Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2015 Sep 24;4:e08969. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08969

A genetic link between discriminative fear coding by the lateral amygdala, dopamine, and fear generalization

Graham L Jones 1, Marta E Soden 1, Cerise R Knakal 1, Heather Lee 1, Amanda S Chung 1, Elliott B Merriam 1, Larry S Zweifel 1,*
Editor: Eunjoon Kim2
PMCID: PMC4621744  PMID: 26402461

Abstract

The lateral amygdala (LA) acquires differential coding of predictive and non-predictive fear stimuli that is critical for proper fear memory assignment. The neurotransmitter dopamine is an important modulator of LA activity and facilitates fear memory formation, but whether dopamine neurons aid in the establishment of discriminative fear coding by the LA is unknown. NMDA-type glutamate receptors in dopamine neurons are critical for the prevention of generalized fear following an aversive experience, suggesting a potential link between a cell autonomous function of NMDAR in dopamine neurons and fear coding by the LA. Here, we utilized mice with a selective genetic inactivation functional NMDARs in dopamine neurons (DAT-NR1 KO mice) combined with behavior, in vivo electrophysiology, and ex vivo electrophysiology in LA neurons to demonstrate that plasticity underlying differential fear coding in the LA is regulated by NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons and alterations in this plasticity is associated non-discriminative cued-fear responses.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.001

Research organism: mouse

eLife digest

When we experience a situation that causes us to feel fearful, the brain processes information about the events that led up to it. This information is encoded by groups of nerve cells called neurons in a region of the brain called the lateral amygdala. The nerve cells communicate with each other through chemicals called neurotransmitters. At a junction between two neurons—called a synapse—neurotransmitters are released from one cell and influence the activity of the other cell.

Long-term changes in the strength of these communications in response to specific cues underlie the formation of memories about fearful events. When these changes occur incorrectly they can lead to memories about particular events becoming inaccurate, which can lead to fear being associated with related, but non-threatening, situations. This ‘generalization’ of fear can lead to generalized anxiety disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder. Dopamine is a neurotransmitter that plays an important role in forming memories of fearful events. However, it is not clear whether neurons that release dopamine are also involved in correctly discriminating fearful events from non-fearful ones.

‘Receptor’ proteins called NMDARs on the surface of neurons that release dopamine are critical for preventing generalized fear. These receptors detect another neurotransmitter called glutamate. Jones et al. used genetics and ‘electrophysiology’ techniques to study these receptors in mice. The experiments show that a gene that encodes part of an NMDAR in dopamine neurons plays a key role in how fear memories are formed. When this gene is selectively switched off in the dopamine neurons, mice are more likely to develop generalized fear and anxiety behaviors after a threatening experience.

The experiments also demonstrate that these generalized threat responses are associated with differences in the way the synaptic connections in the lateral amygdala are strengthened. The next major challenge will be to find out which specific synaptic connections are strengthened and to establish how dopamine neuron activity patterns influences this connectivity.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.002

Introduction

Across taxa the amygdala is a central locus for fear processing (Weiskrantz, 1956; Goldstein, 1965; Slotnick, 1973; Adolphs et al., 1995). Comprised of several interconnected subdivisions that are populated by different types of neurons, the prevailing view is that substructures within the amygdala have specific roles for the acquisition, expression, and extinction of fear-related memories (LeDoux, 2000; Maren and Quirk, 2004; Ehrlich et al., 2009).

Within the amygdala the LA is a major site of convergence for information that arrives from cortical and thalamic nuclei and represents an early processing point for emotionally salient information (LeDoux, 2000). Consistent with an early role of the LA in fear memory formation, inactivation of this area prior to, but not after, fear conditioning prevents cued fear behavior (LeDoux et al., 1990; Wilensky et al., 1999). Optogenetic activation (Johansen et al., 2010) or suppression (Johansen et al., 2014) of principal neurons in the LA facilitates and impairs cued fear memory formation, respectively. Direct stimulation of principal neuron cell bodies in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) can also increase anxiety-like behavior (Tye et al., 2011), thus activation of the LA/BLA is a key neural substrate of fear and anxiety. In addition to principal projection neurons, distinct interneuron populations within the BLA can also potently regulate fear memory formation and fear coding (Wolff et al., 2014), indicating that both inhibitory and excitatory synaptic transmission within this region regulates fear-related behavior and learning.

Fear generalization has been proposed to occur through a failure in an animal's ability to define specific outcome contingencies (Grillon, 2002; Lovibond and Shanks, 2002). Thus, aberrant fear coding in the LA may be an early site of generalized fear manifestation. Consistent with this hypothesis, exposure to fear-inducing stimuli has been demonstrated to increase activity in the amygdala of human subjects (Breiter et al., 1996; Morris et al., 1996; Rauch et al., 2000), and hyper-amygdala activation is observed in numerous disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder (Rauch et al., 2000). In rodents, increasing the intensity of an unconditioned fear stimulus increases fear generalization that correlates with enhanced activation of LA neurons to both conditioned (CS+) and non-conditioned (CS−) stimuli (Ghosh and Chattarji, 2015). In addition, suppression of GABAB-mediated signaling in the LA facilitates non-associative long-term potentiation (LTP) of excitatory synapses that correlates with generalized fear responses (Shaban et al., 2006). Collectively, these data suggest that aberrant plasticity in the LA that facilitates non-selective potentiation of excitatory drive or suppression of inhibitory tone is an important neural substrate of generalized fear.

LA neurons demonstrate short latency responses to auditory, visual, and somatosensory stimuli (Ben-Ari and Le Gal la Salle, 1971). Following fear conditioning the latency of responses to predictive auditory stimuli decreases (Quirk et al., 1995; Maren, 2000) and the response amplitude increases selectively to predictive, but not non-predictive, stimuli (Collins and Pare, 2000), thus demonstrating acquired selectivity in responding to CS+ and CS− stimuli. Acquisition of differential coding of predictive and non-predictive fear-related information is correlated with studies investigating changes in synaptic strength in the LA after fear conditioning. Auditory fear conditioning elicits LTP-like effects in LA neurons (Rogan and LeDoux, 1995; Rogan et al., 1997) and elicits presynaptic enhancement of inputs arriving in the LA from the medial geniculate nucleus of the thalamus and cortex (McKernan and Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997; Tsvetkov et al., 2002; Zinebi et al., 2002).

Dopamine is a potent modulator of plasticity in the LA. In the anesthetized rat, odor-evoked potentials in the LA are potentiated following pairing with a footshock that is dependent on dopamine receptor activation (Rosenkranz and Grace, 2002). In addition, dopamine signaling modulates local inhibitory networks in the LA (Loretan et al., 2004), facilitates LTP induction through suppression of feedforward inhibition (Bissiere et al., 2003), and regulates intrinsic excitability of principal neurons (Yamamoto et al., 2007). Consistent with the modulation of fear-evoked plasticity in the LA, suppression of dopamine signaling attenuates acquisition of conditioned fear memory (Borowski and Kokkinidis, 1996; Lamont and Kokkinidis, 1998; Guarraci et al., 1999, 2000; Greba et al., 2001) and fear memory expression (Nader and LeDoux, 1999).

Subsets of dopamine neurons are activated by aversive stimuli (Schultz and Romo, 1987; Guarraci and Kapp, 1999; Brischoux et al., 2009) and undergo plasticity following an aversive experience (Lammel et al., 2011) or fear conditioning (Guarraci and Kapp, 1999; Brischoux et al., 2009; Gore et al., 2014). NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons regulates both synaptic plasticity (Bonci and Malenka, 1999) and phasic activation of these cells (Overton and Clark, 1997), and disruption of NMDARs in dopamine neurons of mice results in the development of behavioral correlates of generalized fear and anxiety following an aversive experience (Zweifel et al., 2011). These results suggest a potential link between cell autonomous functions of this receptor in dopamine neurons and the disruption of fear coding in the amygdala that may be an underlying cause of fear generalization.

Here we demonstrate that mice lacking NMDARs in dopamine neurons have deficits in cued fear discrimination and that this deficit is associated with altered fear coding in the LA. Contrary to our initial hypothesis, fear generalization in DAT-NR1 KO mice was not associated with a non-specific increased activation of LA neurons to a non-US predictive conditioned stimulus (CS−), but rather a lack of enhanced LA activation to a US predictive conditioned stimulus (CS+). These findings demonstrate that erroneous enhancement of LA activity alone is not necessary for fear generalization, but rather cued fear discrimination coding in general is an essential component for proper fear assignment.

Results

Generalized fear in DAT-NR1 KO mice alters signaling in the LA

We have shown previously that DAT-NR1 KO mice develop non-selective enhancement of acoustic startle reflex and increased anxiety-like behavior following an aversive experience (Zweifel et al., 2011). To determine whether DAT-NR1 KO mice fail to discriminate between fear predictive and non-predictive stimuli we assayed their performance in a delayed Pavlovian cued fear discrimination task (Figure 1A). Mice were presented with 10 trials each of a 10 s light (constant illumination) cue (CS+) that co-terminated with delivery of an unconditioned (US) fear stimulus (0.3 mA, 0.5 s footshock), randomly interspersed with delivery of a distinct light (pulsing illumination) cue (CS−) that did not co-terminate with delivery of the US. 24 hours following conditioning mice were given interspersed presentations of the CS+ and CS− in a novel context. Both DAT-NR1 KO mice and littermate controls displayed enhanced fear (freezing behavior) following both one and two days of conditioning. Control mice demonstrated cue discrimination on both days, a behavior that was significantly impaired in DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 1B).

Figure 1. Impaired fear discrimination and c-Fos activation in the LA of DAT-NR1 KO mice.

Figure 1.

(A) Fear conditioning paradigm. Mice were probed for freezing in context B (top right) to the CS+ and CS− with three presentations of each stimulus (middle) prior to fear conditioning (Pre) and 24 hr after each conditioning session (Test 1 and Test 2). Mice were conditioned in context A (top left) with 10 presentations of a CS− or CS + co-terminating with US delivery (bottom). (B) Freezing behavior (% Time Immobile) during presentation of the CS+ and CS− during pre-conditioning and Test 1 and Test 2. (C) Brain atlas image (left) (Paxinos and Franklin, 2013) illustrating LA subdivisions (gray shading) analyzed for c-Fos induction following fear conditioning. Representative c-Fos immunoreactive cells in the LA of control (control, left) and DAT-NR1KO (KO, right) mice following a single fear conditioning session. Scale bar = 250 μm (D) Average c-Fos positive cells in the LA of Ctrl and KO mice following fear conditioning (n = 3 mice each group, 8 sections/mouse). p < 0.01, unpaired Student's T-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.003

The LA is known to participate in cued-fear processing (LeDoux, 2000) and altered fear coding in this region correlates with generalized fear responses (Ghosh and Chattarji, 2015). Fos expression in the amygdala has been previously shown to be induced following unconditioned footshock and is proposed as an early marker of plasticity (Campeau et al., 1991). To determine whether activity-dependent processes in the LA are altered in DAT-NR1 KO mice, we analyzed the induction of the immediately early gene Fos 90 min following one conditioning session in context A (Figure 1A). Fos expression was significantly reduced in DAT-NR1 KO mice relative to controls (Figure 1C,D) indicating a potential reduced activation of the LA.

Enhancement of LA activity following US delivery is absent in DAT-NR1 KOs

To determine whether an aversive US results in a general increase in LA activation in a manner that is dependent on NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons, we recorded the activity of amygdala neurons during three days of fear conditioning following bilateral implantation of tetrodes into the LA of DAT-NR1 KO and control mice (an independent cohort from those used for behavioral analysis). The majority of tetrodes were localized to the ventral lateral and ventral medial subdivisions of the LA (Figure 2A). Average waveforms of isolated neurons were similar between groups and across days (Figure 2B); similarly, average baseline firing rate and the range of firing rates did not differ between groups or across days of conditioning (Figure 2C).

Figure 2. Population activity in the LA is not enhanced in DAT-NR1 KO mice following footshock.

(A) Brain atlas image (Paxinos and Franklin, 2013) illustrating bilateral tetrode implantation (top) and location of recording electrodes in Ctrl and KO mice. (B) Average waveform of recorded units in Ctrl and KO mice across days of conditioning. (C) Baseline firing rate of individual units in Ctrl and KO mice across days of conditioning (Control: n = 55, 52, and 54 Days 1–3, respectively; DAT-NR1 KO: n = 48, 57, and 58 Days 1–3, respectively). (D) Heat plot of normalized activity in concatenated CS + trials from Ctrl (top) and KO (bottom) mice. (E) Percent change from baseline activity during CS + trials following presentation of the US across days of conditioning. (F) Heat plot of normalized activity in concatenated CS− trials from Ctrl (top) and KO (bottom) mice. (G) Percent change from baseline activity during CS− trials following presentation of the US across days of conditioning. (E, G) Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA, p < 0.001 and p < 0.05, Bonferroni post-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.004

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Population activity during fear conditioning.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Histogram of firing rate of an example neuron on day one of fear conditioning illustrating increased activity following presentation of the US. Arrow indicates onset of first footshock. (B) Proportion of activated neurons is highest in control mice on day 1 and decreases with subsequent conditioning. p < 0.001, Chi-square. (C, D) Heat plots of normalized firing rate for individual cells in control and DAT-NR1 KO mice during CS+/US (C) and CS− (D) presentation and subsequent ITI.

In a number of control neurons we detected an increase in overall firing rate of the cell following presentation of the first US (Figure 2—figure supplement 1), with the highest proportion of activated neurons observed in control mice on the first day of conditioning (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). To determine whether increased activity occurs more prominently following CS + trials, or represents a general increase, we concatenated all CS + trials with subsequent inter-trial intervals (ITIs; Figure 2D) and CS− trials with subsequent ITIs (Figure 2F) and compared them to baseline activity. In control mice, population activity during both CS+ and CS− trials and their subsequent ITIs was significantly enhanced relative to baseline on the first day of conditioning, but this elevated activity diminished across conditioning days (Figure 2D–G). This change in activation was reflected in the proportion of cells activated across days (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We did not detect a significant increase in population activity in DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 2D–G).

Enhancement of US activated neurons is absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice

Visual inspection of our population data revealed discrete phasic events time-locked to the presentation of the US; these results are consistent with previous reports of LA neurons responding to footshock (Ben-Ari and Le Gal la Salle, 1971; Romanski et al., 1993). To determine whether these phasic events undergo plasticity across days of conditioning that is dependent on NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons, we analyzed neurons with US responsiveness. Action potential waveforms, and baseline firing rates of neurons activated by the US did not differ between genotypes (Figure 3A,B) and the proportion of cells responding did not differ between groups or across days (Figure 3C). In control mice we found a transient potentiation of the US response, with day 2 responses significantly higher than day 1 (Figure 3D–F). This effect was not present on the third day of conditioning. Moreover, we did not detect this effect in DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 3D–F). The response to the US on the second day was significantly higher in controls than in DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 3H), but did not differ on the other days (Figure 3G,I).

Figure 3. Transient plasticity in US-activated LA neurons is absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice.

(A) Average waveform of recorded units in Ctrl and KO mice that were activated by the US. (B) Baseline firing rate of individual units in Ctrl and KO mice that were activated by the US (Control: n = 32, 26, and 28 Days 1–3, respectively; DAT-NR1 KO: n = 25, 34, and 35 Days 1–3, respectively). (C) Proportion of neurons from Ctrl and KO mice that were activated or inhibited by the US. (D) Average normalized firing rate of US activated neurons in Ctrl mice across days of conditioning. (E) Average normalized firing rate of US-activated neurons in KO mice across days of conditioning. (F) Average area under the curve (AUC) of activated response for Ctrl and KO mice across days. (GI) Comparison of activated responses of Ctrl and KO mice during day 1 (G), day 2 (H), and day 3 of conditioning (I). Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA, p < 0.001, Bonferroni post-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.006

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. US-inhibited LA neurons do not change across days of conditioning.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Average normalized firing rate of US-inhibited neurons in control mice across days of conditioning (Control: n = 8, 16, and 14 Days 1–3, respectively; DAT-NR1 KO: n = 8, 8, and 9 Days 1–3, respectively). (B) Average normalized firing rate of US- inhibited neurons in DAT-NR1 KO mice across days of conditioning. (C) Average area under the curve (AUC) of inhibited response for control and DAT-NR1 KO mice across days. (DF) Comparison of inhibited responses of control and DAT-NR1 KO mice during day 1 (D), day 2 (E), and day 3 of conditioning (F). Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M.

While the majority of responsive cells increased their firing rates during the US, a small number of neurons showed decreased activity relative to baseline during US presentation (Figure 3C and Figure 3—figure supplement 1), similar to previous reports (Ben-Ari and Le Gal la Salle, 1971). Neurons displaying transient inhibition to the US showed little change across days of conditioning and were not significantly different between genotypes (Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

DAT-NR1 KO mice have impaired discrimination in CS+/CS− activated neurons

Previous studies have shown that LA neurons undergo plasticity in CS−evoked responses, demonstrating increased response magnitude (Quirk et al., 1995, 1997; Maren, 2000) and enhanced discrimination between CS+ and CS− stimuli (Collins and Pare, 2000; Ghosh and Chattarji, 2015). Neurons were detected in both control and DAT-NR1 KO mice that responded to the CS+ and CS− stimuli. Average waveforms, firing rate, and the proportion of neurons responding to these stimuli did not differ between groups (Figure 4A–C). In control mice we observed a progressive enhancement of the CS + response in neurons activated by the stimulus across days (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In contrast, DAT-NR1 KO mice did not show enhancement of CS + responses (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Neither control nor DAT-NR1 KO mice showed changes in CS− responses (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Enhanced responding to CS + across days of conditioning in control mice was not due to repeated presentations of the cue, as this effect was not observed in control mice that received three consecutive days of cue (CS+ and CS−) presentation delivered without footshock (unpaired, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Increased CS + responses in control mice across days of conditioning resulted in significant cue discrimination that was absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 4D,E). This change was not associated with differences in the proportion of neurons responding to both CS+ and CS− stimuli (Control: Day 1, 84%; Day 2, 78%; Day 3, 87% vs DAT-NR1 KO: Day 1, 88%; Day 2, 80%; Day 3, 80%). To determine whether discriminative fear coding emerges early in conditioning, we analyzed the responses of neurons on day 1 of conditioning during the first and last cue presentation. Although there was a trend towards discrimination in both control and DAT-NR1 KO mice by the last conditioning trial this effect did not reach statistical significance (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). These findings indicate that early cue discrimination may occur in both groups of mice, but is only maintained in control mice.

Figure 4. Plasticity in CS activated LA neurons is absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice.

(A) Average waveform of recorded units in Ctrl and KO mice that were activated by the CS+ and CS−. (B) Baseline firing rate of individual units in Ctrl and KO mice that were activated by the CS+ and CS− (Control: n = 21, 22, and 19 Days 1–3, respectively; DAT-NR1 KO: n = 14, 23, and 23 Days 1–3, respectively). (C) Proportion of neurons from Ctrl and KO mice that were activated by the CS+ and CS−. (D) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− activated neurons in Ctrl and KO mice on day 1 of conditioning. (E) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− activated neurons in Ctrl and KO mice on day 3 of conditioning. Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA, p < 0.001 and p < 0.01, Bonferroni post-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.008

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Plasticity in CS activated LA neurons is absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Average normalized firing rate of CS + activated neurons in control mice across days of conditioning. (B) Average normalized firing rate of CS + activated neurons in in DAT-NR1 KO mice across days of conditioning. (C) Average normalized firing rate of CS− activated neurons in control mice across days of conditioning. (D) Average normalized firing rate of CS− activated neurons in in DAT-NR1 KO mice across days of conditioning. (E) Average normalized firing rate of activated neurons during presentation of unpaired CS+ in control mice across days of conditioning. (F) Average normalized firing rate of activated neurons during presentation of unpaired CS− in control mice across days of conditioning. (G) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− activated neurons in unpaired Ctrl mice on day 1 of conditioning. (H) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− activated neurons in unpaired Ctrl mice on day 3 of conditioning. Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA, ****p < 0.0001, Bonferroni post-test.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. CS activated LA neurons are not different at the start of conditioning.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− activated neurons in Ctrl and KO mice during trial 1 of the first day of conditioning. (B) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− activated neurons in Ctrl and KO mice during trial 10 of the first day of conditioning.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Differential response latencies in CS activated neurons between control and DAT-NR1 KO mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

A) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS + presentation in control mice does not change across days. (B) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS + presentation in DAT-NR1 KO mice decrease across days. (C) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS− presentation in control mice decrease across days. (D) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS− presentation in DAT-NR1 KO mice decrease across days. (E) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS + presentation in control vs DAT-NR1 KO mice on day 1. (F) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS + presentation in control vs DAT-NR1 KO mice on day 3. (G) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS− presentation in control vs DAT-NR1 KO mice on day 1. (H) Average cumulative distribution of latency to increase activity in response to CS− presentation in control vs DAT-NR1 KO mice on day 3. Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− activated neurons in unpaired Ctrl mice on day 3 of conditioning. Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA, *p < 0.05, ****p < 0.0001, Bonferroni post-test.

Following auditory fear conditioning the latency of responses to a conditioned stimulus decreases in the LA (Quirk et al., 1995; Maren, 2000). To determine whether the latency of responding to a visually paired stimulus also decreases with fear conditioning we assessed cumulative frequency distributions in the latency to fire across days. We did not observe a significant change in frequency distribution of CS + responses in control mice across days of conditioning (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). In contrast, we did observe a significant difference in the distribution in DAT-NR1 KO mice, with increasing response latencies on the third day of conditioning (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Intriguingly, we observed a similar increase in latency across days of conditioning in response to the CS− in both control and DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 4—figure supplement 3).

DAT-NR1 KO mice have impaired discrimination in CS+/CS− inhibited neurons

In addition to neurons activated by the CS+ and CS− we also observed a number of neurons that displayed transient inhibitions in response to these stimuli. Average waveforms, firing rate, and the proportion of neurons inhibited by these stimuli were similar across groups (Figure 5A–C). Repeated days of conditioning did not alter inhibitory responses to the CS+ in either group (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). However, we did observe a progressive decrease in the magnitude of the inhibitory response to the CS− in control mice, with the largest inhibition observed on day 1 and significant reductions in this inhibition on subsequent days (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). We did not observe this plasticity in DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Similar to the acquisition of cue discrimination in neurons activated by the CS+ and CS−, control, but not DAT-NR1 KO mice, acquired cue discrimination in neurons inhibited by these stimuli (Figure 5D,E). This change was also not associated with differences in the proportion of neurons responding to both CS+ and CS− stimuli (Control: Day 1, 81%; Day 2, 79%; Day 3, 80% vs DAT-NR1 KO: Day 1, 84%; Day 2, 87%; Day 3, 77%). Similar to neurons activated by the CS+ and CS−, neurons inhibited by these stimuli did not differ during the first or last presentation on the first day of conditioning (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

Figure 5. Plasticity in CS inhibited LA neurons is absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice.

(A) Average waveform of recorded units in Ctrl and KO mice that were inhibited by the CS+ and CS−. (B) Baseline firing rate of individual units in Ctrl and KO mice that were inhibited by the CS+ and CS− (Control: n = 21, 15, and 22 Days 1–3, respectively; DAT-NR1 KO: n = 13, 20, and 18 Days 1–3, respectively). (C) Proportion of neurons from Ctrl and KO mice that were inhibited by the CS+ and CS−. (D) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− inhibited neurons in Ctrl and KO mice on day 1 of conditioning. (E) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− inhibited neurons in Ctrl and KO mice on day 3 of conditioning. Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA, p < 0.05, Bonferroni post-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.012

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Plasticity in CS inhibited LA neurons is absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Average normalized firing rate of CS + inhibited neurons in control mice across days of conditioning. (B) Average normalized firing rate of CS + inhibited neurons in in DAT-NR1 KO mice across days of conditioning. (C) Average normalized firing rate of CS− inhibited neurons in control mice across days of conditioning. (D) Average normalized firing rate of CS− inhibited neurons in in DAT-NR1 KO mice across days of conditioning. (E) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− inhibited neurons in Ctrl and KO mice during trial 1 of the first day of conditioning. (F) Average normalized firing rate of CS+ and CS− inhibited neurons in Ctrl and KO mice during trial 10 of the first day of conditioning. Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA,**p < 0.01 and ,***p < 0.001, Bonferroni post-test.

Synaptic plasticity following fear conditioning is impaired in DAT-NR1 KO mice

Synaptic plasticity in the LA following fear conditioning has been previously reported, indicating presynaptic enhancement of excitatory synapses from both thalamic and cortical inputs (McKernan and Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997; Tsvetkov et al., 2002; Zinebi et al., 2002). Dopamine receptor signaling has also been demonstrated to influence plasticity in both excitatory and inhibitory pathways in the LA (Bissiere et al., 2003; Loretan et al., 2004). Our in vivo recordings indicate complex, bidirectional changes in LA cellular activity following multiple days of fear conditioning, including enhanced excitation to the CS+ and reduced inhibition to the CS− in control mice. To determine whether fear generalization associated with disruption of dopamine neuron physiology alters conditioning-evoked plasticity in the LA, we performed whole-cell patch clamp recordings of principal neurons in the LA in acute slices from naïve mice and from mice 24 hr following the second conditioning session.

To test whether fear conditioning elicits pathway-specific changes in the balance of excitatory and inhibitory inputs we measured evoked post-synaptic potentials (PSPs). A stimulating electrode placed in the internal capsule (IC, thalamic), or the external capsule (EC, cortical; Figure 6A) reliably evoked a compound PSP in LA neurons (Figure 6B). The excitatory PSP (EPSP) was isolated using bath application of the GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (100 µM), and the inhibitory PSP (IPSP) was determined through digital subtraction of the EPSP from the compound PSP (Figure 6B). We next calculated the ratio of EPSP:IPSP amplitudes, allowing for the normalized assessment of select changes in either excitatory or inhibitory input. When we compared the ratio of EPSP to IPSP amplitudes in both pathways in both naïve and fear conditioned control and DAT-NR1 KO mice, we saw no significant changes in the balance of excitatory to inhibitory inputs in any group in either the thalamic or cortical pathway (Figure 6C,D) indicating a lack of selective change in either inhibitory or excitatory inputs.

Figure 6. Synaptic plasticity in LA neurons is impaired in DAT-NR1 KO mice.

(A) Brain atlas image52 illustrating placement of stimulating electrodes in the IC and EC and recording electrode in the LA. (B) Representative compound PSP following thalamic stimulation (left) and isolated EPSP and IPSP from Ctrl (middle) and KO (right) mice. (C, D) Excitation/inhibition ratios of EPSP/IPSPs of individual neurons from Ctrl and KO mice following cortical (C, Control: n = 7 naïve, n = 8 shock; DAT-NR1: KO n = 9 naïve, n = 11 shock) and thalamic (D, Control: n = 6 naïve, n = 10 shock; DAT-NR1: KO n = 11 naïve, n = 7 shock) stimulations. (E, F) Representative mEPSCs (E) and mIPSCs (F) from naïve (black) and fear conditioned (gray) Ctrl and KO mice. (G, H) Cumulative distribution of mEPSC frequency from naïve and fear conditioned control (G, n = 18 naïve, n = 14 shock) and KO mice (H, n = 14 naïve, n = 13 shock). (I, J) Cumulative distribution of mIPSC frequency from naïve and fear conditioned control (I) and KO mice (J). Data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Repeated measures ANOVA, p < 0.0001, Bonferroni post-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.08969.014

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. mEPSC and mIPSC amplitude does not change in LA neurons following fear conditioning.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) Average amplitude of mEPSCs in naïve and fear conditioned (shock) control and DAT-NR1 KO mice. (B) Average amplitude of mIPSCs in naïve and fear conditioned (shock) control and DAT-NR1 KO mice.

To further probe for possible changes in synaptic strength evoked by fear conditioning, we recorded miniature excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (mEPSCs and mIPSCs) in the LA of both naïve and fear-conditioned mice (Figure 6E,F). Fear conditioning evoked a significant increase in the frequency of mEPSCs in control mice, which was not observed in DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 6G,H). We did not detect significant changes in the amplitude of mEPSCs in either group (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). Fear conditioning also elicited a significant enhancement of mIPSC frequency in control, but not DAT-NR1 KO mice (Figure 6I,J). We did not detect significant changes in the amplitude of mIPSCs in either group (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). These results are consistent with an enhancement of both excitatory and inhibitory inputs to LA neurons.

Discussion

Our observations that mice lacking NMDARs in dopamine neurons fail to undergo plasticity at multiple levels is consistent with previous reports linking dopamine to plasticity and fear coding in the LA (Rosenkranz and Grace, 2001, 2002; Bissiere et al., 2003) and to the regulation of intrinsic excitability of LA neurons (Yamamoto et al., 2007). Previous studies have demonstrated that failure of LA neurons to discriminate between fear predictive and non-predictive stimuli is highly correlated with fear generalization (Ghosh and Chattarji, 2015). More specifically these authors demonstrate that increased intensity of the US results in non-selective enhancement of LA excitatory responses to a CS−, thus impairing the discrimination between CS+ and CS− (Ghosh and Chattarji, 2015). Consistent with these findings it has also been shown that disruption of GABAB signaling in the LA results in non-associative plasticity, and mice lacking the GABAB receptor subtype GABAB(1a) develop generalized fear responses (Shaban et al., 2006). In contrast to previous reports of generalized fear being associated with an over-exuberance of plasticity and activation of the LA (Shaban et al., 2006; Ghosh and Chattarji, 2015), in DAT-NR1 KO mice with generalized fear the LA neurons appear as though no CS/US association has been made. More specifically, LA neurons in DAT-NR1 KO mice do not potentiate to the CS+, fail to reduce inhibition to the CS−, and demonstrate longer response latencies to the CS+ and CS−, similar to longer response latencies to the CS− in control mice. Although the manner in which discrimination occurs between activated and inhibited neurons differs, the end result is an acquired differential response that is dependent upon NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons. These results indicate that generalized freezing behavior to the CS− is not dependent upon excessive activation of the LA, but rather suggest that discriminative coding in the LA reflects the proper assignment of cue-outcome contingencies. Collectively, these findings suggest that disruption of plasticity underlying discriminative fear coding in the LA is associated with generalized fear responses. Our results extend previous findings and confirm that plasticity in the LA is associated with discriminative fear; however, it is not an over-exuberance of plasticity per se that underlies generalization but rather the lack of appropriate plasticity.

If a lack of synaptic strengthening occurs in the LA in the absence of NMDA receptor signaling in dopamine neurons, then where is the site of heightened threat responses following fear conditioning? It is well established that the LA is an early site of convergent sensory information processing that is essential for fear memory acquisition and expression (LeDoux, 2000); however, fear coding and plasticity are broadly distributed across multiple brain regions including the thalamus, cortex (sensory and prefrontal), and hippocampus (Tovote et al., 2015). In addition, plasticity also occurs within other subdivision of the amygdala including the BLA and lateral subdivision of the central nucleus (CeAl) (Tovote et al., 2015). More specifically, generalized defensive responses to threat have been correlated with non-specific coding of cue information in the CeAl (Ciocchi et al., 2010), and activation of a specific population of CeAl neurons during discriminative fear conditioning results in response generalization (Botta et al., 2015). Thus, it is possible that the generalized threat responses reported here are mediated by exaggerated responses in the CeA or other areas of the brain implicated in fear processing, many of which are innervated by midbrain dopamine neurons.

Consistent with plasticity in the activity of LA neurons in freely moving mice during fear conditioning, we observed synaptic plasticity in LA neurons in acute slice following fear conditioning in control mice that was absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice. Numerous studies have identified pathway-specific plasticity in the LA (Rogan and LeDoux, 1995; McKernan and Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997; Rogan et al., 1997; Zinebi et al., 2002; Schroeder and Shinnick-Gallagher, 2005; Shin et al., 2010; Cho et al., 2012). Examination of the ratio of excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials in response to stimulation of either thalamic or cortical input to LA neurons did not reveal a pathway-specific change in either excitatory or inhibitory drive. These findings indicate that either one or both pathways are enhanced with proportional increases in excitatory and inhibitory drive, or that neither pathway is altered specifically. Our observations of increased frequency of both mIPSCs and mEPSCs suggest a scenario in which one or both pathways are potentiated with proportional changes in excitatory and inhibitory inputs.

There are numerous mechanisms that have been identified that underlie plasticity in LA neurons (Rodrigues et al., 2004) with both pre- and postsynaptic sites of action (Tsvetkov et al., 2002; Zinebi et al., 2002; Apergis-Schoute et al., 2005; Schroeder and Shinnick-Gallagher, 2005; Fourcaudot et al., 2008; Shin et al., 2010; Cho et al., 2012). Our observed increase in the frequency of both mEPSCs and mIPSCs without a concomitant change in amplitude, is consistent with a presynaptic increase in synaptic transmission, similar to previous descriptions of facilitated synaptic transmission from cortical and thalamic inputs to LA neurons (McKernan and Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997; Tsvetkov et al., 2002; Zinebi et al., 2002). Although we did not observe an increase in mEPSC amplitude this does not exclude postsynaptic changes in glutamate receptors. It was previously demonstrated that trafficking of postsynaptic AMPA receptors in the LA is critical for associative memory formation (Rumpel et al., 2005). One potential explanation for these apparent discrepancies is the unmasking of previously silent synapses (Kerchner and Nicoll, 2008). Indeed, silent synapses have been reported in the amygdala (Lee et al., 2013) and stress has been shown to increase the formation of nascent silent synapses in the LA (Suvrathan et al., 2014). Alternatively, it has been proposed that different synaptic sites of LTP in LA neurons (pre- vs postsynaptic) may occur depending on the strength the US, with low US intensities favoring presynaptic LTP and high US intensities favoring postsynaptic LTP (Shin et al., 2010). Because we used a low intensity footshock (0.3 mA) it is possible that our protocol favored the induction of a presynaptic form of synaptic strengthening.

The mechanism by which NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons influences plasticity in fear coding in the LA remains to be elucidated. We have previously demonstrated that NMDARs in dopamine neurons regulate phasic activation of these neurons in response to an aversive stimulus, thus suggesting that phasic dopamine release may facilitate plasticity at excitatory and inhibitory synapses within the LA. Consistent with this hypothesis, previous studies have demonstrated that dopamine signaling within the LA modulates local inhibitory networks (Loretan et al., 2004) and gates LTP induction in LA neurons through a suppression of feedforward inhibition (Bissiere et al., 2003). In addition to local inhibitory neurons, dopamine neurons innervate paracapsular intercalated cell clusters (Marcellino et al., 2012) and potently modulate intercalated neurons through a D1 receptor dependent inhibitory mechanism (Marowsky et al., 2005). More specifically, dopamine signaling in lateral paracapsular intercalated neurons suppresses feedforward inhibition from cortical inputs to the BLA (Marowsky et al., 2005). Thus, phasic dopamine release facilitated by NMDARs in dopamine neurons is likely to regulate excitability and plasticity of LA neurons through both local inhibitory networks and feedforward inhibition from lateral paracapsular intercalated neurons.

In addition to suppression of inhibition, dopamine increases the excitability of LA neurons through induction of a slow afterhyperpolarization (Yamamoto et al., 2007). Collectively, these effects would significantly increase the spike firing of LA neurons. We find that the activity of a significant proportion of LA neurons increases after the first presentation of the US in control mice that parallels our finding of increased Fos levels in the LA following the first conditioning session. Footshock has been demonstrated to increase firing of midbrain dopamine neurons (Brischoux et al., 2009), thus increased dopamine release facilitated by NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons could explain the observed increase in LA activity in control mice that is absent in DAT-NR1 KO mice. Such an increase in the activity of LA neurons would then facilitate the induction of LTP (Bissiere et al., 2003) and lasting changes in synaptic strength.

NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons regulates phasic activation of these neurons as well as synaptic plasticity. Thus, alterations in fear-evoked plasticity within dopamine neurons could also be a major contributor to the disruption of fear coding in the LA of DAT-NR1 KO mice. Using a similar fear conditioning paradigm we have previously shown that dopamine neurons undergo plasticity in fear-evoked increases in calcium signals in dopamine neurons (Gore et al., 2014), which occurs on a similar time course to the results described here. Others have also demonstrated that mesocortical projecting dopamine neurons undergo synaptic plasticity following a painful experience (Lammel et al., 2011), indicating pathway specific activation of dopamine neurons. In support of an interdependent plasticity between dopamine neurons and target structures, it has been demonstrated that NMDAR-dependent cocaine-evoked plasticity in dopamine neurons occurs prior to plasticity in dopaminergic targets of the nucleus accumbens and that plasticity within the accumbens is dependent on NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons (Mameli et al., 2009).

The interrelationship between plasticity and phasic activation of dopamine neurons and the relationship between phasic dopamine and plasticity in dopamine neurons that modulates plasticity in the LA remains to be determined. However, our results provide an important first step in linking NMDAR signaling in dopamine neurons with fear discrimination coding in the LA and demonstrate that fear generalization can occur in the absence of hyperexcitation of the LA.

Materials and methods

Animals

All Materials and methods were approved by the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Control (Grin1Δ/+; Slc6a3Cre/+) and DAT-NR1 KO (Grin1Δ/lox; Slc6a3Cre/+) mice were generated as previously described (Zweifel et al., 2008). 8- to 12-week old male mice were used for behavior and electrophysiology. 5- to 6-week old mice were used for slice electrophysiology. All mice were housed on a 12 hr light/dark cycle in a temperature controlled environment with ad libitum access to food and water for the duration of the study.

Fear conditioning

Behavioral conditioning was performed in a sound attenuating cabinet with a mouse extra wide modular test chamber outfitted with a shock grid and stimulus lights (Med Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT, United States). Mice were habituated to handling each day one week prior to conditioning. For baseline cue responding mice were placeplaced in the box with a white corrugated plastic insert placed in the chamber to cover the walls and shock grid. Mice were assessed for freezing in response to three randomly interspersed presentations of the CS+ and CS− following a two minute baseline period. Each session of the 2-day fear conditioning paradigm included a 2 min baseline period followed by 20 randomly interleaved trials, comprised of 10 CS + trials and 10 CS− trials, each followed by a 110 s inter-trial interval (ITI). The CS + trials consisted of a constant light cue presentation for 9.5 s, terminating with a 0.5 s 0.3 mA footshock (US) and the CS− trials used a different light cue that flashed 5 times for 200 ms every 2 s, ending with the last light flash and the absence of the US. For assessment of cue-specific freezing behavior 24 hr following conditioning mice were placed in the chamber with the white corrugated plastic insert and freezing was assessed in response to three randomly interspersed presentations of the CS+ and CS− following a 2 min baseline period. Freezing responses were scored as immobility, except for movement associated with breathing, by two independent investigators blind to genotype. Data were analyzed for statistical significance by two-way repeated measures ANOVA.

In vivo electrophysiology

Electrophysiology in freely moving mice was performed using microdrives fabricated in house, utilizing 16-channel electrode interface boards (EIB-16; Neuralynx) and tetrodes made from 0.00099" diameter tungsten wire (Tungsten 99.95% CS SFV NATRL; California Fine Wire Company). Microdrive implantations in anesthetized mice were stereotaxically targeted at the LA (−1.65 mm A-P, ±2.85→3.25 mm M-L, −4.3 mm D-V; Paxinos); Bilateral targeting was achieved using coupled polyimide guide tubes (200 µm OD; source) spaced at 6.5 mm. One week after surgery, mice were connected to a 16-channel Medusa Preamplifier and filtered signals (300-5000 Hz) were acquired using a RZ5 Signal Processor (Tucker–Davis Technologies). Tetrodes were lowered daily in ∼40 μm increments until unit activity was observed, at which point the animals began the fear conditioning paradigm; tetrodes were not lowered on subsequent days unless cell activity was absent. For conditioning, mice were treated as above, except they were not exposed to the testing chamber (white corrugated plastic insert) and did not receive cue presentations prior to conditioning. Conditioning proceeded each day following a 10 min baseline period to establish basal neural activity. Tetrode placement was histologically confirmed postmortem using cresyl violet histochemical stain and the presence of implant-induced tissue damage. Neurons were isolated by cluster analysis using Offline Sorter (Plexon) and subsequently formatted and analyzed with MATLAB (Mathworks) and Prism (Graphpad Software). Data were acquired from 13 control and 12 DAT-NR1 KO mice. For unpaired recordings in control mice (N = 5), animals were treated exactly as above, except during the CS + presentation that normally co-terminated with footshock the shock was omitted.

Unit response determination

Reponses for each cell were normalized to baseline firing rate by calculating a Z-score (Z = (Ri-Rm)/S.D.), where Ri = firing rate at an individual time point and Rm = mean firing rate, S.D. = standard deviation of the mean firing rate. Units were characterized as increasing to a stimulus if the Z-score was greater than 1 within the first 500 msec following stimulus presentation. Units were characterized as decreasing to a stimulus if the Z-score was less than −0.5 within the first 500 msec following stimulus presentation. Data were analyzed for statistical significance by two-way repeated measures ANOVA and repeated measures ANOVA, where appropriate.

Slice electrophysiology

For fear conditioned mice, animals were handled for one week prior to conditioning. Mice were conditioned as above with two consecutive days of 10 CS+/US and CS− presentations. Mice were not pre-exposed to cues prior to conditioning. 24 hours following the second conditioning session mice were euthanized for brain slice preparation. Whole-cell recordings were made using an Axopatch 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices) with filtering at 1 KHz using 4–6 MΩ electrodes. Coronal brain slices (250 μm) were prepared in an ice slush solution containing (in mM): 250 sucrose, 3 KCl, 2 MgSO4, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 10 D-glucose, 25 NaHCO3, 0.1 CaCl2. Slices recovered for 1 hr at 34°C in artificial cerebral spinal fluid (ACSF) continually bubbled with O2/CO2 and containing (in mM): 126 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 1.2 MgCl2 11 D-glucose, 18 NaHCO3, 2.4 CaCl2. For evoked PSP and mEPSC recordings patch electrodes were filled with an internal solution containing (in mM): 130 K-Gluconate, 10 KCl, 10 HEPES, 1 EGTA, 5 NaCl, 5 Mg-ATP, 0.5 Na-GTP, 5 QX-314, pH 7.2–7.4, 290 mOsm. For mIPSC recordings patch electrodes were filled with an internal solution containing (in mM): 145 KCl, 10 HEPES, 1 EGTA, 5 Mg-ATP, 0.5 Na-GTP, pH 7.2–7.4, 290 mOsm. ACSF at 32°C was continually perfused over slices at a rate of ∼2 ml/min during recording.

For PSP recordings a concentric bipolar electrode was placed in either the internal (thalamic) or external (cortical) lateral capsule. 1 ms stimuli were delivered at 0.1 Hz, and compound PSPs were recorded in current clamp mode adjusting stimulus intensity until half maximal responses were detected. 15–30 traces were averaged per cell, followed by bath application of picrotoxin (100 μM) to isolate the EPSP. 15–30 EPSP traces were averaged and digitally subtracted from the averaged compound PSP to isolate the IPSP.

mEPSC and mIPSC recordings were made in voltage clamp mode at a holding potential of −60 mV. mEPSCs were recorded in the presence of picrotoxin (100 μM) and tetrodotoxin (500 nM); mIPSCs were recorded in the presence of kynurenic acid (2 mM) and tetrodotoxin (500 nM). Events were detected automatically and were visually inspected and confirmed using Mini Analysis Program (Synaptosoft). Data were analyzed for significance by Student's t-test.

Histology

For Fos protein analysis, mice were handled and conditioned as above. Mice were euthanized and perfused 90 min following the start of a single conditioning session, which consisted of 10 CS + presentations randomly interspersed with 10 CS− presentations. The CS + co-terminated with a 0.3 mA, 0.5 s footshock (US). Following perfusion, 30 µm frozen sections were collected and incubated overnight at 4° in primary antibody (rabbit anti c-Fos, 1:2000, CalBiochem), then washed and incubated for 1 hr at room temperature in a fluorescently labeled secondary antibody (AlexaFluor488 donkey anti rabbit, 1:250, JacksonImmuno). Sections were imaged using a Nikon Diaphot 200 inverted microscope. c-fos positive cells in the lateral amygdala were counted manually by an investigator blind to genotype from −1.0 mm posterior to bregma to −2.3 mm posterior to bregma. Cells were counted bilaterally from every fifth section.

Statistics

Statistical analysis was performed using Prism software (GraphPad).

Acknowledgements

We thank members of the Zweifel lab for scientific discussion. This work was funded by the US National Institutes of Health (R01-MH094536, LSZ and R21-MH098177, LSZ).

Funding Statement

The funder had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health (NIH) R01-MH094536 to Larry S Zweifel.

  • National Institutes of Health (NIH) R21-MH098177 to Larry S Zweifel.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

GLJ, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

MES, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

CRK, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

HL, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

ASC, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

EBM, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

LSZ, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health and approved by the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee protocol (#4249-01). All surgical procedures were performed under isolflurane anesthesia with analagesic pretreatment. All efforts were made to minimize suffering.

References

  1. Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Fear and the human amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15:5879–5891. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-09-05879.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Apergis-Schoute AM, Debiec J, Doyere V, LeDoux JE, Schafe GE. Auditory fear conditioning and long-term potentiation in the lateral amygdala require ERK/MAP kinase signaling in the auditory thalamus: a role for presynaptic plasticity in the fear system. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:5730–5739. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0096-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ben-Ari Y, Le Gal la Salle G. Relationship between spontaneous and evoked unit activity in the amygdala of the cat. Brain Research. 1971;32:474–478. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90342-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bissiere S, Humeau Y, Luthi A. Dopamine gates LTP induction in lateral amygdala by suppressing feedforward inhibition. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:587–592. doi: 10.1038/nn1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bonci A, Malenka RC. Properties and plasticity of excitatory synapses on dopaminergic and GABAergic cells in the ventral tegmental area. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:3723–3730. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-03723.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Borowski TB, Kokkinidis L. Contribution of ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons to expression of conditional fear: effects of electrical stimulation, excitotoxin lesions, and quinpirole infusion on potentiated startle in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1996;110:1349–1364. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.110.6.1349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Botta P, Demmou L, Kasugai Y, Markovic M, Xu C, Fadok JP, Lu T, Poe MM, Xu L, Cook JM, Rudolph U, Sah P, Ferraguti F, Lüthi A. Regulating anxiety with extrasynaptic inhibition. Nature Neuroscience. 2015 doi: 10.1038/nn.4102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Breiter HC, Etcoff NL, Whalen PJ, Kennedy WA, Rauch SL, Buckner RL, Strauss MM, Hyman SE, Rosen BR. Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron. 1996;17:875–887. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brischoux F, Chakraborty S, Brierley DI, Ungless MA. Phasic excitation of dopamine neurons in ventral VTA by noxious stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2009;106:4894–4899. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811507106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Campeau S, Hayward MD, Hope BT, Rosen JB, Nestler EJ, Davis M. Induction of the c-fos proto-oncogene in rat amygdala during unconditioned and conditioned fear. Brain Research. 1991;565:349–352. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)91669-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cho JH, Bayazitov IT, Meloni EG, Myers KM, Carlezon WA, Jr, Zakharenko SS, Bolshakov VY. Coactivation of thalamic and cortical pathways induces input timing-dependent plasticity in amygdala. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:113–122. doi: 10.1038/nn.2993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ciocchi S, Herry C, Grenier F, Wolff SB, Letzkus JJ, Vlachos I, Ehrlich I, Sprengel R, Deisseroth K, Stadler MB, Müller C, Lüthi A. Encoding of conditioned fear in central amygdala inhibitory circuits. Nature. 2010;468:277–282. doi: 10.1038/nature09559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Collins DR, Pare D. Differential fear conditioning induces reciprocal changes in the sensory responses of lateral amygdala neurons to the CS(+) and CS(-) Learning & Memory. 2000;7:97–103. doi: 10.1101/lm.7.2.97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ehrlich I, Humeau Y, Grenier F, Ciocchi S, Herry C, Luthi A. Amygdala inhibitory circuits and the control of fear memory. Neuron. 2009;62:757–771. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.05.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fourcaudot E, Gambino F, Humeau Y, Casassus G, Shaban H, Poulain B, Lüthi A. cAMP/PKA signaling and RIM1alpha mediate presynaptic LTP in the lateral amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2008;105:15130–15135. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806938105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ghosh S, Chattarji S. Neuronal encoding of the switch from specific to generalized fear. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:112–120. doi: 10.1038/nn.3888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Goldstein ML. Effects of hippocampal, amygdala, hypothalamic and parietal lesions on a classically conditioned fear response. Psychological Reports. 1965;16:211–219. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1965.16.1.211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gore BB, Soden ME, Zweifel LS. Visualization of plasticity in fear-evoked calcium signals in midbrain dopamine neurons. Learning & Memory. 2014;21:575–579. doi: 10.1101/lm.036079.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Greba Q, Gifkins A, Kokkinidis L. Inhibition of amygdaloid dopamine D2 receptors impairs emotional learning measured with fear-potentiated startle. Brain Research. 2001;899:218–226. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02243-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Grillon C. Startle reactivity and anxiety disorders: aversive conditioning, context, and neurobiology. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:958–975. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01665-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Guarraci FA, Kapp BS. An electrophysiological characterization of ventral tegmental area dopaminergic neurons during differential pavlovian fear conditioning in the awake rabbit. Behavioural Brain Research. 1999;99:169–179. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00102-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Guarraci FA, Frohardt RJ, Kapp BS. Amygdaloid D1 dopamine receptor involvement in Pavlovian fear conditioning. Brain Research. 1999;827:28–40. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01291-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Guarraci FA, Frohardt RJ, Falls WA, Kapp BS. The effects of intra-amygdaloid infusions of a D2 dopamine receptor antagonist on Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2000;114:647–651. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Johansen JP, Hamanaka H, Monfils MH, Behnia R, Deisseroth K, Blair HT, LeDoux JE. Optical activation of lateral amygdala pyramidal cells instructs associative fear learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2010;107:12692–12697. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002418107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Johansen JP, Diaz-Mataix L, Hamanaka H, Ozawa T, Ycu E, Koivumaa J, Kumar A, Hou M, Deisseroth K, Boyden ES, LeDoux JE. Hebbian and neuromodulatory mechanisms interact to trigger associative memory formation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2014;111:E5584–E5592. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421304111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kerchner GA, Nicoll RA. Silent synapses and the emergence of a postsynaptic mechanism for LTP. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:813–825. doi: 10.1038/nrn2501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lammel S, Ion DI, Roeper J, Malenka RC. Projection-specific modulation of dopamine neuron synapses by aversive and rewarding stimuli. Neuron. 2011;70:855–862. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lamont EW, Kokkinidis L. Infusion of the dopamine D1 receptor antagonist SCH 23390 into the amygdala blocks fear expression in a potentiated startle paradigm. Brain Research. 1998;795:128–136. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00281-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. LeDoux JE, Cicchetti P, Xagoraris A, Romanski LM. The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10:1062–1069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-04-01062.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lee BR, Ma YY, Huang YH, Wang X, Otaka M, Ishikawa M, Neumann PA, Graziane NM, Brown TE, Suska A, Guo C, Lobo MK, Sesack SR, Wolf ME, Nestler EJ, Shaham Y, Schlüter OM, Dong Y. Maturation of silent synapses in amygdala-accumbens projection contributes to incubation of cocaine craving. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1644–1651. doi: 10.1038/nn.3533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Loretan K, Bissiere S, Luthi A. Dopaminergic modulation of spontaneous inhibitory network activity in the lateral amygdala. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47:631–639. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lovibond PF, Shanks DR. The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes. 2002;28:3–26. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.28.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mameli M, Halbout B, Creton C, Engblom D, Parkitna JR, Spanagel R, Lüscher C. Cocaine-evoked synaptic plasticity: persistence in the VTA triggers adaptations in the NAc. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:1036–1041. doi: 10.1038/nn.2367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Marcellino D, Frankowska M, Agnati L, Perez de la Mora M, Vargas-Barroso V, Fuxe K, Larriva-Sahd J. Intercalated and paracapsular cell islands of the adult rat amygdala: a combined rapid-Golgi, ultrastructural, and immunohistochemical account. Neuroscience. 2012;226:324–347. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.08.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Maren S, Quirk GJ. Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:844–852. doi: 10.1038/nrn1535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Maren S. Auditory fear conditioning increases CS-elicited spike firing in lateral amygdala neurons even after extensive overtraining. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;12:4047–4054. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00281.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Marowsky A, Yanagawa Y, Obata K, Vogt KE. A specialized subclass of interneurons mediates dopaminergic facilitation of amygdala function. Neuron. 2005;48:1025–1037. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McKernan MG, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear conditioning induces a lasting potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro. Nature. 1997;390:607–611. doi: 10.1038/37605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AW, Calder AJ, Dolan RJ. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature. 1996;383:812–815. doi: 10.1038/383812a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nader K, LeDoux JE. Inhibition of the mesoamygdala dopaminergic pathway impairs the retrieval of conditioned fear associations. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1999;113:891–901. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.113.5.891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Overton PG, Clark D. Burst firing in midbrain dopaminergic neurons. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews. 1997;25:312–334. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Amsterdam, Boston: Elsevier Academic Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  44. Quirk GJ, Repa C, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat. Neuron. 1995;15:1029–1039. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90092-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Quirk GJ, Armony JL, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning enhances different temporal components of tone-evoked spike trains in auditory cortex and lateral amygdala. Neuron. 1997;19:613–624. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80375-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;47:769–776. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00828-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rodrigues SM, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms underlying emotional learning and memory in the lateral amygdala. Neuron. 2004;44:75–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rogan MT, LeDoux JE. LTP is accompanied by commensurate enhancement of auditory-evoked responses in a fear conditioning circuit. Neuron. 1995;15:127–136. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90070-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rogan MT, Staubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–607. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Romanski LM, Clugnet MC, Bordi F, LeDoux JE. Somatosensory and auditory convergence in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1993;107:444–450. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.3.444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rosenkranz JA, Grace AA. Dopamine attenuates prefrontal cortical suppression of sensory inputs to the basolateral amygdala of rats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:4090–4103. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-11-04090.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rosenkranz JA, Grace AA. Dopamine-mediated modulation of odour-evoked amygdala potentials during pavlovian conditioning. Nature. 2002;417:282–287. doi: 10.1038/417282a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R. Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science. 2005;308:83–88. doi: 10.1126/science.1103944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schroeder BW, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear learning induces persistent facilitation of amygdala synaptic transmission. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;22:1775–1783. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04343.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Schultz W, Romo R. Responses of nigrostriatal dopamine neurons to high-intensity somatosensory stimulation in the anesthetized monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1987;57:201–217. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.57.1.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Shaban H, Humeau Y, Herry C, Cassasus G, Shigemoto R, Ciocchi S, Barbieri S, van der Putten H, Kaupmann K, Bettler B, Lüthi A. Generalization of amygdala LTP and conditioned fear in the absence of presynaptic inhibition. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:1028–1035. doi: 10.1038/nn1732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Shin RM, Tully K, Li Y, Cho JH, Higuchi M, Suhara T, Bolshakov VY. Hierarchical order of coexisting pre- and postsynaptic forms of long-term potentiation at synapses in amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2010;107:19073–19078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009803107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Slotnick BM. Fear behavior and passive avoidance deficits in mice with amygdala lesions. Physiology & Behavior. 1973;11:717–720. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(73)90258-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Suvrathan A, Bennur S, Ghosh S, Tomar A, Anilkumar S, Chattarji S. Stress enhances fear by forming new synapses with greater capacity for long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130151. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tovote P, Fadok JP, Luthi A. Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16:317–331. doi: 10.1038/nrn3945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Tsvetkov E, Carlezon WA, Benes FM, Kandel ER, Bolshakov VY. Fear conditioning occludes LTP-induced presynaptic enhancement of synaptic transmission in the cortical pathway to the lateral amygdala. Neuron. 2002;34:289–300. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00645-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Tye KM, Prakash R, Kim SY, Fenno LE, Grosenick L, Zarabi H, Thompson KR, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K. Amygdala circuitry mediating reversible and bidirectional control of anxiety. Nature. 2011;471:358–362. doi: 10.1038/nature09820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Weiskrantz L. Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1956;49:381–391. doi: 10.1037/h0088009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wilensky AE, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Functional inactivation of the amygdala before but not after auditory fear conditioning prevents memory formation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-j0006.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wolff SB, Grundemann J, Tovote P, Krabbe S, Jacobson GA, Muller C, Herry C, Ehrlich I, Friedrich RW, Letzkus JJ, Lüthi A. Amygdala interneuron subtypes control fear learning through disinhibition. Nature. 2014;509:453–458. doi: 10.1038/nature13258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Yamamoto R, Ueta Y, Kato N. Dopamine induces a slow afterdepolarization in lateral amygdala neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:984–992. doi: 10.1152/jn.00204.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Zinebi F, McKernan M, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Expression of fear-conditioning is accompanied by increased paired-pulse depression within the amygdala. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2002;71:393–400. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00684-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Zweifel LS, Argilli E, Bonci A, Palmiter RD. Role of NMDA receptors in dopamine neurons for plasticity and addictive behaviors. Neuron. 2008;59:486–496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Zweifel LS, Fadok JP, Argilli E, Garelick MG, Jones GL, Dickerson TM, Allen JM, Mizumori SJ, Bonci A, Palmiter RD. Activation of dopamine neurons is critical for aversive conditioning and prevention of generalized anxiety. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:620–626. doi: 10.1038/nn.2808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2015 Sep 24;4:e08969. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08969.016

Decision letter

Editor: Eunjoon Kim1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled “A Genetic Link Between Discriminative Fear Coding by the Lateral Amygdala, Dopamine, and Fear Generalization” for peer review at eLife. Your submission has been favorably evaluated by a Senior editor, a Reviewing editor, and two reviewers.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In the current manuscript, Jones and colleagues demonstrate that genetic inactivation of NR1 receptors in dopamine neurons results in aberrant fear discrimination and alterations of neural plasticity in the lateral amygdala, which they suggest may underlie fear generalization. While this is an important step towards elucidating the influence of dopaminergic signaling in discriminative learning, there are a few important points that should be clarified:

Essential revisions:

1) The c-fos experiment is performed after a single conditioning session. To what extent does this correspond to the behavioral procedure used for the subsequent experiments involving in vivo and in vitro recordings, in particular with regard to handling and pre-exposure?

2) The fear discrimination procedure implemented by the authors does not control for the possibility that constant illumination is more readily conditioned than pulsing illumination. Without this control, it remains possible that the impairment of the DAT-NR1 KO mice is simply due to altered sensory processing.

3) In the design of the in vitro recording experiments, the authors draw an equivalence between naïve mice and mice on Day 1 of in vivo recordings; similarly between mice 24 hr following the second conditioning session and mice on Day 3 of in vivo recordings. However, over the course of each conditioning day, mice receive 10 reinforced CS + trials and 10 non-reinforced CS- trials. Therefore, it seems particularly confounded to consider mice on Day 1 of in vivo recordings to be equivalent to naïve mice.

4) In the second paragraph of the Discussion, the authors conclude: “Our observations of increased frequency of both mIPSCs and mEPSCs, along with previous observations of increased excitatory input from both cortex and thalamus following fear conditioning (McKernan 1997, Zinebi 2002, Tsvetkov 2002), suggest a scenario in which both pathways are potentiated with proportional changes in excitatory and inhibitory inputs.” The authors should therefore provide the data independently for excitatory and inhibitory PSPs in order to substantiate this conclusion.

5) In the third paragraph of the Discussion, the authors write: “In contrast to previous report of generalized fear being associated with an over exuberance of plasticity and activation of the LA (Ghosh 2015), in DAT-NR1 KO mice with generalized fear the LA neurons appear as though no CS-US association has been made. More specifically, LA neurons in DAT-NR1 KO mice do not potentiate to the Cs+, fail to reduce inhibition to the CS-, and demonstrate reduced response latencies to the CS+ and CS-, similar to reduced response latencies to the CS- in control mice.” However, this conclusion appears to be based upon the assumption that the Day 1 recordings represent the baseline CS+ and CS- responses, yet over the course of Day 1 the mice receive 10 reinforced CS + trials and 10 non-reinforced CS- trials. Therefore, it would be very helpful to see the baseline (pre-training) CS+ and CS- responses for the reported units in Figures 4 and 5, in order to evaluate whether any plasticity of excitatory and/or inhibitory responses is already evident on Day 1 of conditioning.

6) Do the authors find a correlation between the magnitude of neural responsivity and/or proportion of CS-responsive neurons with the CS-responses of the mice?

7) In Figure 2–figure supplement 1B, the authors should provide the proportion of neurons that respond to CS+, CS- and CS+/CS-? Does this differ between control and DAT-NR1 KO mice?

8) Is there a difference in response latencies between control and DAT-NR1 KO mice? In Figure 2–figure supplement 1C and D, there appears to be a shorter response latency for both CS+ and CS- in DAT-NR1 KO mice particularly on Day 3.

eLife. 2015 Sep 24;4:e08969. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08969.017

Author response


1) The c-fos experiment is performed after a single conditioning session. To what extent does this correspond to the behavioral procedure used for the subsequent experiments involving in vivo and in vitro recordings, in particular with regard to handling and pre-exposure?

The Fos protein level analysis by immunohistochemistry was designed to address the extent to which activity dependent processes are engaged in control and DAT-NR1 KO mice following and initial conditioning session. The mice were treated the same way as animals that underwent fear conditioning for in vivo electrophysiology analysis except that in this assay mice were euthanized 90 min following conditioning. These animals would therefore be most representative of mice recorded during the first day of conditioning. All mice were handled for one week prior to conditioning. One major difference between mice that underwent conditioning for Fos and electrophysiology recordings (both in vitro and in vivo) vs mice that were analyzed for freezing is that mice for Fos and physiology did not receive any pre-exposure to the conditioning chamber. Mice used for Fos and physiology were also not exposed to the testing chamber and were only exposed to the conditioning chamber. Recording during conditioning allowed us to observe neural responses to both the CS and US presentations within the same session. We have included this additional information in our revised Methods section.

2) The fear discrimination procedure implemented by the authors does not control for the possibility that constant illumination is more readily conditioned than pulsing illumination. Without this control, it remains possible that the impairment of the DAT-NR1 KO mice is simply due to altered sensory processing.

We agree that it is possible that the two cues may be differentially conditioned based on whether they were constant or pulsing. It is also possible that this difference contributes to the discrimination deficits in DAT-NR1 KO mice. However, we have previously demonstrated that DAT-NR1 KO mice develop non-specific fear responses in novel contexts and generalized anxiety using a different conditioning paradigm indicating that generalization in these mice is broader than the context used here. In addition, we now report the electrophysiological responses of neurons activated and inhibited by CS+ and CS- stimuli in both control and DAT-NR1 KO mice during the first and last trial of conditioning on the first day. These results reveal that there are no major differences in the response profiles of these neurons on the first day indicating that at least at the level of the LA sensory information is initially processed equivalently. In addition, we also now provide in vivo electrophysiology data from control mice that received the same cues used in conditioning, but were not paired with footshock. This data reveals that response profiles of activated neurons does not change across multiple days of repeated stimulus presentation. Thus our results demonstrating enhanced responding of LA neurons to the CS+ during conditioning reflects associative fear processing and is not related to differential responding to repeated presentation of the different cue stimuli.

3) In the design of the in vitro recording experiments, the authors draw an equivalence between naïve mice and mice on Day 1 of in vivo recordings; similarly between mice 24 hr following the second conditioning session and mice on Day 3 of in vivo recordings. However, over the course of each conditioning day, mice receive 10 reinforced CS + trials and 10 non-reinforced CS- trials. Therefore, it seems particularly confounded to consider mice on Day 1 of in vivo recordings to be equivalent to naïve mice.

We apologize for the confusion on this matter. We have removed that statement from the manuscript and provided additional detail to the methods to clarify how each group of mice was treated. Although naïve mice were handled the same as conditioned mice they did not receive CS/US pairings. These mice were truly naïve and most closely represented conditioned mice at the start of day one of in vivo electrophysiology recording.

4) In the second paragraph of the Discussion, the authors conclude: “Our observations of increased frequency of both mIPSCs and mEPSCs, along with previous observations of increased excitatory input from both cortex and thalamus following fear conditioning (McKernan 1997, Zinebi 2002, Tsvetkov 2002), suggest a scenario in which both pathways are potentiated with proportional changes in excitatory and inhibitory inputs.” The authors should therefore provide the data independently for excitatory and inhibitory PSPs in order to substantiate this conclusion.

We agree that we cannot support this conclusion based on the mEPSC and mIPSC frequency. We have removed this statement and provided a much more in-depth discussion of these findings. With regard to the quantification of the PSPs, we cannot provide an accurate individual average of the IPSP or EPSPs due to variability in stimulating electrode placement. Typically PSPs are normalized to baseline responses to allow for comparisons across slices and across animals. Because we are comparing two independent groups in which baseline responses cannot be established, an alternative method of normalization is required. To achieve this we compared the ratio of EPSPs to IPSPs, similar to what is done for the generation of AMPA/NMDA receptor ratios. Because we did not see selective effects on EPSPs or IPSPs in either pathway, we analyzed mEPSCs and mIPSCs which do allow for more accurate direct comparisons across groups. In the latter instance we observed and increase in the frequency of both mEPSCs and mIPSCs indicating that both excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs are changed in control mice, but not DAT-NR1 KO mice.

5) In the third paragraph of the Discussion, the authors write: “In contrast to previous report of generalized fear being associated with an over exuberance of plasticity and activation of the LA (Ghosh 2015), in DAT-NR1 KO mice with generalized fear the LA neurons appear as though no CS-US association has been made. More specifically, LA neurons in DAT-NR1 KO mice do not potentiate to the Cs+, fail to reduce inhibition to the CS-, and demonstrate reduced response latencies to the CS+ and CS-, similar to reduced response latencies to the CS- in control mice.” However, this conclusion appears to be based upon the assumption that the Day 1 recordings represent the baseline CS+ and CS- responses, yet over the course of Day 1 the mice receive 10 reinforced CS + trials and 10 non-reinforced CS- trials. Therefore, it would be very helpful to see the baseline (pre-training) CS+ and CS- responses for the reported units in Figures 4 and 5, in order to evaluate whether any plasticity of excitatory and/or inhibitory responses is already evident on Day 1 of conditioning.

We agree that mice receiving CS/US presentations on day 1 are not equivalent to naïve mice and we have modified previous statements. We have also clarified the text to better convey that mice that underwent fear conditioning to assess freezing are not the same mice that were used for in vivo recordings, or slice recordings. Unfortunately, we did not record baseline cue responses prior to conditioning in mice that underwent in vivo recordings. However, we did investigate the initial responses to both the CS+ and CS- cue presentations in both groups (Day 1, trial 1) and compared those responses to the last trial of the day (Day 1, trial 10). We did not observe significant differences in the responses of either group to the CS+ and CS- cue presentation on the first vs last trial. This data is now included in Figure 4–figure supplement 2.

6) Do the authors find a correlation between the magnitude of neural responsivity and/or proportion of CS-responsive neurons with the CS-responses of the mice?

As mentioned above, we did not perform behavioral analysis on mice undergoing in vivo recordings. We find that freezing behavior in mice implanted with microdrives and tethered to pre-amplifiers is unreliable. For this reason we assessed behavior in independent cohorts of mice and cannot directly correlate unit responses to freezing behavior.

7) In Figure 2–figure supplement 1B, the authors should provide the proportion of neurons that respond to CS+, CS- and CS+/CS-? Does this differ between control and DAT-NR1 KO mice?

We have now included the proportion of neurons that respond to both CS+ and CS- stimuli across days of conditioning within the manuscript text. We did not observe differences in this proportion across days or across groups, with the majority showing responses to both cues (∼80%).

8) Is there a difference in response latencies between control and DAT-NR1 KO mice? In Figure 2–figure supplement 1C and D, there appears to be a shorter response latency for both CS+ and CS- in DAT-NR1 KO mice particularly on Day 3.

We have included the analysis comparing the response latencies to the different stimuli on days 1 and 3 of conditioning. We observed a significant difference in response latency in control vs DAT-NR1 KO mice on Day 3 of conditioning, with controls displaying significantly shorter latencies that DAT-NR1 KO mice.


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES