Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;38:237–48. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9142-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kiviniemi MT, Orom H, Giovino GA. Psychological distress and smoking behavior: the nature of the relation differs by race/ethnicity. Nicotine Tob Res. 2011;13:113–9. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntq218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Riley RD, Lambert PC, Abo-Zaid G. Meta-analysis of individual participant data: rationale, conduct, and reporting. BMJ: British Medical Journal. 2010:521–5. doi: 10.1136/bmj.c221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Digman JM. Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:417–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S, Christensen RHB, Singmann H, et al. Package `lme4'. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. 2014 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Rosenthal R. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol Bull. 1979;86:638–641. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kivimäki M, Nyberg ST, Batty GD, Fransson EI, Heikkilä K, Alfredsson L, et al. Job strain as a risk factor for coronary heart disease: a collaborative meta-analysis of individual participant data. The Lancet. 2012;380:1491–7. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60994-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jokela M, Batty GD, Nyberg ST, Virtanen M, Nabi H, Singh-Manoux A, et al. Personality and All-Cause Mortality: Individual-Participant Meta-Analysis of 3,947 Deaths in 76,150 Adults. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2013;178:667–75. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwt170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Leventhal H, Cleary PD. The smoking problem: a review of the research and theory in behavioral risk modification. Psychol Bull. 1980;88:370–405. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.2.370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ockene JK, Mermelstein RJ, Bonollo DS, Emmons KM, Perkins KA, Voorhees CC, et al. Relapse and maintenance issues for smoking cessation. Health Psychology. 2000;19:17–31. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.19.suppl1.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kassel JD, Stroud LR, Paronis CA. Smoking, stress, and negative affect: correlation, causation, and context across stages of smoking. Psychol Bull. 2003;129:270–304. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Floyd RL, Rimer BK, Giovino GA, Mullen PD, Sullivan SE. A review of smoking in pregnancy: effects on pregnancy outcomes and cessation efforts. Annu Rev Public Health. 1993;14:379–411. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pu.14.050193.002115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Hakulinen C, Batty G, Nabi H, Singh Manoux A, et al. Association of personality with the development and persistence of obesity: a meta analysis based on individual–participant data. Obesity Reviews. 2013;14:315–23. doi: 10.1111/obr.12007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jokela M, Elovainio M, Nyberg ST, Tabák AG, Hintsa T, Batty GD, et al. Personality and Risk of Diabetes in Adults: Pooled Analysis of 5 Cohort Studies. Health Psychology. 2014;33:1618–21. doi: 10.1037/hea0000003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Jokela M, Pulkki-Råback L, Elovainio M, Kivimäki M. Personality traits as risk factors for stroke and coronary heart disease mortality: pooled analysis of three cohort studies. J Behav Med. 2014;37:881–9. doi: 10.1007/s10865-013-9548-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hakulinen C, Elovainio M, Batty GD, Virtanen M, Kivimäki M, Jokela M. Personality and Alcohol Consumption: Pooled Analysis of 72,949 Adults from Eight Cohort Studies. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;151:110–4. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Jokela M, Batty GD, Hintsa T, Elovainio M, Hakulinen C, Kivimäki M. Is personality associated with cancer incidence and mortality? An individual-participant meta-analysis of 2156 incident cancer cases among 42 843 men and women. Br J Cancer. 2014;110:1820–4. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2014.58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. Am Psychol. 2009;64:241–256. doi: 10.1037/a0015309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chapman BP, Hampson S, Clarkin J. Personality-informed interventions for healthy aging: Conclusions from a National Institute on Aging work group. Developmental psychology. 2014;50:1426–41. doi: 10.1037/a0034135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Conrod PJ, Castellanos-Ryan N, Mackie C. Long-term effects of a personality-targeted intervention to reduce alcohol use in adolescents. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011;79:296–306. doi: 10.1037/a0022997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010;136:768–821. doi: 10.1037/a0020327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hakulinen C, Elovainio M, Pulkki-Råback L, Virtanen M, Kivimäki M, Jokela M. PERSONALITY AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS: INDIVIDUAL PARTICIPANT META-ANALYSIS OF 10 COHORT STUDIES. 2015 doi: 10.1002/da.22376. doi: 10.1002/da.22376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Grant BF, Hasin DS, Chou SP, Stinson FS, Dawson DA. Nicotine dependence and psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:1107–15. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Connor Gorber S, Schofield-Hurwitz S, Hardt J, Levasseur G, Tremblay M. The accuracy of self-reported smoking: a systematic review of the relationship between self-reported and cotinine-assessed smoking status. Nicotine Tob Res. 2009;11:12–24. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntn010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cervone D. Personality architecture: Within-person structures and processes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;56:423–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Block J. A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychol Bull. 1995;117:187–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jokela M, Batty GD, Nyberg ST, Virtanen M, Nabi H, Singh-Manoux A, et al. The Authors Reply. Am J Epidemiol. 2014;179:792–3. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp MaterialS1

RESOURCES