Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2015 Aug 14;45(16):3539–3548. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001440

A longitudinal, population-based twin study of avoidant and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder traits from early to middle adulthood

L C Gjerde 1,*, N Czajkowski 1,2, E Røysamb 1,2, E Ystrom 2, K Tambs 1, S H Aggen 3,4, R E Ørstavik 1, K S Kendler 3,4,5, T Reichborn-Kjennerud 1,6,7, G P Knudsen 1
PMCID: PMC4623996  NIHMSID: NIHMS717244  PMID: 26273730

Abstract

Background

The phenotypic stability of avoidant personality disorder (AVPD) and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD) has previously been found to be moderate. However, little is known about the longitudinal structure of genetic and environmental factors for these disorders separately and jointly, and to what extent genetic and environmental factors contribute to their stability.

Method

AVPD and OCPD criteria were assessed using the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality in 2793 young adult twins (1385 pairs, 23 singletons) from the Norwegian Institute of Public Health Twin Panel at wave 1 and 2282 (986 pairs, 310 singletons) of these on average 10 years later at wave 2. Longitudinal biometric models were fitted to AVPD and OCPD traits.

Results

For twins who participated at both time-points, the number of endorsed sub-threshold criteria for both personality disorders (PDs) decreased 31% from wave 1 to wave 2. Phenotypic correlations between waves were 0.54 and 0.37 for AVPD and OCPD, respectively. The heritability estimates of the stable PD liabilities were 0.67 for AVPD and 0.53 for OCPD. The genetic correlations were 1.00 for AVPD and 0.72 for OCPD, while the unique environmental influences correlated 0.26 and 0.23, respectively. The correlation between the stable AVPD and OCPD liabilities was 0.39 of which 63% was attributable to genetic influences. Shared environmental factors did not significantly contribute to PD variance at either waves 1 or 2.

Conclusion

Phenotypic stability was moderate for AVPD and OCPD traits, and genetic factors contributed more than unique environmental factors to the stability both within and across phenotypes.

Keywords: Avoidant personality disorder traits, longitudinal studies, Norwegian Twin Registry, obsessive-compulsive personality traits, twin studies

Introduction

Avoidant personality disorder (AVPD) and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD) have, since the introduction of the DSM-III manual (APA, 1980), been placed in the DSM Cluster C classification nosology, characterized by anxiousness and fearfulness. Being two of the most prevalent personality disorders (PDs) (Zimmerman et al. 2005; Bernstein et al. 2014; Reichborn-Kjennerud & Knudsen, 2014), they are associated with major societal costs in terms of medical expenses and loss of productivity (Soeteman et al. 2008).

AVPD and OCPD are, along with the other DSM-5 PDs, assumed to have an onset in adolescence or early adulthood and to be fairly persistent over time (APA, 2013). Stability has always been a central part of the definition of PDs, and the introduction of DSM Axis II from 1980 was partly due to the assumption that PDs are more enduring than other mental disorders (Krueger, 2005). Empirical studies on stability of PDs did, however, not show up until several years later (Lenzenweger, 1999). More recently, longitudinal studies ranging from 2 to 10 years, on the phenotypic stability of AVPD and OCPD, have demonstrated that the absolute stability, defined as consistency in the number of endorsed criteria (Morey & Hopwood, 2013) is moderate, with a decrease in symptom mean levels ranging from 0% to 57% (Johnson et al. 2000; Grilo et al. 2004), and that the rank-order stability, defined as temporal consistency in the ranking of individuals (Morey & Hopwood, 2013) is modest, with Pearson correlations ranging from 0.13 to 0.67 (Lenzenweger, 1999; Johnson et al. 2000; Grilo et al. 2004; Sanislow et al. 2009; Hopwood et al. 2013). AVPD is often found to be more stable than OCPD.

Previous cross-sectional twin studies have established that both AVPD and OCPD are influenced by genetic factors (Torgersen et al. 2000; Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. 2007), and that the heritability of PDs increases substantially when measurement error is taken into account (Kendler et al. 2007; Gjerde et al. 2012; Torgersen et al. 2012). To what extent the latter point applies to OCPD is currently unknown. Despite being placed within the same cluster, the two disorders have been found to have mainly distinct genetic and environmental influences (Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. 2007; Kendler et al. 2008). However, studies on how the genetic and environmental effects of these PDs relate to one another and develop over time are currently lacking.

In the present study, we applied biometric twin models to longitudinal data from the Norwegian Institute of Public Health Twin Panel (NIPHT) on DSM-IV AVPD and OCPD traits assessed in early adulthood and approximately 10 years later. With this approach we sought to: (i) determine the phenotypic stability of AVPD and OCPD, (ii) estimate the time-specific and stable genetic and environmental influences on the two PDs, and (iii) examine the etiology of the association between the parts of AVPD and OCPD that are stable over time.

Material and method

Sample

Twins were recruited from the Norwegian Twin Registry (NTR) at the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. The participants were identified through information from the Norwegian Medical Birth Registry (MBR), which was established 1 January 1967. The twins born between 1967 and 1979 (N = 15 374) were invited to participate in questionnaire studies in 1992 and 1998. Altogether, 12 700 received the second questionnaire, and 8045 twins responded after one reminder (63%). Zygosity was determined by the use of questionnaire items for the entire sample, as well as by microsatellite markers for 676 of the same-sex twin pairs, which in a combined discriminant analysis predicted a zygosity misclassification rate of <1% of pairs (Tambs et al. 2009). The NTR is further described in detail elsewhere (Harris et al. 2006; Nilsen et al. 2013).

The data derive from the studies ‘Mental Health among Twins’ and ‘Axis I and Axis II psychiatric disorders in Norwegian Twins’ (AI/AII), conducted from 1998 to 2004 (wave 1) and a follow-up interview and questionnaire study (AI/AII FU), conducted in 2010 and 2011 (wave 2). Wave 1 interviews were mainly face-to-face, although for practical reasons 231 interviews (8.3%) were done by telephone. Of the 6442 eligible twins, 2801 twins (43.5% of those eligible) participated in wave 1 (1390 complete pairs and 21 singletons). To maximize participation rate, interviews in wave 2 included only a selection of the disorders, and were conducted by telephone. Of the 2801 twins that participated in wave 1, 17 had withdrawn their consent, 14 had unknown addresses and 12 had died, leaving 2758 eligible twins who were invited to participate in a follow-up study. Altogether, after two written reminders and a final telephone contact to non-responders, 2284 twins (82.8%, 987 complete pairs and 310 singletons) were interviewed in wave 2.

Interviewers at wave 1 were mainly senior clinical psychology graduate students, in addition to experienced psychiatric nurses and medical students. Interviewers at wave 2 comprised senior clinical psychology graduate students, experienced psychiatric nurses, experienced clinical psychologists that also were interviewers at wave 1 and clinical nurse specialists. Interviewers received a standardized training program administered by one psychiatrist and two psychologists. The interviewers received supervision during the entire data collection period. At both waves each twin in a pair was interviewed by a different interviewer.

Inter-rater reliability at wave 1 was assessed based on two raters scoring 70 audiotaped interviews. The number of subjects with specific PDs was too low to calculate kappa coefficients. Intra-class (and polychoric) correlations for the number of criteria endorsed at the sub-threshold level (see below) were all high: AVPD 0.96 (0.97), and OCPD 0.92 (0.87). At wave 2, inter-rater reliability was assessed similarly by two raters’ re-scoring of 95 audiotaped interviews. Intra-class (and polychoric) correlations for the number of endorsed sub-threshold criteria were: AVPD 0.84 (0.92), and OCPD 0.75 (0.83).

The studies were approved by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics, and informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

A Norwegian version of the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV; Pfohl & Zimmerman, 1995) was used to assess all 10 DSM-IV PDs at wave 1 and six PDs (two from each DSM-IV cluster) at wave 2; paranoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, avoidant and obsessive-compulsive. The SIDP-IV is a comprehensive semi-structured diagnostic interview that includes non-pejorative questions organized into topical sections (e.g. social relationships and work style) rather than by individual PDs, thereby improving the interview flow.

The SIDP-IV uses a ‘5-year rule’, meaning that behaviors, cognitions and feelings that predominated for most of the past 5 years are considered representative of an individual’s long-term personality. Each DSM-IV criterion is scored on a 4-point scale (0 = absent, 1 = sub-threshold, 2 = present, 3 = strongly present).

We used a dimensional modeling approach that operationalized each of the PDs as ordinal count variables instead of diagnostic cut-off scores. Each criterion for each PD scored ≥1 (sub-threshold) increased the count variable by 1. The two count variables AVPD and OCPD are meant to reflect avoidant and obsessive-compulsive PD traits – that is, PD tendencies that do not necessarily constitute a clinical PD diagnosis (Krueger et al. 2012; APA, 2013).

Using multiple threshold tests, our group has previously shown that the use of individual sub-threshold PD criteria reflected varying levels of severity on a single continuum of liability (Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. 2007).

Due to low frequencies of high counts, the PD variables were further truncated by collapsing the upper criteria counts into four ordered categories (0–3) for AVPD and to five categories (0–4) for OCPD to avoid empty cells in the twin analyses. This approach has been used in previous publications on the same sample (Kendler et al. 2006, 2007; Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. 2007; Torgersen et al. 2008, 2012; Gjerde et al. 2012) and is, according to results from multiple threshold tests, consistent with differences in severity on a normally distributed continuum of liability (Kendler et al. 2008). The phenotypes under study are therefore not AVPD and OCPD at a diagnostic level, but AVPD and OCPD traits.

From the interviews in wave 1, 2793 twins had valid data for DSM-IV PDs; 220 monozygotic male (MZM), 117 dizygotic male (DZM), 449 monozygotic female (MZF), 259 dizygotic female (DZF) and 340 dizygotic unlike-sexed (DZU) twin pairs, in addition to 23 single twins. From the follow-up in wave 2, 2282 twins had valid data for DSM-IV PDs; 154 MZM, 76 DZM, 358 MZF, 179 DZF and 219 DZU twin pairs, in addition to 310 single twins.

Statistical analyses

Stability analyses

In order to investigate absolute stability, we calculated the difference in the average number of endorsed sub-threshold criteria from wave 1 to wave 2. Rank-order stability was measured using polychoric correlations between the sub-threshold-criteria count-variable at waves 1 and 2, as well as Pearson correlations (included to obtain meaningful comparisons with previous studies). All phenotypic analyses were conducted in the statistical environment and programming language R version 2.15 (R Development Core Team, 2012) using the packages ‘polycor’ and ‘psych’ (Fox, 2010; Revelle, 2013), on non-truncated scores.

Longitudinal twin model fitting

Our twin models are based on the classical twin design, where the total variation in the PD traits (disorder liability) studied is assumed to come from three different sources: additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E). MZ twin pairs share all, and DZ twin pairs on average half of their segregating genes. Thus, A would tend to make MZ twins correlate twice as high as DZ twins. C is defined as environmental factors that contribute to similarity between twins (such as childhood socioeconomic conditions, diet, etc.), and is assumed to have an equal effect on MZ and DZ pairs. E is per definition environmentally based experiences not shared between twins in a pair, and assumed therefore not to contribute to twin similarity. The E factor also contains random measurement error. Using this decomposition framework, the influence of the A, C and E factors on AVPD and OCPD at waves 1 and 2 can be estimated.

As the data were ordinal, we applied a liability-threshold approach (Falconer, 1965), where it is assumed that ordered categories (in this case the truncated PD trait scores) are indicators of an unobserved, normally distributed liability that can be estimated as thresholds discriminating between categories. Liability-threshold models were fitted using full information maximum likelihood (FIML) applied to raw data with the open source statistical software OpenMx (Boker et al. 2011). If minimum regularity conditions are satisfied, the difference in −2 times log likelihood (Δ − 2LL) is asymptotically χ2-distributed, which allows testing for significant deterioration in χ2 for nested submodels. Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC), calculated as −2LL – 2df (Akaike, 1987) penalizes more highly parameterized models, and is used for selecting a best-fitting model. The best model is reflected by the lowest AIC value.

With two separate measures of each PD over different periods of time, it is possible to model a latent liability stability factor to each PD trait set as part of the psychometric measurement model. In our model (shown in Fig. 1), we assume that each variable has variance that to some extent is shared between the two measurement time-points. This shared variation between the measured PD variables across waves 1 and 2 can be modeled as common psychometric factors for each PD (FAV for AVPD and FOC for OCPD, also referred to as the latent PD factors). The f pathways (fAV and fOC) from the latent PD factors to the measured PD variables were constrained to be equal across time and index temporal stability in the PDs. The latent PD factors are both influenced by a set of genetic and environmental factors common to the same PD across time (ACAV, CCAV, and ECAV for AVPD, and ACOC, CCOC and ECOC for OCPD) through pathways ac1, cc1, ec1, ac2, cc2, and ec2. Squaring the ac pathways gives the heritability of the stable component of each PD factor. These common genetic and environmental factors are allowed to correlate across PD sets (rac, rcc, rec). They reflect the degree to which additive genetic, shared and unique environmental sources of the across-time PD stability correlate between AVPD and OCPD. A correlation of unity on any of these common factors would indicate that the risk factors are completely shared between the two PD factors.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The full longitudinal structural equation path model used for the twin analyses, shown for one member of a twin pair. AVPD, Avoidant personality disorder; OCPD, obsessive compulsive personality disorder; W1, wave 1; W2, wave 2; A, additive genetic factors; C, shared environmental factors; E, unique environmental factors; ASOC1 and ASOC2, time-specific A factors for OCPD at waves 1 and 2, respectively, ASAV1 and ASAV2, time-specific A factors for AVPD at waves 1 and 2, respectively, CSOC1 and CSOC2, time-specific C factors for OCPD; CSAV1 and CSAV2, time-specific C factors for AVPD; ESOC1 and ESOC2, time-specific E factors for OCPD; ESAV1 and ESAV2, time-specific E factors for AVPD; ACOC, stable A factor for OCPD; ACAV, stable A factor for AVPD; CCOC, stable C factor for OCPD; CCAV, stable C factor for AVPD; ECOC, stable E factor for OCPD; ECAV, stable E factor for AVPD; FOC, latent OCPD construct; FAV, latent AVPD construct; fOC, factor loading reflecting stability coefficient for OCPD; fAV, factor loading reflecting stability coefficient for AVPD; suffix C, common (stable) factors and paths; suffix S, time-specific factors and paths; rAS1 and rAS2, within-wave specific A correlations; rCS1 and rCS2, within-wave specific C correlations; rES1 and rES2, within-wave E correlations; rAC, stable (time-invariant) A correlation; rCC, stable C correlation; rEC, stable E correlation.

The observed unique PD variances are also decomposed into A, C, and E time-specific components (ASAV1, CSAV1, ESAV1, ASOC1, CSOC1, ESOC1 and ASAV2, CSAV2, ESAV2, ASOC2, CSOC2, ESOC2) which are allowed to correlate within time (rAS1, rCS1, rES1 and rAS2, rCS2 and rES2).

Due to limited sample size we did not have statistical power to investigate sex differences in genetic and environmental effects. The differences between the phenotypic correlations for the same sex DZ twins compared to the opposite sex DZ twins at both waves suggested that qualitative sex differences might be present for the phenotypes. However, no significant sex differences for AVPD and OCPD have been found in previous analyses in this sample (Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. 2007; Gjerde et al. 2012). The model parameters were therefore set equal for males and females, but we allowed the thresholds to differ between the sexes as there were differences in the number of endorsed sub-threshold criteria.

Results

Descriptives

The mean age of the participants at wave 1 was 28.2 (range 19–36) years and 37.9 (range 30–44) years at wave 2. On average, there were 9.6 (s.d. = 1.39) years between the two interviews.

Stability

The mean [and standard deviation (s.d.)] of the count of sub-threshold endorsed criteria, indexing absolute stability for AVPD and OCPD traits for the sample participating in both waves 1 and 2 interviews are shown in Table 1. The average count of endorsed sub-threshold criteria decreased 43% for males and 23% for females for AVPD, and 30% for males and 32% for females for OCPD. In total, the decline was 31% from wave 1 to wave 2 for both AVPD and OCPD. Mean change for each individual (in absolute values) was 0.8 criteria for AVPD and 1.4 for OCPD.

Table 1.

Mean number of endorsed sub-threshold criteria for AVPD and OCPD at waves 1 and 2

AVPD
wave 1
AVPD
wave 2
OCPD
wave 1
OCPD
wave 2
Males 0.90 (s.d. = 1.32) 0.51 (s.d. = 1.08) 2.10 (s.d. = 1.65) 1.48 (s.d. = 1.50)
N = 801 N = 801 N = 801 N = 801
Females 0.94 (s.d. = 1.40) 0.72 (s.d. = 1.31) 1.84 (s.d. = 1.61) 1.25 (s.d. = 1.38)
N = 1477 N = 1477 N = 1477 N = 1476
All 0.93 (s.d. = 1.37) 0.64 (s.d. = 1.24) 1.92 (s.d. = 1.63) 1.33 (s.d. = 1.43)
N = 2278 N = 2278 N = 2278 N = 2277

AVPD, Avoidant personality disorder; OCPD, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder; s.d., standard deviation.

The polychoric correlations [95% confidence interval (CI)] between waves 1 and 2, indexing rank-order stability were 0.54 (95% CI 0.50–0.58) for males and 0.54 (95% CI 0.50–0.57) for females for AVPD, and 0.39 (95% CI 0.34–0.44) for males and 0.36 (95% CI 0.32–0.40) for females for OCPD. For the sexes in total, the polychoric correlations were 0.54 (95% CI 0.51–0.57) for AVPD and 0.37 (95% CI 0.34–0.40) for OCPD. Corresponding numbers for the Pearson’s correlation coefficients were 0.47 (95% CI 0.44–0.50) for AVPD, and 0.34 (95% CI 0.30–0.38) for OCPD.

Longitudinal twin model fitting

A full ACE twin model as depicted in Fig. 1 was first fitted to the AVPD and OCPD longitudinal data, and then compared to reduced submodels. As shown in Table 2, dropping all the C paths from the full model did not result in significant deterioration in χ2 and was associated with lower AIC (model 2). Dropping all A paths in model 3, however, resulted in a significant worsening of fit. Subsequent reduced submodels were therefore based on model 2, (an AE model) in order to examine further etiologically informative aspects. In model 4, we constrained the specific A paths to be equal across waves within each PD, resulting in improved fit. Dropping the correlation between the specific A paths in model 5 also did not result in a significant deterioration in χ2 and further improved the fit according to AIC. In model 6, we dropped the specific A paths for AVPD and this restriction resulted in a lower AIC. However, attempting to drop the specific A paths for OCPD as well (model 7) resulted in a higher AIC than in models 4, 5 and 6. The best-fitting model according to AIC was therefore model 6, an AE model without specific A paths for AVPD (Δχ2 = 2.42, Δdf = 16, p = 1.0, ΔAIC = 29.59). The parameter estimates with 95% CIs from the best-fitting model are shown in Fig. 2.

Table 2.

Model fitting results for avoidant personality disorder and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder traits at waves 1 and 2

Model Δχ2 Δdf p ΔAIC
1. ACEa
2. AE 0.27 9 1.0 −17.74
3. CE 28.04 9 0.00 10.03
4. AE (A’s equal within PD) 0.44 13 1.0 −25.57
5. AE (r between A’s fixed to 0) 2.42 15 1.0 −27.59
6. AE (A’s for AVPD fixed to 0) 2.42 16 1.0 −29.59
7. AE (all A’s fixed to 0) 10.57 17 0.88 −23.44

Best fitting model in bold type.

All comparisons of χ2, df and AIC are against the full ACE model.

A, additive genetic effects; C, shared environmental effects; E, unique environmental effects; PD, personality disorder; AVPD, avoidant personality disorder; AIC, Akaike’s Information Criterion.

a

The full model had −2LL = 25 351.53, df = 10 098 and number of parameters estimated = 57.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Parameter estimates (and 95% confidence intervals) for the best-fitting model. AVPD, Avoidant personality disorder; OCPD, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder; W1, wave 1; W2, wave 2; FOC, latent OCPD construct; FAV, latent AVPD construct; ASOC1 and ASOC2, time-specific A factors for OCPD at waves 1 and 2, respectively; ASAV1 and ASAV2, time-specific A factors for AVPD at waves 1 and 2, respectively; ESOC1 and ESOC2, time-specific E factors for OCPD; ESAV1 and ESAV2, time-specific E factors for AVPD; ACOC, stable A factor for OCPD; ACAV, stable A factor for AVPD; ECOC, stable E factor for OCPD; ECAV, stable E factor for AVPD.

The heritability of the stable AVPD factor (FAV) was 0.67 (0.822), whereas the heritability of the observed AVPD measures was 0.35 [(0.72 × 0.82)2]. For the stable OCPD factor (FOC), the heritability was 0.53 (0.732), whereas the heritability of the observed OCPD measures was 0.26 [0.272 + (0.60 × 0.73)2].

The estimated factor loadings equated across waves within AVPD and OCPD were 0.72 and 0.60, respectively. The heritability estimate of the common stable factors is interpreted as the proportion of the total latent stability variance that is attributable to genetic variance (in this case: 67% for AVPD and 53% for OCPD). The rest of the stability was explained by unique environmental sources.

The correlation between the two latent PD constructs (FAV and FOC) was 0.39. Genetic and environmental correlations were modest and estimated to be 0.41 (95% CI 0.26–0.56) and 0.37 (95% CI 0.11–0.40), respectively. The proportion of overlap between the two PD constructs that was due to genetic variance (bivariate heritability) was 0.63.

The full genetic and environmental correlations (not split into time-specific and time-invariant correlations as in Figs 1 and 2) within and between the two PDs at both time-points are shown in Table 3. The genetic correlation between AVPD at waves 1 and 2 was 1.00 (95% CI 1.00–1.00), and slightly lower, at 0.72 (95% CI 0.54–0.93) between OCPD at waves 1 and 2. The extremely narrow confidence interval for AVPD is due to the dropping of specific A paths, which corresponds to fixing the across-wave AVPD correlation to 1. The environmental correlations were much lower: 0.26 (95% CI 0.23–0.27) for AVPD and 0.23 (95% CI 0.16–0.30) for OCPD.

Table 3.

Genetic and environmental correlations between waves 1 and 2 AVPD and OCPD traits

AVPD wave 1 AVPD wave 2 OCPD wave 1 OCPD wave 2
AVPD wave 1 1.00a 0.35 (0.22–0.47) 0.35 (0.22–0.47)
AVPD wave 2 0.26 (0.23–0.27) 0.35 (0.22–0.47) 0.35 (0.22–0.47)
OCPD wave 1 0.20 (0.13–0.25) 0.09 (0.04–0.10) 0.72 (0.54–0.93)
OCPD wave 2 0.09 (0.07–0.10) 0.38 (0.34–0.45) 0.23 (0.16–0.30)

AVPD, Avoidant personality disorder; OCPD, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder.

The genetic correlations between the personality disorders are shown in the upper triangle, the environmental correlations in the lower triangle. 95% confidence intervals are given in parentheses.

a

There is no confidence interval for the genetic correlation between AVPD waves 1 and 2 as this correlation has in effect been fixed to 1.00.

Discussion

Using a population-based sample of Norwegian twins interviewed approximately 10 years apart, we sought to investigate phenotypic as well as genetic and environmental stability of AVPD and OCPD traits.

We found only two relevant studies on absolute stability and four on rank-order stability with which to compare our results. In the clinical Collaborative Longitudinal Study on PDs (Skodol et al. 2005), AVPD symptoms declined 51% for men and 55% for women, whereas OCPD symptoms declined 53% for men and 57% for women over 10 years (Sanislow et al. 2009). This was a larger drop than found in the present study (from 43% to 23% for AVPD for men and women, respectively and from 3% to 32% for OCPD for men and women, respectively), and may be due to the differences in study samples. Our estimates of absolute stability also differ from a community-based sample of adolescent subjects followed to adulthood, where AVPD symptoms were found to decline 57%, while OCPD symptoms did not change at all during 10 years (Johnson et al. 2000). This discrepancy may be due to the differences in age range in the samples. In contrast to other PDs, OCPD is found to have higher prevalence in older adults (Grant et al. 2012), and hence the few OCPD cases in the Johnson et al. (2000) study could be expected to persist rather than decline at this point in life.

A reasonable suggestion for the decrease in symptom levels in the present study might be that self-esteem increases during young adulthood (Erol & Orth, 2011). It is also possible that having children may suppress the reporting of symptoms, in that one may be less self-focused. However, further studies are needed to clarify the mechanisms behind symptom decline in PDs.

The rank-order stability in the present study was moderate; with Pearson correlations of 0.47 for AVPD and 0.34 for OCPD. Pearson’s r for AVPD approximately 10 years apart has previously been found to range between 0.30 and 0.42 (Johnson et al. 2000; Sanislow et al. 2009; Hopwood et al. 2013), which is slightly lower than found in the present study. For OCPD, however, the estimates from previous studies, ranging from 0.13 to 0.32 (Johnson et al. 2000; Sanislow et al. 2009; Hopwood et al. 2013), are closer to our findings.

The most central findings of the present study are the relative contributions from genes and environmental factors to the stability of AVPD and OCPD traits. The stability factor loadings (f) were substantial for both PDs, but highest for AVPD. These coefficients were also to a large extent explained by genetic factors present at both waves. The stability of the genetic effects for AVPD is reflected in the genetic correlations (Table 3), where the correlation across waves was not significantly different from unity. For OCPD, the picture is slightly different, as the total genetic correlation was somewhat lower, in addition to the evidence for time-specific genetic factors contributing significantly to the variation at waves 1 and 2 (Fig. 2). We can only speculate what these age-specific factors might reflect, but as OCPD often has a higher prevalence later in life (Grant et al. 2012), persons who develop OCPD in adolescence or early adulthood might have an additional genetic risk factor operating, for instance through comorbidity with other PDs or mental disorders, or through a particular liability to high conscientiousness or compulsivity. Nevertheless, the genetic stability within the observed PD measures was remarkably high. This implies that genetic influences on PDs stabilize in early adulthood and do not change much throughout middle adulthood. Our findings fit well with what has recently been found for Cluster A and B PDs (Kendler et al. 2014; Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. unpublished data).

The unique environmental influences, however, were to a much higher extent specific to the two waves (Fig. 2). In fact, the environmental time-specific factors contribute with around half the variance to the observed PD trait scores. This probably implies that a non-negligible amount of measurement error is contributing to the total PD variance, but that there are, to some degree, differences between the environmental exposures that influence liability to AVPD or OCPD in early adulthood and 10 years later. Thus, possible environmental targets for interventions might be different at different ages. More research should be conducted to identify what types of exposures are associated with liability to AVPD and OCPD, as this information would be valuable for prevention and treatment of these PDs.

In addition to being specific, the environmental factors also contributed to the stability, and thus in maintaining symptoms of AVPD and OCPD traits. This seems more the case for OCPD, as almost half of the stable variance was explained by environmental factors common to both waves. As the model we have used corrects for measurement errors, these environmental factors contributing to PD stability probably reflect true environmental influences. The most important reason, however, for why there was an association between the stable, time-invariant aspects of AVPD and OCPD was shared genetic influences, explaining 63% of the association. This is in contrast to previous findings, where OCPD was found to be mostly genetically distinct from AVPD and the third Cluster C PD – dependent PD (Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. 2007).

By using the same structured interview at both waves, we found a heritability estimate of 0.67 for the stable AVPD factor. This is reassuringly close to the latent heritability found in a previous publication based on the data from the present sample’s first wave responses in addition to questionnaire data (0.64; Gjerde et al. 2012). For OCPD, there are, to the best of our knowledge, no previous studies with which to compare our result. However, the latent heritability of 0.53 is slightly lower than what has been found for dimensional representations of the other nine DSM-IV PDs – which ranged from 0.55 to 0.72 (Kendler et al. 2007; Gjerde et al. 2012; Torgersen et al. 2012). This fits well with the findings from previous studies on PD heritability from our group (Reichborn-Kjennerud et al. 2007), where OCPD was found to have a lower amount of the total variance explained by genetic variance, compared to AVPD and dependent PD.

Strengths and limitations

This is, to our knowledge, the first population-based longitudinal study of two of the Cluster C PDs investigating genetic and environmental contributions to the PDs at each time-point, measured with the same structured interview (SIDP-IV). This provides a unique opportunity to investigate time-invariant and time-specific influences, as well as estimating the heritability of the stable aspects of AVPD and OCPD traits. Although approximately 10 years separated the first and second interviews, the attrition rate was remarkably low, as 82.8% of the original sample also participated in the second wave.

The results should, however, be interpreted in light of some potentially important limitations. First, as the wave 2 interviews were conducted by phone, it was necessary to reduce the interview length. Assessment of dependent PD was therefore not included in this interview, which prevents giving a full picture of the Cluster C genetic architecture.

Second, analyses of sex differences on genetic and environmental effects in the present study were not included, due to insufficient power. Preliminary exploratory analyses, however, did not reveal evidence for qualitative or quantitative sex differences. Studies with larger sample sizes are needed before firm conclusions can be drawn on this matter.

Third, we used AVPD and OCPD traits instead of categorical diagnoses. It has previously been shown that counts of criteria index the same underlying liability that underlies full clinical PD diagnoses (Kendler et al. 2008). With this approach it follows that much of the variance in the data used originates from individuals that are below the diagnostic threshold for clinical PD diagnoses. However, as the multiple threshold tests indicated that different criteria scores represent different degrees of severity on a single continuum, we expect no qualitative differences in the results compared to if we had used categorical PD diagnoses.

Fourth, although the attrition rate was low from wave 1 to wave 2 (17.2%), there was substantial attrition from the original sample to the first interview wave. Attrition may affect generalizability, and if nonrandom, may also result in biased estimates of genetic and environmental effects (Heath et al. 1998). Detailed analyses have been conducted to investigate the patterns of attrition, and out of a broad range of predictors, including predictors of mental health, participation was clearly influenced only by female sex, monozygosity, older age, and higher educational status (Tambs et al. 2009). We also investigated attrition from wave 1 to wave 2 using GEE corrected logistic regression analyses, controlling for sex and age. Participation at wave 2 was neither significantly predicted by the amount of AVPD sub-threshold criteria at wave 1 (p = 0.06), nor by the amount of OCPD sub-threshold criteria (p = 0.89) at wave 1.

Conclusion

In this population-based twin study of AVPD and OCPD traits measured 10 years apart, we found evidence for moderate phenotypic stability. The stability was mostly influenced by genetic factors shared between the latent PD trait constructs, whereas the unique environmental contributions were much more specific. AVPD and OCPD were moderately associated with each other, and the association was mostly due to genetic influences shared between both disorders. Future studies that can pinpoint which environmental factors are relevant at each time-point will be important to inform AVPD and OCPD treatment, as well as prevention purposes.

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by Research Council of Norway (RCN) – grant number 196148/V50. Previous collection and analysis of twin data from this project was in part supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) – grant number MH-068643 and grants from the RCN, the Norwegian Foundation for Health and Rehabilitation and the Norwegian Council for Mental Health. These funding agencies played no role in the design and conduct of the study, its collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. We are very grateful for the twins’ participation in this study.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

  1. Akaike H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrica. 1987;52:317–332. [Google Scholar]
  2. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  3. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernstein DP, Arntz A, Travaglini L. Schizoid and avoidant personality disorders. In: Blaney PH, Krueger RF, Millon T, editors. Oxford Textbook of Psychopathology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2014. pp. 639–658. [Google Scholar]
  5. Boker MS, Neale MC, Maes H, Wilde M, Spiegel M, Brick T, Spies J, Estabrook R, Kenny S, Bates T, Mehta P, Fox J. OpenMx: an open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrica. 2011;76:306–317. doi: 10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Erol RY, Orth U. Self-esteem development from age 14–30 years: a longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;101:607–619. doi: 10.1037/a0024299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Falconer DS. Inheritance of liability to certain diseases estimated from incidence among relatives. Annual of Human Genetics. 1965;29:51–76. [Google Scholar]
  8. Fox J. polycor: Polychoric and polyserial correlations. R package version 0.7–8. 2010 ( http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=polycor). [Google Scholar]
  9. Gjerde LC, Czajkowski N, Roysamb E, Orstavik RE, Knudsen GP, Ostby K, Torgersen S, Myers J, Kendler KS, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. The heritability of avoidant and dependent personality disorder assessed by personal interview and questionnaire. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2012;126:448–457. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01862.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Grant JE, Mooney ME, Kushner MG. Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidity of DSM-IV obsessive-compulsive personality disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2012;46:469–475. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, Gunderson JG, Pagano ME, Yen S, Zanarini MC, Shea MT, Skodol AE, Stout RL, Morey LC, McGlashan TH. Two-year stability and change of schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:767–775. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Harris JR, Magnus P, Tambs K. The Norwegian Institute of Public Health twin program of research: an update. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2006;9:858–864. doi: 10.1375/183242706779462886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Heath AC, Madden PAF, Martin NG. Assessing the effects of cooperation bias and attrition in behavioral genetic research using data-weighting. Behavior Genetics. 1998;28:415–427. doi: 10.1023/a:1021633127604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Donnellan MB, Samuel DB, Grilo CM, McGlashan TH, Shea MT, Zanarini MC, Gunderson JG, Skodol AE. Ten-year rank-order stability of personality traits and disorders in a clinical sample. Journal of Personality. 2013;81:335–344. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00801.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, Skodol AE, Hamagami F, Brook JS. Age-related change in personality disorder trait levels between early adolescence and adulthood: a community-based longitudinal investigation. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2000;102:265–275. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102004265.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kendler KS, Aggen SH, Czajkowski N, Roysamb E, Tambs K, Torgersen S, Neale MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for DSM-IV personality disorders a multivariate twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:1438–1446. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kendler KS, Aggen SH, Neale MC, Knudsen GP, Krueger RF, Tambs K, Czajkowski N, Ystrom E, Orstavik RE, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. A longitudinal twin study of cluster A personality disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2014;14:1–8. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kendler KS, Czajkowski N, Tambs K, Torgersen S, Aggen SH, Neale MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. Dimensional representations of DSM-IV Cluster A personality disorders in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins: a multivariate study. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:1583–1591. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kendler KS, Myers J, Torgersen S, Neale MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. The heritability of cluster A personality disorders assessed by both personal interview and questionnaire. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:655–665. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Krueger RF. Continuity of axes I and II: toward a unified model of personality, personality disorders, and clinical disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2005;19:233–261. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2005.19.3.233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Krueger RF, Derringer J, Markon KE, Watson D, Skodol AE. Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. Psychological Medicine. 2012;42:1879–1890. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lenzenweger MF. Stability and change in personality disorder features – The longitudinal study of personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:1009–1015. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.11.1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Morey LC, Hopwood CJ. Stability and Change in Personality Disorders. In: NolenHoeksema S, editor. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. Vol. 9. Palo Alto: Annual Reviews; 2013. pp. 499–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Nilsen TS, Knudsen GP, Gervin K, Brandt I, Roysamb E, Tambs K, Orstavik R, Lyle R, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Magnus P, Harris JR. The Norwegian twin registry from a public health perspective: a research update. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2013;16:285–295. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Pfohl BB, Zimmerman M. Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV) Iowa City: University of Iowa, Department of Psychiatry; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  26. R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  27. Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Czajkowski N, Neale MC, Orstavik RE, Torgersen S, Tambs K, Roysamb E, Harris JR, Kendler KS. Genetic and environmental influences on dimensional representations of DSM-IV cluster C personality disorders: a population-based multivariate twin study. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:645–653. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Knudsen GP. Obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. In: Blaney PH, Krueger RF, Millon T, editors. Oxford Textbook of Psychopathology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2014. pp. 707–728. [Google Scholar]
  29. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. Version 1.3.10. Evanston, Illinois, USA: Northwestern University; 2013. ( http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych). [Google Scholar]
  30. Sanislow CA, Little TD, Ansell EB, Grilo CM, Daversa M, Markowitz JC, Pinto A, Shea MT, Yen S, Skodol AE, Morey LC, Gunderson JG, Zanarini MC, McGlashan TH. Ten-year stability and latent structure of the DSM-IV schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118:507–519. doi: 10.1037/a0016478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, Morey LC, Sanislow CA, Bender DS, Grilo CM, Zanarini MC, Yen S, Pagano ME, Stout RL. The Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study (CLPS): overview and implications. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2005;19:487–504. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2005.19.5.487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Soeteman DI, Hakkaart-van Roijen L, Verheul R, Busschbach JJV. The economic burden of personality disorders in mental health care. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2008;69:259–265. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n0212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Tambs K, Ronning T, Prescott CA, Kendler KS, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Torgersen S, Harris JR. The Norwegian Institute of Public Health twin study of mental health: examining recruitment and attrition bias. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2009;12:158–168. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.2.158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Torgersen S, Czajkowski N, Jacobson K, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Roysamb E, Neale MC, Kendler KS. Dimensional representations of DSM-IV cluster B personality disorders in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins: a multivariate study. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:1617–1625. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Torgersen S, Lygren S, Oien PA, Skre I, Onstad S, Edvardsen J, Tambs K, Kringlen E. A twin study of personality disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2000;41:416–425. doi: 10.1053/comp.2000.16560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Torgersen S, Myers J, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Røysamb E, Kubarych T, Kendler KS. The heritability of Cluster B personality disorders assessed both by personal interview and questionnaire. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2012;26:848–866. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2012.26.6.848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Zimmerman M, Rothschild L, Chelminski I. The prevalence of DSM-IV personality disorders in psychiatric outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1911–1918. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES