Abstract
Memory decline is one of the greatest health threats of the twenty-first century. Because of the widespread increase in life expectancy, 20 percent of the global population will be over 60 in 2050 and the problems caused by age-related memory loss will be dramatically aggravated. However, the molecular mechanisms underlying this inevitable process are not well understood. Here we show that the activity of the recently discovered mechanistic target of rapamycin (mTOR) complex 2 (mTORC2) declines with age in the brain of both fruit flies and rodents and that the loss of mTORC2-mediated actin polymerization contributes to age-associated memory loss. Intriguingly, treatment with a small molecule that activates mTORC2 (A-443654) reverses long-term memory (LTM) deficits in both aged mice and flies. In addition, we found that pharmacologically boosting either mTORC2 or actin polymerization enhances LTM. In contrast to the current approaches to enhance memory that have primarily targeted the regulation of gene expression (epigenetic, transcriptional, and translational), our data points to a novel, evolutionarily conserved mechanism for restoring memory that is dependent on structural plasticity. These insights into the molecular basis of age-related memory loss may hold promise for new treatments for cognitive disorders.
During the past century, the global population has witnessed a dramatic increase in life expectancy1. Because individuals are living much longer, cognitive decline has emerged as one of the greatest health threats of old age. Currently, no treatment is available to reverse or delay age-associated cognitive decline. Without an effective intervention, it is estimated that, by 2040, over 90 million people will experience age-mediated memory deficits2. Thus, the development of novel and efficient cognitive enhancers to treat these disorders is of crucial importance. However, in order to develop a therapy that can restore memory in the aging brain, we must first have a better understanding of the molecular and neuronal mechanisms underlying age-associated memory impairment.
Normal aging of the human brain is accompanied by robust and progressive alterations in cognition, mood and motor function3,4. A decline in these behaviors with age is highly correlated with structural and neurophysiological changes in the brain5. Previous studies have shown that the aging brain undergoes a non-uniform loss of grey and white matter volumes, including accelerated shrinkage of the hippocampus and entorhinal cortices4,6. Interestingly, these morphological changes are not a result of increased neuronal death, but rather due to shrinkage of neuronal dendritic arbors and loss of synapses7,8. The question remains whether dysregulated molecular signaling pathways in specific cognitive domains can elicit age-associated structural changes.
Although the evolutionarily conserved mechanistic target of rapamycin (mTOR) has been implicated in aging9, its role in brain aging remains unclear. mTOR forms two functionally distinct complexes. The first complex, mTORC1, consisting of mTOR, Raptor and mLST8 (GβL), is sensitive to the immunosuppressant rapamycin and regulates mRNA translation rates10,11. The second complex, mTORC2, which was recently discovered, is largely insensitive to rapamycin and consists of mTOR, mSIN1, mLST8 and Rictor12,13,14. Although much less is known about its down-stream effectors and up-stream regulation, mTORC2 has been shown to regulate the actin cytoskeleton and seems to play an important role in brain function15,16,17. Indeed, by regulating actin polymerization, mTORC2 controls the structural changes at synapses that are necessary for memory consolidation15. Furthermore, mTORC2 activity is altered in several age-associated cognitive disorders, including Alzheimer’s disease18 and Parkinson’s disease19, in which age is the major risk factor.
Current approaches to reverse memory loss and enhance memory in the aged population have focused on changes in gene expression at the epigenetic20,21 and transcriptional22 levels. However, changes in synaptic actin polymerization are also crucially involved in memory formation23,24,25. Given that a) human brain aging is associated with memory loss3,4,5, b) specific regions of the aging brain exhibit reduced synaptic connectivity7,8, c) inhibition of mTORC2 decreases life span26 and d) mTORC2 regulates structural changes required for memory consolidation15,16,17, we investigated the role of mTORC2 deficiency as a novel mechanism of age-associated memory loss, and the therapeutic potential of mTORC2 as a target for the treatment of memory loss in aged animals.
Results
TORC2 activity decreases with age in flies
TORC2 promotes LTM storage and long-lasting changes in synaptic function15. Given the successful use of Drosophila as a model of age-related disorders27,28,29, we first decided to utilize the fly to investigate the mechanisms that contribute to age-associated memory loss. We determined by Western blotting the levels of Drosophila TORC2 (dTORC2) activity in the brain of wild-type flies at different ages by measuring the phosphorylation of Akt at Ser505, an established readout of dTORC2 activity30. We found that, compared to the brain of young (3 day old) flies, the brain of aged (21 day old) flies contained significantly less phosphorylated Akt (Ser505) (Figs. 1A-B). Hence, dTORC2 activity is reduced in the brain of aged flies.
Aged flies are selectively impaired in long-lasting memory
A decline in LTM is a consequence of aging that is shared between humans and flies29,31,32,33. Given that the mechanisms underlying LTM formation in flies are highly conserved across species34, we investigated the effect of age on different temporal phases of memory in flies. We therefore tested memory in young and aged flies using olfactory classical conditioning, a well-studied form of associative learning35. In this paradigm, flies were trained to associate an odor, the conditioned stimulus (CS), with an aversive electric shock, the unconditioned stimulus (US). This conditioned memory can be dissected into different phases: a short-term memory and two forms of long-lasting memory, anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM)—that lasts only a few days— and a more persistent long-term memory (LTM)36. ARM is formed after either a single associative learning trial or several consecutive trials (massed protocol) whereas LTM is formed only after multiple spaced training trials. Unlike ARM, LTM is protein synthesis - dependent36,37. We observed a significant impairment of both ARM and LTM (Figs. 1D-E) in aged flies, though their short-term memory (STM, measured 3 min post-training) was not affected (Fig. 1C). Consistent with previous findings32,33, aged flies have a selective impairment in long-lasting memories.
Direct activation of dTORC2 and actin polymerization restores LTM in aged flies
If the reduced dTORC2 activity in the brains of aged flies is responsible for their cognitive decline, then its up-regulation activity should ameliorate these memory deficits. Indeed, administration of a small molecule that activates mTORC2-mediated p-Akt (Ser473) (A-443654)38 markedly restored LTM performance in aged flies (Fig. 2A). The improved LTM storage was accompanied by a significant increase in dTORC2 activity levels in aged wild-type flies (Figs. 2B-C), but not in dTORC2-deficient flies (rictorΔ1; Supplementary Fig. S1A online), as measured by the phosphorylation of Akt at Ser505. Furthermore, A-443654 treatment failed to restore LTM in aged rictorΔ1 mutants (see Supplementary Fig. S1B online) and ARM in aged WT flies (Fig. 2D), demonstrating that A-443654 specifically restores LTM in aged flies through the activation of dTORC2. Importantly, A-443654 treatment had no effect on sensory acuity, since both vehicle and drug-treated groups showed normal avoidance of the electric shock and the odorants 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH) and 3-octanol (OCT; see Supplementary Fig. S2 online).
mTORC2 has recently been shown to promote actin-dynamic mediated changes in synaptic strength and LTM formation15. Given the reduction in dTORC2 activity in aged flies (Figs. 1A-B), we predicted that actin polymerization should be reduced in aged flies and that jasplakinolide (JPK), a compound that directly promotes actin polymerization39, should also restore LTM in these flies. Indeed, similar to treatment with A-443654, administration of JPK restored LTM in aged flies (Fig. 2E). Taken together, reduced dTORC2-mediated actin polymerization in aged flies contributes, at least in part, to the LTM impairment, which can be reversed by increasing either dTORC2 or actin polymerization pharmacologically.
Direct activation of dTORC2 and actin polymerization enhances LTM in young flies
In young flies with normal levels of dTORC2, direct pharmacological activation of dTORC2 signaling might not further enhance LTM generated by a strong training protocol (five training sessions with a 15-min rest interval between each). Consistent with this prediction, increasing dTORC2 activity via A-443654 had no effect on LTM in young flies (see Supplementary Fig. S3 online), suggesting that the effect of A-443654 in flies is age-specific. However, direct activation of dTORC2 signaling in young flies given a weak training protocol, which normally does not elicit LTM, might convert relatively short-lasting memories into long-lasting ones. In young flies, a weak training protocol generates a weak memory (Fig. 3C). As we predicted, young flies treated with A-443654 and the same weak training protocol exhibited increased dTORC2 activity in the brain (Figs. 3A-B) and enhanced LTM (Fig. 3C) compared to vehicle-treated controls. These results indicate that a transient activation of dTORC2 activity promotes LTM formation and counteracts cognitive decline in the adult animal. Finally, promoting actin polymerization with JPK also significantly enhanced LTM in young flies given a weak training protocol (Fig. 3D). These data support the notion that direct activation of dTORC2-mediated actin polymerization enhances LTM formation.
Direct activation of mTORC2 restores LTM in aged- mice
Because mTORC2 and its role in memory consolidation are evolutionarily conserved15, we wondered whether deficient mTORC2 also contributes to age-related memory impairment in mice. First, we measured the activity of mTORC2 in the hippocampus of young (3 month old) and old (18 month old) mice. Matching our findings in flies, mTORC2 activity—as measured by the levels of phosphorylated Akt at Ser47330, was indeed decreased in the hippocampus, a key brain region implicated in LTM formation, of aged mice compared to young mice (Figs. 4A-B).
We next tested LTM in young and aged mice using contextual fear conditioning, a form of Pavlovian learning40. In this behavioral assay, a mouse learns to associate an aversive stimulus (US), such as a mild foot shock, with a salient environmental cue (CS), such as the context of the test chamber. The mouse is reintroduced into the test chamber 24 hours after training and fear responses (“freezing”) were recorded. The percentage of time spent freezing was taken as an index of the strength of the association between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. Young and aged mice showed similar amounts of freezing behavior before training (naïve). However, when examined 24 h after training, contextual fear LTM was significantly impaired in aged mice (Fig. 4C), consistent with previous reports4,41,42,43. Remarkably, aged mice injected intraperitoneally with A-443654 immediately after training froze significantly more than vehicle-injected controls, indicating that their contextual LTM was ameliorated (Fig. 4D). Such injections of A-443654 significantly increased mTORC2 activity in the hippocampus of aged mice compared to vehicle-injected controls (Figs. 4E-F). Hence, restoration of TORC2 activity functions in an evolutionarily conserved manner to restore the LTM decline in both flies and mice.
Discussion
Normal aging of the brain is associated with consistent alterations in cognition3,4, but the molecular mechanisms underlying age-related changes in the brain are not well understood. Our findings suggest that mTORC2 deficiency plays a crucial role in age-dependent memory impairment in both flies and mice. We found that mTORC2 activity is significantly reduced in the brain of aged flies and mice (Figs. 1A and 4A). Remarkably, acute pharmacological activation of mTORC2 by A-443654 is sufficient to reverse LTM impairments in both model systems (Figs. 2A and 4D). Interestingly, A-443654 increased mTORC2 activity, as determined by p-Akt (Ser473), in aged mice to comparable baseline levels in young mice (Figs. 4E-F). The comparison of freezing scores in Figs. 4C-D indicate that A-443654 fully restores LTM performance in aged mice.
In aged flies, A-443654 increased dTORC2 activity to levels comparable to those in young flies (Figs. 2B-C). While A-443654 restored LTM in aged flies, this did not reach the LTM performance of young flies (Fig. 2A). We speculate that, given that A-443654 is fed to aged flies after a starvation period (and prior to training), starvation of aged flies may prevent a complete rescue of LTM. Alternatively, there could be additional signaling pathways required in aged flies to boost LTM performance. Given that human brain aging is accompanied by memory loss and reduced synaptic connectivity3,4,7,8 and the molecular mechanisms underlying memory formation are evolutionarily conserved34, deficient mTORC2 activity may account, at least in part, for the age-associated inability to consolidate long-term memories. If so, drugs such as A-443654, that promote mTORC2 activity, could ameliorate some of the cognitive deficits associated with aging.
The small molecule A-443654 was initially discovered to be a potent inhibitor of Akt targets and tumor growth in a number of in vivo tumor models38,44. At the doses required to inhibit tumor growth, significant inhibition of the Akt pathway was observed. Interestingly, A-443654 causes an mTORC2-mediated hyperphosphorylation of Akt at Ser47338. Although we cannot rule out this possibility, it is unlikely that A-443654 restores LTM formation due to the abrogation of Akt-mTORC1 pathway given that previous studies have shown that this signaling pathway is crucial for LTM formation45,46. Indeed, mTORC1 activity was significantly decreased in the hippocampus of aged mice (see Supplementary Fig. S4 online) and A-443654 had no effect on mTORC1 activity38. More importantly, in the absence of mTORC2, A-443654 lost the ability to enhance memory (see Supplementary Fig. S1 online)15, indicating that the memory-enhancing effects of A-443654 are mTORC2-mediated.
A small subset of key signaling pathways can be genetically manipulated to enhance cognition in normal animals47. Our findings show that memory can also be enhanced in both normal and pathological states by targeting proteins involved in synaptic structural changes such as mTORC2 and actin dynamics. Previous evidence indicates that actin polymerization is required for LTM formation and functions to modify neural synaptic transmission and morphology, resulting in long-lasting changes in synaptic efficacy and behavior24. We postulate that in response to neuronal activation, an increase in mTORC2-mediated actin polymerization allows for structural rearrangements and the trafficking and insertion of AMPA receptors at the post-synaptic density. Consistent with this idea, actin polymerization and spine density are reduced in mTORC2-deficient mouse hippocampal neurons15. In addition, both L-LTP-inducing protocols and behavioral learning transiently increase mTORC2 activity in the hippocampus15. Thus, it is possible that brain aging hijacks mTORC2 by reducing its activity. The deficient mTORC2-mediated changes in actin polymerization may not be sufficient to achieve proper trafficking and insertion of AMPA receptors and thus bring about synaptic potentiation and LTM formation. Indeed, a recent study on rhesus macaques revealed an age-related impairment in maintenance of GluA2-containing AMPA receptors in the hippocampus and this is coupled to memory impairment48.
It is widely accepted that aging is a major risk factor for neurodegeneration. However, a crucial question is whether age-related changes in mTORC2 activity are also responsible for the pathological processes associated with neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease. Previous studies have shown that mTORC2 activity is altered in several neurodegenerative disorders including Huntington’s disease49, Parkinson’s disease19, and Alzheimer’s disease18. Further investigation into the mechanism by which mTORC2 regulates age-associated memory impairment may be the key to the understanding the role of mTORC2 in the onset and progression of neurodegenerative disorders in humans.
In conclusion, our work identifies defective mTORC2 signaling as a novel, evolutionarily conserved mechanism of age-associated memory impairment. Although we are only beginning to understand the unique functions of mTORC2 in the brain, therapeutic approaches targeting the mTORC2/Akt signaling pathway may have a profound impact in the prevention and treatment of age-related neurological diseases. Interestingly, a recent study found that depletion of Rictor and mTORC2 activity in adult male mice significantly decreased lifespan26. While further investigation is needed, it is possible that transient activation of mTORC2 activity may have a beneficial effect on animal survival. Therefore, treatment with A-443654 to enhance mTORC2 activity, may potentially act to rejuvenate the brain, delay the rate of brain aging and help preserve memory in the old. Compounds similar to A-443654, which have been used in human clinical trials for cancer progression50, may prove beneficial for treating a wide array of age-related diseases.
Materials and Methods
Mice
The following mouse lines were used: C57BL/6 young (3 month) mice and C57BL/6 aged (18 month) mice (National Institutes of Aging). The mice were kept on a 12-h light/dark cycle, and the behavioral tests were always conducted during the light phase of the cycle. The mice had access to food and water ad libitum, except during tests. This study was conducted in strict accordance to the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals put forth by the National Institutes of Health. Animal care and experimental procedures were approved by the animal care committee of Baylor College of Medicine, according to US National Institutes of Health Guidelines.
Drosophila husbandry
Flies were reared on a standard medium at 25 °C, 60% relative humidity, and a 12 hr light/dark cycle. Wild-type Canton-S and rictorΔ1 mutants51 were used for this study. The rictorΔ1 mutants were out-crossed into a wild-type Canton-S background before behavioral experimentation. For behavioral studies, 50 flies of mixed sex were aliquoted into food vials and transferred to fresh food vials every 3 or 4 d until they reached the age for training and testing.
Behavior
Contextual fear conditioning
Fear conditioning was performed as previously described40,52. Mice were first handled for 5 min for 3 d and then habituated to the conditioning chamber for 20 min for another day. On the training day, after 2 min in the conditioning chamber, mice received two pairings of a tone (2,800 Hz, 85 dB, 30 s) with a co-terminating foot shock (0.7 mA, 2 s), after which they remained in the chamber for an additional minute and were then returned to their home cages. At 24 h after training, mice were tested for freezing (immobility except for respiration) in response to the training context (training chamber). Freezing behavior was hand-scored at 5-s intervals by an observer blind to the genotype. The percentage of time spent freezing was taken as an index of learning and memory. For A-443654 administration, A-443654 was initially dissolved in DMSO to make a 40 mM stock. The compound was then freshly diluted in saline and injected intraperitoneally at a dose of 2.5 mg per kg immediately after training.
Drosophila aversive olfactory conditioning
The negatively reinforced olfactory learning procedure was carried out according to established protocols, with some modifications36,53. Briefly, the training odorants were 0.2% octanol (vol/vol, Sigma-Aldrich) and 0.12% methylcyclohexanol (vol/vol, Sigma-Aldrich) diluted in mineral oil. For each training trial, flies were exposed to the first odorant (CS+) and twelve 1.25-s pulses of electric shock at 90 V for 1 min. This was followed by a 1-min presentation of the second odorant, which was not paired with shocks (CS−). Both spaced and massed training schedules had five training trials. The inter-trial interval was 15 min for spaced training and 30 s for massed training. Memory was then tested in a T maze 24 h after the final training trial and the performance index calculated as: (number of flies that chose the CS−–the number of flies that chose the CS+)/(total number of flies in both tubes), as previously described53. Octanol and methylcyclohexanol alternated as the CS+, and the results were averaged.
For A-443654 and JPK administration, flies were starved for eight hours prior to training and then transferred to vials containing vehicle (1% DMSO +5% sucrose) or 10 μM A-443654/50 nM JPK +5% sucrose for 1 hr. Flies were then transferred to standard medium for 0.5 hr and then subjected to training. For sensory controls, groups of flies were given 2 min to choose between the odorant and air or shocked and non-shocked side in a T-maze. The odorant concentrations and shock voltages were the same as in the LTM experiments. Avoidance was calculated as: (number of flies that chose air/non-shocked side—the number of flies that chose the odorant/shocked side)/(total number of flies in both tubes).
Western Blotting
Samples were homogenized in buffer containing 200 mM HEPES, 50 mM NaCl, 10% glycerol (vol/vol), 1% Triton X-100 (vol/vol), 1 mM EDTA, 50 mM NaF, 2 mM Na3VO4, 25 mM β-glycerophosphate and EDTA-free complete ULTRA tablets (Roche). A total of 30 μg of protein per sample was resolved on SDS-PAGE (10%), transferred onto nitrocellulose membranes and blotted as previously described15. Western blots were quantified by densitometry using the ImageJ software.
Antibodies
For primary antibodies, we used antibodies to p-S6 ribosomal protein (Ser240/244, #4856, 1:1000), p-Akt (Ser473, #9271, 1:1,000), total S6 ribosomal protein (#2217, 1:1,000), total Akt (#9272, 1:1,000), β-actin (#3700, 1:1,000), and Drosophila p-Akt (Ser505, #4504, 1:1,000) (all from Cell Signaling Technologies).
Statistical analysis
All data are presented as means ± s.e.m. The sample size used to result in statistically significant differences was calculated using standard power calculations with α = 0.05 and a power of 0.8. Statistics were calculated by means of Prism (GraphPad software). Shapiro-Wilk normality test was used to test for normal distribution and all means were compared with Student’s t test, one-way or two-way ANOVA and corrected for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni). P < 0.05 was considered significant.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Johnson, J. L. et al. TORC2: a novel target for treating age-associated memory impairment. Sci. Rep. 5, 15193; doi: 10.1038/srep15193 (2015).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the NIH/National Institute of Mental Health grant R01MH096816, the NIH/National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke grant R01NS076708 and the Department of Defense grant AR120254.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author Contributions J.J. and M.C.-M. designed and contributed to all the mouse and fly studies, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. W.H. contributed to the behavioral studies in mice and analyzed the data. G.R. helped design the fly studies and analyzed the data. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.
References
- Gelbard A., Haub C. & Kent M. M. World population beyond six billion. Popul Bull 54, 1–44 (1999). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prince M. et al. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement 9, 63–75.e62, 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.007 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glorioso C. & Sibille E. Between destiny and disease: genetics and molecular pathways of human central nervous system aging. Prog Neurobiol 93, 165–181, 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.11.006 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samson R. D. & Barnes C. A. Impact of aging brain circuits on cognition. Eur J Neurosci 37, 1903–1915, 10.1111/ejn.12183 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park D. C. & Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol 60, 173–196, 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan T. L. et al. Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiol Aging 22, 581–594 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Brabander J. M., Kramers R. J. & Uylings H. B. Layer-specific dendritic regression of pyramidal cells with ageing in the human prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci 10, 1261–1269 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheibel M. E., Lindsay R. D., Tomiyasu U. & Scheibel A. B. Progressive dendritic changes in aging human cortex. Exp Neurol 47, 392–403 (1975). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson S. C., Rabinovitch P. S. & Kaeberlein M. mTOR is a key modulator of ageing and age-related disease. Nature 493, 338–345, 10.1038/nature11861 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guertin D. A. & Sabatini D. M. Defining the role of mTOR in cancer. Cancer Cell 12, 9–22, 10.1016/j.ccr.2007.05.008 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wullschleger S., Loewith R. & Hall M. N. TOR signaling in growth and metabolism. Cell 124, 471–484, 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.016 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacinto E. et al. Mammalian TOR complex 2 controls the actin cytoskeleton and is rapamycin insensitive. Nat Cell Biol 6, 1122–1128, 10.1038/ncb1183 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oh W. J. & Jacinto E. mTOR complex 2 signaling and functions. Cell Cycle 10, 2305–2316 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarbassov D. D. et al. Rictor, a novel binding partner of mTOR, defines a rapamycin-insensitive and raptor-independent pathway that regulates the cytoskeleton. Curr Biol 14, 1296–1302, 10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.054 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang W. et al. mTORC2 controls actin polymerization required for consolidation of long-term memory. Nat Neurosci 16, 441–448, 10.1038/nn.3351 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomanetz V. et al. Ablation of the mTORC2 component rictor in brain or Purkinje cells affects size and neuron morphology. J Cell Biol 201, 293–308, 10.1083/jcb.201205030 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angliker N. & Rüegg M. A. In vivo evidence for mTORC2-mediated actin cytoskeleton rearrangement in neurons. Bioarchitecture 3, 113–118, 10.4161/bioa.26497 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffin R. J. et al. Activation of Akt/PKB, increased phosphorylation of Akt substrates and loss and altered distribution of Akt and PTEN are features of Alzheimer’s disease pathology. J Neurochem 93, 105–117, 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2004.02949.x (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malagelada C., Jin Z. H. & Greene L. A. RTP801 is induced in Parkinson’s disease and mediates neuron death by inhibiting Akt phosphorylation/activation. J Neurosci 28, 14363–14371, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3928-08.2008 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlopoulos E. et al. Molecular mechanism for age-related memory loss: the histone-binding protein RbAp48. Sci Transl Med 5, 200ra115, 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006373 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peleg S. et al. Altered histone acetylation is associated with age-dependent memory impairment in mice. Science 328, 753–756, 10.1126/science.1186088 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukushima H. et al. Upregulation of calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase IV improves memory formation and rescues memory loss with aging. J Neurosci 28, 9910–9919, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2625-08.2008 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cingolani L. A. & Goda Y. Actin in action: the interplay between the actin cytoskeleton and synaptic efficacy. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 344–356, 10.1038/nrn2373 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamprecht R. The actin cytoskeleton in memory formation. Prog Neurobiol 117, 1–19, 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.001 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch G., Rex C. S. & Gall C. M. LTP consolidation: substrates, explanatory power, and functional significance. Neuropharmacology 52, 12–23, 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.07.027 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamming D. W. et al. Depletion of Rictor, an essential protein component of mTORC2, decreases male lifespan. Aging Cell 13, 911–917, 10.1111/acel.12256 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan H. Y. & Bonini N. M. Drosophila models of human neurodegenerative disease. Cell Death Differ 7, 1075–1080, 10.1038/sj.cdd.4400757 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ocorr K., Akasaka T. & Bodmer R. Age-related cardiac disease model of Drosophila. Mech Ageing Dev 128, 1 12–116, 10.1016/j.mad.2006.11.023 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li W. et al. Activation of transposable elements during aging and neuronal decline in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci 16, 529–531, 10.1038/nn.3368 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarbassov D. D., Guertin D. A., Ali S. M. & Sabatini D. M. Phosphorylation and regulation of Akt/PKB by the rictor-mTOR complex. Science 307, 1098–1101, 10.1126/science.1106148 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke S. N. & Barnes C. A. Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 7, 30–40, 10.1038/nrn1809 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tonoki A. & Davis R. L. Aging impairs protein-synthesis-dependent long-term memory in Drosophila. J Neurosci 35, 1173–1180, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0978-14.2015 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mery F. Aging and its differential effects on consolidated memory forms in Drosophila. Exp Gerontol 42, 99–101, 10.1016/j.exger.2006.06.004 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel E. R., Dudai Y. & Mayford M. R. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell 157, 163–186, 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tully T. & Quinn W. G. Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A 157, 263–277 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tully T., Preat T., Boynton S. C. & Del Vecchio M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell 79, 35–47 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin J. C. et al. Induction of a dominant negative CREB transgene specifically blocks long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell 79, 49–58 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han E. K. et al. Akt inhibitor A-443654 induces rapid Akt Ser-473 phosphorylation independent of mTORC1 inhibition. Oncogene 26, 5655–5661, 10.1038/sj.onc.1210343 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bubb M. R., Senderowicz A. M., Sausville E. A., Duncan K. L. & Korn E. D. Jasplakinolide, a cytotoxic natural product, induces actin polymerization and competitively inhibits the binding of phalloidin to F-actin. J Biol Chem 269, 14869–14871 (1994). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell Mol Neurobiol 23, 727–738 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- deToledo-Morrell L., Geinisman Y. & Morrell F. Age-dependent alterations in hippocampal synaptic plasticity: relation to memory disorders. Neurobiol Aging 9, 581–590 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster T. C. Involvement of hippocampal synaptic plasticity in age-related memory decline. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 30, 236–249 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schimanski L. A. & Barnes C. A. Neural Protein Synthesis during Aging: Effects on Plasticity and Memory. Front Aging Neurosci 2, 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00026 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo Y. et al. Potent and selective inhibitors of Akt kinases slow the progress of tumors in vivo. Mol Cancer Ther 4, 977–986, 10.1158/1535-7163.MCT-05-0005 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin C. H. et al. A role for the PI-3 kinase signaling pathway in fear conditioning and synaptic plasticity in the amygdala. Neuron 31, 841–851 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoica L. et al. Selective pharmacogenetic inhibition of mammalian target of Rapamycin complex I (mTORC1) blocks long-term synaptic plasticity and memory storage. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108, 3791–3796, 10.1073/pnas.1014715108 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee Y. S. & Silva A. J. The molecular and cellular biology of enhanced cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10, 126–140, 10.1038/nrn2572 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hara Y. et al. Synaptic distributions of GluA2 and PKMζ in the monkey dentate gyrus and their relationships with aging and memory. J Neurosci 32, 7336–7344, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0605-12.2012 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humbert S. et al. The IGF-1/Akt pathway is neuroprotective in Huntington’s disease and involves Huntingtin phosphorylation by Akt. Dev Cell 2, 831–837 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- GlaxoSmithKline. Open-label Study to Investigate the Safety, Tolerability, PK, and Pharmacodynamics of the AKT Inhibitor GSK690693. In: ClinicalTrials.gov [Internet]. Bethesda (MD): National Library of Medicine (US) 2000-[cited 2015 June 4] Available from: http://clinicaltrials.gov/show/ NCT00493818 NLM Identifier: NCT00493818.
- Hietakangas V. & Cohen S. M. Re-evaluating AKT regulation: role of TOR complex 2 in tissue growth. Genes Dev 21, 632–637, 10.1101/gad.416307 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu P. J. et al. Suppression of PKR promotes network excitability and enhanced cognition by interferon-γ-mediated disinhibition. Cell 147, 1384–1396, 10.1016/j.cell.2011.11.029 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferris J., Ge H., Liu L. & Roman G. G(o) signaling is required for Drosophila associative learning. Nat Neurosci 9, 1036–1040, 10.1038/nn1738 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.