Abstract
The orexin/hypocretin system is important for reward-seeking behaviors, however less is known about its function in non-homeostatic feeding. Environmental influences, particularly cues for food can stimulate feeding in the absence of hunger and lead to maladaptive overeating behavior. The key components of the neural network that mediates this cue-induced overeating in sated rats include lateral hypothalamus, amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), yet the neuropharmacological mechanisms within this network remain unknown. The current study investigated a causal role for orexin in cue-driven feeding, and examined the neural substrates through which orexin mediates this effect. Systemic administration of the orexin-1 receptor (OX1R) antagonist SB-334867 had no effect on baseline eating, but significantly reduced cue-driven consumption in sated rats. Complementary neural analysis revealed that decreased cue-induced feeding under SB-334867 increased Fos expression in mPFC and paraventricular thalamus. These results demonstrate that OX1R signaling critically regulates cue-induced feeding, and suggest orexin is acting through prefrontal cortical and thalamic sites to drive eating in the absence of hunger. These findings inform our understanding of how food-associated cues override signals from the body to promote overeating, and indicate OX1R antagonism as a potential pharmacologic target for treatment of disordered eating in humans.
Environmental and social factors critically control our appetites and food consumption. In particular, food-associated environmental cues have been shown to powerfully stimulate eating in sated (not food-deprived) children and adults1,2. Recent research has employed an animal model of this cue-induced feeding to delineate the critical neural circuits3. In this model, through repeated pairings, rats learn to associate a neutral stimulus (e.g., a tone) with delivery of a palatable food, such that the cue becomes a signal for the food. The ability of the cue to stimulate overeating is later assessed in sated rats by presenting the tone and measuring food consumption. In the presence of the cue, rats eat significantly greater amounts of food.
The neural circuitry underlying this non-homeostatic eating includes the basolateral and basomedial amygdalar nuclei (BLA and BMA), the lateral hypothalamic area (LHA), and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC)4,5. Additionally, specific pathways from the BLA/BMA and mPFC to the LHA are strongly activated by the food cue6, further implicating this network. In contrast, very little is known about the neuropharmacological mechanisms that act within this circuit to promote overeating. One promising candidate is the neuropeptide orexin/hypocretin.
Orexin is synthesized exclusively by neurons in the lateral hypothalamus7,8, a region traditionally associated with the initiation of feeding. This confined population of neurons projects extensively throughout the brain9, and these widespread connections allow orexin to modulate numerous eating and reward-seeking behaviors. Central administration of orexin increases consumption in rats8,10, implicating orexin in homeostatic regulation of feeding. However, presentation of food- or reward-predictive cues alone is sufficient to strongly activate orexin neurons11,12,13, suggesting a broader role for orexin in mediating reward-driven motivated behaviors. Consistent with this view, orexin receptor blockade significantly reduces self-administration for palatable foods in a variety of preparations. For example, orexin receptor antagonism decreased self-administration of high-fat pellets14, sucrose15, and saccharin16. Systemic orexin blockade has also been shown to reduce motivation to respond for high-fat pellets17, and to decrease cue-induced reinstatement of sucrose- and saccharin-seeking behavior15,16.
Orexin mediates its effects via two G protein-coupled receptors, OX1R and OX2R, which are differentially distributed throughout the brain18,19. Accordingly, there is heterogeneity in the function of these receptors, with OX1R activation more closely associated with appetitive and reward behaviors, while OX2R has been predominantly associated with sleep/wake transitions and arousal20,21,22. Selective antagonism of OX1R has been shown to reduce both homeostatic (nocturnal and post-fast) feeding and orexin-induced intake23, while blockade or knockdown of OX1R reduced high-fat diet consumption11,24. Similarly, OX1R antagonism has attenuated operant responding for food pellets25 and palatable, high-fat pellets14, and also prevented cue- and stress-induced reinstatement of sucrose-seeking15,26.
Importantly, orexin fibers and receptors are strongly expressed within the cue-induced feeding circuitry (mPFC, BLA, BMA)18,19,27, suggesting a role for orexin in mediating this overeating effect. In accordance with this hypothesis, we previously showed that presentation of a tone food-cue activates (Fos induction) orexin neurons13. Therefore, here we used systemic administration of an OX1R antagonist coupled with Fos immunohistochemistry to first determine whether OX1R signaling is critical for cue-induced feeding, and then to elucidate the neuroanatomical location/s of its action. Specifically, we examined the key components of the cue-induced feeding circuit (mPFC, BLA, BMA, LHA) as well as two connected regions (the paraventricular thalamic nucleus [PVT] and the central amygdalar nucleus [CEA]) where OX1R action mediates hedonic eating and taste learning respectively24,28.
Results
Experiment 1: Effect of SB-334867 on cue-induced feeding in sated rats
In Experiment 1 we determined the effect of systemic injections of the OX1R antagonist SB-334867 on cue-induced feeding. During training, rats received either tone-food pairings (Paired, n = 7), or tone alone presentations followed by food pellet delivery later in the home cage (Unpaired, n = 7). All animals were then tested for consumption of food pellets under SB-334867 and under vehicle, in a counterbalanced order across two consecutive days. Each test consisted of a baseline consumption test (stimulus-free), followed by a cue consumption test (tone presentations).
Body Weight
Rats in the Paired and Unpaired groups had similar body weights at the beginning of training (P: 297 ± 3 g, U: 291 ± 5 g), and prior to testing (P: 297 ± 5 g, U: 287 ± 6 g). Two-way independent samples t-Tests confirmed there were no significant differences in body weight between groups at either time point (t(12) values = 1.05, 1.27 respectively, p values > 0.05).
Training Chow Consumption
There were no differences between groups in the overall amounts of chow consumed daily or across training days. A group (Paired, Unpaired) by training day ANOVA on daily chow consumption yielded a significant linear trend of training day (F(1,12) = 79.90, P < 0.0001), confirming that chow consumption increased across training days. There was no significant effect of group (F(1,12) = 2.65, p > 0.05), and no group by training day interaction (F(1,12) < 1, p > 0.05).
Test
Baseline Test
Food pellet consumption during the stimulus-free baseline tests are shown in Fig. 1a. Rats in the Paired and Unpaired groups ate similar amounts of the pellets during the baseline test, regardless of whether they had received vehicle or SB-334867. A group by drug treatment (vehicle, SB-334867) ANOVA showed no significant effect of group (F(1,12) < 1, p > 0.05), or drug treatment (F(1,12) = 2.80, p > 0.05), and no significant group by drug treatment interaction (F(1,12) < 1, p > 0.05).
Cue Test
Figure 1b shows the number of food pellets consumed during the cue tests. Under vehicle the Paired group showed cue-induced feeding, but SB-334867 treatment attenuated this effect. A group by drug treatment ANOVA yielded a significant interaction (F(1,12) = 12.25, p = 0.004). Follow-up analyses showed that under vehicle the Paired group ate significantly more than the Unpaired group (F(1,12) = 9.77, p = 0.009), confirming cue-induced feeding. However, following SB-334867 treatment animals in the Paired group no longer showed this effect, with no significant difference in consumption between groups (F(1,12) = 1.90, p > 0.05).
Experiment 2: Fos induction during cue-induced feeding under SB-334867
In Experiment 2 we determined whether blockade of cue-induced feeding after systemic injection of SB-334867 alters activity patterns in brain regions of interest. All rats received tone-food pairings throughout training. On test day, all rats received a drug-free baseline test, followed by an injection of either SB-334867 or vehicle. They then received either a cue test (VEH Cue: n = 6, SB Cue: n = 6), or remained in their home cage with no food to control for non-specific effects of the drug on neural activity (VEH Home: n = 6, SB Home: n = 6). Brain tissue was then collected for later processing for Fos induction as a marker of neuronal activation.
Body Weight
All rats had similar body weights at the beginning of training (SB Cue: 327 ± 7 g, VEH Cue: 329 ± 5 g, SB Home: 343 ± 7 g, VEH Home: 334 ± 7 g), and prior to testing (SB Cue: 326 ± 8 g, VEH Cue: 327 ± 5 g, SB Home: 336 ± 2 g, VEH Home: 330 ± 10 g). One-way ANOVAs confirmed there were no significant differences in body weight between groups at either time point (F(3,23) values = 1.63, 0.45 respectively, p values > 0.05).
Training Chow Consumption
All groups consumed similar overall amounts of chow across training. There was a significant linear trend of training day (F(1,20) = 206.61, p < 0.0001), confirming that chow consumption increased across training days. There were no significant group differences or interactions (F(1,20) values < 1.5, p values > 0.05).
Test
Baseline Test
Consumption during the drug-free baseline test is shown in Fig. 2a. All rats ate similar amounts of pellets. A one-way ANOVA revealed no significant group differences (F(3,23) < 1, p > 0.05).
Post-injection Chow Consumption
During the 30 minutes immediately following injection all rats ate similarly small amounts of chow in their home cage (all <0.2 g). A group (Cue, Home) by drug treatment (vehicle, SB-334867) ANOVA revealed no significant effect of group (F(1,20) = 3.98, p > 0.05), or drug treatment (F(1,20) < 1, p > 0.05), and no significant group by drug treatment interaction (F(1,20) < 1, p > 0.05).
Cue Test
Figure 2b shows the number of pellets consumed during the cue test (tone presentations) for animals in the two Cue groups. Rats that received SB-334867 treatment before test ate significantly less food than those that received vehicle treatment. A two-way independent samples t-Test confirmed this reduction in cue-induced feeding by SB-334867 (t(10) = 2.31, p = 0.043).
Fos Induction
Fos-positive neurons were observed in all brain regions examined (Table 1). Data from the following were not collected due to tissue damage: one SB Cue (ACAd, PL, ILA, BMAp), and two VEH Home (BLAp, BMAp).
Table 1. Number of Fos-Positive Neurons for All Brain Regions Examined.
Brain Region | Cue |
Home |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Vehicle | SB-334867 | Vehicle | SB-334867 | |
ACAd2 | 217 (46) | 338 (30) | 155 (26) | 172 (40) |
PL1 | 177 (28) | 340 (17) | 188 (16) | 189 (38) |
ILA1 | 178 (33) | 314 (16) | 251 (21) | 209 (38) |
BLAa | 71 (15) | 79 (21) | 82 (21) | 61 (10) |
BLAp3 | 74 (14) | 76 (11) | 45 (13) | 58 (14) |
BMAp2 | 93 (8) | 103 (21) | 47 (14) | 60 (27) |
CEAl | 46 (9) | 54 (14) | 33 (11) | 40 (9) |
CEAm | 79 (11) | 78 (6) | 85 (18) | 80 (12) |
CEAc | 59 (3) | 68 (9) | 53 (7) | 57 (11) |
PVTa1 | 154 (29) | 241 (21) | 155 (22) | 148 (22) |
PVTp | 118 (18) | 119 (15) | 109 (12) | 132 (12) |
LHApf | 149 (23) | 176 (9) | 161 (23) | 141 (26) |
LHAl | 66 (16) | 61 (5) | 60 (14) | 44 (10) |
Fos counts are displayed as a mean total number for each area (SEM).
1Group by drug treatment interaction p < 0.01.
2Main effect of group (cue vs. home) p < 0.01.
3Main effect of group (cue vs. home) p = 0.07.
We observed a significant increase in Fos induction that was specific to the SB Cue group in three regions. This was evident as a significant group (Cue, Home) by drug treatment (vehicle, SB-334867) interaction in the ILA (F(1,19) = 10.91, p = 0.004), the PL (F(1,19) = 10.64, p = 0.004), and the PVTa (F(1,20) = 4.74, p = 0.042). Further analyses demonstrated that in each of these regions the SB Cue group had significantly greater Fos induction than the VEH Cue group (F values = 12.15, 20.73, 8.18, p values = 0.002, <0.0001, =0.010 respectively), but there were no such differences between the Home control groups (F values < 1.3, p values > 0.05). Figures 3 and 4 show the average total counts and representative photomicrographs from these three regions.
Exposure to the cue test increased Fos expression in the ACAd and BMAp, independent of drug treatment. In both of these regions a group (Cue, Home) by drug treatment (vehicle, SB-334867) ANOVA found a significant main effect of group (F values = 11.37, 10.24, p values = 0.003, 0.005 respectively), indicating higher levels of Fos induction in the Cue groups compared to the Home groups. There was no significant effect of drug treatment, or group by drug treatment interaction (F values < 4.2, p values > 0.05). The only other region to show this pattern of Fos induction was the BLAp, where the main effect of group approached significance (p = 0.07). There were no significant group differences detected in any of the other brain regions examined.
Discussion
Here, we showed that the OX1R antagonist SB-334867 decreased food intake driven by a food cue selectively, with no effect on baseline eating. In addition, we found elevated Fos induction within the mPFC (PL and ILA) and PVTa in rats tested for cue-induced feeding under SB-334867, compared to rats tested under vehicle. These results demonstrate that orexin signaling at OX1Rs is necessary for cue-induced feeding and that these prefrontal cortical and thalamic regions are important sites of its action.
These effects were specific to a decrease in motivation to eat, and not due to a non-selective effect of the drug on general arousal or locomotor activity. Orexin antagonists, including SB-334867, have been shown to produce sedative effects in rodents29. Nevertheless, the reduction in food intake we observed cannot be attributed to such properties of the drug. SB-334867 had no effect during the baseline test (Experiment 1), demonstrating that it did not affect the animals’ ability to locomote towards the food cup, or to retrieve or consume the food. The dose used (20 mg/kg) was chosen because it has significantly modulated reward motivated behaviors without affecting locomotor activity (e.g.,26), and is lower than the dose that produced motoric effects29. Therefore, the current data show that SB-334867 specifically reduced non-homeostatic feeding during the cue test, in agreement with the proposed orexin function to drive behavior under increased motivational states, translating motivation into action30,31,32.
We found increased Fos induction in the PL, ILA, and PVTa when cue-induced feeding was blocked by SB-334867. This effect was specific to reduction of feeding, and not simply due to a non-specific effect of the drug, because there was no similar increased Fos induction in home cage controls given SB-334867. Therefore, this change in activity reveals orexin action within mPFC and PVTa during cue-induced feeding.
This is the first demonstration that orexin signaling at OX1Rs in the mPFC may be important in mediating feeding behavior. Nevertheless, these findings complement other recent studies implicating PL and ILA in the control of feeding and reward-driven behaviors (e.g.,5,33,34,35). There was also a significant change in the PVTa, a region interconnected with key components of the cue-induced feeding network (PL, ILA, BLA/BMA, LHA)36,37. The PVT is important in negative motivation and affect as well as arousal, but it also mediates feeding and reward behaviors and associated motivation (see38,39). More specifically, a recent study demonstrated knockdown of OX1Rs in the PVT reduced over-consumption of a palatable food in sated rats24. Together, these results suggest that orexin is specifically acting within this network to drive non-homeostatic feeding. Further studies are needed to confirm whether PVTa is necessary for cue-induced feeding, and to determine the exact contribution of orexin signaling within these prefrontal and thalamic regions.
We did not observe significant differences in LHA Fos induction, which suggests that orexin is not acting locally during cue-induced feeding, in agreement with higher expression of OX2Rs than OX1Rs18. However, the LHA is a heterogeneous structure, and it is possible that a small subset of neurons mediates cue-induced feeding (e.g., orexin-containing neurons). Further work is needed to investigate this possibility.
Interestingly, here we found that systemic OX1R antagonism increased Fos expression in a restricted system of PL, ILA and PVTa neurons. This strongly suggests that SB-334867 removed inhibition within these areas, and in turn implies that during cue-induced feeding orexin signaling through OX1R inhibits these targets. This release from inhibition by SB-334867 could be achieved by either direct removal of inhibition (orexin action is inhibitory), or through disinhibition (orexin action is excitatory, but targeting GABAergic cells). The latter mechanism has been observed in recent studies and is therefore more likely. For example, Aracri and colleagues demonstrated that in the PFC, application of orexin excited fast-spiking interneurons, causing the release of GABA onto pyramidal cells40. Furthermore, this effect was specifically mediated by OX1Rs (also see41,42). These findings suggest that the increased Fos in the mPFC is the result of disinhibition.
Given that there are no inhibitory neurons in the rodent midline thalamus43, increased activation in PVT could arise from multiple sources, including disinhibited layer 5/6 mPFC pyramidal cells, other activated inputs (e.g., amygdala, LHA), or from local action of presynaptic OX1Rs. Furthermore, given that mPFC and PVTa are reciprocally connected44,45, these two regions could form a functional loop that would further enhance orexin action within this circuit.
Interestingly, even though SB-334867 treatment reduced cue-driven consumption, there was no corresponding suppression of Fos induction in any of the brain regions examined. This suggests that blockade of OX1R function did not prevent the activation of any of the critical neural sites investigated here. It is possible however, that additional structures important for aspects of cue-driven overeating were inhibited. Candidate areas include the nucleus accumbens (ACB), and the ventral pallidum (VP) (substantia innominata), where modulation of the orexin system alters feeding behavior and hedonic reactivity to food. Infusion of Orexin A into the ACB increased food consumption46,47, blockade of ACB opioid receptors prevented Orexin A-induced feeding48, and Orexin A infusion into the VP significantly increased positive ‘liking’ orofacial reactions to sucrose49. However, whether orexin signaling in the ACB and/or VP is critical for non-homeostatic eating driven by environmental learned cues requires further examination.
In summary, this study demonstrated that pharmacological OX1R blockade specifically decreased food intake driven by a learned food cue. This blockade of cue-induced feeding by OX1R antagonism also increased Fos induction in the mPFC (PL and ILA) and PVTa. These findings clearly show orexin signaling at OX1Rs is critical in cue-induced feeding, and suggest that orexin is acting through these cortical and thalamic sites to drive eating in the absence of hunger.
Methods
Animals
Experimentally naïve, male Long-Evans rats (275–300 g) from Charles River Laboratories (Portage, MI, USA) were used. The animals were individually housed with ad libitum access to food and water except when otherwise noted. The colony room was maintained at 21 °C on a 12 hour light/dark cycle (lights on 06:00) and all behavioral testing was conducted during the light phase of the cycle. The housing and testing procedures were in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guidelines for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the Boston College Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Apparatus
Training and testing were conducted in a set of behavioral chambers described previously50. The floor in each chamber was covered with a black Plexiglas insert. Training stimuli were a 150s tone (75 dB, 2 kHz), and 50 food pellets (5TUL, 45 mg; Test Diets, Richmond, IN, USA; 3.4 kcal/g; 20% protein, 13% fat, 67% carbohydrate) delivered to the food-cup of each chamber unless otherwise noted. The stimuli were controlled by the GraphicState 3.0 software system (Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA, USA).
Drugs
The OX1R antagonist SB-334867 (Tocris Bioscience, Ellisville, MO, USA), was dissolved in 10% (2-Hydroxypropyl)-β-cyclodextrin (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) and 2% dimethyl sulfoxide (Sigma-Aldrich) in sterile water, and administered (20 mg/kg) via intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection at a volume of 2 ml/kg.
Experiment 1: Effect of an OX1R antagonist on cue-induced feeding in sated rats
Animals were assigned to experimental group conditions, which were matched for baseline ad libitum chow consumption (measured twice for 24 hours) and body weight. A day prior to the start of training, chow was removed and all animals received 1 g of the food pellets in their home cage to familiarize them with the pellets. Animals remained food-restricted throughout training, with access to chow for one hour each day immediately following the completion of each training session.
Training
There were eight, 30-minute behavioral training sessions. During each session, rats in the Paired condition received two presentations of a tone. Each tone presentation overlapped and co-terminated with delivery of 50 food pellets. Rats in the Unpaired condition also received two presentations of the tone that overlapped with the sound of the food pump, but no food was delivered. 30–120 minutes following each session, the Unpaired group received food pellets in their home cage, with the timing and quantity matched to that given to the Paired group. Upon completion of training all rats were returned to ad libitum chow consumption for 3 days before testing commenced.
Test
All animals were tested across two consecutive days, under the OX1R antagonist SB-334867 and under vehicle, in a counterbalanced order. On each day animals received the assigned injection and then remained in home cages for 30 minutes. Testing then consisted of a baseline consumption test, followed by the cue test.
Baseline Test
Animals were placed into the behavioral chamber for 20 minutes with ad libitum access to 7 g of food pellets. The rats were briefly removed from the chamber while any remaining pellets were collected and weighed, and then placed back into the chamber for an additional 20 minutes with another 7 g of pellets. Again any remaining pellets were collected and weighed.
Cue Test
Immediately following completion of the baseline test, rats were placed back into the behavioral chamber for a cue test session (approximately 10 minutes). In this session they received three tones, each of which co-terminated with delivery of 50 food pellets. All rats were then returned to the colony room and any remaining food pellets were counted.
Experiment 2: Fos induction during cue-induced feeding under SB-334867
Training
All animals received training as described for the Paired condition in Experiment 1. Upon completion of training all rats were returned to ad libitum chow consumption for 3–4 days before testing commenced.
Test
Baseline Test
All animals underwent drug-free baseline testing as described in Experiment 1, except each exposure was 15 minutes.
Cue Test
Immediately following the baseline test animals were placed back into home cages, returned to the colony room, and injected with either SB-334867 or vehicle. 30 minutes later half of the animals were placed into the behavioral chamber for the cue test as described in Experiment 1, following which they were returned to the colony room in home cages with no access to food (SB Cue, VEH Cue). The other half of the animals remained in home cages to control for non-specific effects of the drug on neural activity (SB Home, VEH Home). For these animals chow was removed 30 minutes following injections to match the timing of chow removal for the Cue animals, and to prevent any Fos induction due to food consumption. Animals were perfused, and the brains removed, two hours from the beginning of the cue test or the equivalent time for Home controls.
Histological Procedures
Rats were anaesthetized by an i.p. injection of tribromoethanol (375 mg/kg body weight) and transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline followed by ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M borate buffer (pH 9.4). The brains were stored for 20–24 hours at 4 °C in the fixative with 12% sucrose and then rapidly frozen in hexanes (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) cooled with dry ice and stored at −80 °C. Frozen brains were cut into 30 μm coronal slices using a sliding microtome (Leica SM200R) and collected into four, serially adjacent, sets. One series of sections was used to identify Fos with standard immunohistochemistry. Free-floating sections were rinsed with potassium phosphate-buffered saline (KPBS), and then incubated with anti-c-fos antibody raised in rabbit (1:40 000, Ab-5, PC38; Calbiochem, San Diego, CA, USA) in a blocking solution of KPBS containing 2% normal goat serum (NGS) (S-1000; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA) and 0.3% Triton X-100 (Sigma–Aldrich) for 72 hours at 4 °C with gentle agitation. Sections were then brought to room temperature, rinsed with KPBS, incubated with biotinylated secondary antibody against rabbit (1:200, BA-1000; Vector Laboratories) in the blocking solution for 90 minutes, rinsed in KPBS, incubated in avidin biotin complex (ABC, PK-6100; Vector Laboratories) for 90 minutes and again rinsed in KPBS. Neurons immunoreactive for Fos were visualized as brown after a one-minute incubation in 3, 3’-diaminobenzidine (SK-4100; Vector Laboratories). Sections were then rinsed, mounted on SuperFrost slides (Thermo Fisher Scientific), dried at 40 °C, dehydrated through graded alcohols, cleared in xylenes, and coverslipped with DPX Mountant (Electron Microscopy Services, Hatfield, PA, USA). A second series of sections were mounted from KPBS onto chrome alum/gelatin-coated slides and stained with thionin for identification of cytoarchitectonic borders.
Image Acquisition and Analysis
The analysis followed parcellation and nomenclature as defined in the Swanson atlas (2004), except for the LHA and the PVT, and these exceptions are described below. Regions of interest were selected based on their involvement in non-homeostatic feeding and appetitive learning, and expression of OX1R. Image acquisition and neuronal counting followed procedures described previously50. Images were acquired bilaterally for each cell group analyzed and left and right counts were summed for each rat, then averaged for each group, resulting in a mean total of Fos-labeled neurons.
The following regions of interest were analyzed. Levels refer to the Swanson51 atlas level, and measurements refer to mm from bregma. Within the mPFC (Level 9, +2.80 mm) we analyzed the anterior cingulate area, dorsal part (ACAd), the prelimbic area (PL), and the infralimbic area (ILA). Within the amygdala, we analyzed the anterior (BLAa) (Level 27, −2.00 mm) and posterior (BLAp) (Level 30, −3.25 mm) parts of the BLA, the posterior part of the BMA (BMAp) (Level 30, −3.25 mm), and the medial (CEAm), lateral (CEAl), and capsular (CEAc) parts of the CEA (Level 27, −2.00 mm). For the LHA (Level 29, −2.85 mm) we defined two areas for analysis: the perifornical area (LHApf) that contained the juxtadorsomedial and suprafornical nuclei52,53, and the lateral area (LHAl), which included the region lateral to the LHApf, and was bordered dorsally by a line that matches the dorsal end of the third ventricle and ventrally by the ventral edge of the fornix. Within the PVT, we sampled from its anterior (PVTa) (Level 26, −1.78 mm) and posterior (PVTp) (Level 30, −3.25 mm) areas, separately.
Statistical Analysis
Body weight and food consumption data were analyzed using either independent sample t-Tests, or ANOVAs, as appropriate. Counts of Fos-positive neurons were analyzed for each structure using a two-way ANOVA with group (Cue, Home) and drug treatment (SB-334867, Vehicle) as between-subjects factors. In Experiment 1 two rats exhibited unusual baseline consumption at test (beyond 2 SEM), in Experiment 2 two rats failed to consistently consume all of the food pellets during training, and six exhibited unusual baseline eating at test (beyond 2 SEM); these rats were excluded from behavioral and neuronal analyses. The number of rats reported refers to those included in the statistical analyses. The statistical packages PSY54 and SPSS (v.21) were used and type I error rate was controlled at 0.05.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Cole, S. et al. Orexin/Hypocretin-1 Receptor Antagonism Selectively Reduces Cue-Induced Feeding in Sated Rats and Recruits Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Thalamus. Sci. Rep. 5, 16143; doi: 10.1038/srep16143 (2015).
Acknowledgments
Supported by NIDDK (DK085721) to GDP. We thank Michael Hobin for technical support.
Footnotes
Author Contributions S.C. and G.D.P. designed the experiments, S.C. and H.S.M. conducted the experimental procedures, S.C., H.S.M. and G.D.P. contributed to the manuscript and reviewed the completed manuscript.
References
- Birch L. L, McPhee L., Sullivan S. & Johnson S. Conditioned meal initiation in young children. Appetite 13, 105–113 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornell C. E., Rodin J. & Weingarten H. Stimulus-induced eating when satiated. Physiol. Behav. 45, 695–704 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrovich G. D. Forebrain networks and the control of feeding by environmental learned cues. Physiol. Behav. 121, 10–18 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrovich G. D., Setlow B., Holland P. C. & Gallagher M. Amygdalo-hypothalamic circuit allows learned cues to override satiety and promote eating. J. Neurosci. 22, 8748–8753 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrovich G. D., Ross C. A., Holland P. C. & Gallagher M. Medial prefrontal cortex is necessary for an appetitive contextual conditioned stimulus to promote eating in sated rats. J. Neurosci. 27, 6436–6441 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrovich G. D., Holland P. C. & Gallagher M. Amygdalar and prefrontal pathways to the lateral hypothalamus are activated by a learned cue that stimulates eating. J. Neurosci. 25, 8295–8302 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Lecea L. et al. The hypocretins: Hypothalamus-specific peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 322–327 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakurai T. et al. Orexins and orexin receptors: a family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell 92, 573–585 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyron C. et al. Neurons containing hypocretin (orexin) project to multiple neuronal systems. J. Neurosci. 18, 9996–10015 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers R. J. et al. Dose-response effects of orexin-A on food intake and the behavioural satiety sequence in rats. Regul. Pept. 96, 71–84 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi D. L., Davis J. F., Fitzgerald M. E. & Benoit S. C. The role of orexin-A in food motivation, reward-based feeding behavior and food-induced neuronal activation in rats. Neuroscience 167, 11–20 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris G. C., Wimmer M. & Aston-Jones G. A role for lateral hypothalamic orexin neurons in reward seeking. Nature 437, 556–559 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrovich G. D., Hobin M. P. & Reppucci C. J. Selective Fos induction in hypothalamic orexin/hypocretin, but not melanin-concentrating hormone neurons, by a learned food-cue that stimulates feeding in sated rats. Neuroscience 224, 70–80 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nair S. G., Golden S. A. & Shaham Y. Differential effects of the hypocretin 1 receptor antagonist SB 334867 on high-fat food self-administration and reinstatement of food seeking in rats. Br. J. Pharmacol. 154, 406–416 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cason A. M. & Aston-Jones G. Role of orexin/hypocretin in conditioned sucrose-seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 226, 155–165 (2013a). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cason A. M. & Aston-Jones G. Attenuation of saccharin-seeking in rats by orexin/hypocretin receptor 1 antagonist. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 228, 499–507 (2013b). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgland S. L. et al. Orexin A/hypocretin-1 selectively promotes motivation for positive reinforcers. J. Neurosci. 29, 11215–11225 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcus J. N. et al. Differential expression of orexin receptors 1 and 2 in the rat brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 435, 6–25 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trivedi P., Yu H., MacNeil D. J., Van der Ploeg L. H. & Guan X. M. Distribution of orexin receptor mRNA in the rat brain. FEBS Lett. 438, 71–75 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akanmu M. A. & Honda K. Selective stimulation of orexin receptor type 2 promotes wakefulness in freely behaving rats. Brain Res. 1048, 138–145 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cason A. M. et al. Role of orexin/hypocretin in reward-seeking and addiction: implications for obesity. Physiol. Behav. 100. 419–428 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willie J. T. et al. Distinct narcolepsy syndromes in Orexin receptor-2 and Orexin null mice: molecular genetic dissection of Non-REM and REM sleep regulatory processes. Neuron 38, 715–730 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haynes A. C. et al. A selective orexin-1 receptor antagonist reduces food consumption in male and female rats. Regul. Pept. 96, 45–51 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi D. L. et al. Orexin signaling in the paraventricular thalamic nucleus modulates mesolimbic dopamine and hedonic feeding in the rat. Neuroscience 210, 243–248 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharf R. et al. Orexin signaling via the orexin 1 receptor mediates operant responding for food reinforcement. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 753–760 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards J. K. et al. Inhibition of orexin-1/hypocretin-1 receptors inhibits yohimbine-induced reinstatement of ethanol and sucrose seeking in Long-Evans rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199, 3109–117 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitt O. et al. Orexinergic innervation of the extended amygdala and basal ganglia in the rat. Brain Struct. Funct. 217, 233–256 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risco S. & Mediavilla C. Orexin-1 receptor antagonist in central nucleus of the amygdala attenuates the acquisition of flavor-taste preference in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 126, 7–12 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers R. J. et al. SB-334867, a selective orexin-1 receptor antagonist, enhances behavioural satiety and blocks the hyperphagic effect of orexin-A in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 1444–1452 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saper C. B. Staying awake for dinner: hypothalamic integration of sleep, feeding, and circadian rhythms. Prog. Brain Res. 153, 243–252 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahler S.V., Moorman D.E., Smith R.J., James M.H. & Aston-Jones G. J. Motivational activation: a unifying hypothesis of orexin/hypocretin function. Nat Neurosci. 17, 1298–303. (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakurai T. The role of orexin in motivated behaviours. Nat Rev Neurosci. 15, 719–31. (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgos-Robles A., Bravo-Rivera H. & Quirk G. J. Prelimbic and infralimbic neurons signal distinct aspects of appetitive instrumental behavior. PLoS ONE 8, e57575. 10.1371/journal.pone.0057575 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calu D. J. et al. Optogenetic inhibition of dorsal medial prefrontal cortex attenuates stress-induced reinstatement of palatable food seeking in female rats. J. Neurosci. 33, 214–226 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nair S. G. et al. Role of dorsal medial prefrontal cortex dopamine D1-family receptors in relapse to high-fat food seeking induced by the anxiogenic drug yohimbine. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 497–510 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Werf Y. D., Witter M. P. & Groenewegen H. J. The intralaminar and midline nuclei of the thalamus. Anatomical and functional evidence for participation in processes of arousal and awareness. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 39, 107–140 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes R. P., Linley S. B. & Hoover W. B. Limbic circuitry of the midline thalamus. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. e-pub ahead of print 20 January 2015. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.014 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colavito V., Tesoriero C., Wirtu A. T., Grassi-Zucconi G. & Bentivoglio M. Limbic thalamus and state-dependent behavior: The paraventricular nucleus of the thalamic midline as a node in circadian timing and sleep/wake-regulatory networks. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. e-pub ahead of print 3 December 2014. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.021 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu D. T., Kirouac G. J., Zubieta J. K. & Bhatnagar S. Contributions of the paraventricular thalamic nucleus in the regulation of stress, motivation, and mood. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 73. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00073 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aracri P., Banfi D., Pasini M. E., Amadeo A. & Becchetti A. Hypocretin (orexin) regulates glutamate input to fast-spiking interneurons in layer V of the Fr2 region of the murine prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex; e-pub ahead of print 1 December 2013. 10.1093/cercor/bht326. (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Burdakov D., Liss B. & Ashcroft F.M. Orexin excites GABAergic neurons of the arcuate nucleus by activating the sodium—calcium exchanger. J. Neurosci. 23, 4951–4957 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu M. et al. Hypocretin increases impulse flow in the septohippocampal GABAergic pathway: implications for arousal via a mechanism of hippocampal disinhibition. J. Neurosci. 22, 7754–7765 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcelli P., Frassoni C., Regondi M. C., De Biasi S. & Spreafico R. GABAergic neurons in mammalian thalamus: a marker of thalamic complexity? Brain Res. Bull. 42, 27–37 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berendse H. W. & Groenewegen H. J. Restricted cortical termination fields of the midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei in the rat. Neuroscience 42, 73–102 (1991). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S. & Kirouac G. J. Sources of inputs to the anterior and posterior aspects of the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus. Brain Struct. Funct. 217, 257–273 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayannavar S., Rashmi K. S., Rao Y. D., Yadav S. & Ganaraja B. Effect of Orexin-A infusion in to the Nucleus Accumbens on consummatory behaviour and alcohol preference in male Wistar rats. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 58, 319–326 (2014). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe A. J. & Kotz C.M. Orexin A in the nucleus accumbens stimulates feeding and locomotor activity. Brain Res. 1050, 156–162 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sweet D. C., Levine A. S. & Kotz C. M. Functional opioid pathways are necessary for hypocretin-1 (orexin-A)-induced feeding. Peptides. 25, 307–314 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ho C. Y. & Berridge K. C. An orexin hotspot in ventral pallidum amplifies hedonic ‘liking’ for sweetness. Neuropsychopharmacology. 38, 1655–1664 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole S., Powell D. J. & Petrovich G. D. Differential recruitment of distinct amygdalar nuclei across appetitive associative learning. Learn. Mem. 20, 295–299 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson L. W. Brain Maps: Structure of the Rat Brain (Elsevier, 2004). [Google Scholar]
- Hahn J. D. & Swanson L. W. Distinct patterns of neuronal inputs and outputs of the juxtaparaventricular and suprafornical regions of the lateral hypothalamic area in the male rat. Brain. Res. Rev. 64, 14–103 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn J. D. & Swanson L. W. Connections of the lateral hypothalamic area juxtadorsomedial region in the male rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 1831–1890 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bird K. D. Analysis of variance via confidence intervals (Sage, 2004). [Google Scholar]