Abstract
The subject of the effects of the active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite on neuronal function has been an active subject of study for more than 40 years. Somewhat unusually, some of these investigations, from the outset have involved an interacting combination of experimental and model-based techniques. This article recounts that 40-year history, and the view of the functional significance of the active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite that has emerged. It specifically considers the emergence from these efforts of what is arguably the first single cell “community” model in neuroscience. The article also considers the implications of the development of this model for future studies of the complex properties of neuronal dendrites.
Keywords: Purkinje cells, modeling, cerebellum, cerebellar, dendrite, active conductnaces, history
Introduction
Analysis of the complex behavior of the mammalian cerebellar Purkinje cell has contributed significantly to our understanding of the role and function of active electrical properties in central nervous system dendrites. Further, as reviewed in this article, the study of the active properties of the dendrites of this neuron is unusual for neuroscience in the extent to which it has involved an interaction between “realistic” biophysically accurate computer models and laboratory-based experiments. Accordingly, in addition to considering the possible functional significance of the active dendritic properties of the mammalian Purkinje cell, this article also recounts in some detail the evolution of the models on which that analysis is based. Ideally, this history should serve as a model for the analysis of all aspects of the functional organization of nervous systems.
It turns out that the co-dependence between modeling and experimental studies of Purkinje cells was established at the earliest stages of study of this neuron’s complex electrical behavior. This early interaction between models and experiments was induced by a claim made by Llinas et al. (1968) based on experimental results, that Purkinje cell dendrites were electrically active. That claim, based on experimentally obtained time delays in shock induced field potentials recorded at different depths of the alligator cerebellum, was immediately challenged by Calvin and Hellerstein (1969) who, citing Rall’s (1964) pioneering cable modeling results, suggested that such delays were likely a simple consequence of passive dendritic current conduction alone. In defending their interpretation, Llinas and colleagues asserted in return that models based on volume conductors rather than cable models were a more appropriate basis for the analysis of extracellular field potentials. A few months later, Zucker (1969) entered the debate by actually performing calculations comparing both types of models, concluding that neither approach, in its classical form, could resolve the issue. However, Zucker pointed out that similarities in simulated field potential results recently obtained from the more active cable theory models for mitral cells developed by Rall and Shepherd (1968) likely supported Llinas’ original interpretation. In response, Calvin suggested that Zucker’s model had too many free parameters, and defended his own argument as based on “the simplest possible model consistent with our objective (to demonstrate that a) commonplace explanation for conduction velocities was as good as the more esoteric” (Calvin, 1969, p. 637). It took 10 more years and the development of experimental brain slice procedures and the application of intracellular recording techniques for Llinas and Sugimori (1980a) to provide conclusive experimental evidence that Purkinje cell dendrites are in fact electrically active.
It is important to point out that while references to computational modeling was at the heart of this very early controversy, no effort was actually made by any of the discussants to actually build a model of the Purkinje cell dendrite (Calvin and Hellerstein, 1969). Instead, the first model of a Purkinje cell dendrite was published by Pellionisz and Szentagothai as the last of a series of early cerebellar network modeling studies (Pellionisz, 1970; Pellionisz and Szentágothai, 1973, 1974). As shown in Figure 1, in that model, the complex Purkinje cell dendrite was represented by only four branches in which synaptic influences were calculated independently, using a simple algebraic summation. On reaching threshold, each branch independently generated dendritic spikes which were then simply summed at the soma. Comparing results of network simulations using these four branch Purkinje cells to previous results with no dendritic structure these authors concluded that: “the simulation experiments are giving quite strong hints in favor of the importance of dendritic geometry” (Pellionisz and Szentágothai, 1974, p. 28).
Perhaps reflecting the influence of the original debate between Llinas and Calvin and Hallerstein in the 1960’s, Llinas and Nicholson (1976) published the first true compartmental model of the Purkinje cell dendrite to specifically test new speculations on cerebellar physiology based on field potential recordings. In this case, the experiments involved climbing fiber-evoked responses in cat cerebellar cortex. As shown in Figure 2, while their compartmental model included conductances represented with Hodgkin Huxley model parameters (Hodgkin and Huxley, 1952), the model included only three dendritic compartments whose active properties were limited to the synapses.
One year later, as shown in Figure 3, Llinas now working with Pellionisz, published the first compartmental Purkinje cell model with more a more complex dendritic tree (Pellionisz and Llinás, 1977). Using as a base a previously published compartmental model of a spinal motorneuron (Dodge and Cooley, 1973), the new Purkinje cell model consisted of 62 compartments with the soma and initial segment incorporating Hodgkin Huxley channels (Hodgkin and Huxley, 1952). With this model the authors sought, for the first time, to use the model to replicate actual experimental responses of frog Purkinje cells including: (1) the rapid “antidromic” decrement in action potential amplitude in the dendrite following somatic current injection (Llinas et al., 1969b; Freeman and Nicholson, 1975); (2) the orthodromic activation of Purkinje cells following parallel fiber stimulation (Eccles et al., 1966a); and (3) the spike burst resulting from climbing fiber synaptic input (Eccles et al., 1966b, 1967). While the authors’ state explicitly in their article that compartmental modeling is an essential technique to: “(handle) a partially or totally active dendritic tree” (Pellionisz and Llinás, 1977, pg. 37) the model they reported still included no active voltage dependent dendritic conductances.
Deducing Function from Structure
In their original justification for building the first more realistic Purkinje cell model, Llinas and Pellionisz explicitly state that: “Rigorous mathematical models of the electrical activity of central neurons (are) a powerful tool to test and interpret experimental data” (Pellionisz and Llinás, 1977, p. 37). However, the model they actually published was clearly built to demonstrate the plausibility of dendritic mechanisms the authors had previously inferred from physiological results. In reviewing the cerebellar (and generally the neuroscience) modeling literature, this type of “demonstration model” is still the most common, with most published models specifically built to demonstrate the plausibility of one prior interpretation or another. Accordingly, these models are not intent on testing or interpreting experimental data, but instead on demonstrating the plausibility of a particular idea.
As described in the rest of this article, models can, and in the case of the cerebellar Purkinje cell have, instead been used to reveal unexpected and new interpretations of experiment and function. These models however, have been built first and foremost on anatomical structure and to replicate basic physiological responses, making as few functional assumptions as possible. As also demonstrated in the following history, models of this sort are also more likely to result in the kind of model sharing by multiple investigators in multiple laboratories which in principle can lead to cooperation, accelerating progress and understanding.
The first published Purkinje cell model that explicitly set out to deduce function from structure, without assuming the function to begin with was published by Shelton (1985) using, for the first time, an actual anatomical dendritic reconstruction of a real Purkinje cell (Figure 4). While structurally realistic, this model, like the earlier Purkinje cell models, did not include active dendritic properties, an omission justified by the authors assertion that: “the part of the dendritic tree of the Purkinje cell which is thought to be essentially passive forms a very large fraction of the total membrane surface area of the cell” (Shelton, 1985, p. 111), although the author later notes that dendritic passivity is an assumption of the model, rather than a conclusion. Instead the model was used to provide a description of the expected passive electrical properties of the Purkinje cell given the morphology of its dendrite. This was accomplished by tuning the model to replicate experimentally observed differences in dendritic and somatic input conductances. It should be noted that while this model was built on an actual anatomical reconstruction of a rat Purkinje cell, for technical reasons the only physiological data available was from Guinea Pigs. Accordingly the author “stretched” the rat dendrite to better resemble a Guinea Pig Purkinje cell. In regard to the possible active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite, Shelton’s explicitly stated that his exploration of the passive properties of the dendrite should “form the substrate for extensions which would treat more complex properties” (Shelton, 1985, p. 111).
Reflecting Shelton’s original intent, the next realistic model of the cerebellar Purkinje cell was published by Rapp et al. (1994, p. 114) explicitly as “an essential step—a skeleton—for constructing biologically more realistic models of PC dendrites”. These authors, who based their model on Guinea Pig morphology, also explicitly tested Shelton’s speculated on the possible influence of active synaptic conductances on passive membrane properties by applying the first synaptic inputs to the dendrite (Rapp et al., 1992). The Rapp et al. modeling publications also, for the first time, included new experimental data obtained by the author’s specifically to parameterize the model, while also considered in some detail the application of newly developed parameter estimation methods for large compartmental models (Holmes and Rall, 1992). Rapp et al. (1994) also tested their results using different reconstructed dendritic morphologies. Harkening back to the original controversy about the appropriate form of modeling to explore dendritic function, these authors also explicitly compared compartmental modeling results to analytical cable model solutions pioneered by Rall (1964), Calvin and Hellerstein (1969), Zucker (1969) and Segev et al. (1985). In publishing their model, Rapp et al. (1994, p. 114) however, explicitly stated, once again, that it was now essential that Purkinje cell models, “incorporate a variety of non-linear voltage- and ligand-gated channels that we know exist in the Purkinje cell dendrite”.
Returning to the community model sub-theme for this article, in addition to being the first Purkinje cell model (and one of the first in neuroscience) to be based on an actual anatomically reconstructed dendrite, the Shelton model was also the first Purkinje cell model whose components were reused by other modelers (Bush and Sejnowski, 1991; Genet et al., 2010; Blum and Wang, 1990; Brown et al., 2011), in each case adding active dendritic properties to the model. However, once again, in each modeling study, the intent was to demonstrate a previous idea about the functional significance of this property.
While Shelton’s model was the first realistic Purkinje cell model, and was used by others to build new models, these versions of the Shelton models have not generated further versions. Likely this is due in part to the fact that these models were intended to demonstrate, rather than discover function, but also because the models were not written in a form easily transmitted to others. Instead, it is the original Rapp et al. (1992, 1994) Purkinje cell model (Figure 5) that lead to the model that has emerged as “among the most successful, cited, and re-used/updated in computational neuroscience” (Ascoli, 2007, p. 156). It is clear from the history that my laboratory played a critical role, first by translating the Rapp model into GENESIS, the general purpose simulator also built in my laboratory (Bower and Beeman, 1995, 2007) and second, because by adding a full set of active conductances to the model, independent of a set of underlying functional assumptions or objectives. This second feature of our modeling efforts I think is especially important, because it means that other investigators don’t have to “buy” our interpretations or assumptions about function.
After obtaining a copy of the model from Rapp and colleagues even before their final article was published (De Schutter et al., 1993; Jaeger et al., 1993), we used GENESIS to included 10 active conductances differentially distributed in the soma and dendrite, parametrized on data from a wide range of in vitro voltage clamp experiments. The initial model-based results of the consequences of active dendritic processes for the basic physiological responses of recorded Purkinje cells were published in a series of three articles published in De Schutter and Bower (1994a,b,c). The first of these articles De Schutter and Bower (1994a) explicitly extended the work of Shelton (1985) and Rapp et al. (1992, 1994) with an analysis of the electrical structure of the Purkinje cell dendrite now including active voltage dependent conductances (Figure 6). The second article De Schutter and Bower (1994b) explored dendritic responses to climbing fiber input extending the study of the model to understand the possible influence of background excitatory synaptic inputs again first explored by Rapp et al. (1992, 1994) but now also including inhibitory synapses. The third article considered for the first time the response of an active Purkinje cell dendrite to the type of synaptic activity expected to result from stimulus driven input (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c). As the first neuronal model to use concurrent supercomputers (De Schutter and Bower, 1992), these simulations involved a much more extensive test of parameter space than previously possible, demonstrating that modeled responses were quite robust to changes in its primary parameters. Importantly for the reuse of this model by others, the use of the GENESIS simulation system specifically developed for sharing realistic neurobiological models (Bower and Beeman, 1995) made the Purkinje cell model one of the first if not the first published online (De Schutter, 1994). Again, availability of the model to anyone—its construction within a modeling platform, and I believe its focus on physiological rather than functional interpretations has led this model to be one of the first, if not the first community model in neuroscience.
Emergence of a Community Purkinje Cell Model
The articles by Rapp et al. (1992, 1994) and De Schutter and Bower (1994a,b,c) have collectively been cited more than 500 times, with the first description of the active Purkinje cell model De Schutter and Bower (1994a) responsible for almost half those citations. Importantly, the model, we now refer to as the “R-DB model”, has formed the basis for considerable subsequent work from my own students both within my laboratory (Jaeger et al., 1996; Baldi et al., 1998; Sultan and Bower, 1998; Jaeger and Bower, 1999; Mocanu et al., 2000; Santamaria et al., 2002, 2007; Santamaria and Bower, 2004; Lu et al., 2005, 2009; Cornelis et al., 2010) and within their own independent laboratories and research (Staub et al., 1994; De Schutter, 1998; Vos et al., 1999; Howell et al., 2000; Steuber and De Schutter, 2001, 2002; Gauck and Jaeger, 2003; Solinas et al., 2003, 2006; Kreiner and Jaeger, 2004; Koekkoek et al., 2005; Santamaria et al., 2006, 2011; Shin and De Schutter, 2006; Shin et al., 2007; Steuber et al., 2007; Achard and De Schutter, 2008; De Schutter and Steuber, 2009; Anwar et al., 2012, 2013, 2014; Coop et al., 2010; Tahon et al., 2011; Cao et al., 2012; Couto et al., 2015). Perhaps more importantly the R-DB model has become a true “community model” as it is now being used by a growing number of authors as a base for further modeling work outside its laboratories of origin (Coop and Reeke, 2001; Mandelblat et al., 2001; Miyasho et al., 2001; Roth and Häusser, 2001; Chono et al., 2003; Khaliq et al., 2003; Steuber and Willshaw, 2004; Ogasawara et al., 2007; Yamazaki and Tanaka, 2007; Kulagina et al., 2008; Traub et al., 2008; Brown et al., 2011; Brown and Loew, 2012; Forrest et al., 2012; Forrest, 2015; Masoli et al., 2015). Several of these modeling efforts have now started their own lineage sequences, with, for example, the adaptation of the original R-DB Model by Miyasho et al. (2001), being further extended by Chono et al. (2003), Kulagina et al. (2008), and Brown et al. (2011). Importantly, the model has also been translated from the original GENESIS files to multiple other modeling platforms. As described in this next section, much of that modeling work has been focused on replicating and understanding the complex responses of Purkinje cells resulting from the active properties of its dendrite.
One of the first uses of the R-DB Model outside of my own laboratory’s lineage, explicitly tested the model’s ability to replicate PC responses obtained from new in vitro experimental studies using ion channel blockers (Miyasho et al., 2001). Using dendritic morphology from the rat (Shelton, 1985) parameterized with data from the R-DB Model, Miyasho et al. (2001) modified channel descriptions and conductance densities to reproduce the repetitive Ca2+ spike firing they had found experimentally after the application of TTX in vitro. Importantly, these authors also refined the kinetics of the K+ delayed rectifier current, applying a new mechanism for calculating intracellular Ca2+ concentration while also changing the Ca2+ sensitivity of the calcium-activated dendritic K+ conductance. With these changes, the model was extended to replicate physiological responses including: (1) characteristic Ca2+ dendritic spikes in the presence of TTX; (2) repetitive Ca2+ spiking patterns resulting from the presence of TTX; (3) the lack of Ca2+ spikes found after application of a P-type Ca2+ channel blocker; (4) the slow onset of the Ca2+ spikes in response to a depolorizing current steps; and (5) the marked shortening of the Ca2+ spike onset seen in the presence of 4-AP. Two years later, Chono et al. (2003) further refined the Miyasho et al. (2001) model by adding new channel descriptions as well as refinements in the conductance values for the simulated Ca2+ and Ca2+ dependent K+ channels. These enhancements have since been incorporated into Purkinje cell modeling efforts by other groups (Traub et al., 2008; Brown et al., 2011).
Having extended the ability of the R-DB Model to replicate physiological data obtained under new pharmacological conditions, Miyasho et al. (2001) then explored the possible contribution to dendritic calcium spike generation of two low threshold dendritic calcium related conductances they had recently discovered in their own experimental studies (Watanabe et al., 1998). Adding Ni2+ sensitive Ca2+ channels to the dendrites, these authors demonstrated that the model could now replicate the longer onset Ca2+ spikes found in the presence of Ni2+.
This is the kind of cumulative refinement and advancement that can best, or perhaps can only take place with community models. However, equally important to changes in the structure of a community model, is the use of that model to explore new forms of behavior or perform new forms of analysis not considered by the original model’s authors. To this end, several authors have used the R-DB Model in a reduced from to more closely examine neuronal dynamics (Mandelblat et al., 2001; Fernandez et al., 2007). In a series of publications, Brown et al. (2011) adapted the original R-DB Model to explore how mechanisms at the subcellular (biochemical) levels could be linked to somatic output (Rapp et al., 1992; Brown et al., 2011; Brown and Loew, 2012). While building a new model in Fortran, Traub et al. (2008) never-the-less extended R-DB Model parameters to explore the possible role of gap junctions between the initial axon segments of Purkinje cells in cerebellar cortical oscillations. To do so, he reduced overall dendritic complexity while maintaining a “realistic” path from the distal dendrite to the soma (see Figure 7).
The R-DB Model is also now being used in the context of both subcellular and network level scales. Sub-cellularly, the model has been used as a base to consider the effects of molecular or biophysical mechanisms on Purkinje cell function (Holmes and Rall, 1992; Brown et al., 2011; Brown and Loew, 2012), and to provide a larger context for studies of subcellular modeling of calcium diffusion (Santamaria et al., 2006, 2011; Anwar et al., 2012) as well as biophysical mechanisms of synaptic plasticity (Vladimirescu et al., 1981; Antunes and De Schutter, 2012; De Schutter, 2013). The model has also been used to build network level simulations in reduced (Yuen et al., 1995; Coop and Reeke, 2001; Sarro, 2004), and full form (Howell et al., 2000; Solinas et al., 2003; Santamaria et al., 2007).
The R-DB model has also been applied to new analytical studies, including, for example, questions involving the information processing potential of dendrites (Coop et al., 2010) as well as possible spike coding strategies (Jaeger and Bower, 1999; Steuber and De Schutter, 2001, 2002; De Schutter and Steuber, 2009). Efforts have also been made to link the structure of the R-DB Model to the kind of analysis involved in the field of artificial neural networks (Steuber and De Schutter, 2001; Sarro, 2004).
Finally, the R-DB Model is being used as a base for assessing modeling technology itself, including parameter estimation techniques (Van Geit et al., 2007) and the relationship between parameter variations and modeling results (Achard and De Schutter, 2008).
Understanding Purkinje Cell Responses to Different Types of Input
Having established the community status of the R-DB model, the remainder of this article will consider what has been learned as a result of the use of the model. While general reuse and improvement are important, ultimately the utility of any model, whether used by the community or not, is its ability to generate and truly test hypothesis regarding function (De Schutter, 1999). This is also the most complex and challenging application for any model, especially given the tendency of all scientists to want to see what they want to see. Accordingly especially important, in my view, is a clear establishment of community standards for model performance. In this regard, the next section is organized around a set of Purkinje cell behaviors actually identified by Pellionisz and Llinás, (1977, p. 42) as necessary for, “any Purkinje cell model which claims to be adequate”. As described in subsequent sections of this article, all of these behaviors turn out to depend on the active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite, and replicating and understanding these core response properties has provided the basis for further analysis of the functional significance of active dendritic processes.
Antidromic Spike Activation of the Purkinje Cell Dendrite
Perhaps the most straightforward characteristic Purkinje cell response, identified by Pellionisz and Llinas, is the fact that action potentials generated in the Purkinje cell soma do not propagate into its dendrite (Figure 8). At the time of the first Purkinje cell modeling studies, this lack of antidromic dendritic invasion had already been predicted based on field potential recordings (Llinas et al., 1969b; Freeman and Nicholson, 1975), although the phenomenon was not directly observed experimentally until much later (Llinas and Sugimori, 1980b). In the early passive models, the lack of back propagation was attributed to the relative surface area of the cell dendrite compared to its soma (Pellionisz and Llinás, 1977; Rapp et al., 1994). This explanation was further elaborated in another passive modeling study using parameters obtained from the R-DB Model (although with different dendritic morphology) as due to a large cumulative impedance mismatch resulting from the high branching density of the Purkinje cell dendrite (Roth and Häusser, 2001). With respect to active dendritic mechanisms the models have shown that the very low Na+ channel density in Purkinje cell dendrites provides no mechanism to overcome these morphological effects (De Schutter, 1999; Kitamura and Häusser, 2011) a result also reported in models of other types of mammalian neurons (Vetter et al., 2001).
Responses to Somatic Current Injection
It has been known since intracellular recordings were first made in Purkinje cells, that their response to current injection is complex (Llinas and Sugimori, 1980b). The modeling results shown in Figure 8 were obtained from a passive Purkinje cell dendritic model after current injection in the soma. In fact, as shown in Figure 9, current injection in a real Purkinje cell (and the active R-DB model), produces a much more complex pattern of somatic and dendritic activity (Gähwiler and Llano, 1989; Hirano and Hagiwara, 1989; Kaneda et al., 1990; Regan, 1991; Wang et al., 1991; Lev-Ram et al., 1992). In part for this reason, although not explicitly a part of the original Pellionisz and Llinás (1977) standard for Purkinje cell models, the ability to replicate the results of in vitro current injection studies has become the defacto standard for testing and tuning active Purkinje cell models (Bush and Sejnowski, 1991; De Schutter and Bower, 1994b; Coop and Reeke, 2001; Mandelblat et al., 2001; Miyasho et al., 2001; Forrest et al., 2012).
While a full description of the mechanisms responsible for these in vitro response patterns is beyond the scope of this article, the general result from modeling studies is that this behavior of the Purkinje cell is a function of a complex interaction between all its biophysical and anatomical properties (De Schutter, 1999). This conclusion is somewhat in contrast with the more typical analysis from experimental studies which usually associate different features of the in vitro response properties to specific kinds of afferent input (Gähwiler and Llano, 1989; Hirano and Hagiwara, 1989; Kaneda et al., 1990; Regan, 1991; Wang et al., 1991; Lev-Ram et al., 1992; Miyasho et al., 2001), i.e., fast events associated with somatic action potential generation; the somewhat slower Ca2+ related dendritic bursting behavior assumed to be related to climbing fiber inputs; and longer time course events assumed to be influenced by granule cell related synaptic inputs (Traub et al., 2008; Brown et al., 2011; Isope et al., 2012; Kitamura and Kano, 2012). The models clearly show that these responses are actually related to the entire structure of the Purkinje cell and the interaction between its different afferent inputs. Bursting responses to climbing fiber inputs, for example, are also dependent on the level of background granule cell synaptic input.
It turns out that this co-dependence discovered in the models sheds new light on the importance of the experimental conditions under which Purkinje cells are studied. For example, it has actually been known for many years that the spontaneous behavior of Purkinje cells in vitro is quite different from the spontaneous behavior of Purkinje cell in vivo (Llinas and Sugimori, 1980b). As shown in the modeling results of Figure 10A, in vitro behavior consists of relatively rapid (usually >100 Hz) action potentials, interrupted periodically by spontaneous dendritic calcium spikes. In contrast, as simulated in Figure 10C, Purkinje cells in vivo generate spontaneous action potentials at lower frequencies (usually <80 Hz) that are quite irregular. Dendritic Ca2+ spikes are also believed to only appear in vivo in response to climbing fiber inputs (Llinas and Nicholson, 1976) whereas in vitro they occur spontaneously. Understanding how the response properties of the cell changes in vitro is important given how much of the study of the active properties of neurons has been done using this technique. What modeling results have suggested is that it is the lack of background synaptic input in what is essentially a deafferented brain slice preparation that is reasonable for differences in in vivo and in vitro behavior (Jaeger et al., 1996). Perhaps particularly important in Purkinje cells which are known to receive 150,000 excitatory parallel fiber inputs. However, when provided with background excitatory input alone, the R-DB Model produced a pattern of output that resembled neither the in vitro nor in vivo conditions (Figure 10B; De Schutter, 1999). Instead, replication of in vivo patterns required spontaneous input from both excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs (Figure 10C). Accordingly, the models predict both in single cell (Jaeger et al., 1996; Watanabe et al., 1998) and network form (Howell et al., 2000) that normal Purkinje cell behavior likely depends on current from constant background synaptic inputs, interacting with the active Ca2+ and K+ dependent channels in the dendrite and soma (De Schutter, 1998). Experimental studies specifically designed to test these modeling predictions are consistent with this interpretation (Jaeger and Bower, 1999; Kreiner and Jaeger, 2004). Realistic models have therefore provided an essential tool to relate in vitro response properties to the natural in vivo behavior of Purkinje cells especially challenging given the complexity of this cells active dendritic properties.
Purkinje Cell Responses to Climbing Fiber Activation
The fact that the Purkinje cell responds to climbing fiber activation in vivo with a burst of action potentials has been known for many years (Eccles et al., 1966b). In fact as already noted, the first compartmental Purkinje cell model was specifically constructed to test this experimentally derived prediction (Llinas and Hillman, 1969) that this response behavior was a consequence of the multiple synaptic contacts distributed over the Purkinje cell dendrite by a single climbing fiber (Llinas and Nicholson, 1976), with subsequent modeling focused on the actual biophysical mechanisms responsible for producing the “oscillatory wavelets” or “spike burst” characteristic (see Figure 11F) of climbing fiber responses (Pellionisz and Llinás, 1977). At the time, these authors concluded that the different peaks in the somatic burst response were generated by repetitive firing of the initial segment of the axon rather than by an active dendritic mechanism as had been previously proposed (Eccles et al., 1966b).
Neither Shelton (1985) nor Rapp et al. (1992, 1994) attempted to replicate Purkinje cell responses to climbing fiber activation, however, this was an important component of the initial analysis of the active dendritic and somatic model of De Schutter and Bower (1994b). In fact, after tuning model parameters to replicate responses to somatic current injection data, the ability of the model to generate climbing fiber burst responses without further tuning parameters was the first indication of the model’s likely realism (see Figure 11). As already described, the model predicted that the correct in vivo form of the climbing fiber response was dependent on background patterns of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input. However, analysis of the model also predicted that the dendritic response was dependent on the activation of P type Ca2+ channels in both the cells smooth and the spiny dendrites, with the duration of the dendrite spike being regulated by Ca2+ activated K+ conductances. The modeling results also suggested that the biphasic reversal potential of the climbing fiber induced EPSP, with an early portion reversing before the later portion (i.e., the climbing fibers “duel reversal potential”) previously shown experimentally (Llinas and Hillman, 1969) and attributed solely to the spatial distribution of climbing fiber synapses (Llinas and Nicholson, 1976) was also likely dependent on the details of the active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite. Further, an unexpected but important prediction of the model was that climbing fiber activation resulted in substantial increases in intracellular calcium not only in the smooth dendrites, where climbing fiber synapses actually terminate, but also in the smallest spiny dendritic branches receiving granule cell synaptic inputs (Gundappa-Sulur et al., 1999; Lu et al., 2009) again showing the interrelatedness of the anatomical and physiological components of the dendrite. The involvement of the entire dendrite in the climbing fiber event was simultaneously shown experimentally (Konnerth et al., 1992; Miyakawa et al., 1992). The model also predicted that inhomogeneity in local levels of calcium activation in the dendrite did not depend on a non-uniform distribution of Ca2+ channels as had previously been proposed Tank et al. (1988) and Llinas and Sugimori (1992). Instead the pattern of calcium response was a consequence of the non-uniform geometry of the Purkinje cell dendrite, and likely varied from Purkinje cell to Purkinje cell. Thus, unlike Rapp et al. (1994), who reported little effect of individual dendritic variations on cellular passive properties, the active model suggested that differences in individual Purkinje cell morphologies might, in fact have important functional significance.
Replication of the Simple Spike Firing of Purkinje Cells
The final, and it turns out most difficult standard for Purkinje cell modeling proposed by Pellionisz and Llinás (1977) was the ability to replicate simple spike firing in response to granule cell (parallel fiber) input. This is, of course, mor difficult because, in principle, understanding the important features of this behavior is likely linked directly to questions of neuronal coding, which we really know about nothing about. Never-the-less, it is the attempt to replicate this behavior of the Purkinje cell with the R-DB Model has produced the most interesting and provocative structural and functional predictions resulting in several new hypotheses regarding the cell’s overall function and in fact the function of the cerebellum itself (Bower, 2002). The following sections will consider several examples.
The Natural Function of the Purkinje Cell Dendrite Depends on the Presence of Background Synaptic Inputs
As already described, one important prediction of the R-DB Model is that the natural behavior of the Purkinje cell dendrite depends on the presence of continuous excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input from the granule cell pathway. Again, while background excitatory granule cell (parallel fiber) synaptic activity had been anticipated for some time to influence ongoing Purkinje cell firing (Llinas et al., 1969a), in order to get realistic patterns of spiking out of the active Purkinje cell model it was necessary to also add background inhibitory synaptic inputs (De Schutter and Bower, 1994a). These modeling efforts resulted in several predictions. First the model predicted that Purkinje cell behavior was dependent on the ability of the soma, itself, to spontaneously generate action potentials. This ability has now been demonstrated experimentally (Pugh and Raman, 2009), and has recently also been further studied using a model derived from the R-DB line (Forrest et al., 2012). Second, as shown in Figure 12, the model predicted that the large intrinsic voltage gated currents and not the relatively smaller currents associated with synaptic activation most influenced ongoing somatic spiking (Jaeger et al., 1996; De Schutter, 1998; Jaeger and Bower, 1999). In fact, the model predicted that the Purkinje cell dendrite is actually dominantly a current sink rather than a source, making the behavior of the Purkinje cell very different from that of a traditional integrate and fire neuron (see below). Further, the model suggested that background spontaneous parallel fiber inputs had much less of an effect on the actual timing of Purkinje cell spikes than did inhibitory synaptic input (Jaeger et al., 1996). While a full description of the dendritic dynamics underlying this behavior is beyond the scope of this chapter (for more details, see De Schutter and Bower, 1994b,c; Jaeger et al., 1996; De Schutter, 1999; Jaeger and Bower, 1999), experimental (Jaeger and Bower, 1999; Womack and Khodakhah, 2002a,b, 2004; Womack et al., 2004; Santamaria et al., 2007) and subsequent R-DB Model related studies (Howell et al., 2000; Miyasho et al., 2001; Coop et al., 2010; Brown et al., 2011; Forrest et al., 2012) have supported these unexpected but model-predicted interactions between the Purkinje cell dendrite and soma.
“Dendritic Democracy” and the Influence of Distal Synaptic Inputs
The influence of excitatory synaptic input in such a large dendrite has been a central issue for Purkinje cell modeling for many years. In fact, the publication by Llinas et al. (1968) that sparked the first consideration of modeling in Purkinje cells (Calvin and Hellerstein, 1969; Calvin, 1969; Zucker, 1969) started by posing the following fundamental question: “In studying the anatomy of the Purkinje cell, one wonders how the distal region of (these large) dendrites can act upon the soma and axon …” (Llinas et al., 1968, p. 1132). That article went on to identify two possibilities: (i) by direct electrotonic spread from the distal dendrite to the soma, or (ii) by the initiation of action potentials or local responses which can be conducted either in an all- or-none manner or in a decremental fashion down to the axon.” (Llinas et al., 1968, p. 1132). Considering this question was also a primary objective of the modeling efforts of both Shelton (1985) and Rapp et al. (1992, 1994); who both predicted, based on their passive models, that the Purkinje cell dendrite was actually electrotonically compact and therefore that distal synaptic inputs, in principle should have an influence on the soma similar to those more proximal. Shelton specifically describes the functional significance of the high passive dendritic input resistance as “a specialization which optimizes the dendrites for signaling (the soma) with minimum (synaptic) attenuation” (Shelton, 1985, p. 127). This apparent characteristic of the passive electrical properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite has been described as promoting “dendritic democracy” so that: “somatic EPSP amplitude is only weakly dependent on synaptic location on Purkinje cell spiny branchlets” (Roth and Häusser, 2001, p. 469).
In the description of the behavior of their passive models, Llinas et al. (1968), Pellionisz and Llinás (1977), Shelton (1985) and Rapp et al. (1994) all mentioned that this baseline “dendritic democracy” likely only applied to the passive electrical properties of the dendrite, and was therefore likely to change with the addition of active conductances. Shelton (1985, p. 128), specifically predicted that the addition of synaptic conductances would likely “swamp” the passive membrane conductivity significantly extending the electrotonic length of the dendrite. Actual simulations by Rapp et al. (1992, 1994) again using a passive model, supported Shelton’s speculation, predicting that individual parallel fiber synapses “essentially loose their functional meaning (in the presence of large amounts of background synaptic input) and only activation of a large number of parallel fibers will significantly displace the membrane potential” (Rapp et al., 1992, p. 530).
In fact, in our active dendritic models adding both synaptic conductances as well as the large voltage dependent dendritic Ca2+ related membrane conductances did further extend the electrotonic length of the dendrite (De Schutter and Bower, 1994b) a modeling result subsequently confirmed experimentally (Staub et al., 1994; Ascoli, 2007). However, as described in the third article in the series (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c), the addition of dendritic voltage dependent Ca2+ membrane conductances revealed a new and unexpected biophysical mechanism in which synchronously activated granule cell inputs induced a sub-threshold Ca2+ dependent amplification mechanism that restored “democracy” to the dendrite even in the presence of ongoing background synaptic input (Figure 13). While Llinas had suggested the general possibility that active membrane properties could facilitate the influence of synapses on the soma, and Shelton (1985, p. 128), specifically speculated that “active dendritic spikes or active graded potentials may act as a booster mechanism to overcome the electrotonic lengthening of the dendrite due to synaptic activation”, the specific mechanism that emerged from the R-DB Model was unexpected. Instead of being dependent on a dendritic calcium spiking mechanism as previously assumed (Pellionisz and Szentágothai, 1974), the mechanism involved activation of a sub-spiking threshold calcium event (Figure 13). As a result, in these simulations, a small number of synchronously activated granule cell synaptic inputs produced a similar level of depolarization in the soma regardless of where they were located on the dendrite, a form of “dendritic democracy” that turned out to be dependent and reflect the actual temporal pattern of synaptic input. Further, and importantly, while generating a somatic spike in the passive dendritic models required the activation of large numbers of excitatory synapses (Llinas and Sugimori, 1980b; Rapp et al., 1992, 1994), the active model predicted that somatic spike generation due to synchronously activated synaptic input required an order of magnitude fewer active synapses (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c). This prediction was subsequently confirmed experimentally (Isope and Barbour, 2002). The model has also predicted a similar amplification effect on synchronized inhibitory inputs (Solinas et al., 2006).
Purkinje Cells are Tuned to Operate in Context of Activity in the Overall Cerebellar Cortical Network
Another very general but critically important insight gained from the models is that understanding neuronal function requires that a neurons physiological properties be considered in the context of the network in which they are embedded, and in particular in the context of the temporal and spatial patterns of afferent information converging on that cell as a consequence of network structure. While this might at first seem completely obvious, by embedding the R-DB Model within realistic network simulations, very specific new predictions were obtained on this relationship (Santamaria et al., 2007). As with single cell modeling, it is our view that for models to generate new predictions (rather than simply demonstrate pre-conceived functional notions) network level modeling must also be tested against a clearly defined set of physiological behaviors, preferably not yet well understood (Bower, 1990). To be able to interpret the significance of the active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite with respect to network organization, it will be necessary to first consider these network level physiological behaviors.
As it turns out the original motivation for cerebellar modeling in my laboratory was to investigate an unexpected and counterintuitive pattern of Purkinje cell responses to peripheral sensory stimuli (see Figure 14) observed in vivo (Bower and Woolston, 1983). Specifically, the spatial extent of Purkinje cell responses to peripheral stimuli was found to be far more restricted than was expected from the organization of cerebellar cortical circuitry and in particular the considerable anatomical spread of the parallel fibers within cerebellar cortex (Eccles et al., 1967, 1971; Bell and Grimm, 1969; Bower and Woolston, 1983). Results consistent or directly supporting this finding have now been reported in numerous subsequent experiments (Kolb et al., 1997; Cohen and Yarom, 1998; Lu et al., 2005; Holtzman et al., 2006; Heck et al., 2007; Rokni et al., 2008; de Solages et al., 2008; Brown and Ariel, 2009; Walter et al., 2009; Dizon and Khodakhah, 2011).
In the original experimental studies published in the early 1980’s, the restricted extent of Purkinje cells activated by peripheral stimuli was interpreted in the most obvious way by suggesting that parallel fibers were simply less influential on Purkinje cell output than had previously been assumed (Bower et al., 1980; Bower and Woolston, 1983). However, it was not clear why responding Purkinje cells were only found over regions of active granule cell layer. In Llinas (1982) suggested that this experimental result (Bower et al., 1980; Bower and Woolston, 1983) could be explained if Purkinje cells were driven by synchronous input from synapses made by granule cells as they ascend through the molecular layer past the Purkinje cell dendrite (Mugnaini, 1972), but not by more asynchronous parallel fiber inputs (Llinas, 1982). Llinas, however, attributed this effect simply to the reduced synchrony of parallel fiber inputs.
When considered now in the context of the R-DB Modeling results, this explanation seemed perfectly consistent with the relative lack of direct influence of background parallel inputs on Purkinje cell spiking, combined with the amplification mechanism for synchronize excitatory inputs (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c). Accordingly it was fully expected that the R-DB Model, once placed in a network context, would confirm Llinas speculation, that the effect simply had to do with the timing of the different synaptic inputs. It was surprising therefore, that even the most desyncronized pattern of parallel fibers, still induced the dendritic boosting mechanism driving somatic output (Santamaria et al., 2007). Resolving this difference between experimental data and modeling results required the introduction of feed-forward inhibitory synaptic inputs to the network model (Santamaria et al., 2007; Walter et al., 2009).
The modeling efforts intended to replicate the restricted pattern of Purkinje cell activation to afferent input (Bower and Woolston, 1983), have perhaps most fundamentally changed how we think of cerebellar cortical processing (Bower, 2010). While most previous theories of cerebellar function have focused on the parallel fiber system as the primary driver of Purkinje cell somatic firing (Braitenberg, 1967; Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971; Pellionisz and Szentágothai, 1974; Medina and Mauk, 2000; Vetter et al., 2001; Heck and Sultan, 2002; Ito, 2006; Kitamura and Kano, 2012), model analysis suggests that it is actually the synapses associated with the ascending segment of the granule cell axon, firing nearly synchronously and not the parallel fibers, which influence spike timing in the soma (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c). Further, the model has also predicted that ongoing somatic spiking activity is not directly influenced by synaptic input, but instead is mediated through the large active conductances in the soma and dendrite. In this view, the synchronous ascending input simply modifies the timing of action potentials that would have been generated anyway (Santamaria and Bower, 2004).
As just briefly described, perhaps one of the more important consequences of the modeling effort has been to clarify and make quite clear predictions regarding different functional roles of the parallel fibers and the ascending segment synapses of the same granule cell axon (Bower, 1997c). While parallel fiber inputs modulates the overall state of the dendrite, it is the ascending segment inputs that more closely drive output. Interestingly, this functional difference turns out to actually be manifest in the fine physical structure of the Purkinje cell dendrite itself. As shown in Figure 15, anatomical studies have demonstrated that the synapses associated with the ascending granule cell axon segments are found only on the distal regions of the dendrite (Gundappa-Sulur et al., 1999; Lu et al., 2009), where our network models predict that these synapses will be synchronously active in response to afferent mossy fiber stimuli (Santamaria et al., 2007). Our single cell models predict that the active properties of the dendrite mediate a boosting mechanism allowing this distant input to influence somatic spiking (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c). Anatomical studies have also shown that parallel fiber synapses are found primarily on the more proximal spiny dendrites (Gundappa-Sulur et al., 1999; Lu et al., 2009), where both the network (Santamaria et al., 2007) and single cell (Jaeger et al., 1996) models suggest they interact with feed forward inhibition to regulate the activation state of the large dendritic voltage dependent Ca2+ and Ca2+ activated K+ conductances. This places parallel fibers in a position to influence or modulate, the response of the dendrite to the synchronous ascending segment synapses. The models predict that this modulation by the parallel fibers and molecular layer inhibition is mediated through their control of the membrane voltage in the dendrite, and thus the state of activation of the large dendritic voltage dependent conductances. Thus the same active voltage dependent dendritic conductances are responsible for mediating the amplification mechanism for distal synchronous ascending segment inputs as well as the spiking behavior of the soma in general (Bower, 2010). As an aside, these results also suggest that climbing fiber activation resets these modulatory mechanisms (Bower, 1997b), a role consistent with another original prediction of the R-DB Model, that calcium influx from climbing fiber activation would spread to the distal most regions of the dendrite (De Schutter and Bower, 1994b). In this way, the use of anatomically and physiologically realistic models has resulted in predictions that, in effect, merge the anatomical and physiological properties of this cell. In my view, this is what is meant by exploring structure function relationships. Importantly again, the models were not built with these relationships in mind, they came out of running the models.
Implications and the Importance of Community Models
In summary, while it was first suggested more than 40 years ago that the active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite significantly influence the computation performed by this neuron, it has taken 40 years of combined modeling and experimental work to reach the beginning of an understanding about this relationship. Further, that emerging understanding suggests that much of our intuition over the last 40 years has been largely wrong. Changes in thinking about the physiological structure of the Purkinje cell dendrite has, in turn, driven, at least in our laboratory, a fundamental reconsideration regarding the function of the cerebellum as a whole (Bower, 1997a,b; Bower et al., 2012; for context for in the overall field see: Manto et al., 2012).
While general speculations on this subject can still be found in many experimental papers, the combination of realistic modeling and experimental studies described here has specifically revealed that Purkinje cell responses to granule cell-related excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs are quite different from the parallel fiber dominant, integrate and fire type cellular dynamics assumed by the most current theories of cerebellar function (Braitenberg, 1967; Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971; Pellionisz and Szentágothai, 1974; Medina and Mauk, 2000; Vetter et al., 2001; Heck and Sultan, 2002; Ito, 2006; Hong and Optican, 2008; Kitamura and Kano, 2012). In fact, while the 500+ references in the literature for the R-DB Model is very high compared to almost all models of its kind, in the last 20 years, there have actually been over 10,000 Purkinje cell experimental papers published, almost none of reference models of any kind. It is also completely standard, 40 years after Purkinje cell modeling started, for review articles on Purkinje cell function to make no mention what-so-ever of these modeling efforts or their results (e.g., from the last 2 years; Gallian and De Zeeuw, 2014; Grasselli and Hansel, 2014; Jörntell, 2014; Lewis and Raman, 2014; Voogd, 2014; Cerminara et al., 2015; Cheron et al., 2015; Dar, 2015; Louis, 2015; Tada et al., 2015). In fact, even review articles on subjects as central to the modeling as the active properties of the Purkinje cell dendrite can quite remarkably be published with hardly any mention of modeling results (Kitamura and Kano, 2012). Yet, many of the issues raised in those reviews, as well as the experimental papers they are based on raise issues that modelers have been investigating for years and many that have been resolved years ago.
How then are we to proceed in an organized way to understand function at any level, from the cerebellum itself down to the voltage dependent conductances in the Purkinje cell dendrite. This article is an example of how such study can proceed if based on realistic models shared by a community. Yet most published models are still designed to demonstrate a preexisting functional idea. In this regard, it is a remarkable fact that Pellionisz and Llinas first proposed more than 25 years ago a standard of “adequacy” for representing Purkinje cells (Pellionisz and Llinás, 1977). Yet most published models of Purkinje cells and certainly almost all published network models make no attempts what-so-ever to demonstrate that their Purkinje cells behave like actual Purkinje cells (Blum et al., 1993; Buonomano and Mauk, 1994; Yuen et al., 1995; Barto et al., 1999; Chauvet and Chauvet, 1999; Medina and Mauk, 2000; Spoelstra et al., 2000; Kistler and De Zeeuw, 2002; Brunel et al., 2004; Mauk and Ohyama, 2004; Yamazaki and Tanaka, 2007; Carrillo et al., 2008; Kulagina et al., 2008; de Gruijl et al., 2009; Abrams et al., 2010; Dean et al., 2010; Ohyama et al., 2010; Dean and Porrill, 2011; Li et al., 2012; Yamazaki and Nagao, 2012). It is entirely unclear what the value of a model is if the properties of its neurons, in this case a neuron with important active dendritic conductances, bears little resemblance to its actual physiological properties. Philosophers of science have long recognized the distinction between observation-based story telling and quantitative model-based analysis (Kuhn, 1962). In my view, models that misrepresent the actual physical properties of their neurons, including in this case usually neglecting the active properties of their dendrites, are essentially an extension of the story telling tradition. It is also worth noting that many of the models referenced above concern, perhaps, issues that many consider to be more directly related to cerebellar function, aging, learning, ataxia, effects of alcohol abuse, etc. These are clearly of interest to the cerebellar community, especially with the pressure for so-called translational science. In my view, a real understanding of these kinds of issues will absolutely depend on the continued construction and further elaboration of the level of realistic model described here, best done as part of a community. However, given the current state of the model, I see no reason why questions involving synaptic plasticity, pharmacological effects on specific ion channels, and even, possibly the kinds of aberrant behavior seen in Purkinje cells in some conditions of ataxia, can’t begin to be studied with a model of this type.
This in fact, is perhaps the most important reason that over the next 20 years it will be critical for the computational neuroscience community to adopt and build community models (Bower and Bower, 2013). If we are all simply working on our own disconnected individual models, we have little chance of establishing the kind of tested and accepted underlying quantitative framework that is likely essential for real scientific progress. By committing to the use of community models we also establish a common structure that can be presented to the larger neuroscience community, not as just another model, but as a model that has been built, tested, verified and accepted by multiple researchers. Why shouldn’t these models, then find their way into graduate training programs, or neuroscience textbooks? Why shouldn’t such a model be used as a standard against which other models are tested? As long as modelers fail to cooperate, they will likely continue to be largely ignored, not only be experimentalists, but also by their fellow modelers. It is only through the cooperative building and testing of models that an underlying quantitative infrastructure will begin to be constructed for neuroscience. In my view, the last 40 years demonstrates that it is only through that kind of infrastructure that we will ever understand complex phenomena, like, for example, the functional implications of active neuronal processes.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
- Abrams Z. R., Warrier A., Trauner D., Zhang X. (2010). A signal processing analysis of purkinje cells in vitro. Front. Neural Circuits 4:13. 10.3389/fncir.2010.00013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Achard P., De Schutter E. (2008). Calcium, synaptic plasticity and intrinsic homeostasis in Purkinje neuron models. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2:8. 10.3389/neuro.10.008.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albus J. S. (1971). A theory of cerebellar function. Math. Biosci. 10, 25–61. 10.1016/0025-5564(71)90051-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Antunes G., De Schutter E. (2012). A stochastic signaling network mediates the probabilistic induction of cerebellar long-term depression. J. Neurosci. 32, 9288–9300. 10.1523/jneurosci.5976-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anwar H., Hepburn I., Nedelescu H., Chen W., De Schutter E. (2013). Stocastic calcium mechanisms cause dendritic calcium spike variability. J. Neurosci. 40, 15848–15867. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1722-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anwar H., Hong S., Schutter E. (2012). Controlling Ca2+-activated K+ channels with models of Ca2+ buffering in Purkinje cells. Cerebellum 11, 681–693. 10.1007/s12311-010-0224-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anwar H., Roome C. J., Nedelescu H., Chen W., Kuhn B., De Schutter E. (2014). Dendritic diameters affect the spatial variability of intracellular calcium dynamics in computer models. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 8:168. 10.3389/fncel.2014.00168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ascoli G. A. (2007). Successes and rewards in sharing digital reconstructions of neuronal morphology. Neuroinformatics 5, 154–160. 10.1007/s12021-007-0010-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldi P., Vanier M. C., Bower J. M. (1998). On the use of Bayesian methods for evaluating compartmental neural models. J. Comput. Neurosci. 5, 285–314. 10.1023/A:1008887028637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barto A. G., Fagg A. H., Sitkoff N., Houk J. C. (1999). A cerebellar model of timing and prediction in the control of reaching. Neural Comput. 11, 565–594. 10.1162/089976699300016575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell C. C., Grimm R. J. (1969). Discharge properties of Purkinje cells recorded on single and double microelectrodes. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 1044–1055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blum E. K., Khademi P. M., Thompson R. F. (1993). “Model and simulation of a simplified cerebellar neural network for classical conditioning of the rabbit eyeblink response,” in Proceedings of Analysis and Modeling of Neural Systems, Vol. 2 (Boston: Kluwer Academic). [Google Scholar]
- Blum E. K., Wang X. (1990). “Design for Purkinje cells,” in Analysis and Modeling of Neural Systems, ed. Eeck A. F. H. (Boston: Kluwer Academic Press; ), 123–129. [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (1990). “Reverse engineering the nervous system: an anatomical, physiological and computer based approach,” in An Introduction to Neural and Electronic Networks, eds Zornetzer S., Davis J., Lau C. (New York: Academic Press; ), 3–24. [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (1997a). Is the cerebellum sensory for motor’s sake, of motor for sensory’s sake: the view from the whiskers of a rat? Prog. Brain Res. 114, 463–496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (1997b). “The cerebellum and the control of sensory data aquisition,” in International Review of Neurobiology Vol 41, The Cerebellum and Cognition. International Review of Neurobiology, ed. Schmahmann J. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 489–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (1997c). What do parallel fibers do? commentary on “The detection and generation of sequences as a key to cerebellar function: Experiments and Theory” by: Valentino Braitenberg, Detlef Heck, and Fahad Sultan. Behav. Brain Sci. 20:247. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (2002). The organization of cerebellar cortical circuitry revisited: implications for function. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 978, 135–155. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07562.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (2010). Model-founded explorations of the roles of molecular layer inhibition in regulating purkinje cell responses in cerebellar cortex: more trouble for the beam hypothesis. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 4:27. 10.3389/fncel.2010.00027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (2012). “The computational structure of the cerebellar molecular layer,” in Handbook of Cerebellum and Cerebellar Disorders, eds Schmahmann M., Manto D. L., Gruol J. D., Koibuchi N., Rossi F. (Berlin: Springer; ), 1359–1380. [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M. (2013). “The Purkinje cell as a community model in neuroscience,” in 20 Years of Computational Neuroscience, ed. Bower J. M. (Berlin: Springer; ) 103–140. [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M., Beeman D. (1995). The Book of GENESIS. New York: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M., Beeman D. (2007). Constructing realistic neural simulations with GENESIS. Methods Mol. Biol. 401, 103–125. 10.1007/978-1-59745-520-6_7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M., Woolston D. C. (1983). Congruence of spatial organization of tactile projections to granule cell and Purkinje cell layers of cerebellar hemispheres of the albino rat: vertical organization of cerebellar cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 49, 745–766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M., Woolston D. C., Gibson J. M. (1980). Congruence of spatial patterns of receptive field projections to Purkinje cell and granule cell layers in the cerebellar hemispheres of the rat. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 6, 511. [Google Scholar]
- Braitenberg V. (1967). Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond range? Prog. Brain Res. 25, 334–346. 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)60971-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown M. E., Ariel M. (2009). Topography and response timing of intact cerebellum stained with absorbance voltage-sensitive dye. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 474–490. 10.1152/jn.90766.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown S.-A., Loew L. M. (2012). Computational analysis of calcium signaling and membrane electrophysiology in cerebellar Purkinje neurons associated with ataxia. BMC Syst. Biol. 6:70. 10.1186/1752-0509-6-70 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown S.-A., Moraru I. I., Schaff J. C., Loew L. M. (2011). Virtual NEURON: a strategy for merged biochemical and electrophysiological modeling. J. Comput. Neurosci. 31, 385–400. 10.1007/s10827-011-0317-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunel N., Hakim V., Isope P., Nadal J. P., Barbour B. (2004). Optimal information storage and the distribution of synaptic weights: perceptron versus Purkinje cell. Neuron 43, 745–757. 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00528-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buonomano D. V., Mauk M. D. (1994). Neural-network model of the cerebellum: temporal discrimination and the timing of motor-responses. Neural Comput. 6, 38–55. 10.1162/neco.1994.6.1.38 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bush P. C., Sejnowski T. J. (1991). Simulations of a reconstructed cerebellar Purkinje cell based on simplified channel kinetics. Neural Comput. 3, 321–332. 10.1162/neco.1991.3.3.321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvin W. H. (1969). Dendritic spikes revisited. Science 166, 637–638. 10.1126/science.166.3905.637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvin W. H., Hellerstein D. (1969). Dendritic spikes versus cable properties. Science 163, 96–97. 10.1126/science.163.3862.96 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao Y., Maran S. K., Dhamala M., Jaeger D., Heck D. H. (2012). Behavior-related pauses in simple-spike activity of mouse Purkinje cells are linked to spike rate modulation. J. Neurosci. 32, 8678–8685. 10.1523/jneurosci.4969-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrillo R. R., Ros E., Boucheny C., Coenen O. J. M. D. (2008). A real-time spiking cerebellum model for learning robot control. Biosystems 94, 18–27. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2008.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cerminara N. L., Lang E. J., Sillitoe R. V., Apps R. (2015). Redefining the cerebellar cortex as an assembly of non-uniform Purkinje cell microcircuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 79–93. 10.1038/nrn3886 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chauvet P., Chauvet G. A. (1999). Purkinje local circuits with delays: mathematical conditions of stability for learning and retrieval. Neural Netw. 12, 59–77. 10.1016/s0893-6080(98)00106-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheron G., Márquez-Ruiz J., Dan B. (2015). Oscillations, timing, plasticity and learning in the cerebellum. Cerebellum [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1007/s12311-015-0665-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chono K., Takagi H., Koyma S., Suzuki H., Ito E. (2003). A cell model study of calcium influx mechanism regulated by calcium-dependent potassium channels in Purkinje cell dendrites. J. Neurosci. Methods 129, 115–127. 10.1016/s0165-0270(03)00194-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen D., Yarom Y. (1998). Patches of synchronized activity in the cerebellar cortex evoked by mossy-fiber stimulation: questioning the role of parallel fibers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 15032–15036. 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coop A. D., Cornelis H., Santamaria F. (2010). Dendritic excitability modulates dendritic information processing in a Purkinje cell model. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4:6. 10.3389/fncom.2010.00006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coop A. D., Reeke G. N. J. (2001). The composite neuron: a realistic one-compartment Purkinje cell model suitable for large-scale neuronal network simulations. J. Comput. Neurosci. 10, 173–186. 10.1023/A:1011269014373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornelis H., Coop A. D., Rodriquez M., Beeman D., Bower J. M. (2010). “Using models to collaborate, communicate and publish: an introduction to GENESIS 3.0 and the future of computational neurobiology,” in Proceedings of CNS 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Couto J., Linaro D., De Schutter E., Giugliano M. (2015). On the firing rate dependency of the phase response curve of rat Purkinje neurons in vitro. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11:e1004112. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dar M. S. (2015). Ethanol-induced cerebellar ataxia: cellular and molecular mechanisms. Cerebellum 14, 447–465. 10.1007/s12311-014-0638-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dean P., Porrill J. (2011). Evaluating the adaptive-filter model of the cerebellum. J. Physiol. 589, 3459–3470. 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.201574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dean P., Porrill J., Ekerot C.-F., Jörntell H. (2010). The cerebellar microcircuit as an adaptive filter: experimental and computational evidence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 30–43. 10.1038/nrn2756 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Gruijl J. R., van der Smagt P., De Zeeuw C. I. (2009). Anticipatory grip force control using a cerebellar model. Neuroscience 162, 777–786. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.02.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E. (1994). Modelling the cerebellar Purkinje cell: experiments in computo. Prog. Brain Res. 102, 427–441. 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)60557-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E. (1998). Dendritic voltage and calcium-gated channels amplify the variability of postsynaptic responses in a Purkinje cell model. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 504–519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E. (1999). Using realistic models to study synaptic integration in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 233–245. 10.1515/revneuro.1999.10.3-4.233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E. (2013). The importance of stochastic signaling processes in the induction of long-term synaptic plasticity. Neural. Netw. 47, 3–10. 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.11.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1992). “Purkinje neuron simulation on the Intel Touchstone Delta with GENESIS,” in Proceedings of the Grand Challenge Computing Fair, eds Mihaly T., Messina P. (Pasadena, CA: CCSF Publishing; ), 268–279. [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1994a). An active membrane model of the cerebellar Purkinje cell. I. Simulation of current clamps in slice. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 375–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1994b). An active membrane model of the cerebellar Purkinje cell II. Simulation of synaptic responses. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 401–419. 10.1115/1.802953.paper47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1994c). Simulated responses of cerebellar Purkinje cells are independent of the dendritic location of granule cell synaptic inputs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 91, 4736–4740. 10.1073/pnas.91.11.4736 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1993). “Integration of synchronous and asynchronous synaptic inputs in a detailed compartmental model of the cerebellar Purkinje cell,” in Computation and Neural Systems 1992, eds Bower J. M., Eeckman F. (Boston, MA: Kluwer Press; ), 355–362. [Google Scholar]
- De Schutter E., Steuber V. (2009). Patterns and pauses in Purkinje cell simple spike trains: experiments, modeling and theory. Neuroscience 162, 816–826. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.02.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Solages C., Szapiro G., Brunel N., Hakim V., Isope P., Buisseret P., et al. (2008). High-frequency organization and synchrony of activity in the purkinje cell layer of the cerebellum. Neuron 58, 775–788. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dizon M. J., Khodakhah K. (2011). The role of interneurons in shaping Purkinje cell responses in the cerebellar cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 10463–10473. 10.1523/jneurosci.1350-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge F. A., Cooley J. W. (1973). Action potential of the motorneuron. IBM J. Res. Dev. 17, 219–229. 10.1147/rd.173.0219 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eccles J. C., Faber D. S., Murphy J. T., Sabah N. H., Taborikova H. (1971). Investigation on integration of mossy fiber inputs to Purkyne cells in the anterior lobe. Exp. Brain Res. 13, 54–77. 10.1007/bf00236430 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eccles J. C., Ito M., Szentagothai J. (1967). The Cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine. Berlin: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Eccles J. C., Llins R., Sasaki K. (1966a). Parallel fibre stimulation and the responses induced thereby in the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Exp. Brain Res. 1, 17–39. 10.1007/bf00235207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eccles J. C., Llinás R., Sasaki K. (1966b). The excitatory synaptic action of climbing fibres on the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. J. Physiol. 182, 268–296. 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp007824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez F. R., Engbers J. D. T., Turner R. W. (2007). Firing dynamics of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 278–294. 10.3410/f.1086747.539661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forrest M. D. (2015). Simulation of alcohol action upon a detailed Purkinje cell neuron model and a simplier surrogate model that runs >400 times faster. BMC Neuroscience 16:27. 10.1186/s12868-015-0162-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forrest M. D., Wall M. J., Press D. A., Feng J. (2012). The sodium-potassium pump controls the intrinsic firing of the cerebellar Purkinje neuron. PLoS One 7:e51169. 10.1371/journal.pone.0051169 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman J. A., Nicholson C. (1975). Experimental optimization of current source-density technique for anuran cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 38, 369–382. 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90494-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gähwiler B. H., Llano I. (1989). Sodium and potassium conductances in somatic membranes of rat Purkinje cells from organotypic cerebellar cultures. J. Physiol. 417, 105–122. 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017793 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallian E., De Zeeuw C. I. (2014). Questioning the cerebellar doctrine. Prog. Brain Res. 210, 59–77. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00003-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauck V., Jaeger D. (2003). The contribution of NMDA and AMPA conductances to the control of spiking in neurons of the deep cerebellar nuclei. J. Neurosci. 23, 8109–8118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genet S., Sabarly L., Guigon E., Berry H., Delord B. (2010). Dendritic signals command firing dynamics in a mathematical model of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Biophys. J. 99, 427–436. 10.1016/j.bpj.2010.04.056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grasselli G., Hansel C. (2014). Cerebellar long-term potentiation: cellular mechanisms and role in learning. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 117, 39–51. 10.1016/b978-0-12-420247-4.00003-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gundappa-Sulur G., De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1999). Ascending granule cell axon: an important component of cerebellar cortical circuitry. J. comp. Neurol. 408, 580–596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heck D., Sultan F. (2002). Cerebellar structure and function: making sense of parallel fibers. Hum. Mov. Sci. 21, 411–421. 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00123-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heck D. H., Thach W. T., Keating J. G. (2007). On-beam synchrony in the cerebellum as the mechanism for the timing and coordination of movement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 7658–7663. 10.1073/pnas.0609966104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirano T., Hagiwara S. (1989). Kinetics and distribution of voltage-gated Ca, Na and K channels on the somata of rat cerebellar Purkinje cells. Pflugers Arch. 413, 463–469. 10.1007/bf00594174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgkin A. L., Huxley A. F. (1952). A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J. Physiol. 117, 500–544. 10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes W. R., Rall W. (1992). Electrotonic length estimates in neurons with dendritic tapering or somatic shunt. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 1421–1437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtzman T., Rajapaksa T., Mostofi A., Edgley S. A. (2006). Different responses of rat cerebellar Purkinje cells and Golgi cells evoked by widespread convergent sensory inputs. J. Physiol. 574, 491–507. 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.108282 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hong S., Optican L. M. (2008). Interaction between Purkinje cells and inhibitory interneurons may create adjustable output waveforms to generate timed cerebellar output. PLoS One 3:e2770. 10.1371/journal.pone.0002770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howell F. W., Dyhrfjeld-Johnsen J., Maex R., Goddard N., De Schutter E. (2000). A large scale model of the cerebellar cortex using PGENESIS. Neurocomputing 32–33, 1041–1046. 10.1016/s0925-2312(00)00277-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Isope P., Barbour B. (2002). Properties of unitary granule cell–>Purkinje cell synapses in adult rat cerebellar slices. J. Neurosci. 22, 9668–9678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isope P., Hildebrand M. E., Snutch T. P. (2012). Contributions of T-type voltage-gated calcium channels to postsynaptic calcium signaling within Purkinje neurons. Cerebellum 11, 651–665. 10.1007/s12311-010-0195-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. (2006). Cerebellar circuitry as a neuronal machine. Prog. Neurobiol. 78, 272–303. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaeger D., Bower J. M. (1999). Synaptic control of spiking in cerebellar Purkinje cells: dynamic current clamp based on model conductances. J. Neurosci. 19, 6090–6101. 10.1007/978-0-387-89279-5_3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaeger D., De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1993). “Prolonged activation following brief synaptic input in the cerebellar Purkinje cell: intracellular recording and compartmental modeling,” in Computation and Neural Systems 1992, eds Bower J. M., Eeckman F. (Boston, MA: Kluwer Press; ), 343–348. [Google Scholar]
- Jaeger D., De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (1996). The role of synaptic and voltage-gated currents in the control of Purkinje cell spiking: a modeling study. J. Neurosci. 17, 91–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jörntell H. (2014). Cerebellar synaptic plasticity and the credit assignment problem. Cerebellum [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1007/s12311-014-0623-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaneda M., Wakamori M., Ito C., Akaike N. (1990). Low-threshold calcium current in isolated Purkinje cell bodies of rat cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 63, 1046–1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khaliq Z. M., Gouwens N. W., Raman I. M. (2003). The contribution of resurgent sodium current to high-frequency firing in Purkinje neurons: an experimental and modeling study. J. Neurosci. 23, 4899–4912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kistler W. M., De Zeeuw C. I. (2002). Dynamical working memory and timed responses: the role of reverberating loops in the olivo-cerebellar system. Neural Comput. 14, 2597–2626. 10.1162/089976602760407991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitamura K., Häusser M. (2011). Dendritic calcium signaling triggered by spontaneous and sensory-evoked climbing fiber input to cerebellar Purkinje cells in vivo. J. Neurosci. 31, 10847–10858. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2525-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitamura K., Kano M. (2012). Dendritic calcium signaling in cerebellar Purkinje cell. Neural Netw. 47, 11–17. 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koekkoek S. K. E., Yamaguchi K., Milojkovic B. A., Dortland B. R., Ruigrok T. J. H., Maex R., et al. (2005). Deletion of FMR1 in Purkinje cells enhances parallel fiber LTD, enlarges spines and attenuates cerebellar eyelid conditioning in Fragile X syndrome. Neuron 47, 339–352. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolb F. P., Arnold G., Lerch R., Straka H., Buttner-Ennever J. (1997). Spatial distribution of field potential profiles in the cat cerebellar cortex evoked by peripheral and central inputs. Neuroscience 81, 1155–1181. 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00255-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konnerth A., Dreessen J., Augustine G. J. (1992). Brief dendritic calcium signals initiate long-lasting synaptic depression in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 89, 7051–7055. 10.1073/pnas.89.15.7051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreiner L., Jaeger D. (2004). Synaptic shunting by a baseline of synaptic conductances modulates responses to inhibitory input volleys in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Cerebellum 3, 112–125. 10.1080/14734220410031990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn T. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. 4 Edn. Chicago, IL: University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kulagina I. B., Korogod S. M., Horcholle-Bossavitt G., Batini C., Tyc-Dumont S. (2008). Phase relationships between calcium and voltage oscillations in different dendrites of purkinje neurons. Neurophysiol 40, 404–411. 10.1007/s11062-009-9066-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lev-Ram V., Miyakawa H., Lasser-Ross N., Ross W. N. (1992). Calcium transients in cerebellar Purkinje neurons evoked by intracellelular stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 1167–1177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis A. H., Raman I. M. (2014). Resurgent current of voltage gated Na(+) channels. J. Physiol. 592, 4825–4838. 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.277582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li W.-K., Hausknecht M. J., Stone P., Mauk M. D. (2012). Using a million cell simulation of the cerebellum: network scaling and task generality. Neural Netw. 47, 95–102. 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.11.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R. (1982). “General discussion: radial connectivity in the cerebellar cortex: a novel view regarding the functional organization of the molecular layer,” in The Cerebellum: New Vistas, (Exp. Brain Res. Suppl. Vol. 6), eds Palay S. L., Chan-Palay V. (New York: Springer Verlag; ), 189–194. [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Bloedel J. R., Hillman D. E. (1969a). Functional characterization of neuronal circuitry of frog cerebellar cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 847–870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Bloedel J. R., Roberts W. (1969b). Antidromic invasion of Purkinje cells in frog cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 881–891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Hillman D. E. (1969). “Physiological and morphological organization of cerebellar circuits in various vertebrates,” in Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development, ed. Llinas R. (Chicago, IL: American Medical Association; ), 43–76. [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Nicholson C., Freeman J. A., Hillman D. E. (1968). Dendritic spikes and their inhibition in alligator purkinje cells. Science 163, 1132–1135. 10.1126/science.160.3832.1132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Nicholson C. (1976). Reversal properties of climbing fiber potential in cat Purkinje cells: an example of a distributed synapse. J. Neurophysiol. 39, 311–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Sugimori M. (1980a). Electrophysiological properties of in vitro Purkinje cell dendrites in mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. 305, 197–213. 10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013358 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Sugimori M. (1980b). Electrophysiological properties of in vitro Purkinje cell somata in mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. 305, 171–195. 10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013357 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas R., Sugimori M. (1992). “The electrophysiology of the cerebellar Purkinje cell revisited,” in The Cerebellum Revisited, eds Llinas R., Sotelo C. (New York: Springer Verlag; ), 167–181. [Google Scholar]
- Louis E. D. (2015). Essential tremor: a common disorder of purkinje neurons. Neurscientist [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1177/1073858415590351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu H., Esquivel A. V., Bower J. M. (2009). 3D electron microscopic reconstruction of segments of rat cerebellar purkinje cell dendrites receiving ascending and parallel fiber granule cell synaptic inputs. J. Comp. Neurol. 514, 583–594. 10.1002/cne.22041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu H., Hartmann M. J., Bower J. M. (2005). Correlations between purkinje cell single-unit activity and simultaneously recorded field potentials in the immediately underlying granule cell layer. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1849–1860. 10.1152/jn.01275.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandelblat Y., Etzion Y., Grossman Y., Golomb D. (2001). Period doubling of calcium spike firing in a model of a Purkinje cell dendrite. J. Comp. Neurosci. 11, 43–62. 10.1023/A:1011252730249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manto M., Bower J. M., Conforto A. B., Delgado-García J. M., da Guarda S. N. F., Gerwig M., et al. (2012). Consensus paper: roles of the cerebellum in motor control–the diversity of ideas on cerebellar involvement in movement. Cerebellum 11, 457–487. 10.1007/s12311-011-0331-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marr D. (1969). A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. 202, 437–471. 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008820 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masoli S., Solinas S., D’Angelo E. (2015). Action potential processing in a detailed Purkinje cell model reveals a critical role for axonal compartmentalization. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 9:47. 10.3389/fncel.2015.00047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mauk M. D., Ohyama T. (2004). Extinction as new learning versus unlearning: considerations from a computer simulation of the cerebellum. Learn Mem. 11, 566–571. 10.1101/lm.83504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medina J. F., Mauk M. D. (2000). Computer simulation of cerebellar information processing. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1205–1211. 10.1038/81486 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyakawa H., Lev-Ram V., Lasser-Ross N., Ross W. N. (1992). Calcium transients evoked by climbing fiber and parallel fiber synaptic inputs in guinea pig cerebellar Purkinje neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 1178–1189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyasho T., Takagi H., Suzuki H., Watanabe S., Inoue M., Kudo Y., et al. (2001). Low-threshold potassium channels and a low-threshold calcium channel regulate Ca2+ spike firing in the dendrites of cerebellar Purkinje neurons: a modeling study. Brain Res. 891, 106–115. 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)03206-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mocanu O. D., Oliver J., Santamaria F., Bower J. M. (2000). Branching point effects on the passive properties of the cerebellar granule cell axon. Neurocomputing 32, 207–212. 10.1016/s0925-2312(00)00165-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mugnaini E. (1972). “The histology and cytology of the cerebellar cortex,” in The Comparitive Anatomy and Histology of the Cerebellum: The Human Cerebellum, Cerebellar Connections and Cerebellar Cortex, eds Larsell O., Jansen J. (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press; ), 201–262. [Google Scholar]
- Ogasawara H., Doi T., Doya K., Kawato M. (2007). Nitric oxide regulates input specificity of long-term depression and context dependence of cerebellar learning. PLoS Comput. Biol. 3:e179. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohyama T., Voicu H., Kalmbach B., Mauk M. D. (2010). A decrementing form of plasticity apparent in cerebellar learning. J. Neurosci. 30, 16993–17003. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2455-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellionisz A. (1970). Computer simulation of the pattern transfer of large cerebellar neuronal fields. Acta Biochim. Biophys. Acad. Sci. Hung. 5, 71–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellionisz A., Llinás R. (1977). A computer model of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neuroscience 2, 37–48. 10.1016/0306-4522(77)90066-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellionisz A., Szentágothai J. (1973). Dynamic single unit simulation of a realistic cerebellar network model. Brain Res. 49, 83–99. 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90403-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellionisz A., Szentágothai J. (1974). Dynamic single unit simulation of a realistic cerebellar network model. II. Purkinje cell activity within the basic circuit and modified by inhibitory systems. Brain Res. 68, 19–40. 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90531-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pugh J. R., Raman I. M. (2009). Nothing can be coincidence: synaptic inhibition and plasticity in the cerebellar nuclei. Trends Neurosci. 32, 170–177. 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rall W. (1964). “Theoretical significance of dendritic trees for neuronal input-output relations,” in Neural Theory and Modeling, ed. Reiss R. (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press; ), 73–97. [Google Scholar]
- Rall W., Shepherd G. M. (1968). Theoretical reconstruction of field potentials and dendrodendritic synaptic interactions in olfactory bulb. J. Neurophysiol. 31, 884–915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapp M., Segev I., Yarom Y. (1994). Physiology, morphology and detailed passive models of guinea-pig cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Physiol. 474, 101–118. 10.1113/jphysiol.1994.sp020006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapp M., Yarom Y., Segev I. (1992). The impact of parallel fiber background activity on the cable properties of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neural Comput. 4, 518–533. 10.1162/neco.1992.4.4.518 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Regan L. J. (1991). Voltage-dependent calcium currents in Purkinje cells from rat cerebellar vermis. J. Neurosci. 11, 2259–2269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rokni D., Llinas R., Yarom Y. (2008). Stars and stripes in the cerebellar cortex: a voltage sensitive dye study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 1:1. 10.3389/neuro.06.001.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth A., Häusser M. (2001). Compartmental models of rat cerebellar Purkinje cells based on simultaneous somatic and dendritic patch-clamp recordings. J. Physiol. 535, 445–472. 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.00445.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santamaria F., Bower J. M. (2004). Background synaptic activity modulates the response of a modeled Purkinje cell to paired afferent input. J. Neurophyiol. 93, 237–250. 10.1152/jn.00458.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santamaria F., Jaeger D., De Schutter E., Bower J. M. (2002). Modulatory effects of parallel fiber and molecular layer interneuron synaptic activity on purkinje cell responses to ascending segment input: a modeling study. J. Comput. Neurosci. 13, 217–235. 10.1023/A:1020266315730 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santamaria F., Tripp P. G., Bower J. M. (2007). Feedforward inhibition controls the spread of granule cell-induced Purkinje cell activity in the cerebellar cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 248–263. 10.1152/jn.01098.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santamaria F., Wils S., De Schutter E., Augustine G. J. (2006). Anomalous diffusion in Purkinje cell dendrites caused by spines. Neuron 52, 635–648. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santamaria F., Wils S., De Schutter E., Augustine G. J. (2011). The diffusional properties of dendrites depend on the density of dendritic spines. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 561–568. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07785.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarro L. M. (2004). Characterization of dendrites as nonlinear computation devices. Neurocomputing 58–60, 581–586. 10.1016/j.neucom.2004.01.098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Segev I., Fleshman J. W., Miller J. P., Bunow B. (1985). Modeling the electrical behavior of anatomically complex neurons using a network analysis program: passive membrane. Biol. Cybern. 53, 27–40. 10.1007/bf00355688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shelton D. P. (1985). Membrane resistivity estimated for the Purkinje neuron by means of a passive computer model. Neuroscience 14, 111–131. 10.1016/0306-4522(85)90168-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin S. L., De Schutter E. (2006). Dynamic synchronization of Purkinje cell simple spikes. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 3485–3491. 10.1152/jn.00570.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin S.-L., Hoebeek F. E., Schonewille M., De Zeeuw C. I., Aertsen A., De Schutter E. (2007). Regular patterns in cerebellar Purkinje cell simple spike trains. PLoS One 2:e485. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solinas S., Maex R., De Schutter E. (2003). Synchronization of Purkinje cell pairs along the parallel fiber axis: a model. Neurocomputing 52–54, 97–102. 10.1016/s0925-2312(02)00846-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Solinas S. M. G., Maex R., Schutter E. (2006). Dendritic amplification of inhibitory postsynaptic potentials in a model Purkinje cell. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 1207–1218. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04564.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spoelstra J., Schweighofer N., Arbib M. A. (2000). Cerebellar learning of accurate predictive control for fast-reaching movements. Biol. Cybern. 82, 321–333. 10.1007/s004220050586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staub C., De Schutter E., Knöpfel T. (1994). Voltage-imaging and simulation of effects of voltage- and agonist-activated conductances on soma-dendritic voltage coupling in cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Comput. Neurosci. 1, 301–311. 10.1007/bf00961878 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steuber V., De Schutter E. (2001). Long-term depression and recognition of parallel fibre patterns in a multi-compartmental model of a cerebellar Purkinje cell. Neurocomputing 38–40, 383–388. 10.1016/s0925-2312(01)00458-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Steuber V., De Schutter E. (2002). Rank order decoding of temporal parallel fibre input patterns in a complex Purkinje cell model. Neurocomputing 44–46, 183–188. 10.1016/s0925-2312(02)00388-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Steuber V., Mittmann W., Hoebeek F. E., Silver R. A., De Zeeuw C. I., Häusser M., et al. (2007). Cerebellar LTD and pattern recognition by Purkinje cells. Neuron 54, 121–136. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.03.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steuber V., Willshaw D. (2004). A biophysical model of synaptic delay learning and temporal pattern recognition in a cerebellar Purkinje cell. J. Comput. Neurosci. 17, 149–164. 10.1023/b:jcns.0000037678.26155.b5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sultan F., Bower J. M. (1998). Quantitative Golgi study of the rat cerebellar molecular layer interneurons using principal component analysis. J. Comp. Neurol. 393, 353–373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tada M., Nishizawa M., Onodera O. (2015). Redefining cerebellar atazia in degenerative ataxias: lessons from recent research on cerebellar systems. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 86, 922–928. 10.1136/jnnp-2013-307225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tahon K., Wijnants M., De Schutter E., Maex R. (2011). Current source density correlates of cerebellar Golgi and Purkinje cell responses to tactile input. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 1327–1341. 10.1152/jn.00317.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tank D. W., Sugimori M., Connor J. A., Llinás R. R. (1988). Spatially resolved calcium dynamics of mammalian Purkinje cells in cerebellar slice. Science 242, 773–777. 10.1126/science.2847315 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub R. D., Middleton S. J., Knöpfel T., Whittington M. A. (2008). Model of very fast (>75 Hz) network oscillations generated by electrical coupling between the proximal axons of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 1603–1616. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06477.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Geit W., Achard P., De Schutter E. (2007). Neurofitter: a parameter tuning package for a wide range of electrophysiological neuron models. Front. Neuroinform. 1:1. 10.3389/neuro.11.001.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetter P., Roth A., Häusser M. (2001). Propagation of action potentials in dendrites depends on dendritic morphology. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 926–937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vladimirescu A., Zhang K., Newton A. R., Pederson D. O., Sangiovani-Vincentelli A. (1981). SPICE. Berkeley California: University of California. [Google Scholar]
- Voogd J. (2014). What we do not know about cerebellar systems neuroscience. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:227. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vos B. P., Maex R., Volny-Luraghi A., De Schutter E. (1999). Parallel fibers synchronize spontaneous activity in cerebellar Golgi cells. J. Neurosci. 19:RC6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter J. T., Dizon M.-J., Khodakhah K. (2009). The functional equivalence of ascending and parallel fiber inputs in cerebellar computation. J. Neurosci 29, 8462–8473. 10.1523/jneurosci.5718-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y., Strahlendorf J. C., Strahlendorf H. K. (1991). A transient voltage-dependent outward potassium current in mammalian cerbellar Purkinje cells. Brain Res. 567, 153–158. 10.1016/0006-8993(91)91449-b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe S., Takagi H., Miyasho T., Inoue M., Kirino Y., Kudo Y., et al. (1998). Differential roles of two types of voltage-gated Ca2+ channels in the dendrites of rat cerebellar Purkinje neurons. Brain Res. 791, 43–55. 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00048-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womack M. D., Chevez C., Khodakhah K. (2004). Calcium-activated potassium channels are selectively coupled to P/Q-type calcium channels in cerebellar Purkinje neurons. J. Neurosci 24, 8818–8822. 10.1523/jneurosci.2915-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womack M. D., Khodakhah K. (2002a). Characterization of large conductance Ca2+-activated K+ channels in cerebellar Purkinje neurons. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 1214–1222. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02171.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womack M., Khodakhah K. (2002b). Active contribution of dendrites to the tonic and trimodal patterns of activity in cerebellar Purkinje neurons. J. Neurosci. 22, 10603–10612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womack M. D., Khodakhah K. (2004). Dendritic control of spontaneous bursting in cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Neurosci. 24, 3511–3521. 10.1523/jneurosci.0290-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamazaki T., Nagao S. (2012). A computational mechanism for unified gain and timing control in the cerebellum. PLoS One 7:e33319. 10.1371/journal.pone.0033319 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamazaki T., Tanaka S. (2007). A spiking network model for passage-of-time representation in the cerebellum. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 2279–2292. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05837.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuen G. L., Hockberger P. E., Houk J. C. (1995). Bistability in cerebellar Purkinje cell dendrites modelled with high-threshold calcium and delayed-rectifier potassium channels. Biol. Cybern. 73, 375–388. 10.1007/bf00199473 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zucker R. S. (1969). Field potentials generated by dendritic spikes and synaptic potentials. Science 165, 409–413. 10.1126/science.165.3891.409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]