Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2015 Sep 22;282(1815):20151278. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1278

Quantifying uncertainty in the phylogenetics of Australian numeral systems

Kevin Zhou 1, Claire Bowern 2,
PMCID: PMC4614748  PMID: 26378214

Abstract

Researchers have long been interested in the evolution of culture and the ways in which change in cultural systems can be reconstructed and tracked. Within the realm of language, these questions are increasingly investigated with Bayesian phylogenetic methods. However, such work in cultural phylogenetics could be improved by more explicit quantification of reconstruction and transition probabilities. We apply such methods to numerals in the languages of Australia. As a large phylogeny with almost universal ‘low-limit' systems, Australian languages are ideal for investigating numeral change over time. We reconstruct the most likely extent of the system at the root and use that information to explore the ways numerals evolve. We show that these systems do not increment serially, but most commonly vary their upper limits between 3 and 5. While there is evidence for rapid system elaboration beyond the lower limits, languages lose numerals as well as gain them. We investigate the ways larger numerals build on smaller bases, and show that there is a general tendency to both gain and replace 4 by combining 2 + 2 (rather than inventing a new unanalysable word ‘four'). We develop a series of methods for quantifying and visualizing the results.

Keywords: phylogenetics, linguistics, Australian languages, evolution, cultural reconstruction

1. Introduction

Researchers have long been interested in the evolution of culture, and the ways in which change in cultural systems can be reconstructed and tracked. Debates about the ways in which such systems change have focused on the relative frequency of horizontal diffusion versus vertical transmission. That is, are cultural traits primarily inherited, or do they spread across geographical areas [14]?

Within the realm of language and culture, these questions are increasingly investigated with Bayesian phylogenetic methods. Recent research has investigated questions in kinship [5,6], basic vocabulary [711] and grammatical patterns [12,13]. Such work uses Bayesian inference to design and test evolutionary scenarios. But results are often presented without full and explicit quantification of the uncertainties associated with models, reconstructions and trees used to formulate descent hypotheses. For example, some papers [13] use tree samples for their phylogenetic trait analysis, yet present only a single model for transition probabilities, without quantifying the support for that model or discussing alternatives. Such work, important though it is, could be improved by more explicit quantification of reconstruction and transition probabilities.

We address these issues by developing a series of methods for quantifying and visualizing results of ancestral trait reconstruction. We test them using data from numeral system reconstruction in Australian languages of the Pama-Nyungan family [14]. Numeral systems across the world show great variation [1517], but within Australia, the systems tend to be low limit (with an upper bound of less than 20, and frequently less than 5). However, while there is variation in both the composition and the extent of the systems, that variation is highly constrained. Low-limit numeral systems are not unique to Australian languages, but are characteristic of hunter–gatherer societies throughout the world, which presumably have a lesser functional need for a more general system [15]. Variation in Australian numeral systems stems not only from the systems' upper extents but also from whether larger numbers are composed of smaller numbers (e.g. 3 = 2 + 1, 4 = 2 + 2, etc.; compare English ‘twenty-five’) or show unanalysable, opaque etymology (such as English ‘three’ and ‘four’) [15,16]. While previous work [16,17] recognizes some variation in the extent of low-limit numeral systems, there has not been any investigation into how low-limit systems change over time, particularly with respect to their upper limits. Do such systems gradually increment, for example? And can such systems lose numerals, and revise their upper limits downwards? Finally, Australian languages are also claimed to show extensive horizontal transfer of features [18], but Bayesian phylogenetic trait analyses allow us to investigate the level of phylogenetic signal in a dataset. All these factors make Australian numeral systems an ideal test case for phylogenetic investigation.

2. Methods

(a). Numeral systems

We investigate two questions about the evolution of Australia's numeral systems. First, we explore the phylogenetics of numeral system limits across the Pama-Nyungan family (the most populous family of Australian languages, covering almost 90% of the land mass and containing approx. 70% of Australian languages). Bowern & Zentz [14] show that upper limits vary from 3 to 20, with the median at 4. We investigate whether limits are structured phylogenetically. Within this domain, we test four hypotheses about numeral evolution. One hypothesis is that languages add numerals incrementally over time. If this is so, we would expect the root (Proto-Pama-Nyungan) to reconstruct to 2, and for numeral extent to increase serially approaching the tips. A second hypothesis is that upper limits are random, or the products of local system enhancement. A third hypothesis is that languages with low-limit numeral systems may lose numeral terms over time (an alternative hypothesis to hypothesis 1 that limits strictly increase). A fourth hypothesis is that languages might borrow numerals from one another, thus leading to horizontal transfer of features of numeral systems as well as of the words used in those systems.

Secondly, we investigate whether numeral creation and replacement evolves phylogenetically. Languages vary in whether their numerals are compositional or opaque. Compositional numerals are formed from two other numbers (e.g. English ‘twenty-one’ = ‘twenty’ + ‘one’). In the Wiradjuri language, for example, 3 is buula-nguunbaay (‘two’ + ‘one’) and 4 is buula-buula (‘two’ + ‘two’). There is substantial variation across the languages, however, as to whether both 3 and 4 are compositional or opaque, and if only one is compositional, which numeral shows the compositional form. We investigate this further by comparing evolutionary models where the form of 4 is dependent on the form of 3, versus models where the two numerals are assumed to change independently. We further hypothesize that opaque numerals will tend to be renewed or replaced by compositional forms.

(b). Bayesian statistics

Bayesian phylogenetic methods are relatively new to the study of linguistic prehistory, serving as a complement to the comparative method [19,20]. They were adapted from methods developed for evolutionary biology upon the realization that many of the properties of evolution have direct parallels in language change [7,21]. These statistical methods, particularly Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC [22]), have been successful in verifying conclusions drawn from more traditional linguistic methods as well as offering support for deeper and less obvious relationships (e.g. [10,11]). As is well known, MCMC numerically approximates the posterior distribution of the parameters given the data, which is often intractable to calculate analytically. It involves searching the n-dimensional space of possible parameter values, where n is the number of parameters that attempt to describe the data. Parameter vectors that explain the data well, based on the likelihood and prior distributions, will be visited frequently; those that poorly explain the data will be visited less often. In this way, the stationary distribution of the Markov chain informs us of the range of possible parameter values.

Here, we are interested in transition rates between the different states (i.e. numeral extent or opacity) throughout the language tree, as well as the state reconstructions at various nodes within the tree, which are represented as probability mass functions, allocating different probabilities to each state in proportion to the degree of their support. We use an extension of MCMC known as Reversible Jump MCMC (RJMCMC; [23], see [24] for its introduction to (and development in) evolutionary biology), which allows the number of parameters to change. This technique is particularly suited for evolutionary models with a large number of possible states. RJMCMC traverses not only the parameter space but also the model space. In this way, models that poorly explain the data are visited less often.

In our case, the parameters are transition rates between the different states. Given a set of k independent traits, there are k(k − 1) transition rates. However, calculating all of the transition rates is not only computationally demanding but also unnecessary because it leads to overparametrization. To reduce the number of transition rates, we can either delete rates or restrict them to the values of other rates, but there is no systematic method of determining these deletions and groupings without external knowledge. RJMCMC overcomes this issue by allowing the Markov chain to ‘jump’ from one dimension to another by deleting or grouping parameters together in addition to varying them. As with conventional MCMC, such jumps are more likely to occur if the candidate parameter vector better fits the data. Hence, we can draw conclusions about the different transition rates based on, for example, how often they are deleted.

To model numerical system extent, we use BayesTraits [25] to infer ancestral states at different nodes. To investigate correlated evolution for numerical opacity, we examine two classes of models, one that assumes independent evolution and one that assumes dependent evolution, which we then compare via the Bayes factor (BF) [26]. Fitting the data to both models and comparing their posterior likelihoods via the BF indicates whether coevolution is favoured. We adopt the RJMCMC approach, but we modify the coevolution model of opacity of ‘3’ and ‘4’ by introducing two new paired states (electronic supplementary material figures S1 and S4). These are required because the opacity of ‘4’ is ternary—opaque, compositional, or not attested. We retain the opacity of ‘3’ as a binary term because no languages have a term for ‘4’ but not for ‘3’.

(c). Representation of RJMCMC reconstructions

Given that each RJMCMC run fits the data to multiple models, there is potential for the state reconstructions to vary more significantly than those deduced by conventional MCMC methods, as we shall see. For example, there might be two significantly different sets of models that both explain the data well. As such, representing the uncertainty in the reconstructions using a simple mean and confidence interval in place of a histogram of the reconstructions would obscure the true nature of the reconstructions. Here, we propose a new method that uses the Shannon entropy of the reconstructions as a sorting criterion. Briefly, the entropy of a discrete trait X with a probability mass reconstruction p(x) is given by H(X) = −Σxp(x)logkp(x). If p(x) = 0 for some x, then p(x)logkp(x) is defined to be 0. In most applications, k = 2 and the units are in bits; however, for convenience we assume k to be the number of possible values X can take so that the entropy is bounded between 0 and 1. Entropy has many interpretations depending on the application, but here it is useful to think of it as how certain the reconstruction is. For example, an entropy of 0 indicates that one particular value of X has a probability of 1, meaning that the reconstruction is completely certain. On the other hand, a maximum entropy of 1 indicates that the reconstruction is uniform across all the states—that is, it is completely uncertain.

By sorting reconstructions based on their entropies, we achieve a better sense of the diversity of the reconstructions returned by RJMCMC. Furthermore, as we will see, this representation is more compact than using histograms and provides more information. Generating one-dimensional histograms is an inherently marginalizing procedure—in other words, it discards any joint relationships. Our procedure displays the joint reconstruction and simultaneously visualizes all the reconstructions given by the Markov chain. Also of interest is the distribution of the entropies themselves, the shape of which conveys the overall degree of certainty the procedure was able to confer. Hence, we weighted each sorted entropy by how frequently each reconstruction of that entropy appeared. Doing so accentuates the reconstructions and the entropies that appear most often. Estimates of the entropy density were given by spline interpolation of histograms.

(d). Analysis of rates

To investigate the transition rates that figure importantly in the analyses, we rank the rates. Ranking rates by their speed across the MCMC chain allows us to quantify which rates are relatively fast: for each state in the RJMCMC chain, we ranked the rates from fastest to slowest and tabulated the proportion of the chain each rate was fastest. We also plot (for comparison) the proportion of runs for which each rate is deleted. Doing so exploits the joint Bayesian relationship among all the parameters, while merely comparing the mean rate, as some authors have previously done [24], ignores the joint relationship.

3. Results

(a). Extent

Figure 1 displays the entropy-sorted reconstructions at various ancestral nodes throughout the tree. The reconstructions vary in certainty at any given node and across all nodes. The closer to zero the reconstructions cluster on the x-axis, the lower the entropy and the greater the confidence in the reconstruction. For example, the Mayi and Ngayarta subgroup node reconstructions have entropies approaching 0, where an extent of four and three (respectively) are confidently reconstructed. On the other hand, the Central New South Wales node is not as confidently reconstructed, as illustrated by the relatively high entropy levels, even though reconstructions of an extent of 4 predominate.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Entropy plots for the Pama-Nyungan root and major subgroups (that is, selected internal tree nodes), plotted on a consensus tree derived from Bowern & Atkinson [10]. Major subgroups of the family are labelled. Internal node labels give the posterior probabilities for the nodes. Colours for the reconstructions are dark blue, 3; light blue, 4, green, 5; yellow, 6; red, 7; brown, 8+. Upper limit on gamma-distributed rate prior = 2.

Marrngu provides an illustration of conflicting models. Lower entropy reconstructions favour a limit of three, but the peak density for reconstructions indicates greater overall support for limit-five, although with less certainty. Likewise, Paman ancestral state reconstructions suggest two solutions; either limit-three or limit-five. Since reconstructions of limit-three are allocated a higher posterior probability across the entropy range, they are to be preferred. The root (Proto-Pama-Nyungan) reconstruction appears to be limit-four. Certainty of the reconstructions is affected by the degree of posterior support for the particular node in the tree. As Pagel et al. [25] put it, ‘the probability that the node exists puts an upper limit on how certain one can ever be about the ancestral state of that node’. Most of the nodes under study here have probabilities in the sample at or close to 1; however, it should be borne in mind that trait reconstruction confidence can never exceed the probability of the node it is assigned to.

Low-limit numeral systems do not only increase their limits over time, and they do not simply increase limits serially. Languages can also lose numerals, supporting hypothesis 3, and contrary to strong versions of hypotheses 1 and 2. In the Central subgroup of Pama-Nyungan, for example, the most likely reconstruction is limit-four, but Karnic, one of the daughter subgroups, has substantial support for limit-three. Several subgroups, including Paman and Western Pama-Nyungan, show support for a reconstruction limit of three, in comparison to the root, with limit-four.

Transition rates provide further information about which changes in extent are particularly common. A transition rate from numeral extent x to y is denoted by qxy. Thus, q34 is the transition rate from state 3 to state 4 (or, in real-world terms, the transition from a language having three numerals to having four). Rates that are frequently deleted have less importance in the model. We rank rates by speed and plot two pieces of information: the proportion of runs in which the rate is the fastest, and the proportion in which the rate is deleted (figure 2). Three rates stand out as particularly fast: q34, q35 and q43. This implies that in our data, limited elaboration of the numeral system is common (from extent 3 to extent 4 or 5), as is some simplification (from extent 5 to 4). These results also imply that strict stepwise elaboration of low-limit systems is not necessary, given the rate of extent elaboration from 3 to 5.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Rank frequencies of transition rates for numeral extent, showing (in dashed red) the proportion of samples in which that rate is the fastest and (in solid blue) the proportion of samples in which the rate is deleted. Rates are given in the form qxy, which denotes the estimate rate of evolution from trait value x to trait value y. (Online version in colour.)

The next fastest rate is q49. This perhaps implies that there is a threshold for numerical complexity (that is, if elaboration beyond 4 or 5 is found, significant rather than marginal elaboration is found). This accords with research on acquisition of numerals [27,28], which shows that children encode the numerals involved in subitizing (1–3) earlier. They map small numerosities to specific lexical items and only later generalize to higher numerals [27]. Rates q37, q47 and q57 are overall slow and are most frequently deleted. Stepwise rates (e.g. q34, q45) are, on the whole, faster than rates where limits increase or decrease non-incrementally (e.g. q35, q24), implying that while hypothesis 1 is false, stepwise incrementation is still more common than non-stepwise elaboration. These data are consistent with a situation in which numeral extent rates fluctuate stochastically, but where variation in numeral extent is mostly (though not entirely) concentrated around accretion and deletion of numerals between 3 and 5. Hypothesis 3, that languages can lose numerals, even in low-limit systems, is thus well-supported in our dataset.

The final hypothesis concerns the borrowing of numeral systems. The data suggest elaboration of numeral systems in the languages of the South subgroup (Kulin, Waka-Kabi, Central NSW and Lower Murray). While there is no direct evidence from the forms of the numerals that borrowing has taken place, it is striking that the only subgroup outside the Southern clade with numeral extent above 5 is Thura-Yura, a subgroup that is directly adjacent to the Lower Murray languages. Wati, the subgroup with the next highest extent reconstruction, borders Thura-Yura to the west. This implies that horizontal transmission may well play a role in the elaboration of numeral systems, supporting hypothesis 4.

(b). Opacity

We next consider the internal structure of numeral systems. As described above, languages vary in the extent to which higher numerals are built from smaller numerals; that is, whether they are compositional, or opaque. One obvious question is whether the compositionality of higher numerals depends on that of lower numerals; for example, if the word for ‘4’ is based on 2 + 2, does that imply that the word for ‘3’ is also compositional (based on 2 + 1)? Compositionality changes over time, through a process known in linguistics as grammaticalization [29]. As shown in electronic supplementary material, figure S1, all possible combinations of opaque and compositional numerals exist in our dataset, though at unequal frequencies.

To test for correlated evolution of compositionality, we ran two models. In the first, numerals were assumed to evolve independently. In the second, they were assumed to evolve dependently. The BF was then compared to evaluate the extent of support for one model compared with the other. A BF range of 7.8–10.4, verified from three independent Markov chains, indicates that the dependent model is superior to the independent model, offering strong support for correlated evolution between the opacity of ‘3’ and ‘4’. Electronic supplementary material, figures S9 and S10 give the entropy-sorted reconstructions at ancestral nodes using both models.

If we compare the reconstructions produced by the dependent and independent models, we note that some reconstructions are similar (e.g. Paman, Kalkatungic) while others are different (e.g. Root, Kulin and Mayi). The South node is interesting in that although state 1 (compositional 3 and 4) has the greatest a posteriori probability under both models, the dependent model sometimes gives state 5 (i.e. compositional 3 and absent 4) as a strong alternative. Furthermore, the entropy ranges differ; in general, it appears that the dependent model produces reconstructions with a broader range of entropies, often extending closer to 0. It also tends to produce results without a clear reconstruction across entropies. Furthermore, if we compare the reconstructions with those considering extent data (without regard for numeral etymology, from figure 1 above), we see that there is overall consistency between the two sets of reconstructions, and subgroups reconstructed with extent 3 tend to be reconstructed with absent 4. Within Western Pama-Nyungan, for example, both Warluwaric and Yolngu reconstruct with high probability to extent-3 numeral systems. For Warluwaric, compositional 3/absent 4 is the most likely node value, while for Yolngu opaque 3/absent 4 is most likely.

To get a sense of the mechanism of correlated evolution, we can compare the various transition rates of the dependent model. Figure 3 summarizes the reconstructed transition rates in the dependent model, showing particular dependencies. Arrows are weighted by frequency of the rate occurring in the sample. Therefore, the thickest arrows (e.g. between 0,0 and 0,1 where opaque 4 becomes compositional) show the transitions that are most relevant for the model.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Summary of the transition rates with support for a model of dependent trait evolution for numeral opacity. This information summarizes graphically the same information in electronic supplementary material, figure S14, which gives relative speeds of all rates. Arrows are weighted by the transition speed, with fastest rates the heaviest. 0 denotes a compositional form, 1 an opaque one. Thus 0,0 is the state in which both numerals 3 and 4 are compositional; 0,1 is a state with compositional 3 but opaque 4 and so forth. Solid line denotes a missing state (these are languages with upper limit of three numerals, and hence no form for 4).

The data provide evidence for the ways in which languages both lose and gain numerals. The three fastest rates involve the creation of a compositional 4 either through replacing an earlier opaque 4 or incrementing a smaller system. Note, however, that an opaque 4 may be also be both gained or lost. This behaviour is in contrast to 3 which, in our data, becomes opaque only if 4 is also opaque. This is presumably in large part because compositional 3 is usually ‘2 + 1’; this leads to a pressure for 4 to be compositional in base 2 as well (i.e. ‘2 + 2’; see further discussion of this by Bowern and Zentz [14]). These findings make sense in a model of language change where new elements in a numeral system are created and renewed analogically to other numerals. An alternative would be where speakers of languages recruit opaque terms from other domains [15]. This is the usual source of words for ‘5’, which frequently originate in words for ‘hand’ in Pama-Nyungan languages (infrequently, words for 5 based on ‘3 + 2’ are found). This is not the dominant mode of evolution of terms for 4 in Pama-Nyungan languages, however. These data also imply a cycle of grammaticalization where numerals (particularly 4) tend to be innovated compositionally, grammaticalized or fossilized so that their origin becomes opaque to their speakers, then lost or replaced by a new compositional form.

Compositional 3 (e.g. 2 + 1) and opaque 4 seems to be an unstable state, with fast transition rates leading to loss of 4, replacement of opaque 4 with compositional 4, or replacement of compositional 3 with opaque 3. Finally, 4 may be gained or lost, whether 3 is opaque or compositional. This implies that the upper limit of a numeral system is, at least to some extent, independent of numeral etymology, as one would expect if these are true numerals. If the numeral words in these languages are ad hoc compounds rather than true numerals, we would expect there to be a clearer dependency between the presence of a compositional 3 and a compositional 4.

4. Conclusion

In conclusion, entropy sorting provides a convenient way to visualize the relative likelihoods of reconstructions of multistate data at different nodes in the phylogeny. Both entropy plots and rate comparisons allow a way to evaluate the outcomes of Bayesian phylogenetic analysis. We use this approach to investigate the phylogenetics of small numeral systems in Australia. We reconstruct the most likely extent of the system at the root and use that information to explore the ways in which numeral systems evolve. We show that the mode of evolution among low-limit numeral systems in Australia is variation between limits of 3, 4 and 5, and that languages may both lose and gain numerals across time. We provide evidence for a rapid elaboration of numerals above 5, and that there is some evidence that higher numeral extents have spread areally. Below extent 5, however, languages fluctuate in their upper limits across time. These findings argue against a model of numeral evolution which begins with gradual incrementation or elaboration. We investigate the etymologies of numerals and show that numeral 4 can be lost or gained independently of the structure of 3, numeral 4 is usually innovated compositionally.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary materials
rspb20151278supp1.docx (6.3MB, docx)

Acknowledgements

Thanks to members of the Pama-Nyungan/Historical Language Lab at Yale, and to Mark Pagel.

Data accessibility

See electronic supplementary materials for coded datasets and references to original sources.

Authors' contributions

C.B. and K.Z. developed the research; K.Z. wrote the code for the analysis; C.B. sourced data; K.Z. and C.B. analysed the data; C.B. and K.Z. wrote the paper.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

This work was funded by NSF grants BCS-0844550 and BCS-1423711.

References

  • 1.Kroeber AL. 1940. Stimulus diffusion. Am. Anthropol. 42, 1–20. ( 10.1525/aa.1940.42.1.02a00020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kroeber AL. 1948. Anthropology: race, language, culture, psychology, prehistory. New York, NY: Harcourt-Brace. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mace R, Holden CJ. 2005. A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 116–121. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2004.12.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mace R, Jordan FM. 2011. Macro-evolutionary studies of cultural diversity: a review of empirical studies of cultural transmission and cultural adaptation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 402–411. ( 10.1098/rstb.2010.0238) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jordan FM, Gray RD, Greenhill SJ, Mace R. 2009. Matrilocal residence is ancestral in Austronesian societies. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 1957–1964. ( 10.1098/rspb.2009.0088) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fortunato L, Jordan F. 2010. Your place or mine? A phylogenetic comparative analysis of marital residence in Indo-European and Austronesian societies. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 365, 3913–3922. ( 10.1098/rstb.2010.0017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Atkinson QD, Gray RD. 2005. Curious parallels and curious connections—phylogenetic thinking in biology and historical linguistics. Syst. Biol. 54, 513–526. ( 10.1080/10635150590950317) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bouckaert R, Lemey P, Dunn M, Greenhill SJ, Alekseyenko AV, Drummond AJ, Gray RD, Suchard MA, Atkinson QD. 2012. Mapping the origins and expansion of the Indo-European language family. Science 337, 957–960. ( 10.1126/science.1219669) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Holden CJ. 2002. Bantu language trees reflect the spread of farming across sub-Saharan Africa: a maximum-parsimony analysis. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 793–799. ( 10.1098/rspb.2002.1955) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bowern C, Atkinson Q. 2012. Computational phylogenetics and the internal structure of Pama-Nyungan. Language 88, 817–845. ( 10.1353/lan.2012.0081) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gray RD, Drummond AJ, Greenhill SJ. 2009. Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and pauses in Pacific settlement. Science 323, 479–483. ( 10.1126/science.1166858) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dunn M, Terrill A, Reesink G, Foley RA, Levinson SC. 2005. Structural phylogenetics and the reconstruction of ancient language history. Science 309, 2072–2075. ( 10.1126/science.1114615) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dunn M, Greenhill SJ, Levinson SC, Gray RD. 2011. Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature 473, 79–82. ( 10.1038/nature09923) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bowern C, Zentz J. 2012. Numeral systems in Australian languages. Anthropol. Linguist. 54, 130–166. ( 10.1353/anl.2012.0008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Epps P, Bowern C, Hansen C, Hill J, Zentz J. 2012. On numeral complexity in hunter–gatherer languages. Linguist. Typol. 16, 41–109. ( 10.1515/lity-2012-0002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hanke T. 2010. Additional rarities in the typology of numerals. In Rethinking universals: how rarities affect linguistic theory (eds Wohlgemuth J, Cysouw M), pp. 61–90. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Comrie B. 2005. Numeral bases. In The world atlas of language structures (eds Haspelmath M, Dryer MS, Gil D, Comrie B), pp. 530–533. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dixon RMW. 2002. Australian languages: their nature and development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hock HH, Joseph BD. 2009. Language history, language change, and language relationship. In An introduction to historical and comparative linguistics. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rankin RL. 2008. The comparative method. In The handbook of historical linguistics (eds Joseph BD, Janda RD), pp. 199–212. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jordan FM, Huber BR. 2013. Evolutionary approaches to cross-cultural anthropology. Cross-Cult. Res. 47, 91–101. ( 10.1177/1069397112471800) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Metropolis N, Rosenbluth AW, Rosenbluth MN, Teller AH, Teller E. 1953. Equation of state calculations by fast computing machines. J. Chem. Phys. 21, 1087–1092. ( 10.1063/1.1699114) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Green PJ. 1995. Reversible jump Markov chain Monte Carlo computation and Bayesian model determination. Biometrika 82, 711–732. ( 10.1093/biomet/82.4.711) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pagel M, Meade A. 2006. Bayesian analysis of correlated evolution of discrete characters by reversible-jump Markov chain Monte Carlo. Am. Nat. 167, 808–825. ( 10.1086/503444) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pagel M, Meade A, Barker D. 2004. Bayesian estimation of ancestral character states on phylogenies. Syst. Biol. 53, 673–684. ( 10.1080/10635150490522232) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Pagel M, Meade A. 2004. A phylogenetic mixture model for detecting pattern-heterogeneity in gene sequence or character-state data. Syst. Biol. 53, 571–581. ( 10.1080/10635150490468675) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wynn K. 1992. Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognit. Psychol. 24, 220–251. ( 10.1016/0010-0285(92)90008-P) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Benoit L, Lehalle H, Jouen F. 2004. Do young children acquire number words through subitizing or counting? Cognit. Dev. 19, 291–307. ( 10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.03.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Saitou N, Nei M. 1987. The neighbor-joining method: a new method for reconstructing phylogenetic trees. Mol. Biol. Evol. 4, 406–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary materials
rspb20151278supp1.docx (6.3MB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

See electronic supplementary materials for coded datasets and references to original sources.


Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES