Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2015 Nov 3;10(11):e0141607. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141607

An Investigation of Emotion Recognition and Theory of Mind in People with Chronic Heart Failure

Tina Habota 1,*, Skye N McLennan 1, Jan Cameron 2, Chantal F Ski 2, David R Thompson 2, Peter G Rendell 1
Editor: Marina A Pavlova3
PMCID: PMC4631439  PMID: 26529409

Abstract

Objectives

Cognitive deficits are common in patients with chronic heart failure (CHF), but no study has investigated whether these deficits extend to social cognition. The present study provided the first empirical assessment of emotion recognition and theory of mind (ToM) in patients with CHF. In addition, it assessed whether each of these social cognitive constructs was associated with more general cognitive impairment.

Methods

A group comparison design was used, with 31 CHF patients compared to 38 demographically matched controls. The Ekman Faces test was used to assess emotion recognition, and the Mind in the Eyes test to measure ToM. Measures assessing global cognition, executive functions, and verbal memory were also administered.

Results

There were no differences between groups on emotion recognition or ToM. The CHF group’s performance was poorer on some executive measures, but memory was relatively preserved. In the CHF group, both emotion recognition performance and ToM ability correlated moderately with global cognition (r = .38, p = .034; r = .49, p = .005, respectively), but not with executive function or verbal memory.

Conclusion

CHF patients with lower cognitive ability were more likely to have difficulty recognizing emotions and inferring the mental states of others. Clinical implications of these findings are discussed.

Introduction

Chronic heart failure (CHF) is a complex condition characterized by an underlying structural abnormality that impairs the function of the heart to deliver sufficient blood flow to meet the metabolic needs of the body and brain [1]. In many patients with CHF, accumulated ischemic damage to the brain results in secondary cognitive impairment [2,3]. The level of cognitive impairment varies from patient to patient, but some degree of dysfunction is seen in up to 80% of patients in select CHF populations [4,5]. Over the past few decades, multiple neuropsychological studies have established that people with CHF are affected by deficits in cognitive processes such as executive function, memory, and attention (for review see, [5]). However, no study has assessed social cognition in this group.

Core aspects of social cognition are emotion recognition, which is the ability to perceive and correctly distinguish emotions displayed by others [6], and Theory of Mind (ToM), which is the ability to make inferences about the mental states of others [7]. These two processes of social cognition are vital because they facilitate effective social interaction and allow people to form and maintain strong relationships with others [8] by enabling them to understand subtle social cues [9]. Profound examples of deficits in these processes are seen in people with autism spectrum disorders [10] and schizophrenia [11,12].

In clinical groups, deficits in social cognition have been linked to poor functional outcomes, for example poor community and/or psychological functioning [13,14]. Therefore, social cognition may be particularly important for patients with CHF who experience debilitating physical symptoms that impact on their physical and emotional wellbeing, thereby increasing their need for support from others [15,16]. Social cognitive impairment may also contribute to isolation through poor social functioning [13]. This is important because social isolation is a significant predictor of mortality in CHF, while social support increases overall quality of life [17].

The overarching aim of this study was to examine emotion recognition and ToM in patients with CHF. It is possible that the deficits CHF patients experience with other cognitive abilities also extend to deficits in social cognition. This is because the neuropathology observed in these patients involves white matter hyperintensities and reduced grey matter [2,18] in regions of the brain that are implicated in emotion recognition and ToM, including the prefrontal cortex and the limbic system (temporal lobe) [19,20]. In particular, the observed white matter pathology is associated with disconnection within fronto-subcortical brain tracts [21] known to be involved in the processing of emotional signals [22,23].

The second aim of the study was to assess whether deficits in emotion recognition and ToM were associated with more general cognitive impairment. Although general cognition and social cognition are supported by different neural circuits [2426], the process of understanding others’ thoughts and emotions has been shown to make substantial demands on cognitive control processes, such as inhibition and mental flexibility [2729]. Given that executive control mechanisms are supported by frontal brain regions [26], which are amongst the most affected in CHF, it might be that in the context of CHF, any observed deficits in emotion recognition and ToM are related to more general cognitive difficulties. Indeed, a relationship between emotion recognition and/or ToM and general cognition has been observed in other clinical populations [30,31].

To address each of these aims, a group of CHF patients was compared to a group of matched controls. It was hypothesized that the CHF group would show deficits in emotion recognition and ToM compared to the group of healthy controls. It was also predicted that within the CHF group, emotion recognition and ToM would positively correlate with global cognition, executive function, and verbal memory.

Methods

This research was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees at Eastern Health and the Australian Catholic University. All participants provided written informed consent.

Participants

The CHF group was recruited from a pool of participants taking part in a larger study (n = 72); 13 participants were paid AUD $10 per hour, and the rest were volunteers. To be eligible for the parent study participants had to be aged over 18, and actively engaged in a nurse-led CHF management program at one of three public hospitals in metropolitan Melbourne, Australia. All recruited participants had a confirmed diagnosis of CHF. Specifically, cardinal symptoms and clinical features of congestion, and objective evidence of cardiac impairment on echocardiogram [1].

Participants with CHF were excluded if they resided in a high care residential aged facility, had a terminal diagnosis, a documented history of dementia, or could not read English. All 72 participants were approached from the parent study; 25 declined, and five were unreachable. No participants had head injury or psychiatric illness. We screened participants’ global cognition; initially, we recruited 42 participants but excluded six who could not complete the primary measures because they either declined or ran out of time. Another five participants were excluded who showed signs of potential dementia as operationalized by a score lower than 82 on the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination—Revised (ACE-R) [32]. The final sample of CHF patients included 31 adults. A subset of this final sample has been reported on previously [33], but not with regards to social cognitive performance.

The control group was recruited from the general community; 13 participants were paid AUD $10 per hour, the rest were volunteers. Participants in the control group were excluded if they reported a history of CHF or neurological disease, had recent treatment (past three months) for an acute cardiovascular problem, or could not read English. We initially recruited 43 participants, but excluded four who could not complete the primary measures, and one who had an ACE-R score below the cut off. The final control group included 38 adults.

Materials

Health

The New York Heart Association (NYHA) classification [34] was used as an index of functional status. The NYHA is an extensively used and validated clinical measure. Classification I indicates no limitations and classification IV indicates the worst functional status.

The Charlson Co-morbidity Index [35] was used to assess the severity of co-morbid conditions. Overall index scores are categorized as mild, moderate, or severe; higher scores indicate more severe co-morbidity.

Global cognition

The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination—Revised [32] is a test of global cognition. The ACE-R is a reliable and sensitive test of early cognitive dysfunction [36] and was used to identify and exclude participants with possible dementia (scores <82). Higher scores indicate better cognitive functioning.

General intelligence

The National Adult Reading Test (NART) [37] was used as an index of premorbid intelligence. Standardized IQ scores were calculated using the formula in the administration manual [37].

Psychopathology

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [38] was used to screen for symptoms of anxiety and depression. The level of emotional symptomology was assessed separately for anxiety and depression (seven items each). Higher scores indicate higher levels of symptomology.

Executive function—cognitive flexibility

The Trail Making Test (TMT) [39] was used to assess cognitive flexibility. The Trails B minus Trails A difference score was used as an index of cognitive flexibility [40]. Lower scores indicate better performance.

Executive function—inhibition

The Hayling Sentence Completion test [41] was used to assess cognitive inhibition. A total score was obtained by tallying errors and time taken (in seconds) to complete the task. Standardized scores were calculated; higher scores indicate better performance.

Executive function—initiation

The final measure of executive functioning was verbal fluency, which was extracted from the ACE-R and used to assess cognitive initiation. Two types of verbal fluency were assessed; phonemic and categorical. In the phonemic task, participants were given one minute to orally generate as many words beginning with the letter ‘P’ as they could, excluding proper nouns and the same word with a different suffix. In the categorical task, participants were given one minute to name as many animals as they could. A composite verbal fluency score was used in the present study. Higher scores indicate better performance.

Verbal memory

The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [42] was used to measure verbal memory. A single composite score for the first five trials was used as the measure of immediate recall. The total number of correctly recalled words after a 20-minute delay was used as a measure of delayed recall. Higher scores indicate better performance.

Primary Measures

Emotion recognition

The Ekman 60 Faces test [43] was used to assess recognition of six basic human emotions; happy, anger, sadness, disgust, surprise, and fear. Participants were presented with 60 slides, featuring extensively and universally validated photographs of human faces [44]. Participants were asked to choose one of six emotions that best described the emotion that the person in the picture displayed. This measure takes approximately 15 minutes to administer, and it has been used extensively to assess emotion recognition in various groups.

Theory of Mind

The Reading the Mind in the Eyes test [45] was used to assess ToM. Participants were presented with 36 black and white images of the eye region of human faces, and asked to indicate which of four given emotional states each image best represents. In comparison to measures of emotion recognition, which require participants to identify basic facial expressions, this task required participants to identify more complex and finely nuanced mental states (e.g., “perplexed”, “flirtatious”). This measure is administered in approximately 10 minutes. It is a reliable and valid measure of social cognitive dysfunction, and it is commonly used to assess ToM in various clinical and non-clinical groups [46].

Procedure

Participants with CHF were tested approximately three months after recruitment into the parent study. The delay of three months was built in to ensure that participants were medically stable when they completed the neuropsychological assessment. Participants were tested in a quiet room, either at their residences, in a hospital consultation room, or in a university-testing lab in a single session, lasting approximately two hours.

Design and data analysis

This study used a matched-group comparison design. Missing value analysis was conducted, which showed that data was missing at random. Therefore missing data were not substituted. Descriptive statistics were generated for all variables. Univariate analyses were conducted to assess group differences on the background cognitive measures, and the ToM task. A mixed two-way ANOVA was used to examine differences in performance on the emotion recognition measure.

Pearson correlations were used to examine associations between the social cognitive measures and each of the other cognitive measures. Before undertaking the correlational analyses, all variables were assessed for normality. In the CHF group, all variables were normally distributed. In the control group, the Ekman Faces score was negatively skewed. Skewness was corrected by adjusting one outlier (which was 3 standard deviations below the mean) to two standard deviations below the mean [47].

To reduce the possibility of Type I error, we created a composite executive function score given that the three measures of executive function (TMT, Hayling, verbal fluency) were correlated: TMT with Hayling (r = -.38, p = .003), TMT with verbal fluency (r = -.25, p = .048), and Hayling with verbal fluency (r = .41, p = .001). The composite executive function score was created by converting scores on the three measures of executive function to z scores, reversing-coding the TMT such that higher scores indicated higher performance, then calculating a mean z of the three scores for each participant. The two measures of verbal memory (immediate and delayed recall, RAVLT) were also correlated (r = .79, p < .001) so a composite verbal memory score was created using the same approach. Both composite scores were normally distributed.

Results

CHF group characteristics

The CHF group consisted of adults aged 40 to 86 (M = 69.77, SD = 11.23) who were predominantly male (65%). Table 1 shows that the majority of the CHF sample was classified as functional classification II on the NYHA. Systolic and ischemic CHF were the most common etiologies in this group, and hypertension was the most frequently reported risk factor. On average, participants had a moderate level of comorbid disease burden (Charlson Comorbidity Index mean = 3.48, SD = 2.03), and the average length of time living with CHF was three years (M = 36.17 months, SD = 55.49).

Table 1. Clinical Characteristics of the CHF Group.

Health characteristics n %
NYHA classification
 Class I 2 6.5
 Class II 18 58.1
 Class III 10 32.3
 Class IV 1 3.2
Heart failure type
 Systolic 22 71.0
 Diastolic 4 12.9
 Mixed 3 9.7
 Unspecified 2 6.5
Heart failure etiology
 Ischemic 17 54.8
 Non ischemic 8 25.8
 Idiopathic 1 3.2
 Other 5 16.1

Note. NYHA = New York Heart Association.

Group comparisons on demographics and cognition

Table 2 shows that there was a trend towards higher proportions of cardiac risk factors in the CHF group, but these group differences were not significant (all ps > .060). Table 2 also shows that the two groups were closely matched in gender distribution, age, education, and estimated IQ as indexed by the NART. Independent samples t-tests were conducted to examine differences between groups on cognitive measures (Table 2). The control group performed significantly better on two of the three measures of executive function; cognitive flexibility t(62) = 3.07, p = .003, and cognitive inhibition t(61) = 5.88, p < .001, but not on either measures of verbal memory. The control group reported more symptoms of anxiety t(67) = 2.23, p = .029.

Table 2. Participant Characteristics.

CHF group Control group
n % n % χ 2
Proportion of men (%) 20 65.0 27 71.0 0.34
Cardiac risk factors (%)
 Hypercholesterolemia 13 41.9% 15 39.5% 0.04
 Hypertension 21 67.7% 18 47.4% 2.88
 Smoking 13 41.9% 8 21.1% 3.52
 Diabetes 7 22.6% 4 10.5% 1.85
 Obesity 5 16.1% 2 5.3% 2.21
Demographic (M) M SD M SD t d
 Age (years) 31 69.77 11.23 38 67.13 7.53 1.12 0.28
 Education (years) 31 11.65 3.74 38 13.07 3.57 1.61 0.39
 Estimated IQ 30 112.54 5.89 38 114.33 6.38 1.19 0.29
Global cognition and mental health
 Global cognition (ACE-R) 31 91.00 4.89 38 92.08 4.55 0.95 0.23
 Anxiety (HADS) 31 6.03 3.73 38 8.29 4.51 2.23* 0.55
 Depression (HADS) 31 5.19 2.91 38 5.55 3.29 0.48 0.12
Executive functions
 Cognitive flexibility (TMT) 27 74.83 35.17 37 49.19 31.35 3.07** 0.77
 Inhibition (Hayling) 28 2.89 2.01 35 5.57 1.61 5.88*** 1.47
 Initiation (Verbal fluency) 31 29.94 6.51 38 32.21 7.85 1.29 0.31
Verbal memory (RAVLT)
 Immediate recall 26 41.58 8.89 38 45.00 8.96 1.51 0.38
 Delayed recall 24 8.83 2.51 38 9.13 2.84 0.42 0.11

d = Cohen’s d index of effect size. Effect sizes: small = 0.2; medium = 0.5; large = 0.8 [48].

* p < .05.

** p < .01.

*** p < .001.

Notes. ACE-R = Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination—Revised; HADS = Hospital Anxiety Depression Scale; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; TMT = Trail Making Test (B minus A).

Group comparisons on measures of emotion recognition and ToM

Emotion recognition: Ekman Faces test

Fig 1 shows the results of the Ekman Faces test as a function of group (CHF, control) and emotion type (happiness, surprise, anger, disgust, sadness, fear). These data were analyzed with a mixed 2 x 6 ANOVA with the between-groups variable of group and the within-groups variable of emotion type. Mauchly’s test indicated that the sphericity assumption was violated; therefore the Huynh-Feldt correction was used. Of primary interest, there was no significant main effect of group F(1, 67) = 0.01, p = .932 ηp2 < .001, and no interaction effect F(4.38, 293.15) = 0.76, p = .566, ηp 2 = .011, which indicates that recognition of basic emotions did not differ as a function of group status. Of secondary interest was the main effect of emotion type, F(4.38, 293.15) = 99.99, p < .001, ηp 2 = 0.60. Post hoc revealed that for all participants, the recognition accuracy significantly differed for each comparison of each type of emotion, with the order from best to worst recognized being: happiness (M = 9.91, SD = 0.33), surprise (M = 9.04; SD = 1.14), sadness (M = 7.99; SD = 1.82), disgust (M = 7.87; SD = 1.62), anger (M = 6.94, SD = 1.91), and fear (M = 4.99, SD = 2.17). The one exception was that recognition accuracy did not differ for the comparison of sadness and disgust.

Fig 1. Mean number of correct responses for each emotion type on the Ekman Faces test for the CHF and control groups.

Fig 1

Theory of Mind: Mind in the Eyes test

An independent samples t-test revealed that the groups did not significantly differ on their ability to accurately infer the mental states of others, t(66) = 0.63, p = .450, d = 0.18 (CHF M = 23.87, SD = 4.39; controls M = 23.08, SD = 4.23). The effect size was less than the cut off for a small Cohen’s d (0.2) [48].

Correlations between emotion recognition and ToM and other cognitive measures

Pearson correlations were computed separately for the CHF group and controls to assess the relationship between emotion recognition and ToM and the other cognitive measures. Separate correlations were run for the Ekman Faces test and the Mind in the Eyes test (Table 3). All correlations were in the expected direction for both groups with better cognitive performance associated with better social cognition performance. In the CHF group, both emotion recognition and ToM showed significant moderate positive correlations with global cognition (r = .38 p = .034; r = .49 p = .005, respectively). However, neither emotion recognition nor ToM significantly correlated with executive function or verbal memory. In the control group, neither executive function nor verbal memory significantly correlated with the Ekman Faces test and the Mind in the Eyes test, however there was a significant correlation between the Ekman Faces test and the Mind in the Eyes test (r = .45, p = .004), as might be expected.

Table 3. Relationships between Emotion Recognition (Ekman) and ToM (Mind in the Eyes) Scores and Cognitive Measures.

Ekman Faces Mind in the Eyes
CHF Controls CHF Controls
n r (p) n r (p) n r (p) n r (p)
Global cognition (ACE-R) 31 .38 (.034) * 38 .10 (.565) 31 .49 (.005) ** 38 .07 (.659)
Executive function 26 .02 (.957) 34 .15 (.410) 26 .14 (.495) 34 .25 (.158)
Verbal memory 24 .30 (.154) 38 .21 (.208) 24 .21 (.327) 38 .11 (.530)
Mind in the Eyes 31 .29 (.104) 38 .45 (.004) ** - -

* p < .05.

** p < .01.

Note. ACE-R = Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination—Revised; Executive function and verbal memory are both composite scores.

Discussion

A large body of research has shown that people with CHF present with deficits in a range of cognitive abilities [3,49]. In the present study the CHF group’s cognitive performance varied across cognitive domains. People with CHF showed deficits in some, but not all, cognitive functions, with relatively preserved memory function. No previous studies have investigated whether these deficits might extend to social cognition. The present study compared the emotion recognition and ToM abilities of people with CHF to a group of demographically matched controls. Contrary to expectations, the findings indicated that the performance of the two groups did not differ on either aspect of social cognition. This is also the first study to examine the association between social cognition and more general cognition. Importantly, in the CHF group, people with lower global cognitive ability were more likely to have difficulty recognizing emotions and inferring the mental states of others, as expected. However, contradictory to our prediction, emotion recognition and ToM were not significantly correlated with measures of executive function or verbal memory.

The absence of group differences in emotion recognition and ToM is surprising because people with CHF are affected by diffuse damage to neural structures, including frontal and temporal regions [2,18], which have specifically been implicated in both of these social cognitive processes [19,20]. The lack of group differences is further surprising because similar diffuse neural damage and widespread cognitive impairment is seen in other neurocognitive disorders, including people with traumatic brain injury [31,50,51], autism spectrum disorders [10,52] and multiple sclerosis [30,53,54]. Each of these groups has shown significant deficits in general cognition, but also in emotion recognition and ToM. Furthermore, social cognition deficits have also been observed in a range of neuropsychiatric disorders, most commonly schizophrenia [11,12], but also mood disorders, such as major depression and anxiety for review see, [55]. Thus, the common finding that CHF patients are affected by elevated rates of depression and anxiety [56], might have been expected to further increase their vulnerability to social cognition deficits.

There are several likely explanations for the null findings of this study. In the CHF group, correlations between emotion recognition and ToM with variables that had missing data (i.e., composite verbal memory n = 24; executive function n = 26) were underpowered; a post-hoc power analysis showed that the study power was .42 for a medium effect size (r = .30; the strongest correlation observed with a reduced sample size). Additionally, participants were a select and relatively high functioning group, cognitively and symptomatically. Specifically, we were interested in the performance of non-demented participants and therefore excluded anyone who showed signs of dementia. In addition, 64.6% of the CHF sample had no, or only mild, heart failure symptoms, and the overall subjective rating of depression was within the normal range. Thus, in the wider CHF population, where medical [2,5759] and emotional [6062] symptoms are often more severe, brain pathology may also be more severe. Consequently, the ability to successfully recognize emotions and make inferences about the mental states of others is likely to be more impaired in CHF patients with greater comorbidity and worse functioning.

Finally, we chose measures of emotion recognition and ToM that have been used extensively with other clinical groups. However, they may not have been sensitive enough to detect subtle group differences because the CHF group was high functioning. Other studies have found that traditional and static measures of emotion recognition and ToM, like those used in the present study, do not always detect deficits that are picked up by dynamic measures [6365]. Thus, future research could extend this study by using dynamic and/or morphed images (emotionally neutral expressions that are morphed into expressions of a specific emotion; e.g., [66]) and include measures of reaction time. These measurement approaches could be more sensitive to subtle deficits in emotion recognition in high functioning groups.

Although preliminary, the significant correlations between global cognition and emotion recognition and ToM suggest that CHF patients with lower cognitive functioning are also more likely to have social cognitive deficits. If these findings are confirmed by other studies, then patients with poor cognitive function might require tailored intervention that focuses on improving psychosocial functioning. This is an important consideration because a key factor in successful CHF self-care is social support [67]. The available evidence has shown that social isolation and lack of social support are associated with increased risk of rehospitalization and death [6870]. Therefore, for patients with low global cognition, emotion recognition and ToM may indirectly impact self-care decision-making and quality of life through impoverished relationships or social isolation. Indeed, this indirect effect has been reported in people with schizophrenia where general cognitive abilities affected social abilities, which consequently exerted a negative influence on general functioning and quality of life [13,14].

In conclusion, in this group of high functioning CHF patients, which was matched to the control group on many important characteristics, capacity for emotion recognition and ToM was not found to be impaired, but each social cognitive construct was related to global cognition. Considering these preliminary findings, it seems likely that people with CHF who present with low general cognitive ability may also be affected by difficulties with accurately recognizing emotions and inferring the mental states of others. This information is important given that psychosocial status, including social support and isolation are important influences of CHF self-care [67].

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the heart failure nurses across Eastern Health who generously assisted with recruitment, and we thank all of the participants who took part in this study. We acknowledge the help of Mollie Flood in recruiting and testing some of the participants. The study was funded by a Mona Menzies Post-Doctoral Nursing Research Grant from the Nurses Board Victoria Legacy Ltd, Discovery Project Grant from the Australian Research Council, and an Australian Postgraduate Award scholarship. There are no conflicts of interest to declare.

Data Availability

Please note that data underlying the findings of this manuscript are available through a public repository – Figshare (http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1529850).

Funding Statement

The study was funded by a Mona Menzies Post-Doctoral Nursing Research Grant from the Nurses Board Victoria Legacy Ltd, Discovery Project Grant from the Australian Research Council, and an Australian Postgraduate Award scholarship.

References

  • 1. Krum H, Jelinek MV, Stewart S, Sindone A, Atherton JJ, Hawkes AL. Guidelines for the prevention, detection and management of people with chronic heart failure in Australia 2006. Med J Aust. 2006;185: 549–446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Vogels RLC, van der Flier WM, van Harten B, Gouw AA, Scheltens P, Schroeder-Tanka JM, et al. Brain magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in patients with heart failure. Eur J Heart Fail. 2007;9: 1003–1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Vogels RLC, Scheltens P, Schroeder-Tanka JM, Weinstein HC. Cognitive impairment in heart failure: A systematic review of the literature. Eur J Heart Fail. 2007;9: 440–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Cameron J, Worrall-Carter L, Page K, Riegel B, Lo SK, Stewart S. Does cognitive impairment predict poor self-care in patients with heart failure? Eur J Heart Fail. 2010;12: 508–515. 10.1093/eurjhf/hfq042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Cannon JA, McMurray JJ, Quinn TJ. “Hearts and minds”: Association, causation and implication of cognitive impairment in heart failure. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2015;7: 22–39. 10.1186/s13195-015-0106-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Adolphs R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12: 169–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Frith C, Frith U. Theory of mind. Curr Biol. 2005;15: R644–R645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Grossmann T. The development of emotion perception in face and voice during infancy. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2010;28: 219–236. 10.3233/RNN-2010-0499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Radice-Neumann D, Zupan B, Babbage DR, Willer B. Overview of impaired facial affect recognition in persons with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2007;21: 807–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Harms MB, Martin A, Wallace GL. Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20: 290–322. 10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, Healey KM, Moberg PJ. Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Schizophr Bull. 2009;36: 1009–1019. 10.1093/schbul/sbn192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, Van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191: 5–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Fett AKJ, Viechtbauer W, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35: 573–588. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: Empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37: S41–S54. 10.1093/schbul/sbr079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. O’Loughlin C, Murphy NF, Conlon C, O’Donovan A, Ledwidge M, McDonald K. Quality of life predicts outcome in a heart failure disease management program. Int J Cardiol. 2010;139: 60–67. 10.1016/j.ijcard.2008.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Volz A, Schmid JP, Zwahlen M, Kohls S, Saner H, Barth J. Predictors of readmission and health related quality of life in patients with chronic heart failure: A comparison of different psychosocial aspects. J Behav Med. 2011;34: 13–22. 10.1007/s10865-010-9282-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Årestedt K, Saveman B-I, Johansson P, Blomqvist K. Social support and its association with health-related quality of life among older patients with chronic heart failure. Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2013;12: 69–77. 10.1177/1474515111432997 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Woo MA, Macey PM, Fonarow GC, Hamilton MA, Harper RM. Regional brain gray matter loss in heart failure. J Appl Physiol. 2003;95: 677–684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ. Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30: 2313–2335. 10.1002/hbm.20671 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional Neuroanatomy of Emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage. 2002;16: 331–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Jokinen H, Kalska H, Mäntylä R, Pohjasvaara T, Ylikoski R, Hietanen M, et al. Cognitive profile of subcortical ischaemic vascular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77: 28–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D, Cooper G, Damasio A. A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. J Neurosci. 2000;20: 2683–2690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Ruffman T, Henry JD, Livingstone V, Phillips LH. A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: Implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32: 863–881. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Lieberman J. Implications for the neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160: 815–824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19: 2767–2796. 10.1093/cercor/bhp055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16: 17–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Apperly IA, Samson D, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: Neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16: 1773–1784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Bailey PE, Henry JD. Growing less empathic with age: Disinhibition of the self-perspective. J Gerontol Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2008;63: 219–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Decety J, Jackson PL. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004;3: 71–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Henry JD, Phillips LH, Beatty WW, McDonald S, Longley WA, Joscelyne A, et al. Evidence for deficits in facial affect recognition and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15: 277–285. 10.1017/S1355617709090195 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Henry JD, Phillips LH, Crawford JR, Ietswaart M, Summers F. Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44: 1623–1628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Mathuranath PS, Nestor PJ, Berrios GE, Rakowicz W, Hodges JR. A brief cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. 2000;55: 1613–1620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Habota T, McLennan SN, Cameron J, Henry JD, Ski CF, Thompson DR, et al. Prospective memory impairment in chronic heart failure. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2015;21: 183–192. 10.1017/S1355617715000119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Bennett JA, Riegel B, Bittner V, Nichols J. Validity and reliability of the NYHA classes for measuring research outcomes in patients with cardiac disease. Heart Lung J Acute Crit Care. 2002;31: 262–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Charlson ME, Pompei P, Ales KL, MacKenzie CR. A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: Development and validation. J Chronic Dis. 1987;40: 373–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR. The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): A brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21: 1078–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Nelson HE. National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. Windsor: NFER; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67: 361–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Reitan Neuropsychology Labratory; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Lamberty GJ, Putnam SH, Chatel DM, Bieliauskas LA, Adams KM. Derived Trail Making Test indices: A preliminary report. Cogn Behav Neurol. 1994;7: 230–234. [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Burgess PW, Shallice T. The Hayling and Brixton tests. Thurston: Thames Valley Test Co. Ltd.: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Rey A. L’examen clinique en psychologie. Oxford: Presses Universitaries De France; 1958. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Measuring facial movement. Environ Psychol Nonverbal Behav. 1976;1: 56–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Edwards J, Jackson HJ, Pattison PE. Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: A methodological review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2002;22: 789–832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning Autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42: 241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Vellante M, Baron-Cohen S, Melis M, Marrone M, Petretto DR, Masala C, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: Systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18: 326–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Tabachnick B, Fidell L. Using multivariate statistics. 4th ed New York: Allyn & Bacon; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Pressler SJ. Cognitive functioning and chronic heart failure: A review of the literature (2002-July 2007). J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2008;23: 239–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Babbage DR, Yim J, Zupan B, Neumann D, Tomita M, Willer B. Meta-analysis of facial affect recognition difficulties after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2011;25: 227–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Martín-Rodríguez JF, León-Carrión J. Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48: 1181–1191. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Baron-Cohen S. Theory of mind and autism: A review. Int Rev Res Ment Retard. 2000;23: 169–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Phillips LH, Henry JD, Scott C, Summers F, Whyte M. Specific impairments of emotion perception in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology. 2011;25: 131–136. 10.1037/a0020752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Pöttgen J, Dziobek I, Reh S, Heesen C, Gold SM. Impaired social cognition in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84: 523–528. 10.1136/jnnp-2012-304157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Kret ME, Ploeger A. Emotion processing deficits: A liability spectrum providing insight into comorbidity of mental disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;52: 153–171. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Yohannes A, Willgoss T, Baldwin R, Connolly M. Depression and anxiety in chronic heart failure and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: Prevalence, relevance, clinical implications and management principles. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;25: 1209–1221. 10.1002/gps.2463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Ekman I, Fagerberg B, Skoog I. The clinical implications of cognitive impairment in elderly patients with chronic heart failure. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2001;16: 47–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Pressler SJ, Subramanian U, Kareken D, Perkins SM, Gradus-Pizlo I, Sauve MJ, et al. Cognitive deficits in chronic heart failure. Nurs Res. 2010;59: 127–139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Vogels RLC, Oosterman JM, Van Harten B, Scheltens P, Van Der Flier WM, Schroeder-Tanka JM, et al. Profile of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55: 1764–1770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Hawkins MAW, Dolansky MA, Schaefer JT, Fulcher MJ, Gunstad J, Redle JD, et al. Cognitive function in heart failure is associated with nonsomatic symptoms of depression but not somatic symptoms. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2015;30: E9–E17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Insel KC, Badger TA. Deciphering the 4 D’s: Cognitive decline, delirium, depression and dementia—a review. J Adv Nurs. 2002;38: 360–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Li Y, Meyer JS, Thornby J. Depressive symptoms among cognitively normal versus cognitively impaired elderly subjects. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16: 455–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Golan O, Baron-Cohen S, Hill J. The Cambridge mindreading (CAM) face-voice battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36: 169–183. 10.1007/s10803-005-0057-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Golan O, Baron-Cohen S, Hill JJ, Rutherford M. The “Reading the Mind in the Voice”test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37: 1096–1106. 10.1007/s10803-006-0252-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Rutherford MD, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32: 189–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Isaacowitz DM, Wadlinger HA, Goren D, Wilson HR. Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study. Psychol Aging. 2006;21: 40–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Moser DK, Watkins JF. Conceptualizing self-care in heart failure: A life course model of patient characteristics. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2008;23: 219–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Friedmann E, Thomas SA, Liu F, Morton PG, Chapa D, Gottlieb SS. Relationship of depression, anxiety, and social isolation to chronic heart failure outpatient mortality. Am Heart J. 2006;152: 940.e1–940.e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Luttik ML, Jaarsma T, Moser D, Sanderman R, van Veldhuisen DJ. The importance and impact of social support on outcomes in patients with heart failure: An overview of the literature. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2005;20: 162–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Murberg TA. Long-term effect of social relationships on mortality in patients with congestive heart failure. Int J Psychiatry Med. 2004;34: 207–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Please note that data underlying the findings of this manuscript are available through a public repository – Figshare (http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1529850).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES